Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Mar 4.
Published in final edited form as: J Autism Dev Disord. 2010 Sep;40(9):1139–1145. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0961-7

Brief Report: Glutamate Transporter Gene (SLC1A1) Single Nucleotide Polymorphism (rs301430) and Repetitive Behaviors and Anxiety in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Kenneth D Gadow 1, Jasmin Roohi 2, Carla J DeVincent 3, Sarah Kirsch 4, Eli Hatchwell 5
PMCID: PMC4348063  NIHMSID: NIHMS666639  PMID: 20155310

Abstract

Investigated association of single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs301430 in glutamate transporter gene (SLC1A1) with severity of repetitive behaviors (obsessive–compulsive behaviors, tics) and anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Mothers and/or teachers completed a validated DSM-IV-referenced rating scale for 67 children with autism spectrum disorder. Although analyses were not significant for repetitive behaviors, youths homozygous for the high expressing C allele had more severe anxiety than carriers of the T allele. Allelic variation in SLC1A1 may be a biomarker for or modifier of anxiety symptom severity in children with ASD, but study findings are best conceptualized as tentative pending replication with larger independent samples.

Keywords: Autism, Autism spectrum disorder, SLC1A1, Obsessive–compulsive behaviors, Separation anxiety, Generalized anxiety


Obsessive–compulsive behaviors (OCB) and anxiety are common co-occurring features of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (e.g., Gadow et al. 2005; White et al. 2009), and the three groups of syndromes evidence overlapping symptoms and family histories in ASD and non ASD samples (e.g., Bolton et al. 1998; Cath et al. 2008; Comings 1990; Cullen et al. 2008; Gadow et al. 2002; Piven and Palmer 1999). Nevertheless, relatively few studies have actually examined whether common gene variants purportedly associated with obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD) may actually be biomarkers for or potential modifiers of these symptoms in children with ASD. The clinical significance of this effort is underscored by the facts that repetitive behaviors are stressful for parents (e.g., Lecavalier et al. 2006) and singled out as being a source of concern (Fong et al. 1993); have an adverse effect on quality of life (Bishop et al. 2007); and are highly stable over time (Billstedt et al. 2007; Lecavalier et al. 2006), high priorities for intervention (Matson and Dempsey 2009) and associated with poorer long-term outcome (e.g., Billstedt et al. 2007; Howlin et al. 2004). The repetitive behaviors associated with ASD include a complex assortment of vocalizations, motor movements, and cognitions whose nosology is not well-established (e.g., Carcani-Rathwell et al. 2006; Lam et al. 2008; Mandy and Skuse 2008) and not easily differentiated from the classic features of OCD (e.g., Wood et al. 2008; Zandt et al. 2007), which is also true for other neurobehavioral syndromes that are characterized by repetitive behaviors and associated with OCB such as tic disorders (e.g., Grados et al. 2008).

Although a number of studies have examined the molecular genetics of ASD repetitive behaviors (e.g., Coon et al. 2005; Mulder et al. 2005; Sacco et al. 2007; Sakurai et al. 2008; Sutcliffe et al. 2005), few have actually evaluated relations of purported OCD candidate genes with measures of OCB behaviors or with other anxiety-related behaviors. The glutamate transporter gene (SLC1A1), which encodes the neuronal glutamate transporter excitatory amino acid carrier 1, is highly expressed in regions of the brain that are implicated in the pathogenesis of OCD (Arnold et al. 2006; Brune et al. 2008; Dickel et al. 2006; Stewart et al. 2007; Wendland et al. 2009). The glutamate transporter regulates neurotransmission by removing glutamate from the extracellular space thus terminating synaptic transmission. It is also involved in the biosynthesis of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), which may also be involved in the etiology of OCD. Dickel et al. (2006) examined nine single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in SLC1A1 and found two SNPs associated with early onset OCD, one of which was a coding synonymous SNP in exon 10 (rs301430). TDT analyses indicated overtransmission of the lower-frequency C allele. In a similar vein, Stewart et al. (2007) found that a haplotype, which included rs301430, was associated with early-onset OCD in a family-based association study. In a more recent study Wendland et al. (2009) reported on three SLC1A1 SNPs previously found to be associated with OCD, one of which was rs301430. This SNP was found to be associated with mRNA levels in post mortem brain tissue, and alleles were shown to have a differential effect on reporter gene expression in human and rat cell lines. The T allele evidenced approximately 50% reduced expression compared with the C allele. Others, however, have not found differences in allele frequencies between child-onset OCD cases and controls for rs301430 (Wang et al. 2009). In one of the few studies to investigate its potential role in ASD pathogenesis, Brune et al. (2008) found preliminary evidence for differential transmission of a haplotype that included rs301430.

Based on findings for SLC1A1 SNP rs301430 in nonASD patients with OCD, we examined whether rs301430 was a biomarker for OCB severity in children with ASD. Owing to the aforementioned nosological uncertainties about movement disorders within the ASD clinical phenotype, we also examined tics. As we and others have found that common polymorphisms of genes such as DAT1 (Comings et al. 1996; Gadow et al. 2008a; Rowe et al. 1998), BDNF (Gadow et al. 2009b) and DRD4 (Gadow et al. 2009a) are associated with both movement and anxiety symptoms, a secondary objective was to assess possible relations with anxiety as well. However, as there are no published data about these symptoms and rs301430 in ASD samples, our analyses are exploratory (i.e., hypothesis generating, not hypothesis confirming).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants in this study were recruited from referrals to a university hospital developmental disabilities specialty clinic located on Long Island, New York. All families with at least one child with a confirmed diagnosis of ASD were contacted by mail for participation in a genetic study. A total of 92 individuals were initially recruited, but to maximize homogeneity, the study sample (N = 67) was limited to individuals who were children (4–14 years old) when the diagnostic and behavioral evaluations were conducted. Child participants were excluded from the study if a Rett MECP2 or a Fragile X mutation was discovered. Demographic characteristics were as follows: age (M = 6.9; SD = 2.6), gender (87% male), ethnicity (96% Caucasian), IQ (M = 79.2; SD = 23.2), socioeconomic status (SES) assessed with Hollingshead’s (1975) index of occupational and educational social status (M = 42.4; SD = 11.4), single-parent household (1%), and psychotropic medication use (24%). Mothers’ and teachers’ ratings of psychiatric symptoms were available for 62/57 of the children, respectively. This study was approved by a university Institutional Review Board, informed consent was obtained, and appropriate measures were taken to protect patient (and rater) confidentiality.

Procedure

Diagnoses of ASD were confirmed by an expert diagnostician and based on five sources of information about ASD symptoms to verify DSM-IV criteria: (a) comprehensive developmental history, (b) clinician interview with child and caregiver(s), (c) direct observations of the child, (d) review of validated ASD rating scales including the Child Symptom Inventory-4 (CSI-4) (Gadow et al. 2008b), and (e) prior evaluations and, additionally (n = 49), with (f) the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord et al. 2000) and/or Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Rutter et al. 2003).

Prior to scheduling their initial clinic evaluation, the parents of potential participants were mailed a packet of materials including behavior rating scales, background information questionnaire, and permission for release of school reports, psycho-educational, and special education evaluation records. Rating scales included parent and teacher versions of the CSI-4, which in most cases were completed by the child’s mother. Child’s genotype status was determined using DNA isolated from peripheral blood cells and polymerase chain reaction (PCR).

Genotyping

SNP analysis was performed for the SLC1A1 SNP rs301430 genotypes with high-resolution melting. PCR was carried out in a 10 μl volume containing the following primers: SLC1A1SNPF-ACAATCAACATGGATGGGAC and SL C1A1SNPR-CGCTGCCACTGCTTCATA. Each amplification was overlaid with mineral oil and contained 20 ng of DNA, 0.25 μM of each primer, and 1× Light Scanner Master Mix (Idaho Technology Inc). Reaction conditions were: an initial denaturation at 95°C for 2 min, followed by 45 cycles of 94°C for 30 s, 58°C for 30 s, 72°C for 30 s, with a final extension step (72°C for 10 min) and a final ‘hold’ at 4°C. Melt analysis was performed between 70 and 98°C with a Light Scanner (Idaho Technology, Inc) (Zhou et al. 2004), and SNP status determined using the Small Amplicon Module. One individual with each of the three expected genotypes (C/C, C/T, T/T) was sequenced to confirm correct genotype calling, and these samples were included in the melt analysis (data not shown). Genotype analyses were conducted by an investigator who was blind to the behavioral characteristics of the study sample.

Measures

The CSI-4 (Gadow and Sprafkin 1986, 2002) is a behavior rating scale that assesses the behavioral symptoms of a broad range of psychiatric syndromes and has both parent and teacher versions. Individual items bear one-to-one correspondence with DSM-IV symptoms (i.e., high content validity). To assess symptom severity, items are scored (never = 0, sometimes = 1, often = 2, and very often = 3) and summed separately for each symptom dimension. In the present study, analyses pertained to two symptom domains: repetitive behaviors (obsessive–compulsive behavior, tics) and anxiety (separation, generalized, social). The findings of numerous studies indicate that the CSI-4 demonstrates satisfactory psychometric properties in community-based normative, clinic-referred non-ASD, and ASD samples (see Gadow and Sprafkin 2008). Moreover, confirmatory factor analysis in large a (N = 730) sample of children with diagnosed ASD supports the construct validity of DSM-IV syndromes (Lecavalier et al. 2009) as well as the tripartite model of ASD symptom domains (Lecavalier et al. 2009). As with almost all behavior rating scales, mother and teacher ratings evidence modest convergence, which likely in part reflects important gene x environment interactions. In other words, settings (e.g., home, school) vary considerably in the demands they place on children resulting in setting-specific phenotypic variation as well as source-specific (e.g., mother, teacher) perceptions of deviance. The complexity of these relations is compounded by the fact that the same “susceptibility” allele may be adaptive in one context but maladaptive in another (see Belsky et al. 2009).

Statistical Analyses

Chi-square tests (categorical variables), correlations (continuous variables), and ANOVAs (combined categorical and continuous variables) of demographic characteristics with genotype groups and the dependent variables were examined to identify potential covariates for subsequent analyses. Next, we conducted MANOVAs comparing genotype group differences in the two domains of symptoms to control for multiple comparisons thereby reducing the risk of Type 1 error. Subsequent univariate analyses were examined for evidence of gene-behavior relations. Follow-up pairwise comparisons were examined to identify specific differences between genotype groups. Analyses were conducted separately for the two informants and the two domains of behaviors: repetitive behaviors (OCB, tics) and anxiety. For the latter, data were available for separation anxiety (mothers’ ratings), generalized anxiety (mothers’ and teachers’ ratings), and social phobia (teachers’ ratings). We calculated partial eta-squared (ηp2) to gauge the magnitude of group differences and to address in part the inherent limitations of significance testing (Cohen 1994; Feise 2002; Perneger 1998; Rothman 1990; Zhang et al. 1997). A rule of thumb for determining the magnitude of ηp2 suggests the following: 0.01–0.06 = small, 0.06–0.14 = moderate, and >0.14 = large (Cohen 1988).

Results

Child SLC1A1 rs301430 allele frequencies were C (32%) and T (68%) and genotype frequencies were as follows: C/C (n = 5; 8%), C/T (n = 33; 49%), and T/T (n = 29; 43%), which does not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ2 = 1.34, p = .29). Comparisons between genotype groups did not evidence statistically significant differences in child’s age, gender, ethnicity, IQ level, psychotropic medication, special education, family’s SES, or whether the mother was a single-parent. Importantly, genotype groups did not differ significantly in severity of any of the three ASD domains (communication deficits, socialization deficits, perseverative behaviors) as assessed with the CSI-4.

OCB and Tic Behaviors

MANOVAs did not indicate multivariate effects for repetitive behaviors (OCB, tics) for either mothers’ or teachers’ ratings. Therefore, follow-up univariate analyses were not conducted as part of our planned analyses. Nevertheless, owing to the possibility of Type 2 error with an uncommon genotype, we present the results of these analyses in Table 1 to include effect sizes so readers can better evaluate the likelihood of this risk.

Table 1.

Group differences in severity of psychiatric symptoms for SLC1A1 genotypes

Variable (CSI-4)a C/C
Mean
(SD)
C/T
Mean
(SD)
C/T
Mean
(SD)
F P = ηp2
Parent ratings
 Generalized
 anxiety
4.8 (2.9) 2.4 (2.4) 2.9 (3.0) 1.53 0.23 0.05
 Separation
 anxietya
8.4 (7.5) 2.2 (2.9) 2.3 (3.1) 7.91 0.001 0.22
 Tics 2.6 (1.9) 1.3 (1.5) 1.3 (1.5) 1.69 0.19 0.06
 OCB 1.4(1.1) 0.8 (1.0) 1.3 (1.5) 1.22 0.30 0.06
Teacher ratings
 Generalized
 anxiety
5.6 (2.6) 2.3 (2.1) 2.6 (2.2) 3.48 0.04 0.12
 Social phobiab 3.0 (2.4) 1.9 (1.7) 1.3 (1.6) 2.12 0.13 0.08
 Tics 2.0 (2.3) 1.9 (2.0) 2.3 (2.3) 0.33 0.72 0.01
 OCB 2.8 (2.2) 1.0 (1.4) 1.5 (1.8) 2.64 0.08 0.09
a

Included in the parent version of the CSI-4 only

b

Owing to modifications in the item content of the CSI-4, we were unable to directly compare parent and teacher ratings of social phobia CSI-4 child symptom inventory-4, OCB obsessive-compulsive behavior

Anxiety Behaviors

The MANOVA for mothers’ ratings of anxiety was significant (F = 3.78, p = .006) as was the univariate analysis for separation anxiety (F = 7.71, p = .001, ηp2 = .22) but not generalized anxiety (p = .23; ηp2 = .12) (Table 1). Planned comparisons indicated that children homozygous for the C allele had more severe separation anxiety than either the C/T (p < .001) or T/T (p < .001) groups (see Fig. 1). The multivariate effect was marginally significant for teachers’ ratings of anxiety (F = 2.25, p = .07) and univariate analyses were significant for generalized anxiety (F = 3.48, p = .04, ηp2 = .12) but not social phobia (p = .13; ηp2 = .08). Planned comparisons indicated C/C group had more severe symptoms of generalized anxiety than the C/T (p = .02) or the T/T (p = .01) groups.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Child SLC1A1 rs301430 genotype and mothers’ and teachers’ ratings of separation anxiety disorder (SAD) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) symptoms, respectively. Results are presented as M ± SEM

Discussion

The notion that common gene polymorphisms may be biomarkers for and possibly modifiers of co-occurring behavioral disturbances within the ASD clinical phenotype as well as severity of ASD symptoms finds support in a growing number of studies (e.g., Brune et al. 2006; Cohen et al. 2003; Gadow et al. 2008a, b, 2009a, b; Mulder et al. 2005; Roohi et al. 2009; Tordjman et al. 2001). Not unexpectedly, the results of these same studies also provide evidence of polygeny, pleiotropy, epistasis, and contextual variability (informant specificity), consistent with what is known about the genetic architecture of physical and behavioral characteristics (Flint and Mackay 2009) as well as commonalities with similar traits in other neurobehavioral syndromes. The present study provides tentative evidence for an association of the C/C genotype with separation anxiety and generalized anxiety in children with ASD. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of a potential genetic biomarker for separation anxiety in the ASD clinical phenotype. Similar to other common candidate genes that we have studied, the magnitude of genotype group differences was in the moderate range. An association with different types of anxiety suggests that the SLC1A1 rs301430 polymorphism may be a more general risk factor for trait anxiety in this clinical phenotype. Importantly, these relations were still evident after controlling for ASD symptom severity and intellectual ability and therefore less likely to be linked to the etiology of ASD. Unfortunately, owing to limited research in this area to include the molecular mechanisms by which the rs301430 polymorphism influences gene expression, it is premature to construct a model for a potential role of the C/C genotype in the pathogenesis of anxiety in ASD.

Unexpectedly, allelic variation was not associated with either OCB or tic severity, which is nevertheless consistent with our finding of equivalent severity of perseverative ASD behaviors in the three genotype groups. Our prior research with other genes, however, has found unique gene-behavior linkage for each type of movement disturbance even when associations with perseverative behavior scores were nonsignificant (Gadow et al. 2008a; Gadow et al. 2009a, b). In other words, evidence clearly supports the notion that repetitive behaviors are multi-factorial both within (Carcani-Rathwell et al. 2006) and between (Moss et al. 2009) neurodevelopmental syndromes. Moreover, the marginally significant (p = .08) main effect of genotype for teacher ratings of OCB suggests that further research is warranted. It is noteworthy that the C/C group received the highest mean OCB ratings as was the case for generalized anxiety.

Although our results do not provide compelling support for dysfunctional glutamatergic neurotransmission in the etiology of OCB in children with ASD, they are subject to several qualifications. The percentage of youths with the C/C genotype was small, and though consistent with no-nASD samples, increased the risk of a Type 2 error. Moreover, gene-behavior relations may have been more evident had we examined a wider array of SNPs or conducted haplotype analyses (e.g., Brune et al. 2008; Dickel et al. 2006; Wendland et al. 2009; Stewart et al. 2007). For example, it is possible that other loci, some of which in linkage disequilibrium with rs301430, would provide stronger evidence of or better explanation for an association with OCB severity. Although the study sample was primarily Caucasian, this does not rule out population stratification as a possible confound. For these and other reasons, our findings must be considered tentative pending replication in larger independent samples.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported, in part, by grants from the National Institutes of Health (GCRC grant No. M01RR10710), the National Alliance for Autism Research, the Matt and Debra Cody Center for Autism and Developmental Disorders, and charitable contributions. The authors wish to thank Dr. John Pomeroy for supervising the clinical diagnoses and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions.

Footnotes

Financial Disclosures Kenneth D. Gadow: shareholder in Checkmate Plus, publisher of the Child Symptom Inventory-4; Jasmin Roohi: none; Carla J. DeVincent: none; Sarah Kirsch: none; Eli Hatchwell: none.

Contributor Information

Kenneth D. Gadow, Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, New York, NY, USA; Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Putnam Hall, South Campus, Stony Brook, NY 11794-8790, USA

Jasmin Roohi, Department of Genetics, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA.

Carla J. DeVincent, Department of Pediatrics, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA

Sarah Kirsch, Department of Pathology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA.

Eli Hatchwell, Department of Pathology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA.

References

  1. Arnold PD, Sicard T, Burroughs E, Richter MA, Kennedy JL. Glutamate transporter gene SLC1A1 associated with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:769–776. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Belsky J, Jonassaint C, Pluess M, Stanton M, Brummett B, Williams R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes. Molecular Genetics. 2009;14:746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism in adult: Symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:1102–1110. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01774.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bishop SL, Richler J, Cain AC, Lord C. Predictors of perceived negative impact in moterhs of children with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112:450–461. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[450:POPNII]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bolton PF, Pickles A, Murphy M, Rutter M. Autism, affective and other psychiatric disorders: Patterns of familial aggregation. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:385–395. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797006004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brune CW, Kim S-J, Hanna GL, Courchesne E, Lord C, Leventhal BL, et al. Family-based association testing of OCD-associated SNPs of SLC1A1 in an autism sample. Autism Research. 2008;1:108–113. doi: 10.1002/aur.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brune CW, Kim SJ, Salt J, Leventhal BL, Lord C, Cook EH., Jr. 5-HTTLPR genotype-specific phenotype in children and adolescents with autism. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:2148–2156. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.12.2148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carcani-Rathwell I, Rabe-Hasketh S, Santosh PJ. Repetitive and stereotyped behaviours in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:573–581. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01565.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cath DC, Ran N, Smit JH, van Balkom AJLM, Comijs HC. Symptom overlap between autism spectrum disorder, generalized anxiety disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults: A preliminary case-controlled study. Psychopahtology. 2008;41:101–110. doi: 10.1159/000111555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen J. The earth is round (p < .05) American Psychologist. 1994;49:997–1003. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cohen IL, Liu X, Schultz C, White BN, Jenkins EC, Brown WT, et al. Association of autism severity with a monoamine oxidase a functional polymorphism. Clinical Genetics. 2003;64:90–197. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2003.00115.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Comings DE. Tourette syndrome and human behavior. Hope Press; Duarte, CA: 1990. [Google Scholar]
  14. Comings DE, Wu S, Chiu C, Ring RH, Gade R, Ahn C, et al. Polygenetic inheritance of Tourette syndrome, stuttering, attention deficit hyperactivity, conduct, and oppositional defiant disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 1996;67B:264–288. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960531)67:3<264::AID-AJMG4>3.0.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coon H, Dunn D, Lainhart J, Miller J, Hamil C, Battaglia A, et al. Possible association between autism and variants in the brain-expressed tryptophan hydroxylase gene (TPH2) American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2005;135B:42–46. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cullen B, Samuels J, Grados M, Landa R, Bienvenu OJ, Liang K-Y, et al. Social and communication difficulties and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychopathology. 2008;41:194–200. doi: 10.1159/000120988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dickel DE, Veenstra-Vander Weele J, Cox NJ, Wu X, Fischer DJ, Van Etten-Lee M, et al. Association testing of the positional and functional candidate gen SLOC1A1/EAAC1 in early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:778–785. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Feise RJ. Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment? BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2002:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-2-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Flint J, Mackay TFC. Genetic architecture of quantitative traits in mice, flies, and humans. Genome Research. 2009;19:723–733. doi: 10.1101/gr.086660.108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fong L, Wilgosh L, Sobsey D. The experience of parenting an adolescent with autism. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education. 1993;40:105–113. [Google Scholar]
  21. Gadow KD, DeVincent CJ, Olvet DM, Hatchwell E. Association of DRD4 polymorphism with severity oppositional defiant, separation anxiety, and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorder. 2009a doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07382.x. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gadow KD, DeVincent CJ, Pomeroy J, Azizian A. Comparison of DSM-IV symptoms in elementary school-aged children with PDD versus clinic and community samples. Autism. 2005;9:392–415. doi: 10.1177/1362361305056079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gadow KD, Nolan EE, Sprafkin J, Schwartz J. Tics and psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2002;44:330–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gadow KD, Roohi J, DeVincent CJ, Hatchwell E. Association of ADHD, tics, and anxiety with dopamine transporter (DAT1) genotype in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008a;49:1331–1338. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01952.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gadow KD, Roohi J, DeVincent CJ, Kirsch S, Hatchwell E. Association of COMT (Val158Met) and BDNF (Val66Met) gene polymorphisms with anxiety, ADHD and tics in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009b;39:67–74. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0794-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gadow KD, Schwartz J, DeVincent C, Strong G, Cuva S. Clinical utility of autism spectrum disorder scoring algorithms for the child symptom inventory. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008b;38:419–427. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0408-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Stony Brook Child Psychiatric Checklist-3. Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York; Stony Brook: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gadow KD, Sprafkin J. Child Symptom Inventory-4 Screening and Norms Manual. Checkmate Plus; Stony Brook, NY: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gadow KD,, Sprafkin J. The symptom inventories: An annotated bibliography [On-line] Checkmate Plus; Stony Brook, NY: 2008. Available: www.checkmateplus.com. [Google Scholar]
  30. Grados MA, Mathews CA, Tourette Syndrome Association International Consortium for Genetics Latent class analysis of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome using comorbidities: Clinical and genetic implications. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:219–225. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hollingshead AB. Four Factor Index of Social Status. Department of Sociology, Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1975. [Google Scholar]
  32. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45:212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lam KSL, Bodfish JW, Piven J. Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiarity and association with other symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:1193–1200. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01944.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lecavalier L, Gadow KD, DeVincent CJ, Edwards MC. Validation of DSM-IV model of psychiatric syndromes in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009a;39:278–289. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0622-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lecavalier L, Gadow KD, DeVincent CJ, Houts C, Edwards MC. Deconstructing the PDD clinical phenotype: Internal validity of the DSM-IV. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009b;50:1246–1254. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lecavalier L, Leone S, Wiltz J. The impact of behaviour problems on caregiver stress in young people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2006;50:172–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00732.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mandy WPL, Skuse DH. Research review: What is the association between the social-communication element of autism and repetitive interests, behaviours and activities? Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49:795–808. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Matson JL, Dempsey T. The nature and treatment of compulsions, obsessions, and rituals in people with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2009;30:603–611. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2008.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Moss J, Oliver C, Arron K, Burbidge C, Berg K. The prevelance and phenomenology of repetitive behavior in genetic syndromes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39:572–588. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0655-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mulder EJ, Anderson GM, Kema IP, Brugman AM, Ketelaars CEJ, de Bildt A, et al. Serotonin transporter intron 2 polymorphism associated with rigid-compulsive behaviors in Dutch individuals with pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2005;B133:93–96. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Perneger TV. What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ. 1998;316:1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Piven J, Palmer P. Psychiatric disorder and the broad autism phenotype: Evidence from family study of multiple-incidence autistic families. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:557–563. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Roohi J, DeVincent CJ, Hatchwell E, Gadow KD. Association of a monoamine oxidase-A gene promoter polymorphism with ADHD and anxiety in boys with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39:67–74. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0600-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rothman K. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology. 1990;1:43–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rowe DC, Stever C, Gard JMC, Cleveland HH, Sanders ML, Abramowitz A, et al. The relation of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) to symptoms of internalizing disorders in children. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28:215–225. doi: 10.1023/a:1021427314941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rutter M, LeCouteur A, Lord C. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles, CA: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  48. Sacco R, Papaleo V, Hager J, Rousseau F, Moessner R, Militerni R, et al. Case-control and family-based association studies of candidate gene in autistic disorder and its endophenotypes: TPH2 and GLO1. BMC Medical Genetics. 2007;8:11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2350-8-11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sakurai T, Reichert J, Hoffman EJ, Cai G, Jones HB, Faham M, et al. A large-scale screen for coding variants in SERT/SLC6A4 in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research. 2008;1:251–257. doi: 10.1002/aur.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Stewart SE, Fagerness JA, Platko J, Smoller JW, Scharf JM, Illmann C, et al. Association of the SLC1A1 glutamate transporter gene and obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B Neurospychiatric Genetics. 2007;144B:1027–1033. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sutcliffe JS, Delahanty RJ, Prasad HC, McCauley JL, Han Q, Jiang L, et al. Allelic heterogeneity at the serotonin transporter locus (SLC6A4) confers susceptibility to autism and rigid-compulsive behaviors. American Journal of Human Genetics. 2005;77:265–279. doi: 10.1086/432648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tordjman S, Gutknecht L, Carlier M, Spitz E, Antoine C, Slama F, et al. Role of the serotonin transporter gene in the behavioral expression of autism. Molecular Psychiatry. 2001;6:434–439. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wang Y, Adamczyk A, Shugart YY, Samuels JF, Grados MA, Greenberg BD, et al. A screen of SLC1A1 for OCD-related alleles. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B. 2009 doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31001. doi 10.1002/ajmg.b.31001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wendland JR, Moya PR, Timpano KR, Anavitarte AP, Kruse MR, Wheaton MG, et al. A haplotype containing quantitative trait loci for SLC 1A1 gene expression and its association with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:408–416. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. White SW, Oswald D, Ollendick T, Scahill L. Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29:216–229. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wood JJ, Drahota A, Sze K, Har K, Chiu A, Langer DA. Cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety in children with autism Spectrum disorders: a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;50:224–234. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01948.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Zandt F, Prior M, Kyrios M. Repetitive behaviour in children with high functioning autism and obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37:251–259. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0158-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zhang J, Quan H, Ng J, Stepanavage ME. Some statistical methods for multiple endpoints in clinical trials. Controlled Clinical Trials. 1997;18:204–221. doi: 10.1016/s0197-2456(96)00129-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Zhou L, Myers AN, Vandersteen JG, Wang L, Wittwer CT. Closed-tube genotyping with unlabeled oligonucleotide probes and a saturating DNA dye. Clinical Chemistry. 2004;50:328–335. doi: 10.1373/clinchem.2004.034322. Epub 2004 May 27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES