Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Biol. 2014 Aug 29;20(5):956–967. doi: 10.1111/adb.12180

Serial longitudinal MRI data indicates non-linear regional gray matter volume recovery in abstinent alcohol dependent individuals

Timothy C Durazzo a,b,*, Anderson Mon a,b, Stefan Gazdzinski a,b, Ping-Hong Yeh a,b, Dieter J Meyerhoff a,b
PMCID: PMC4345147  NIHMSID: NIHMS621014  PMID: 25170881

Abstract

The trajectory of regional volume changes during the first year of sustained abstinence in those recovering from an alcohol use disorder is unclear because previous research typically employed only two assessment points. To better understand the trajectory of regional brain volume recovery in treatment-seeking alcohol dependent individuals (ALC), regional brain volumes were measured after 1-week, 1-month, and 7.5-months of sustained abstinence, via magnetic resonance imaging at 1.5 Tesla. ALC showed significant volume increases in frontal, parietal, and occipital gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM), total cortical GM and total lobar WM, thalamus and cerebellum, and decreased ventricular volume over 7.5-months of abstinence. Volume increases in regional GM were significantly greater over 1-week to 1-month than from 1-month to 7.5-months of abstinence, indicating a non-linear rate of change in regional GM over 7.5-months. Overall, regional lobar WM showed linear volume increases over 7.5-months. With increasing age, smoking ALC showed lower frontal and total cortical GM volume recovery than non-smoking ALC. Despite significant volume increases, ALC showed smaller GM volumes in all regions, except the frontal cortex, than controls after 7.5-months of abstinence. ALC and controls showed no regional WM volume differences at any assessment point. In non-smoking ALC only, increasing regional GM and WM volumes were related to improving processing speed. Findings may indicate a differential rate of recovery of cell types/cellular components contributing to GM and WM volume during early abstinence, and that GM volume deficits persist after 7.5-months of sustained sobriety in this ALC cohort.

Keywords: alcohol use disorders, brain volume, cigarette smoking, cognition, recovery

INTRODUCTION

Magnetic resonance (MR)-based neuroimaging studies of treatment-seeking alcohol dependent individuals (ALC), during early abstinence from alcohol (i.e., 1-week to 1-month of abstinence), have consistently shown regional gray matter (GM) volume loss that is most prominent in the anterior frontal lobe, posterior parietal lobe, cingulate gyrus, insula, hippocampus, and cerebellum (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Demirakca et al., 2011; Durazzo et al., 2014b; van Eijk et al., 2012). White matter (WM) volume loss in early abstinent ALC during is reported in all four lobes, particularly in the frontal WM (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Monnig et al., 2013). Longitudinal studies indicate treatment-seeking ALC demonstrate variable levels of regional GM and WM volume recovery during the first year of sustained abstinence (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Monnig et al., 2013; van Eijk et al., 2012). Although treatment-seeking ALC show regional volume recovery with sustained sobriety, most ALC continue to demonstrate significantly smaller volumes in multiple brain regions after 6–12 months of abstinence compared to controls (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Monnig et al., 2013).

Investigations of brain volume recovery with sobriety have typically employed two assessment points - a baseline study after approximately 1-month of abstinence and a follow-up study after 6–12 months of abstinence, where only linear rates of volumes changes could be assessed (Buhler and Mann, 2011). Consequently, little is known about the actual trajectory of regional volume changes during the first year of abstinence. Some studies suggest that treatment-seeking ALC experience more rapid brain volume increases over the first month of sobriety than during later months of sustained abstinence (Agartz et al., 2003; Gazdzinski et al., 2005; Mon et al., 2011; Pfefferbaum et al., 1995; Yeh et al., 2007), which indicates volume change is not necessarily linear over the first year of sobriety. Additionally, few studies have examined associations between changes in brain volume and neurocognition during early abstinence, therefore the functional relevance of volume changes during this period are unclear. Differential rates of change in brain morphology over the first year of abstinence may be clinically relevant because ALC with the smallest regional cortical GM volume, particularly in anterior frontal regions, at approximately 1-week (Cardenas et al., 2011; Durazzo et al., 2011b) and 1-month (Rando et al., 2010) of abstinence had an increased relapse risk within the year following treatment. Furthermore, ALC with the lowest processing speed at 1-month of abstinence showed a significantly increased risk for relapse following treatment (Durazzo et al., 2008).

The rate and extent of brain volume recovery in abstinent ALC may be influenced by age, sex, diet/nutrition, genetic factors, and comorbid medical, psychiatric, substance use disorders, and chronic cigarette smoking (Durazzo et al., 2007b; Mon et al., 2013; Oscar-Berman and Marinkovic, 2007). It is well established that medical, psychiatric, and substance use disorders, and cigarette smoking, are highly prevalent in alcohol use disorders (Durazzo and Meyerhoff, 2007; Hasin et al., 2007; Mertens et al., 2005; Moss et al., 2010). Cigarette smoking in non-clinical samples (Durazzo et al., 2013; Durazzo et al., 2010, 2012) and in alcohol use disorders (Durazzo et al., 2007a; Durazzo et al., 2011a; Durazzo et al., 2014b; Gazdzinski et al., 2010; Gazdzinski et al., 2008; Luhar et al., 2013) is associated with significant morphological abnormalities, primarily in the anterior frontal, posterior parietal, and mesial temporal regions. Serial assessment of the influence of the foregoing variables on brain morphological changes during abstinence will assist in clarifying the factors promoting the considerable heterogeneity in the rate and extent of brain volume recovery demonstrated by ALC during abstinence.

The goal of this longitudinal study was to investigate regional changes of lobar and subcortical brain volumes in treatment-seeking ALC over approximately 7.5-months of sustained abstinence. Assessment points after approximately 1-week, 1-month, and 7.5-months of abstinence enabled a comparison of the rates of change of regional brain volumes during early abstinence (i.e., 1-week to 1-month) to changes occurring over an intermediate period of abstinence (i.e., 1 to 7.5-months). We specifically examined the effects of comorbid cigarette smoking, medical conditions, and psychiatric and substance use disorders on longitudinal volume changes in ALC, as well as the associations between changes of regional brain volumes and neurocognition. We predicted that:

  1. The rate of regional volume changes in ALC is greater from 1-week to 1-month than from 1 to 7.5-months.

  2. Smoking ALC (sALC) demonstrate less recovery than non-smoking ALC (nsALC) in frontal GM and WM volumes over 7.5-months; age interacts with smoking status (sALC vs. nsALC), where, with increasing age, sALC show less GM and WM recovery in the frontal and parietal lobes than nsALC.

  3. Over 7.5-months of abstinence, increasing frontal and parietal GM and WM in ALC is associated with improving learning and memory, and working memory; increasing lobar GM and WM across the brain relate to improving processing speed.

  4. After 7.5-months of abstinence, both nsALC and sALC continue to demonstrate significantly smaller regional brain volumes than never-smoking controls (CON).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

ALC were recruited from the VA Medical Center (SFVAMC) Substance Abuse Day Hospital and the Kaiser Permanente Chemical Dependence Recovery Program outpatient clinics in San Francisco, and CON were recruited from the local community. Participants provided written consent before engaging in study procedures, which conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the University of California, and SFVAMC. A total of 111 unique ALC (44 nsALC and 67 sALC) participants were enrolled. Thirty-six nsALC and 46 sALC were first studied after 7±4 days of abstinence (assessment point 1 = AP1) and 82 abstinent ALC were re-assessed after approximately 1-month of abstinence (AP2). An additional 29 ALC presented for treatment at our site after 2–3 weeks of sobriety, because they underwent detoxification at other facilities; these 29 participants completed their first assessment after approximately 1-month of abstinence at AP2. Of the 111 total ALC participants studied at AP2, 66 (59%) relapsed between AP2 and AP3, three sALC were excluded after AP2 because they stopped smoking, and six participants were lost to follow-up (e.g., moved out-of-state or no longer interested in participating). Of the 111 ALC studied at AP2, 36 (18 nsALC and 18 sALC) maintained continuous sobriety from alcohol (and consistent smoking levels) for at least 6 months following AP2, and were studied again after 226±60 days of abstinence (AP3). Demographic and alcohol consumption variables between ALC groups studied at AP1, AP2, or AP3 were not significantly different, and ALC groups did not differ in length of abstinence at any AP. Thirty-two CON completed a baseline study, and 15 were assessed again after 278±104 days. Table 1 provides demographic and clinical information for the 82 ALC participants studied at AP1 and 32 CON studied at baseline. Primary inclusion criteria for ALC were a current DSM-IV [APA (1994)] diagnosis of alcohol dependence, fluency in English, consumption of >150 alcohol-containing drinks/month (1 alcoholic drink equivalent=13.6g pure ethanol) for at least 8 years before enrollment for men, and consumption of >80 drinks per month for at least 6 years before enrollment for women.

Table 1.

Group Demographics and Clinical Measures for ALC at Assessment Point 1 and CON at Baseline

Measure CON n = 32 nsALC n = 36 sALC n = 46

Days abstinent NA
AP1 7 (4) 7 (3)
AP2 34 (9) 32 (9)
AP3 225 (57) 227 (65)

Agea 47 (9) 52 (11) 49 (9)

Educationb 17 (3) 14 (2) 14 (2)

Male (%) 91 88 93

Percent Caucasian 72 75 74

Positive family history for alcoholismc 43 84 80

1-year average drinks/monthd 16 (17) 328 (182) 446 (209)

Lifetime average drinks/monthe 16 (14) 163 (107) 277 (147)

History of comorbid substance abuse/dependence (%) NA 22 19

History of comorbid psychiatric disorder (%) NA 42 40

History of comorbid medical disorder (%) NA 50 52

Beck Depression Inventory 4 (2) 14 (2) 15 (2)

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – Trait 32 (8) 47 (10) 50 (13)

Fagerstrom Test of Nicotine Dependence NA NA 5 (2)

Pack-years NA NA 25 (18)

Lifetime years of smoking NA NA 25 (12)

Note. Mean (standard deviation);

a

nsALC > CON;

b

CON > nsALC = sALC;

c

CON < nsALC = sALC; sALC > nsALC > CON;

d,e

sALC > nsALC > CON; p < .05 for all group differences;

NA = not applicable; Positive family history for alcoholism = mother, father, maternal grandparent and/or paternal grandparent indicated by participant to have a history of an alcohol use disorder.

Primary exclusion criteria for ALC and CON are fully detailed elsewhere (Durazzo et al., 2004). In brief, all participants were free of psychiatric, neurological, physical, and medical conditions known or suspected to influence brain morphology and neurocognition, with the exceptions of mood disorders, hepatitis C, hypertension, and type-2 diabetes for ALC; these medical conditions are highly prevalent in ALC (Mertens et al., 2005; Moss et al., 2010). For ALC, current/past unipolar mood disorders (e.g., major depression) were not exclusionary given the high comorbidity with both alcohol dependence (Mertens et al., 2003) and cigarette smoking (Durazzo et al., 2010). In ALC, dependence on any substance (other than alcohol or nicotine) within 5-years prior to enrollment was exclusionary. Participants were screened for recent alcohol and illicit substances at each AP.

Clinical Assessment

At their first assessment, participants completed the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition, Version 2.0, and standardized questionnaires assessing depressive (Beck Depression Inventory [BDI]) and anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, for Y-2 [STAI]) symptomatology, lifetime alcohol consumption [Lifetime Drinking History (LDH)], lifetime substance use consumption [questionnaire assessing substance type, and quantity and frequency of use based on the Addiction Severity Index and NIDA Addictive Drug Survey] and level of nicotine dependence via the Fagerstrom Tolerance Test for Nicotine Dependence. From the LDH, average number of drinks/month over 1-year prior to enrollment and average number of drinks/month over lifetime were calculated. Family history of alcoholism (Family History of Alcoholism Questionnaire; FHA) was obtained via questionnaire by asking participants to classify all first- and second-degree blood relatives who experienced an alcohol use disorder during their lifetime. A participant was considered FHA-positive if they reported their mother, father, and/or grandparent (maternal or paternal) had any lifetime history of “problematic” alcohol consumption. At all APs, the total number of cigarettes smoked per day and lifetime years of smoking were recorded for sALC. At AP3, the Timeline Follow-Back Interview was used in ALC to assess any alcohol consumption and the quantity/frequency of any other substance use. For ALC, review of electronic medical records, and/or telephone interview of collateral sources (i.e., family or friends) was used to verify their abstinence/relapse status [see (Durazzo et al., 2011b) for details and corresponding references for the above measures].

ALC participants with a current/past history of a non-exclusionary medical condition that may have influenced brain morphology and/or neurocognition were considered to be positive for the medical comorbidity factor; the most common medical comorbidities were current hypertension and hepatitis C seropositivity. ALC were considered positive for the substance use disorder comorbidity factor if they met criteria DSM-IV for past dependence (≥5 years prior to enrollment), or current/past substance abuse; most met criteria for cocaine or methamphetamine abuse/dependence. ALC were considered to be positive for a psychiatric comorbidity if they met current or lifetime DSM-IV criteria for a unipolar mood or anxiety disorder; the majority met criteria for a major depressive disorder or substance (alcohol)-induced mood disorder, with depressive features.

Neurocognitive Assessment

Participants completed a battery of measures that assessed working memory, processing speed, and auditory-verbal and visuospatial learning and memory within 48 hours of their MR scan. Alternate forms were used, where available, at follow-up assessments. The following measures were administered: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 3rded. (WAIS-III) - Digit Span (working memory), Symbol Search, and Digit Symbol (processing speed); California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) - Immediate Recall trials 1–5 (auditory-verbal learning), Short-and-Long Delay Free Recall (auditory-verbal memory); Brief Visual Memory Test (BVMT) Revised-Total Recall (visuospatial learning) and Delayed Recall (visuospatial memory). See (Pennington et al., 2013) for corresponding references for the above measures.

Magnetic Resonance (MR) Data Acquisition and Processing

Structural images were acquired on a 1.5 Tesla MR system (Vision, Siemens Medical Systems, Iselin NJ) with a T1-weighted Magnetization Prepared Rapid Acquisition Gradient Echo sequence (1 × 1 mm2 in-plane resolution, 1.5 mm slabs) oriented orthogonal to the long axis of the hippocampus. Tissue intensity-based segmentation of cortical and subcortical GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) from T1-weighted images was conducted with the semi-automated Expectation-Maximization Segmentation method (Van Leemput et al., 1999). This method employs a probabilistic segmentation of GM, WM, and CSF to each MRI voxel based on T1-weighted tissue intensity [see (Mon et al., 2013) for method details and reliability]. Absolute volumes (in cc) for GM and WM of the four major lobes and subcortical regions [cerebellum, thalamus, caudate, lenticular nucleus (sum of putamen and globus pallidus)], and ventricles were obtained by non-linear coregistration of tissue maps to a reference atlas [see (Studholme et al., 2001; Studholme et al., 2003) for method details and reliability]. Groups showed no significant differences in magnitudes of change for left and right hemisphere regions in all longitudinal analyses (data not shown), so results for summed left and right hemisphere volumes are presented. Total cortical GM and WM volumes were calculated by summing the respective GM and WM volumes from the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes. Intracranial volume (ICV) was calculated as the sum of total GM, WM, ventricular and sulcal CSF volumes. Due to high within-group variability, caudate volume was excluded from all analyses.

Data Analyses

Cross-sectional analyses

Comparisons between groups on demographic and clinical variables were completed with multivariate analysis of variance or Fisher’s Exact Test where appropriate. Volume comparisons between nsALC, sALC, and CON at each AP were conducted with generalized linear modeling with group (nsALC, sALC, CON), age, and ICV as predictors. Significant main effects for group (p<.05) were followed-up with pairwise t-tests. In comparisons between nsALC and sALC, lifetime average number of drinks/month and 1-year average drinks/month were separately employed as covariates because of the significantly greater alcohol consumption in sALC (see Table 1). Standard Bonferroni correction was applied to pairwise comparisons for each region (adjusted p ≤ .017). Effect sizes for pairwise comparisons (see Table 6) were calculated with Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988).

Table 6.

Regional volumes and effect sizes for group differences for all assessment points

ROI AP1 AP1 Effect Size AP2 AP2 Effect Size AP3 AP3 Effect Size
CON
n=32
nsALC
n=36
sALC
n= 46
nsALC
vs.
CON
sALC
vs.
CON
sALC
vs.
nsALC
CON
n=32
nsALC
n=44
sALC
n= 67
nsALC
vs.
CON
sALC
vs.
CON
sALC
vs.
nsALC
CON
n=32
nsALC
n=18
sALC
n=18
nsALC
vs.
CON
sALC
vs.
CON
nsALC
vs.
sALC
Frontal GM 221.1 (10.4) 213.1 (10.4) 212.8 (10.3) 0.86* 0.90$ 0.03 222.4 (10.2) 215.9 (10.3) 214.4 (10.1) 0.63* 0.78$ 0.09 222.0 (9.6) 218.6 (9.3) 218.0 (9.2) 0.36 0.43 0.07
Parietal GM 123.2 (6.4) 117.3 (6.5) 117.2 (6.4) 0.90* 0.92$ 0.01 123.1 (6.7) 118.9 (6.7) 118.1 (6.7) 0.62* 0.75$ 0.12 123.8 (6.0) 120.1 (6.1) 119.9 (5.8) 0.83* 0.65$ 0.17
Temporal GM 152.0 (6.1) 147.1 (6.1) 145.8 (6.0) 0.70* 1.02$ 0.12 152.2 (7.0) 147.3 (7.1) 147.1 (6.9) 0.69* 0.72$ 0.03 153.4 (7.0) 147.2 (7.2) 147.8 (6.9) 0.87* 0.80$ 0.09
Occipital GM 56.3 (5.0) 55.4 (5.0) 53.4 (4.9) 0.18 0.59$ 0.40^ 56.6 (4.8) 55.9 (4.8) 54.2 (4.7) 0.15 0.52$ 0.37 56.5 (4.8) 56.4 (4.9) 53.6 (4.7) 0.02 0.63$ 0.58^
Total Cortical GM 553.9 (19.6) 533.6 (19.8) 529.2 (19.9) 1.03* 1.25$ 0.07 555.2 (20.0) 538.6 (20.5) 534.8 (19.8) 0.82* 1.02$ 0.19 554.6 (17.9) 542.1 (17.8) 538.9 (17.9) 0.70* 0.88$ 0.18
Frontal WM 206.9 (13.0) 207.4 (13.1) 207.3 (12.8) 0.04 0.3 0.01 206.5 (13.1) 208.5 (13.2) 208.8 (13.1) 0.22 0.23 0.01 206.8 (11.8) 211.0 (12.2) 210.4 (11.6) 0.35 0.30 0.06
Parietal WM 94.5 (6.5) 94.5 (6.4) 95.0 (6.3) 0.01 0.08 0.08 94.6 (6.8) 94.8 (6.8) 94.7 (6.7) 0.02 0.01 0.01 95.0 (6.9) 96.9 (7.1) 97.2 (6.7) 0.28 0.32 0.04
Temporal WM 57.6 (3.5) 56.5 (3.5) 56.8 (3.4) 0.31 0.24 0.06 57.8 (3.8) 56.7 (3.8) 56.7 (3.7) 0.29 0.29 0.00 58.0 (3.3) 56.9 (3.4) 57.5 (3.2) 0.39 0.14 0.18
Occipital WM 35.6 (3.2) 34.1 (3.2) 34.0 (3.2) 0.14 0.07 0.07 34.9 (3.5) 34.6 (3.5) 34.2 (3.4) 0.21 0.09 0.12 34.8 (3.5) 35.4 (3.6) 35.0 (3.4) 0.44 0.34 0.10
Total Lobar WM 394.8 (20.5) 391.5 (20.6) 390.8 (20.3) 0.16 0.20 0.04 393.9 (21.6) 392.7 (22.1) 392.8 (21.5) 0.05 0.03 0.01 395.8 (20.8) 399.5 (21.4) 400.3 (20.3) 0.22 0.22 0.04
Cerebellum 123.3 (9.6) 119.9 (9.7) 119.3 (9.5) 0.36 0.42 0.06 123.6 (9.1) 121.8 (9.2) 120.9 (9.0) 0.19 0.29 0.10 124.1 (10.0) 125.3 (10.4) 120.7 (9.8) 0.11 0.34 0.46
Thalamus 6.7 (1.1) 5.7 (1.1) 5.8 (1.0) 0.99* 0.93$ 0.08 6.7 (1.1) 5.8 (1.1) 5.7 (1.1) 0.80* 0.88$ 0.07 6.7 (1.1) 6.3 (1.1) 5.8 (1.0) 0.44 0.91$ 0.47
Lenticular nucleus 7.0 (1.0) 6.5 (1.0) 6.6 (1.0) 0.59* 0.50$ 0.09 7.0 (1.0) 6.4 (1.0) 6.5 (1.0) 0.57* 0.50$ 0.08 7.0 (0.9) 6.4 (1.0) 6.4 (1.0) 0.57* 0.57$ 0.00
Ventricular CSF 25.7 (8.8) 30.3 (9.0) 27.5 (8.8) 0.52 0.21 0.31 25.8 (9.1) 28.3 (9.1) 27.6 (9.0) 0.27 0.20 0.08 26.2 (9.3) 25.8 (9.8) 28.4 (9.0) 0.04 0.34 0.28

Note. Mean (standard deviation); reported values are estimated means from pairwise comparisons of CON, nsALC, and sALC;

*

nsALC mean volume < nsCON mean volume, p ≤ .017;

$

sALC mean volume < nsCON mean volume, p ≤ .017;

^

sALC mean volume < nsALC mean volume, p ≤ .017.

GM: gray matter. WM: white matter. CSF: cerebrospinal fluid

Longitudinal analyses (see Supplementary information for details)

ALC

Regional volume change over 7.5-months (i.e., over AP1, AP2, and AP3) for ALC was evaluated with linear mixed modeling. Main effects and interactions for all analyses were considered significant at p<.05. In all longitudinal analyses described below, medical, psychiatric, and substance abuse comorbidities (binary factors) were separately added to models as secondary predictors, following examination for the effects of smoking status, age, ICV, and months abstinent.

Analysis 1 tested for a non-linear trajectory of regional volume changes in ALC over AP1, AP2, and AP3. For each region, a base model with smoking status (nsALC vs. sALC), age, ICV, lifetime average drinks/month (or 1-year average drinks/month), months abstinent (linear), smoking status x months abstinent (or smoking status x age) was statistically compared to a second model containing the foregoing predictors plus a quadratic term for months abstinent (i.e., months abstinent2).

Analysis 2 examined rates of change (i.e., slopes) in ALC for regional volumes between AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3. This analysis also tested for differences in slopes for AP1-AP2 versus AP2-AP3 for each region.

Analysis 3 tested associations between change in regional brain volumes and change in neurocognitive measures across all assessment points, separately for nsALC and sALC. Neurocognitive and regional volumes were standardized to CON to form z-scores. The neurocognitive measure (e.g., BVMT Delayed Recall) was the dependent measure, with age, education, months abstinent (linear), lifetime average drinks/month, and regional brain volume (e.g., frontal GM) as predictors. False discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) was used to control for multiplicity of associations across models. Due to highly similar findings for Digit Span and Digit Symbol (see Supplementary Table 1), these tests were combined into a single “Processing Speed” measure by calculating the arithmetic mean of z-scores for each measure.

ALC vs. CON

CON had two assessment points (baseline and follow-up). Therefore, comparisons of regional volume changes between nsALC, sALC, and CON involved assessment of volumes changes over AP1-3 interval for nsALC and sALC versus change over the baseline-follow-up interval for CON. Linear mixed modeling was used and predictors included group (nsALC, sALC, CON), age, ICV, time (months abstinent for ALC and inter-scan interval for CON), and group x time interaction.

RESULTS

Participant demographics and clinical measures

There were no differences between nsALC, sALC, and CON on the percentage of males and Caucasians. CON were younger than nsALC and had more years of education than nsALC and sALC (all p<.05). Both nsALC and sALC had a higher percentage of relatives (i.e., mother, father and/or grandparents) who had a history of an alcohol use disorder. Lifetime and 1-year-average drinks/month were higher in sALC than nsALC (both p<.05). No differences were apparent between nsALC and sALC on the BDI, STAI, and frequency of comorbid substance use, psychiatric, or medical conditions (see Table 1).

Regional volume changes for ALC over 7.5-months

Analysis 1 – ALC rates of change in regional volumes over all APs (AP1-AP2-AP3; see Table 2 and Supplementary Figure 1)

Table 2.

Linear and Quadratic Rates of Change for ALC in Regional Volumes over 7.5 months of Abstinence

Region Linear Quadratic
β months abstinent SE p-value β months abstinent SE p-value
Frontal GM 1.68 0.28 < .001 −0.0048 0.0011 < .001
Parietal GM 0.95 0.17 < .001 −0.0030 0.0006 < .001
Temporal GM 0.49 0.26 .07 −0.0018 0.0010 .09
Occipital GM 0.30 0.10 .004 −0.0015 0.0004 .004
Total cortical GM 3.93 0.57 < .001 −0.0120 0.00007 < .001
Frontal WM 0.40 0.08 < .001 −0.0003 0.0011 .82
Parietal WM 0.20 0.04 < .001 0.0003 0.0003 .79
Temporal WM 0.13 0.03 < .001 −0.0006 0.0004 .17
Occipital WM 0.13 0.09 .13 −0.0006 0.0003 .06
Total lobar WM 0.75 0.14 < .001 0.0000 0.0017 .95
Cerebellum 0.93 0.13 < .001 −0.0027 0.0005 < .001
Thalamus 0.12 0.03 < .001 −.00033 0.00010 < .001
Lenticular nucleus −0.01 0.31 .15 .00003 0.00010 .91
Ventricular CSF −1.05 0.20 < .001 .00282 0.00070 < .001

Note. β = slope.

CSF: cerebrospinal fluid. GM: gray matter. SE: standard error of the estimate. WM: white matter.

Cortical GM and subcortical regions

Models containing both the linear (all p ≤ .004) and quadratic (all p ≤ .004) terms for months abstinent were significant for frontal, parietal, occipital, and total cortical GM, as well as for the cerebellum, thalamus, and ventricular CSF volume. There were no main effects for smoking status, and the smoking status x linear/quadratic months abstinent interaction was not significant (all p>.20). A smoking status x age interaction was observed for the frontal and total cortical GM (both p<.03); with increasing age, smokers showed significantly less volume recovery than non-smokers over 7.5-months. Temporal GM and lenticular nucleus showed no significant changes with abstinence. Given there were no significant effects for smoking status or smoking status x months abstinent interactions, the positive slopes for the linear term for months abstinent indicated that the ALC group showed significant volume increases in frontal, parietal, occipital, and total cortical GM, cerebellum and thalamus over 7.5-months of abstinence. The significant negative slope (i.e., “frowning” parabola) of the quadratic term for months abstinent in the above GM regions indicated ALC showed different rates of regional volume change between AP1-AP2 than between AP2-AP3.

Lobar WM

For frontal, parietal, temporal WM, and total lobar WM, only the linear term for months abstinent was significant. There was no main effect for smoking status or interactions among smoking status, age, and months abstinent. The positive linear slope for each region indicated that ALC, as a group, demonstrated a significant linear increase in frontal, parietal, temporal, and total lobar WM over 7.5-months.

Greater lifetime and 1-year-average drinks/month were associated with less recovery of frontal GM volume in ALC (both p<.03). In sALC, greater lifetime years of smoking were related to lower frontal WM recovery (p=.03). Medical, psychiatric, and substance abuse comorbidities were not significant predictors of volume change in any region (all p>.30).

Analysis 2 – ALC rates of change for regional volumes between AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3 (see Table 3)

Table 3.

Linear Rates of Change in Regional Volumes for ALC over AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3 Intervals

Region AP1-AP2 AP2-AP3 Difference in β AP1-AP2 vs. AP2-AP3
β months abstinent SE p-value β months abstinent SE p-value p-valuea
Frontal GM 2.21 0.49 < .001 0.39 0.10 .001 < .001
Parietal GM 0.90 0.25 < .001 0.07 0.05 .21 .002
Temporal GM 0.27 0.55 .62 0.02 0.05 .67 .49
Occipital GM 0.16 0.11 .15 0.05 0.05 .23 .13
Total cortical GM 3.94 0.77 < .001 0.35 0.22 .13 < .001
Frontal WM 1.27 0.75 .09 0.38 0.09 .002 < .01
Parietal WM 0.14 0.27 .61 0.30 0.05 < .001 .45
Temporal WM 0.31 0.16 .06 0.15 0.03 < .001 .34
Occipital WM 0.04 0.13 .75 0.06 0.03 .039 .74
Total lobar WM 1.43 0.83 .09 0.92 0.19 < .001 .55
Cerebellum 0.86 0.15 < .001 0.21 0.05 .001 .02
Thalamus 0.18 0.05 .001 0.02 0.01 .001 .002
Lenticular nuclei −0.01 0.05 .88 −0.01 0.01 .62 .99
Ventricular CSF −1.83 0.28 < .001 −0.22 0.06 < .001 < .001

Note.

a

significant p-value (p < .05) indicates rate of volume change over AP1-AP2 is statistically greater than volume change over AP2-AP3. β = slope.

CSF: cerebrospinal fluid. GM: gray matter. SE: standard error of the estimate. WM: white matter.

Cortical GM and subcortical regions

There were no significant main effects for smoking status or smoking status x months abstinent interactions, so nsALC and sALC were combined into a single ALC group. Over the AP1-AP2 interval, ALC showed increased frontal, parietal and total cortical GM, increases in the cerebellar and thalamic volumes, and concomitant decreases in ventricular CSF (all p<.05). Over the AP2-AP3 interval, ALC demonstrated a significant increase in frontal GM. The rate of volume change over AP1-AP2 was significantly greater than over AP2-AP3 in the frontal, parietal and total cortical GM, cerebellum, thalamus, and ventricular CSF (all p<.05). This indicates the significant quadratic term for months abstinent in these regions was driven by steep volume increases over AP1-AP2 and a flatter positive trajectory over AP2-AP3. For example, the frontal GM increased approximately 2.2 cc/month over AP1-AP2 but only 0.39 cc/month over AP2-AP3.

Lobar WM

ALC showed no statistically significant changes in lobar WM over the AP1-AP2 interval, but significant increases in all lobes and total lobar WM over the AP2-AP3 interval. Except for the frontal WM, there were no differences in regional volume change rates between the AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3 intervals. This supports the findings from Analyses 1 and 2, which demonstrated significant linear volume changes across WM regions over approximately 7.5-months of abstinence.

Greater lifetime and 1-year-average drinks/month were associated with less recovery of frontal GM volume (both p<.03) over the AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3 intervals. In smokers, greater lifetime years of smoking were related to less frontal WM recovery over both AP1-AP2 and AP2-AP3 (both p ≤ .03). Medical, psychiatric, and substance abuse comorbidities were not significant predictors of volume change in any region across either interval (all p>.30).

Analysis 3 – Associations between regional brain volume and neurocognitive changes in ALC (see Table 4)

Table 4.

Linear rates of Change in Regional Volumes as Predictors of change in Processing Speed over 7.5 months for non-smoking ALC.

Region Processing Speed
β SE p-value (FDR corrected)
Frontal GM 0.16 0.03 < .001
Parietal GM 0.18 0.04 < .001
Temporal GM 0.11 0.03 < .001
Occipital GM 0.18 0.05 < .001
Total cortical GM 0.11 0.03 < .001
Frontal WM 0.15 0.04 < .001
Parietal WM 0.16 0.04 < .01
Temporal WM 0.19 0.04 < .001
Occipital WM 0.17 0.05 < .01
Total lobar WM 0.15 0.04 < .001
Cerebellum 0.21 0.07 < .01
Thalamus 0.08 0.07 .28
Lenticular nucleus 0.19 0.07 < .01
Ventricular CSF −0.07 0.06 .28

Note. β = slope. CSF: cerebrospinal fluid. GM: gray matter. SE: standard error of the estimate. WM: white matter.

For nsALC, improving Processing Speed was associated with increasing volumes in all GM and WM regions over 7.5-months of abstinence, except the thalamus. In nsALC, there were also multiple associations between changes in between changes in working memory and auditory-verbal/visuospatial learning and memory and regional volumes (p=.03 to .04), but these did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. There were no significant associations between changes in neurocognitive measures and regional volumes in sALC before FDR correction (all p>.07). nsALC and sALC showed statistically equivalent rates of change and variances in both regional brain volumes and Processing Speed over 7.5-months (data not shown), so the lack of association between changes in volumes and Processing Speed in sALC was not attributable to a restriction of range in sALC.

Comparisons of ALC and CON on regional volume changes (see Table 5)

Table 5.

Comparisons of CON and ALC on Regional Volumes over Study Interval.

Region Baseline AP1 Follow-up AP3 CON change Baseline to Follow-up (%) ALC change AP1 to AP3 (%) Group x time interaction p-value
CON n =32 ALC n =82 CON n = 15 ALC n =36
Frontal GM 221.3 (8.0) 212.6 (10.4) 221.1 (7.9) 218.3 (8.6) 0.1 2.7 .001
Parietal GM 123.8 (6.0) 117.3 (6.5) 123.1 6.0) 119.9 (6.0) 0.6 2.2 .01
Temporal GM 151.2 (7.7) 146.8 (6.1) 150.4 (7.6) 147.5 (7.1) −0.5 0.5 .17
Occipital GM 57.3 (4.4) 54.3 (5.0) 57.4 (4.4) 55.1 (4.8) 0.2 1.5 .01
Total cortical GM 553.7 (17.2) 531.8 (19.9) 552.0 (16.8) 540.1 (16.0) 0.3 1.6 .01
Frontal WM 205.4 (12.3) 207.4 (13.0) 207.1 (12.6) 210.7 (11.9) 0.8 1.6 .001
Parietal WM 94.2 (5.9) 94.8 (6.4) 95.1 (6.8) 97.6 (6.9) 1.0 3.0 .02
Temporal WM 57.5 (3.1) 56.7 (3.5) 57.1 (3.0) 57.2 (3.3) −0.7 1.0 .08
Occipital WM 35.3 (3.3) 34.1 (3.2) 34.9 (3.3) 35.2 (3.5) 1.3 3.1 .001
Total lobar WM 394.7 (20.6) 391.2 (20.5) 395.5 (20.8) 399.6 (20.9) 0.2 2.1 .004
Cerebellum 124.2 (10.3) 119.6 (9.6) 124.3 (10.1) 123.0 (10.1) 0.03 2.8 .02
Thalamus 6.8 (1.2) 5.8 (1.1) 6.9 (1.3) 6.1 (1.1) 1.4 5.2 .02
Lenticular nucleus 7.0 (1.3) 6.6 (1.0) 6.9 (1.3) 6.4 (1.0) −1.4 −2.3 .23
Ventricular CSF 25.5 (5.5) 28.9 (8.8) 26.4 (5.6) 27.4 (9.4) 1.9 −5.5 < .001

Note. Mean (standard deviation); reported values are estimated means from longitudinal model comparing volume change in CON and ALC; a significant group x time interaction (p < .05) indicates volume change over time was greater in ALC than for CON. CSF: cerebrospinal fluid. GM: gray matter. WM: white matter.

There were no main effects for smoking status or smoking status x months abstinent interactions for regional volumes in ALC, so nsALC and sALC were combined into a single group. A group (ALC vs. CON) x time interaction was observed for all regions (all p<.05), except the temporal GM and WM and the lenticular nucleus. ALC showed significant volume increases in all regions (all p<.05), except the temporal GM and lenticular nucleus, which is consistent with findings from Analyses 1 and 2. CON showed no changes between baseline and follow-up in any region.

Cross-sectional comparisons between ALC and CON on regional volumes at AP1, AP2, and AP3 (see Table 6)

Since no significant volume changes were observed in CON, the larger baseline sample (n=32) was used in all cross-sectional comparisons to ALC. At both AP1 and AP2, both nsALC and sALC showed smaller frontal, parietal, and total cortical GM, thalamus, and lenticular nucleus volumes than CON (all p ≤ .017), with moderate-to-large effect sizes for the observed differences. At AP1, sALC showed smaller occipital GM volume than nsALC, and a trend (p=.03) for smaller occipital GM volume. There were no group differences for the cerebellum, ventricular CSF volume, and WM in any region at AP1, AP2, or AP3. At AP3, groups were not different on frontal GM volume, and nsALC was equivalent to CON on thalamic volume. Otherwise, the group differences in GM, WM, and subcortical structures demonstrated at AP1 was still apparent after 7.5-months of abstinence at AP3.

DISCUSSION

The primary findings from this study are: (1) ALC (both nsALC and sALC) showed significant volume increases in all GM and WM regions over approximately 7.5-months of abstinence, except the temporal GM and the lenticular nucleus, as well as decreased ventricular volume over 7.5-months; medical, psychiatric, and substance misuse comorbidities were not significant predictors of regional volume change. (2) Rates of volume change for ALC for the cortical and subcortical GM were significantly greater over 1-week to 1-month of abstinence than over 1 to 7.5-months of abstinence; linear WM volume changes were observed in ALC over 7.5 months of abstinence. (3) sALC showed significantly less recovery with increasing age than nsALC in the frontal GM and total cortical GM over 7.5-months. (4) Over 7.5-months of abstinence, improving Processing Speed was associated with increasing volumes in multiple regions in nsALC, but not in sALC. (5) After 7.5-months of abstinence, nsALC and sALC were statistically equivalent to CON on frontal GM volume, but continued to demonstrate significantly lower parietal, temporal, and total cortical GM, and thalamic volumes than CON. Neither nsALC nor sALC differed significantly from CON on regional WM volumes at AP1, AP2, or AP3.

The divergent rates of volume increases for regional GM and WM demonstrated by ALC suggest that the neuronal components (e.g., dendrites/dendritic spines, cell bodies) and glial cells (e.g., protoplasmic astrocytes) that primarily constitute the tissue mass of cortical and subcortical GM may recover at a different rate than the components contributing to WM (e.g., myelin, oligodendrocytes, neuronal axons, fibrous astrocytes). In ALC, 58% of total cortical GM volume recovery (i.e., 5.3 of 9.1 cc) occurred over the first month of abstinence, and was driven by frontal GM increases. For lobar WM, the majority of volume recovery in ALC occurred over the AP2-AP3 interval (approximately 6.5-months), with the exception of the frontal WM, where the rate of change was greater over the first month of abstinence. Previously, we found ALC demonstrated significant increases in frontal and parietal GM N-acetylaspartate (NAA; marker of neuronal integrity) and choline-containing compounds (Cho, marker of cell membrane turn-over/synthesis) over 1-month of abstinence (Durazzo et al., 2006). Additionally, higher frontal GM NAA level was associated with greater frontal GM thickness (Durazzo et al., 2011a) in 1-week-abstinent ALC, and increasing frontal Cho was related to increasing global brain volume in ALC over 7-weeks of sobriety (Bartsch et al., 2007). Additionally, data from humans and animal models suggest that the morphological recovery in ALC during early and extended abstinence may be related to increases in neuronal dendritic arbor, soma/cell volume, synaptic density, glial proliferation (particularly microglia), and remyelination (Crews and Nixon, 2009; Dlugos and Pentney, 1997; Sullivan and Pfefferbaum, 2005). Taken together, the rapid volume increases in cortical GM volumes observed in this ALC cohort during the first month of abstinence may be related to increases of neuronal structural and metabolic integrity, and glial cell proliferation. However, the in vivo recovery trajectory of the specific cellular components comprising the cortical and subcortical GM and lobar WM in human ALC is unclear.

Although volume recovery in ALC within 1-year of abstinence was observed in multiple studies, many cross-sectional reports indicated ALC demonstrated significantly smaller regional GM and/or WM volumes after 6 or more months of sobriety than controls (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Monnig et al., 2013). In the current study, both nsALC and sALC exhibited volume increases in multiple cortical and subcortical GM regions after approximately 7.5-months of sustained abstinence, but both groups continued to manifest lower GM volumes than CON in most regions (i.e., −3.2% lower total cortical GM in ALC at AP3). Cerebellar volume also increased significantly in ALC over 7.5-months of abstinence, but it was not significantly different from CON at any AP. Frontal GM was the sole cortical GM region in ALC that was statistically equivalent to CON after 7.5-months of abstinence. Recovery of frontal GM volume is clinically relevant because ALC with lower volumes in frontal subregions (e.g., orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices) during early abstinence were more likely to relapse within approximately 1-year after treatment (Cardenas et al., 2011; Durazzo et al., 2011b; Rando et al., 2010). For lobar WM, ALC demonstrated significant WM volume increases in all regions except the occipital WM, but cross-sectionally, ALC WM volumes were not significantly different from CON in any region at any AP. This is consistent with animal models (Kroenke et al., 2014) and several human studies that reported no significant differences in lobar WM volumes between controls and ALC during early and extended abstinence (Monnig et al., 2013). Additional longitudinal studies are needed to specifically examine if major subcomponents within the GM and WM regions of interest measured in this study show equivalents rate of change during abstinence.

sALC and nsALC did not differ on regional volume recovery rates, but sALC showed significantly lower frontal and total cortical GM recovery with increasing age than nsALC. The greater age-related effects on longitudinal GM volume recovery in sALC are consistent with our cross-sectional studies, where 1-week-abstinent sALC demonstrated significantly greater age-related frontal GM volume loss than nsALC (Durazzo et al., 2014b). Taken together, cigarette smoking in ALC appears to adversely affect the structural integrity of brain parenchyma comprising the GM of ALC over time [see (Durazzo et al., 2014a; Durazzo et al., 2010) for discussion of potential smoking-related mechanisms].

In this study, higher alcohol consumption and cigarette exposure measures were only related to lower recovery of frontal GM and WM volumes, respectively. This supports assertions that the frontal lobe is particularly vulnerable to the effects of hazardous alcohol consumption (Crews and Nixon, 2009) and chronic smoking (Durazzo et al., 2010). However, alcohol consumption and cigarette exposure measures in ALC showed weak associations (r ≤ −0.25) with regional volumes in cross-sectional analyses at AP1, AP2, and AP3 (data not shown). Other cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in ALC reported weak or non-significant associations of alcohol consumption variables with regional brain volumes (Buhler and Mann, 2011; Monnig et al., 2013). Additionally, adolescent offspring of alcohol dependent individuals, with minimal alcohol exposure, demonstrated smaller regional brain volumes than offspring of non-alcohol dependent individuals (Tessner and Hill, 2010). Given the limited or lack of associations of alcohol consumption, smoking severity, and medical, psychiatric and substance comorbidities with volume changes and cross-sectional volumes in ALC, we believe that the persistent lower regional cortical and subcortical GM volumes in ALC relative to CON after 7.5 months of abstinence likely reflect the influence of premorbid factors (e.g., genetic vulnerability to alcohol use disorders) or comorbid factors not assessed in this study.

For nsALC only, linear volumes increases in lobar GM and WM, cerebellum, and lenticular nucleus were robust predictors of improved Processing Speed over 7.5-months. The measures that formed the Processing Speed domain require speed and accuracy (i.e., efficiency), which interrogates the integrity of nodes of multiple anterior and posterior circuits and their interconnections (Kolb and Whishaw, 2009; Salthouse, 2000). The absence of associations between change regional volumes and neurocognition in sALC was not attributable to restriction of range in these measures, and may indicate that sALC and nsALC potentially employ different circuits during completion of the Processing Speed measures. Alternately, sALC may show abnormal functional and/or anatomical connectivity in the multiple cortical-subcortical circuits that subserve Processing Speed. This may have implications for neuroplasticity-based cognitive remediation approaches for treatment-seeking ALC (e.g., Rupp et al., 2012).

This report has limitations that may influence the generalizability of the findings. The level of volume change in ALC in the lobar regions or subcortical structures (e.g., cerebellum) may not be representative of magnitude of change of all subcomponents (e.g., cerebellar vermis) comprising the larger region of interest. The modest number of observations at TP3 increases the risk of model over-fitting and corresponding Type I error, despite all critical model assumptions having been met in all analyses. The results may have been influenced by factors not assessed in this study, such as personality disorders, diet/nutrition, exercise, subclinical liver dysfunction, and genetic predispositions [e.g., (Mon et al., 2013)]. Only approximately 10% of participants were female, which precluded examination of sex effects.

In conclusion, this cohort of treatment-seeking ALC demonstrated significant recovery of regional GM and WM volumes over 7.5-months of abstinence, the rate of volume change for regional GM was greatest during the first 30-days of abstinence, and volume recovery was not influenced by common medical, psychiatric, or substance misuse comorbidities; however, sALC showed lower frontal and total cortical GM recovery with increasing age than nsALC. Despite significant regional volume recovery, sALC and nsALC continued to demonstrate significantly smaller GM volumes in most regions after 7.5-months of sustained abstinence than CON. Additional longitudinal studies are needed to determine the neuropsychological, psychosocial, and treatment implications of these findings.

Supplementary Material

Supp DataS1
Supp FigureS1
Supp TableS1

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health [DA24136 to T.C.D and AA10788 to D.J.M.] administered by the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and by the use of resources and facilities at the San Francisco Veterans Administration Medical Center. We thank Mary Rebecca Young, Bill Clift, Ricky Chen, and Drs. Ellen Herbst, Peter Banys, and Steven Batki of the San Francisco VA Medical Center Substance Abuse Day Hospital, and Dr. David Pating, Karen Moise and their colleagues at the Kaiser Permanente Chemical Dependency Recovery Program in San Francisco for their valuable assistance in participant recruitment. We also thank Mary Vertinski and Molly Estrin for assistance in the neurocognitive and clinical assessment of participants. Finally, we thank the study participants, who made this research possible.

Footnotes

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION

Drs. Durazzo and Meyerhoff conceptualized and designed the study. Dr. Durazzo recruited and conducted the clinical and neuropsychological assessment of study participants or directly supervised participant recruitment and assessment. Dr. Durazzo was responsible for all statistical analyses, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. Drs. Mon, Gazdzinski, and Yeh were responsible for magnetic resonance data acquisition. Dr. Mon completed the quantitative morphological processing and quality control. All authors edited and contributed significant content to the manuscript.

The authors have no disclosures and no conflicts of interest to report.

References

  1. Agartz I, Brag S, Franck J, Hammarberg A, Okugawa G, Svinhufvud K, Bergman H. MR volumetry during acute alcohol withdrawal and abstinence: a descriptive study. Alcohol Alcohol. 2003;38:71–78. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agg020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bartsch AJ, Homola G, Biller A, Smith SM, Weijers HG, Wiesbeck GA, Jenkinson M, De Stefano N, Solymosi L, Bendszus M. Manifestations of early brain recovery associated with abstinence from alcoholism. Brain. 2007;130:36–47. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Royal Stat Soc. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  4. Buhler M, Mann K. Alcohol and the human brain: a systematic review of different neuroimaging methods. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1771–1793. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01540.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cardenas VA, Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Mon A, Studholme C, Meyerhoff DJ. Brain Morphology at Entry into Treatment for Alcohol Dependence Is Related to Relapse Propensity. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  7. Crews FT, Nixon K. Mechanisms of neurodegeneration and regeneration in alcoholism. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44:115–127. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Demirakca T, Ende G, Kammerer N, Welzel-Marquez H, Hermann D, Heinz A, Mann K. Effects of alcoholism and continued abstinence on brain volumes in both genders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1678–1685. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01514.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, D.C: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dlugos CA, Pentney RJ. Morphometric evidence that the total number of synapses on Purkinje neurons of old F344 rats is reduced after long-term ethanol treatment and restored to control levels after recovery. Alcohol Alcohol. 1997;32:161–172. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.alcalc.a008250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Durazzo TC, Cardenas VA, Studholme C, Weiner MW, Meyerhoff DJ. Non-treatment-seeking heavy drinkers: Effects of chronic cigarette smoking on brain structure. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007a;87:76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Banys P, Meyerhoff DJ. Cigarette smoking exacerbates chronic alcohol-induced brain damage: a preliminary metabolite imaging study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:1849–1860. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000148112.92525.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Meyerhoff DJ. The neurobiological and neurocognitive consequences of chronic cigarette smoking in alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Alcohol. 2007b;42:174–185. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agm020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Rothlind JC, Banys P, Meyerhoff DJ. Brain metabolite concentrations and neurocognition during short-term recovery from alcohol dependence: Preliminary evidence of the effects of concurrent chronic cigarette smoking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:539–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00060.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Yeh PH, Meyerhoff DJ. Combined neuroimaging, neurocognitive and psychiatric factors to predict alcohol consumption following treatment for alcohol dependence. Alcohol Alcohol. 2008;43:683–691. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Durazzo TC, Mattsson N, Weiner MW Initiative. Smoking and increased Alzheimer’s disease risk: A review of potential mechanisms. Alzheimer’s and Dementia. 2014a;10:S122–S145. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ. Neurobiological and neurocognitive effects of chronic cigarette smoking and alcoholism. Front Biosci. 2007;12:4079–4100. doi: 10.2741/2373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Nixon SJ. Chronic cigarette smoking: implications for neurocognition and brain neurobiology. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7:3760–3791. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7103760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Nixon SJ. A comprehensive assessment of neurocognition in middle-aged chronic cigarette smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;122:105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ, Nixon SJ. Interactive effects of chronic cigarette smoking and age on hippocampal volumes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2013;133:704–711. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Durazzo TC, Mon A, Gazdzinski S, Meyerhoff DJ. Chronic cigarette smoking in alcohol dependence: associations with cortical thickness and N-acetylaspartate levels in the extended brain reward system. Addict Biol. 2011a;18:379–391. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00407.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Durazzo TC, Mon A, Pennington D, Abe C, Gazdzinski S, Meyerhoff DJ. Interactive effects of chronic cigarette smoking and age on brain volumes in controls and alcohol-dependent individuals in early abstinence. Addict Biol. 2014b;19:132–143. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00492.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Durazzo TC, Tosun D, Buckley S, Gazdzinski S, Mon A, Fryer SL, Meyerhoff DJ. Cortical Thickness, Surface Area, and Volume of the Brain Reward System in Alcohol Dependence: Relationships to Relapse and Extended Abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011b;35:1187–1200. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01452.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ. Temporal dynamics and determinants of whole brain tissue volume changes during recovery from alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;78:263–273. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Mon A, Yeh PH, Meyerhoff DJ. Cerebral white matter recovery in abstinent alcoholics--a multimodality magnetic resonance study. Brain. 2010;133:1043–1053. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Weiner MW, Meyerhoff DJ. Are treated alcoholics representative of the entire population with alcohol use disorders? A magnetic resonance study of brain injury. Alcohol. 2008;42:67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hasin DS, Stinson FS, Ogburn E, Grant BF. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:830–842. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kolb B, Whishaw IQ. Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. 6. Worth Publishers; New York, NY: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kroenke CD, Rohlfing T, Park B, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, Grant KA. Monkeys that voluntarily and chronically drink alcohol damage their brains: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuropsychopharm. 2014;39:823–830. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Luhar RB, Sawyer KS, Gravitz Z, Ruiz SM, Oscar-Berman M. Brain volumes and neuropsychological performance are related to current smoking and alcoholism history. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2013;9:1767–1784. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S52298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mertens JR, Lu YW, Parthasarathy S, Moore C, Weisner CM. Medical and psychiatric conditions of alcohol and drug treatment patients in an HMO: comparison with matched controls. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:2511–2517. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.20.2511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mertens JR, Weisner C, Ray GT, Fireman B, Walsh K. Hazardous drinkers and drug users in HMO primary care: prevalence, medical conditions, and costs. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:989–998. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000167958.68586.3d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mon A, Delucchi K, Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Meyerhoff DJ. A mathematical formula for prediction of gray and white matter volume recovery in abstinent alcohol dependent individuals. Psychiatry Res. 2011;194:198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mon A, Durazzo TC, Gazdzinski S, Hutchison KE, Pennington D, Meyerhoff DJ. Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) Genotype is Associated with Lobar Gray and White Matter Volume Recovery in Abstinent Alcohol Dependent Individuals. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12:98–107. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00854.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Monnig MA, Tonigan JS, Yeo RA, Thoma RJ, McCrady BS. White matter volume in alcohol use disorders: a meta-analysis. Addict Biol. 2013;18:581–592. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00441.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Moss HB, Chen CM, Yi HY. Prospective follow-up of empirically derived Alcohol Dependence subtypes in wave 2 of the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol And Related Conditions (NESARC): recovery status, alcohol use disorders and diagnostic criteria, alcohol consumption behavior, health status, and treatment seeking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:1073–1083. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01183.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K. Alcohol: effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev. 2007;17:239–257. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9038-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pennington DL, Durazzo TC, Schmidt T, Mon A, Abe C, Meyerhoff DJ. The Effects of Chronic Cigarette Smoking on Cognitive Recovery during Early Abstinence from Alcohol. Alc Clin Exp Research. 2013;37:1220–1227. doi: 10.1111/acer.12089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Mathalon DH, Shear PK, Rosenbloom MJ, Lim KO. Longitudinal changes in magnetic resonance imaging brain volumes in abstinent and relapsed alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19:1177–1191. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01598.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rando K, Hong KI, Bhagwagar Z, Li CS, Bergquist K, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Association of Frontal and Posterior Cortical Gray Matter Volume With Time to Alcohol Relapse: A Prospective Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;168:183–192. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10020233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rupp CI, Kemmler G, Kurz M, Hinterhuber H, Fleischhacker WW. Cognitive remediation therapy during treatment for alcohol dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73:625–634. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Salthouse TA. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol Psychol. 2000;54:35–54. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00052-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. Neurocircuitry in alcoholism: a substrate of disruption and repair. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;180:583–594. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2267-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tessner KD, Hill SY. Neural circuitry associated with risk for alcohol use disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20:1–20. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9111-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. van Eijk J, Demirakca T, Frischknecht U, Hermann D, Mann K, Ende G. Rapid Partial Regeneration of Brain Volume During the First 14 Days of Abstinence from Alcohol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01853.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Van Leemput K, Maes F, Vandermeulen D, Suetens P. Automated model-based tissue classification of MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 1999;18:897–908. doi: 10.1109/42.811270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yeh PH, Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Sjostrand K, Meyerhoff DJ. Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) of longitudinal brain structural and cognitive changes in alcohol-dependent individuals during sobriety. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;91:195–204. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp DataS1
Supp FigureS1
Supp TableS1

RESOURCES