Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Psychol. 2015 Jun 1;3:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.004

Culture and emotion regulation

Brett Q Ford 1, Iris B Mauss 1
PMCID: PMC4341898  NIHMSID: NIHMS652042  PMID: 25729757

Abstract

While anthropological research has long emphasized cultural differences in whether emotions are viewed as beneficial versus harmful, psychological science has only recently begun to systematically examine those differences and their implications for emotion regulation and well-being. Underscoring the pervasive role of culture in people’s emotions, we summarize research that has examined links between culture, emotion regulation, and well-being. Specifically, we focus on two questions. First, how does culture lead individuals to regulate their emotions? And second, how does culture modulate the link between emotion regulation and well-being? We finish by suggesting directions for future research to advance the study of culture and emotion regulation.

Introduction

Anthropological research has long emphasized cultural differences in how emotions are viewed, often focusing on the fundamental distinction of seeing emotions as beneficial versus harmful [13]. Psychological science has only more recently begun to systematically examine those differences and their implications for emotion regulation (i.e., how individuals modify their own emotional experiences and expressions [4]). In our review, we integrate these two approaches, suggesting that how cultures view emotions critically shapes whether individuals engage in emotion regulation and whether that emotion regulation is adaptive [5].

How does culture influence individuals?

Culture – patterns of historically derived and selected ideas and their embodiment in institutions, practices, and artifacts [6] – pervasively influences how individuals think, feel, and behave. One framework often employed to characterize this influence focuses on the extent to which a culture promotes interdependence (where individuals define themselves more on relationships and prioritize harmony with others) versus independence (where individuals define themselves more on unique attributes and prioritize distinguishing themselves from others) [7]. While these value dimensions represent just one example of the many values that vary among cultural groups, we focus on them in the present review because they are fundamental to how individuals conceive of themselves and their emotions and thus, have clear links to emotion regulation. We focus on East Asian heritage (e.g., Japan or China; Asian Americans) as an example of a relatively interdependent context, and European heritage (e.g., northern or western Europe; European-Americans) as an example of a relatively independent context. We focus on these groups because interdependence and independence have been particularly clearly instantiated within them and because these groups have dominated the literature on culture and emotion regulation. We build our review on the hypothesis that the extent to which a cultural group promotes independence versus interdependence entails a particular understanding of the harmfulness of emotions and, in turn, whether they should be regulated.

Culture shapes whether individuals are motivated to regulate their emotions

Most fundamentally, culture should influence whether people are motivated to regulate their emotions. Theoretically, because emotions are powerful internal experiences that can both assert someone’s individuality and potentially disrupt social harmony [8] (e.g., anger can be used to assert one’s opinion, but it may also make others uncomfortable), members of interdependent cultures should be motivated to regulate their emotions more readily than members of independent cultures [9]. Supporting this basic idea, Asian Americans reported using emotion regulation more frequently [10] and reported a stronger preference for emotion regulation (e.g. “people in general should control their emotions more”) compared to European Americans [11]. Suggesting that these preferences may translate to actual emotion regulation, Asian Americans experienced and facially expressed less anger than European Americans in a standardized laboratory anger provocation, and this effect of cultural group was mediated by Asian-Americans’ stronger preferences for emotion regulation [11]. Thus, initial evidence suggests that culture shapes the extent to which individuals are motivated to initiate emotion regulation, and perhaps whether emotion regulation is likely to take place.

While some research has assessed cultural differences in the motivation to regulate emotion in general, much of the research on cultural differences in emotion regulation – by a wide margin – has focused on cultural differences in using the emotion regulation strategy of expressive suppression. This strategy involves inhibiting the outward expression of an ongoing emotion and is often assessed with items like “I control my emotions by not expressing them”. Numerous studies have demonstrated that individuals from Asian backgrounds (e.g., Hong Kong Chinese, Japanese, and Asian Americans) are more likely to report using suppression than individuals from European backgrounds [1216]. When using countries as the unit of analysis, samples from countries higher (vs. lower) on interdependence (Hong Kong vs. Canada) also reported higher levels of suppression [14].

Importantly, it is not simply membership in a cultural group that should shape whether someone is motivated to regulate their emotions. Rather, it is the extent to which an individual is oriented toward a particular culture’s values that should predict their emotion regulation. Consequently, even within a cultural group, engagement in and sensitivity to one’s cultural context – and the values embedded in it – should be associated with emotion regulation. Supporting this idea, the extent to which Asian American or European American participants endorsed Asian vs. European American cultural values predicted their use of suppression more strongly than their cultural group membership [9]. Similarly, Koreans who were more (vs. less) genetically sensitive to their social environments (GG carriers of the oxytocin receptor polymorphism) were more likely to use suppression, whereas Americans who were genetically more (vs. less) sensitive to their social environments were less likely to use suppression [17]. Overall, it appears that individuals oriented toward interdependent cultural values – and not necessarily individuals of a particular racial or genetic background – are more motivated to regulate their emotions using suppression, whereas the reverse is true for individuals oriented toward independent cultural values.

Culture shapes whether emotion regulation is adaptive

The above review suggests that culture shapes whether individuals are motivated to regulate their emotions. Once underway, culture may also shape the adaptiveness of that emotion regulation (i.e., whether emotion regulation is good or bad for a person’s well-being) [18]. Because culture reinforces behaviors that promote culturally-supported values [5], behaviors that are consistent with a culture’s values may become more practiced (and thus easier to implement) and more socially-rewarded, both of which may lead to greater well-being. Thus, emotion regulation may be adaptive when it is consistent with its cultural context, and maladaptive when it is inconsistent. Supporting the notion that culturally-consistent emotion regulation is more positively valued, Chinese individuals (but not European Americans) associate suppression with interpersonal harmony [19]. On the other hand, European Americans (but not Chinese individuals) associate suppression with experiential avoidance [20].

This analysis casts doubt on the all-but-axiomatic view that suppressing one’s emotions is maladaptive. Suppression has been associated with worse psychological health [12, 21, 22], physical health [23], and social functioning [16, 24]. However, this research either focused on American samples or did not take into consideration participants’ culture. Our culturally-grounded analysis suggests that in interdependent contexts, suppression is not necessarily maladaptive, and may even be adaptive.

Recent research provides some support for this idea. Several studies have shown that while suppression is linked with worse well-being for individuals from independent cultural backgrounds, this negative effect is significantly weaker for individuals from interdependent cultural backgrounds [9, 25, 26]. For example, in an experiment where participants were instructed to suppress their emotions while discussing an upsetting film with a stranger, suppressors who were relatively higher in Asian cultural values (vs. European values) were viewed as less hostile by their interaction partners and were subsequently treated with less hostility from that partner [9].

In even stronger support of the notion that suppression is less harmful in interdependent cultural contexts, research has shown that suppression is linked to worse functioning for individuals from independent cultural backgrounds but is unrelated to psychological and social functioning for individuals from interdependent backgrounds [15, 27, 28]. For example, when instructed to suppress their emotions in response to negative images, European Americans exhibited a pronounced parietal late positive potential event-related potential signal – an index of heightened emotional processing – but an Asian sample exhibited a significant reduction in this signal and the signal was completely attenuated within a matter of seconds [29].

Some evidence suggests that suppression can even be beneficial for interdependent individuals. During a negative emotion induction, a stronger preference to regulate emotions led to a more adaptive pattern of physiological responding in Asian-American cultural contexts, while a stronger preference to regulate emotions led to a maladaptive pattern of physiological responding in European-American cultural contexts [30]. Moreover, individuals who identified as highly interdependent had higher well-being and relationship satisfaction when they suppressed negative emotions during a sacrifice for their romantic partner, while those who were lower on interdependence reported lower well-being and relationship satisfaction when they suppressed negative emotions [31]. Furthermore, Chinese-American children who used suppression in response to peer-induced stressors had better mood, but only when they were more oriented toward Chinese (vs. American) culture [32].

Of note, some studies found that suppression is equally harmful for individuals from both interdependent and independent cultures [16, 33]. Overall, then, empirical research largely supports the hypothesis that suppression has fewer well-being costs for individuals from interdependent (vs. independent) cultures. However, the precise nature of this effect varies across studies, with some studies showing less or no costs in interdependent contexts, some showing benefits of suppression in interdependent contexts, and some showing no moderating effect of culture on the well-being effects of suppression.

It is unclear exactly what factors account for this pattern because relatively few studies exist to draw inferences from. However, most published studies demonstrating an adaptive (or less maladaptive) side of suppression either employed an Asian sample or assessed cultural values directly. Conversely, published studies demonstrating no moderation by culture compared Asian Americans with European Americans. This pattern highlights the nuanced nature of the links between culture, emotion regulation, and well-being and underscores the importance of recruiting participants from different countries to capture subtle effects, or – perhaps more importantly – directly measuring cultural orientation and values. When culture is operationalized with precision, there is a robust pattern such that suppression carries fewer costs for more interdependent compared to independent individuals.

Conclusions and directions for future research

The present review summarized recent research suggesting that culture shapes (1) whether individuals are motivated to regulate their emotions and (2) the adaptiveness of emotion regulation. We believe several directions for future research are especially promising.

First, given robust cultural differences in preferences for specific emotions, it is striking that very little research on cultural differences in emotion regulation has considered the emotion being targeted. For example, independent (vs. interdependent) cultures more strongly value positive emotions (especially high-arousal positive emotions like excitement [34]) [3537] and more highly devalue negative emotions [3739]. Critically, valuing an emotion should lead to attempts to increase that emotion through emotion regulation and devaluing an emotion should lead to attempts to decrease that emotion [40]. Thus, the general conclusion that interdependent individuals value emotion regulation more than independent individuals may need to be qualified depending on the emotion being targeted.

Second, cross-cultural research has largely focused on an emotion-regulation strategy that targets emotional behavior (i.e., suppression). This focus may be due to emotional behavior directly and visibly promoting versus conflicting with cultural values. But what about other emotion-regulation strategies? A few studies have examined reappraisal (cognitively re-evaluating an emotional situation to change its emotional impact). Unlike suppression, the frequency of using reappraisal largely does not differ across cultures [12, 1417, 26]. This could be because reappraisal targets the less visible internal experience of emotion, and thus reappraisal may be equally important across cultures as individuals discreetly up- or down-regulate any culturally-valued emotion. At the same time, some evidence suggests that individuals from interdependent cultures may benefit more from using reappraisal [26], perhaps because adjusting one’s emotions to the social environment is more important in this cultural context. The research on reappraisal suggests that different emotion–regulation strategies likely operate quite differently in different cultures. More research is required to understand these differences in a wider range of emotion-regulation strategies (e.g., social support [41], attentional focus [42], automatic emotion regulation [43]).

Third, much of the available research has focused on two dimensions of cultural values (independence and interdependence) and their instantiation in two cultural groups (Asians and Asian-Americans compared to European Americans). Although there is a strong foundation for this focus, it will be important to expand this research to other cultural value systems (e.g., hierarchy [14], tradition [44]) and forms of culture [45] (e.g., socioeconomic status [46], region [47]).

Finally, it will be useful to move beyond documenting that cultural differences exist, and focus on locating mechanisms behind these cultural differences. How does culture transmit values that shape emotion regulation and its outcomes? What are these values? Similarly, what are the origins of these cultural differences? One recent study suggests, for example, that China’s regional differences in agriculture (the requirements of rice versus wheat farming, specifically) have promoted different cultural values (interdependence versus independence, respectively), within different regions of the country [48]. Answering these questions becomes increasingly important as rising rates of globalization and multiculturalism shape emotion regulation and its outcomes across the world.

Highlights.

  • Culture shapes whether individuals are motivated to regulate their emotions

  • Culture shapes whether emotion regulation is adaptive or maladaptive

  • Nuanced cultural analysis will advance our understanding of emotion regulation

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a National Institutes of Health Affective Science Training Fellowship (5T32MH020006) awarded to B.Q.F.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Lutz C, White GM. The anthropology of emotions. Annual Review of Anthropology. 1986;15:405–436. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Shweder R. You're not sick, you're just in love": Emotion as an interpretive system. In: Ekman P, Davidson RJ, editors. The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford University Press; 1994. pp. 32–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wierzbicka A. Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. In: Kitayama S, Markus HR, editors. Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association; 1994. pp. 133–196. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. 2nd Ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2014. pp. 3–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Mesquita B, De Leersnyder J, Albert D. The cultural regulation of emotions. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. 2nd Edition. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2014. pp. 284–304. ** This chapter comprehensively reviews a cultural perspective on emotion regulation, highlighting foundational psychological research and key anthropological research often ignored by psychological science.
  • 6.Kroeber AL, Kluckholm CK. Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. New York: Random House; 1952. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review. 1991;98(2):224–253. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Matsumoto D. Cultural similarities and differences in dispay rules. Motivation and Amotion. 1990;14(3):195–214. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Butler EA, Lee TL, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and culture: are the social consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion. 2007;7(1):30–48. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Gross JJ, Richards JM, John OP. Emotion regulation in everyday life. In: Snyder DK, Simpson JA, Hughes JN, editors. Emotion regulation in families: Pathways to dysfunction and health. Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mauss IB, et al. Emotion control values and responding to an anger-provocation in Asian-American and European-American individuals. Cognition and Emotion. 2010;24(6):1026–1043. doi: 10.1080/02699930903122273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85(2):348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Matsumoto D. Are cultural differences in emotion regulation mediated by personality traits? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2006;37(4):421–437. [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Matsumoto D, Yoo SH, Nakagawa S. Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008;94(6):925–37. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.925. * This empirical paper is unique in its focus on country-level differences in emotion regulation as a function of country-level characteristics: Even when using countries as the unit of analysis, samples from countries higher (vs. lower) on interdependence (Hong Kong vs. Canada) reported higher levels of suppression.
  • 15.Soto JA, et al. Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion. 2011;11(6):1450–1455. doi: 10.1037/a0023340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. English T, John OP. Understanding the social effects of emotion regulation: the mediating role of authenticity for individual differences in suppression. Emotion. 2013;13(2):314–329. doi: 10.1037/a0029847. * This empirical paper provides support for a mechanism in the link between suppression and worse well-being outcomes for individuals from independent cultural backgrounds (i.e., the sense of not being authentic and true to inner feelings and beliefs). This paper is also an example of an investigation that did not find evidence for culture moderating the link between suppression and adaptive outcomes.
  • 17. Kim HS, et al. Gene-culture interaction: Oxytocin Receptor Polymorphism (OXTR) and emotion regulation. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2011;2(6):665–672. * This empirical paper represents the developing field of gene-by-culture interactions: Koreans who were more (vs. less) genetically sensitive to their social environments (GG carriers of the oxytocin receptor polymorphism) were more likely to use suppression, whereas Americans who were genetically more (vs. less) sensitive to their social environments were less likely to use suppression. This investigation helps clarify the potential biological underpinnings of cultural differences, and in this case, suggests that biological sensitivities to culture may be one path through which culture shapes emotion regulation.
  • 18.Butler EA. Emotion regulation in cultural context: Implications for wellness and health. In: Barnow S, Balkir N, editors. Cultural variations in psychopathology: From research to practice. Hogrefe Publishing; 2012. pp. 93–114. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wei M, et al. Suppression and interpersonal harmony: a cross-cultural comparison between Chinese and European Americans. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2013;60(4):625–633. doi: 10.1037/a0033413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Su JC, Wei M, Tsai H-TT. Running away from unwanted feelings: culture matters. Cognition and Emotion. 2014;28(7):1313–1327. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.881322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kashdan TB, et al. Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44(9):1301–1320. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(2):217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Mauss IB, Gross JJ. Emotion suppression and cardiovascular disease: Is hiding feelings bad for your heart? In: Nyklicek I, Temoshok L, Vingerhoets A, editors. Emotional expression and health: Advances in theory, assessment and clinical applications. New York: Brunner-Routledge; 2004. pp. 61–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Srivastava S, et al. The social costs of emotional suppression: A prospective study of the transition to college. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2009;96(4):883–897. doi: 10.1037/a0014755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cheung RY, Park IJK. Anger suppression, interdependent self-construal, and depression among Asian American and European American college students. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2010;16(4):517–525. doi: 10.1037/a0020655. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kwon H, et al. Cultural and gender differences in emotion regulation: relation to depression. Cognition and Emotion. 2013;27(5):769–782. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.792244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lee E, et al. Is emotion suppression that bad? Comparing the emotion suppression and subjective well-being link in two cultures. Korean Journal of Social and Personality Psychology. 2009;23(1):131–146. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Su JC, Lee RM, Oishi S. The role of culture and self-construal in the link between expressive suppression and depressive symptoms. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2012;44(2):316–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Murata A, Moser JS, Kitayama S. Culture shapes electrocortical responses during emotion suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8(5):595–601. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Mauss IB, Butler EA. Cultural context moderates the relationship between emotion control values and cardiovascular challenge versus threat responses. Biological Psychology. 2010;84(3):521–530. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.010. *This empirical paper illustrates that culture moderates the link between emotion regulation and adaptive outcomes: In response to an experimental anger provocation, Asian Americans who were more motivated to control their emotions showed a “challenge” pattern of physiological responding characteristic of healthy physiological responding, whereas European Americans who were more motivated to control their emotions showed a “threat” pattern of physiological responding more characteristic of harmful physiological responding.
  • 31. Le BM, Impett EA. When holding back helps: suppressing negative emotions during sacrifice feels authentic and is beneficial for highly interdependent people. Psychological Science. 2013;24(9):1809–1815. doi: 10.1177/0956797613475365. *This empirical paper highlights the importance of both assessing within-culture variation in cultural values and assessing potential mechanisms in the link between cultural differences and outcomes, rather than simply documenting a cultural difference: whereas individuals who identified as highly interdependent had higher well-being and relationship satisfaction when they suppressed negative emotions more (vs. less) during a sacrifice for their romantic partner, those who were lower on interdependence reported lower well-being and relationship satisfaction when they suppressed their negative emotions, and these cultural differences were accounted for by differences in feelings of authenticity.
  • 32.Huang K, Leong FTL. Coping with peer stressors and associated dysphoria: Acculturation differences among Chinese-American children. Counselling Psychology Quarterly. 1994;7(1):53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Roberts NA, Levenson RW, Gross JJ. Cardiovascular costs of emotion suppression cross ethnic lines. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2008;70(1):82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Tsai JL, Knutson B, Fung HH. Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;90(2):288–307. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Joshanloo M, et al. Cross-cultural validation of fear of happiness scale across 14 national groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2013;45(2):246–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Miyamoto Y, Ma X. Dampening or savoring positive emotions: a dialectical cultural script guides emotion regulation. Emotion. 2011;11(6):1346–1357. doi: 10.1037/a0025135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Miyamoto Y, Ma X, Petermann AG. Cultural differences in hedonic emotion regulation after a negative event. Emotion. 2014;14(4):804–815. doi: 10.1037/a0036257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chentsova-Dutton YE, Tsai JL. Self-focused attention and emotional reactivity: the role of culture. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010;98(3):507–519. doi: 10.1037/a0018534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Koopmann-Holm B, Tsai JL. Focusing on the negative: Cultural differences in expressions of sympathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology doi: 10.1037/a0037684. , in press. * This empirical paper represents a relatively small but crucially important literature on the objects and artifacts embedded in our cultures that serve both as an instantiation and a mechanism of culturally-supported values: Mass-produced American condolence cards were more likely to express negative emotion avoidance (“hold onto hope”), compared to German cards (“sharing your sorrow”), reflecting that Americans are particularly likely to want to reduce their experience of negative emotions.
  • 40.Tamir M, Mauss IB. Social cognitive factors in emotion regulation: Implications for well-being. In: Nyklicek AVI, Zeelenberg M, Denollet J, editors. Emotion regulation and well-being. Springer; 2011. pp. 31–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kim HS, Sherman DK, Taylor SE. Culture and social support. Americal Psychologist. 2008;63(6):518–526. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Masuda T, et al. Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008;94(3):365–381. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.3.365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mauss IB, Bunge SA, Gross JJ. Culture and automatic emotion regulation. In: Ismer S, et al., editors. Regulating emotions: Culture, social necessity and biological inheritance. London: Blackwell Publishing; 2008. pp. 39–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bardi A, Schwartz SH. Values and behavior: strength and structure of relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2003;29(10):1207–1220. doi: 10.1177/0146167203254602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Cohen AB. Many forms of culture. American Psychologist. 2009;64(3):194–204. doi: 10.1037/a0015308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Snibbe AC, Markus HR. You can't always get what you want: educational attainment, agency, and choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88(4):703–720. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cohen D, et al. Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An "experimental ethnography". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70(5):945–960. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.945. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Talhelm T, et al. Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science. 2014;344:603–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES