Abstract
A hallmark of the developing auditory cortex is the heightened plasticity in the critical period, during which acoustic inputs can indelibly alter cortical function. However, not all sounds in the natural acoustic environment are ethologically relevant. How does the auditory system resolve relevant sounds from the acoustic environment in such an early developmental stage when most associative learning mechanisms are not yet fully functional? What can the auditory system learn from one of the most important classes of sounds—animal vocalizations? How does naturalistic acoustic experience shape cortical sound representation and perception? To answer these questions, we need to consider an unusual strategy—statistical learning—where what the system needs to learn is embedded in the sensory input. Here, I will review recent findings on how certain statistical structure of natural animal vocalizations shapes auditory cortical acoustic representations, and how cortical plasticity may underlie learned categorical sound perception. These results will be discussed in the context of human speech perception.
Keywords: Development, Perceptual learning, Auditory cortex, Statistical learning, Categorical perception
Learning has been defined as an enduring change in the mechanisms of behavior that results from experience with the environmental events (Domjan, 2010). Perceptual learning is the specific and relatively permanent modification of perception and behavior following sensory experience (Fahle & Poggio, 2002). Exposure to specific acoustic experience in the critical period of early sensory development alters cortical sound representations (Zhang et al., 2001) and perceptual behavior (Han et al., 2007; Kover et al., 2013), and therefore is a process of perceptual learning. However, the type of perceptual learning through sensory exposure is unique in that it does not involve an explicit training process (Keuroghlian & Knudsen, 2007)—there is no instruction of the desired response or feedback on the actual response. In the absence of instructions or feedbacks, how does the auditory system know what and how to learn in order to adapt to its specific acoustic environment?
Perceptual learning may be classified into three categories: unsupervised, supervised and reinforcement learning. In reinforcement learning, feedback is provided on whether the response is correct (e.g., tell me what sound you are hearing; no, that is not correct.). In supervised learning, the desired response is provided (e.g., now I am going to play the sound of /la/; learn it.). Reinforcement learning is sometimes regarded as a form of supervised learning. In unsupervised learning, however, the subject is left alone to discover the structures of a stimulus ensemble without instructions or feedback (e.g., I am going to play a long sound stream; tell me all the structures in it that you can find.). This is achieved through statistical learning, a process of reshaping perception according to the statistical structures of a stimulus ensemble (e.g., an acoustic environment). Although statistical learning per se does not need feedback, it can be performed in a reinforcement context (Toro & Trobalon, 2005).
Both humans and rodents are sensitive to statistical structure of acoustic input such as stimulus probability and conditional probability distributions. For example, exposure to sounds distributed along a phonetic continuum affects the subsequent discrimination of those phonemes in rats and humans (Maye et al., 2002; Pons, 2006). Human infants and adults are sensitive to stimulus transitional probability, and can use it to identify speech sound sequences or tone sequences that are repeating in a continuous acoustic stream (Saffran et al., 1996; Saffran et al., 1999). Rats are also sensitive to conditional probability such as co-occurrence of sounds in a sequence (Toro & Trobalon, 2005). The neural mechanisms underlying this type of statistical learning are unknown.
The auditory cortex is remarkably adaptive to sensory input. During an epoch of early development, exposure to the acoustic environment can change sound representations without external instructions or feedback (Zhang et al., 2001). Early studies indicate that cortical sound representations are sensitive to simple statistics of the sensory input such as frequency of occurrences—more frequently experienced sounds gain larger cortical representations (Zhang et al., 2001). More recently studies suggest that auditory cortex is also sensitive to conditional probabilities (e.g., co-occurrence of sounds in a sequence) (Kover et al., 2013). Most importantly, early experience-dependent reorganization of cortical acoustic representations is correlated with altered perception and perceptual behavior (Han et al., 2007; Kover et al., 2013). Here I will discuss recent findings on the cortical mechanisms underlying developmental perceptual learning. For more comprehensive reviews on cortical plasticity and its perceptual consequences, please see (Sanes & Bao, 2009; Schreiner & Polley, 2014).
The developing auditory cortex selectively represents animal vocalizations
Nature sounds typically comprise environmental sounds (e.g., wind, water …), animal vocalizations (including human speech) and non-vocalization animal sounds (e.g., from footsteps, wing flaps …). These sounds carry different behavioral significances. Animal vocalizations of the same and different species are likely to be crucial for reproduction and survival of the animal. By contrast, environmental sounds are likely to be less important. Human infants preferentially attend to speech over non-speech sounds (Vouloumanos & Werker, 2007), suggesting an intrinsic preference for behaviorally important sounds. At the level of sensory neural processing, it would be advantageous to preferentially allocate more neurons to process behaviorally important sounds such as animal vocalizations (Wang & Kadia, 2001; Garcia-Lazaro et al., 2006; Kim & Bao, 2013).
Among the different types of natural sounds, animal vocalizations are arguably the most structured (Singh & Theunissen, 2003). They are complex and diverse, but also have some common characteristics that distinguish them from non-vocalization sounds. For example, most vocalizations of mammals are repeated at an ethological range of 4–10 Hz (Liu et al., 2003; Schnupp et al., 2006). Human speech is also temporally modulated (Rosen, 1992), and the temporal modulation in the same ethological range is critical for human speech perception (Elliott & Theunissen, 2009). By contrast, non-vocalization sounds are often random in nature and are not repeated in the ethological modulation range. Studies have indicated that sounds that are repeatedly encountered in the ethological rate range become over-represented in the auditory cortex –i.e., more neurons become tuned to the sounds (Zhang et al., 2001; Chang & Merzenich, 2003; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2008; Insanally et al., 2009). Sounds that are repeated at higher or lower rates are not over-represented (Figure 1 and (Kim & Bao, 2009)). Indeed, unmodulated sounds, similar to a constant environmental sound, can retard cortical development (Chang & Merzenich, 2003; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2008). Unmodulated sounds may even be under-represented in the developing auditory cortex (de Villers-Sidani et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2008). This type of temporal repetition rate-dependent cortical plasticity likely contributes to over-representation of conspecific vocalizations in rat auditory cortex(Figure 1 and (Kim & Bao, 2013)).
Statistics of the acoustic input shape cortical sound representations
Humans and rodents are sensitive to stimulus statistics such as simple stimulus probability distributions and more complex transitional/conditional probability distributions (Saffran et al., 1996; Saffran et al., 1999; Maye et al., 2002; Toro & Trobalon, 2005; Pons, 2006). Electrophysiological studies suggest that developing auditory cortex can utilize these statistics to shape acoustic representations. Early studies indicate that more neurons become tuned to repeatedly presented (i.e., high probability) sounds(Zhang et al., 2001; Chang & Merzenich, 2003; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2008; Insanally et al., 2009). It may be hypothesized that the size of cortical representation encodes stimulus probability (Simoncelli, 2009; Fischer, 2010; Kover & Bao, 2010). A recent study indicated that developing auditory cortex can also encode higher-order conditional stimulus probability—the probability of sounds occurring in a sequence played at the ethological rate (Kover et al., 2013). Sounds that are presented in a sequence tend to be represented by the same population of neurons, whereas sounds that are never presented in a sequence tend to be represented by separate populations of neurons (Figure 2 and (Kover et al., 2013)). Rodent vocalizations occur in bouts with similar temporal rates (Liu et al., 2003; Holy & Guo, 2005; Kim & Bao, 2009; 2013). Early experience of those vocalizations could thus theoretically lead to similar cortical representations, and reduced perceptual contrast, of the individual calls despite their substantial trial-by-trial variability. By contrast, functionally different call types (e.g., pup vs. adult encounter calls) that do not occur in the same bout may be represented by distinct populations of neurons, resulting in perceptual boundaries and categorical perception of the calls (Ehret & Haack, 1981).
Experience-dependent cortical plasticity can account for altered perceptual behaviors
How does statistical learning in the developing auditory cortex impact perception and perceptual behavior? Early acoustic experience has a profound impact on auditory perception and perceptual behaviors. Human fetuses gradually become sensitive to mother’s voice and native speech during late gestation (Kisilevsky et al., 2009; Kisilevsky & Hains, 2011), presumably due to acoustic experience in utero. Prenatal experience shapes perception and neural responses to speech in neonatal infants (Nazzi et al., 1998; Partanen et al., 2013). Language-specific perception and neural representation of speech sounds continue to refine and consolidate during the first year of life and beyond (Kuhl et al., 1992; Kuhl et al., 2006). A profound consequence of early experience of speech sound is the sharpening of categorical perception of native speech sounds—within-category perceptual contrast is reduced and between-category contrast is enhanced—resulting in more efficient recognition of native speech sounds and often loss of sensitivity to some foreign phonemic contrasts (Kuhl et al., 1992; Iverson et al., 2003; Kuhl et al., 2006). Can cortical statistical learning as shown in electrophysiological studies result in categorical perception of conspecific vocalizations (Ehret & Haack, 1981)?
An early study examined perceptual consequences of early acoustic experience in rats (Han et al., 2007). Rats were exposure to a 7.1-kHz tone repeated at the ethological rate during the critical period of auditory cortical development. The animals were then placed in a normal animal room for one month before being tested in a tonal frequency difference detection task. The difficulty of the task was carefully chosen to allow measurement of both improvement and impairment of the performance. The results indicate that discrimination performance was impaired at the exposure frequency, and improved at the flanking frequencies (Figure 3 and (Han et al., 2007)). Subsequent electrophysiological examination of the primary auditory cortex confirmed that the exposure frequency was over-represented (Figure 3). Although the impaired discrimination performance for over-represented stimuli is somewhat counter-intuitive, it is consistent with findings that discrimination between prototypical exemplars of the same speech sounds is more difficult than discrimination between non-prototypical exemplars (Kuhl et al., 1992; Iverson et al., 2003; Kuhl et al., 2006). This phenomenon, also known as the “perceptual magnet effect”, depends on speech sound experience and is specific to the native language (Kuhl, 1991). Thus, it appears that early experience of speech sounds reduces the perceptual sensitivity to subtle differences between prototypical exemplars of the same speech sounds (Kuhl, 1991; Iverson et al., 2003). A computational analysis indicates that the difference detection performance in the sound-exposed animals can be quantitatively accounted for by their cortical frequency representation (Figure 3 and (Han et al., 2007)).
Recent probabilistic models of sensory perception suggest that the percept of a stimulus may be shifted towards the stimuli with larger representations (Simoncelli, 2009; Fischer, 2010; Kover & Bao, 2010). In one of the models, all neurons vote for their preferred stimuli, and their votes are weighed by their firing rates(Fischer, 2010). The model percept is shifted towards the stimulus with larger representation because more neurons vote for it (Fischer, 2010). Empirical observations supported the probabilistic models (Fischer & Pena, 2011; Girshick et al., 2011; Ganguli & Simoncelli, 2014). For example, owls accurately localize sound sources near the center of gaze, but systematically underestimate peripheral source directions (Fischer & Pena, 2011). This behavior is correlated with over-representation of the space near the center of gaze and under-representation of lateral space (Fischer & Pena, 2011).
Higher-order stimulus probability can also shape perception and perceptual behavior. For example, the transitional probability boundary shown in Figure 2 has been shown to result in a perceptual boundary where difference detection performance is improved (Kover et al., 2013). The altered behavior is correlated with segregated representation of the two frequency bands divided by the probability boundary, and steepened tuning curve slopes at the probability boundary (Kover et al., 2013).
Early experience of natural sounds shapes categorical sensory representation
Above studies suggest that early experience shapes categorical representation and perception of sounds. However, they used simple tone pips. Natural sounds are complex and highly structured. To investigate whether natural sounds shape categorical auditory representation and perception, in a recently published study, a group of juvenile rats were exposed to a set of natural animal vocal sounds, referred to as “jungle sounds” (Bao et al., 2013). The jungle sounds CD loop was an hour of spectrotemporally complex sounds, in which there were at least 40 distinctive repeating motifs of bird songs, mammalian vocalizations and insect sounds (for examples, see Figure 4A). Cortical neurons became more selective to spectrotemporal features in the experienced sounds (Bao et al., 2013). At the neuronal population level, more neurons were involved in representing the whole set of complex sounds, but fewer neurons actually responded to each individual sound, and with greater firing rates (Bao et al., 2013). A comparison of population-temporal responses to the experienced complex sounds revealed that cortical responses to different renderings of the same song motif were more similar, indicating that the cortical neurons became less sensitive to natural acoustic variations associated with stimulus context and sound renderings (Figure 4B). By contrast, cortical responses to sounds of different motifs became more distinctive, suggesting that cortical neurons were tuned to the defining features of the experienced sounds. These effects lead to emergent categorical representations of the experienced sounds (Figure 4C). Further behavioral studies are needed to determine whether the jungle sound-exposure result in behavioral categorical perception of the experienced sounds.
Cortical plasticity is a mechanism for warped and categorical sound perception
Sensory representation and perception have two modes, continuous and categorical, and they serve different purposes (Harnad, 1987). For example, precise localization of sound in space would require continuous and faithful representation of the auditory space. By contrast, recognition of vocalizations or speech sounds would be facilitated by categorical representation. Categorical perception is a result of warped perceptual space, as the perceived difference between stimuli is no longer proportional to their physical difference—stimuli in a categorical center are perceived as being more similar, and stimuli across categorical boundaries are perceived as being more different, than they are (Harnad, 1987). The distortion may allow the system to tune out of irrelevant stimulus variability and noises that otherwise would interfere with the perception. Although categorical readout neurons, those that response selectively or exclusively to a category of stimuli, are often found in prefrontal cortex (Russ et al., 2007), the underlying neural computation and sensory transformation may happen in the sensory cortex (Ohl et al., 2001; Steinschneider et al., 2003; Chang et al., 2010; Tsunada et al., 2011; Bathellier et al., 2012).
Early sensory exposure results in selective cortical representations of experienced behaviorally important sounds (Zhang et al., 2001; Chang & Merzenich, 2003; de Villers-Sidani et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2008; Insanally et al., 2009), and representational boundaries that separate different categories of sounds (Kover et al., 2013). These cortical reorganizations may lead to reduced perceptual sensitivity near the over-represented sounds(Han et al., 2007), shift of percept towards over-represented sounds(Fischer & Pena, 2011), and enhance perceptual sensitivity at the boundaries of sound categories (Kover et al., 2013). These perceptual distortions are hallmarks of categorical perception. Thus, sensory exposure-induced auditory cortical plasticity during early develop may be a mechanism for learning perceptual categories.
Unsupervised learning solves the chicken-and-egg problem in the sensory development
Mammalian sensory systems are remarkably adaptive to the ever changing environment. This is achieved through different types of learning mechanisms. Supervised and reinforcement learning allow for rapid improvement in sensory processing for stimuli that are behaviorally relevant for the current and specific context. However, these types of learning often require a certain level of sensory processing in order to understand instructions or feedback. In addition, the development of the supervised and reinforcement learning mechanisms may also be experience-dependent, and requires some level of sensory processing. For example, it is impossible to teach newborns speech in the more traditional and associative form, because they cannot understand verbal instructions, and their brain learning systems are still developing.
The sensory system solves this “chicken-and-egg” problem by unsupervised, statistical learning. Neural circuits in the auditor system organize themselves according to the statistical structures of the sensory input, so that the sounds that are likely to be behaviorally relevant, such as animal vocalizations, are preferentially and categorically represented.
Cortical plasticity underlies perceptual learning
A central question in the field of auditory research is the role of cortical plasticity in perceptual learning. Some studies have found correlations between altered cortical stimulus representations and perceptual learning (Recanzone et al., 1993; Ohl & Scheich, 1996; Bao et al., 2004; Polley et al., 2004; Polley et al., 2006; Schnupp et al., 2006; Han et al., 2007; Froemke et al., 2013), but others have failed to observe those correlations(Talwar & Gerstein, 2001; Brown et al., 2004; Reed et al., 2011; Ranasinghe et al., 2012). As evidence supporting both views accumulates, it is increasingly evident that the conclusion depends on the specifics of the tasks used to measure behavioral performances as well as the types of plasticity effects that are considered (Berlau & Weinberger, 2008). Some auditory tasks preferentially measures procedure or motor learning, and involve corresponding brain substrates of learning. For example, classical conditioning of simple movement to auditory stimuli appears to be mediated by brainstem and the cerebellum (Thompson et al., 1997; Bao et al., 2002), and can be retained even without the forebrain (Mauk & Thompson, 1987). To avoid the confound of non-perceptual influences, tasks that require more sensory processing and less procedure/motor learning are preferred for measuring perceptual learning. Some perceptual tasks measure discrimination of subtle differences between stimuli, while others examine how animals classify very different stimuli. Theoretical and experimental research indicates that fine stimulus discrimination is better achieved by steeper tuning curve slopes, whereas stimulus classification along a large stimulus continuum may be better served by strong responses at the peaks of tuning curves (Butts & Goldman, 2006; Kim & Bao, 2008; Simoncelli, 2009; Kover & Bao, 2010). The multi-dimensional cortical plasticity (e.g., in characteristic frequency, tuning bandwidth, tuning curve slopes, response magnitude, response timing…) could have dramatically different effects on different behavioral tasks. Appropriate behavior tests and quantitative models integrating different aspects of cortical plasticity will help elucidate whether and how cortical plasticity influences perception.
Summary
Based on the reviewed evidence, the following hypothetical model emerges. During the critical period of auditory cortical development, sounds with certain properties of natural vocalizations, such as temporal repetition at an ethological rate, profoundly shape acoustic representation in the auditory cortex. Vocalizations that are repeated at the ethological rate in a bout, such as rat pup calls, are represented similarly by a population of neurons. By contrast, different classes of vocalizations that are not produced at the ethological rates in a bout, such as pup and adult rat calls, are represented by separate populations of neurons. The segregated representations of vocalizations lead to perceptual shifts towards prototypical vocalizations and elevated perceptual contrasts between different classes of vocalizations, resulting in categorical perception of the experienced vocalizations. This model (1) considers categorical perception as a functional outcome of experience-dependent sensory plasticity; (2) emphasizes the history of sensory experience in understanding how the auditory system represents vocalizations and other ethologically relevant natural sounds; and (3) hypothesizes a causal relationship between sensory representations and perception. Further research that integrates neurophysiological and neuroethological approaches under a quantitative theoretical framework of categorical perception is required to evaluate this model of developmental perceptual learning.
Acknowledgement
The work was supported by National Institute of Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (DC-009259).
Footnotes
The author declares no conflict of interest.
References
- Bao S, Chang EF, Teng CL, Heiser MA, Merzenich MM. Emergent categorical representation of natural, complex sounds resulting from the early post-natal sound environment. Neuroscience. 2013;248C:30–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bao S, Chang EF, Woods J, Merzenich MM. Temporal plasticity in the primary auditory cortex induced by operant perceptual learning. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:974–981. doi: 10.1038/nn1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bao S, Chen L, Kim JJ, Thompson RF. Cerebellar cortical inhibition and classical eyeblink conditioning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2002;99:1592–1597. doi: 10.1073/pnas.032655399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bathellier B, Ushakova L, Rumpel S. Discrete neocortical dynamics predict behavioral categorization of sounds. Neuron. 2012;76:435–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlau KM, Weinberger NM. Learning strategy determines auditory cortical plasticity. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;89:153–166. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown M, Irvine DR, Park VN. Perceptual learning on an auditory frequency discrimination task by cats: association with changes in primary auditory cortex. Cerebral cortex. 2004;14:952–965. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butts DA, Goldman MS. Tuning curves, neuronal variability, and sensory coding. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e92. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang EF, Merzenich MM. Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science. 2003;300:498–502. doi: 10.1126/science.1082163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang EF, Rieger JW, Johnson K, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:1428–1432. doi: 10.1038/nn.2641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Villers-Sidani E, Simpson KL, Lu YF, Lin RC, Merzenich MM. Manipulating critical period closure across different sectors of the primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:957–965. doi: 10.1038/nn.2144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domjan M. Principles of learning and behavior. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Cengage; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Ehret G, Haack B. Categorical perception of mouse pup ultrasound by lactating females. Die Naturwissenschaften. 1981;68:208–209. doi: 10.1007/BF01047208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott TM, Theunissen FE. The modulation transfer function for speech intelligibility. PLoS computational biology. 2009;5:e1000302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahle M, Poggio T. Perceptual Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer BJ. Bayesian Estimates from Heterogeneous Population Codes. Proc. IEEE Int'l. Joint Conf. on. Neural Networks. 2010 [Google Scholar]
- Fischer BJ, Pena JL. Owl's behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1061–1066. doi: 10.1038/nn.2872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froemke RC, Carcea I, Barker AJ, Yuan K, Seybold BA, Martins AR, Zaika N, Bernstein H, Wachs M, Levis PA, Polley DB, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. Long-term modification of cortical synapses improves sensory perception. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:79–88. doi: 10.1038/nn.3274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli D, Simoncelli EP. Efficient Sensory Encoding and Bayesian Inference with Heterogeneous Neural Populations. Neural computation. 2014:1–32. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Lazaro JA, Ahmed B, Schnupp JW. Tuning to natural stimulus dynamics in primary auditory cortex. Current biology : CB. 2006;16:264–271. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girshick AR, Landy MS, Simoncelli EP. Cardinal rules: visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:926–932. doi: 10.1038/nn.2831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han YK, Kover H, Insanally MN, Semerdjian JH, Bao S. Early experience impairs perceptual discrimination. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:1191–1197. doi: 10.1038/nn1941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harnad SR. Categorical Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Holy TE, Guo Z. Ultrasonic songs of male mice. PLoS biology. 2005;3:e386. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insanally MN, Kover H, Kim H, Bao S. Feature-dependent sensitive periods in the development of complex sound representation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:5456–5462. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5311-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iverson P, Kuhl PK, Akahane-Yamada R, Diesch E, Tohkura Y, Kettermann A, Siebert C. A perceptual interference account of acquisition difficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition. 2003;87:B47–B57. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00198-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keuroghlian AS, Knudsen EI. Adaptive auditory plasticity in developing and adult animals. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;82:109–121. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, Bao S. Distributed representation of perceptual categories in the auditory cortex. Journal of computational neuroscience. 2008;24:277–290. doi: 10.1007/s10827-007-0055-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, Bao S. Selective increase in representations of sounds repeated at an ethological rate. J Neurosci. 2009;29:5163–5169. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0365-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim H, Bao S. Experience-dependent overrepresentation of ultrasonic vocalization frequencies in the rat primary auditory cortex. Journal of neurophysiology. 2013;110:1087–1096. doi: 10.1152/jn.00230.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kisilevsky BS, Hains SM. Onset and maturation of fetal heart rate response to the mother's voice over late gestation. Developmental science. 2011;14:214–223. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kisilevsky BS, Hains SM, Brown CA, Lee CT, Cowperthwaite B, Stutzman SS, Swansburg ML, Lee K, Xie X, Huang H, Ye HH, Zhang K, Wang Z. Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant behavior & development. 2009;32:59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kover H, Bao S. Cortical plasticity as a mechanism for storing Bayesian priors in sensory perception. PloS one. 2010;5:e10497. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kover H, Gill K, Tseng YT, Bao S. Perceptual and neuronal boundary learned from higher-order stimulus probabilities. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3699–3705. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3166-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK. Human adults and human infants show a "perceptual magnet effect" for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Percept Psychophys. 1991;50:93–107. doi: 10.3758/bf03212211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK, Stevens E, Hayashi A, Deguchi T, Kiritani S, Iverson P. Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental science. 2006;9:F13–F21. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00468.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stevens KN, Lindblom B. Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science. 1992;255:606–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1736364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu RC, Miller KD, Merzenich MM, Schreiner CE. Acoustic variability and distinguishability among mouse ultrasound vocalizations. J Acoust Soc Am. 2003;114:3412–3422. doi: 10.1121/1.1623787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauk MD, Thompson RF. Retention of classically conditioned eyelid responses following acute decerebration. Brain research. 1987;403:89–95. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90126-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maye J, Werker JF, Gerken L. Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition. 2002;82:B101–B111. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nazzi T, Bertoncini J, Mehler J. Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance. 1998;24:756–766. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohl FW, Scheich H. Differential frequency conditioning enhances spectral contrast sensitivity of units in auditory cortex (field Al) of the alert Mongolian gerbil. The European journal of neuroscience. 1996;8:1001–1017. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01587.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohl FW, Scheich H, Freeman WJ. Change in pattern of ongoing cortical activity with auditory category learning. Nature. 2001;412:733–736. doi: 10.1038/35089076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Partanen E, Kujala T, Naatanen R, Liitola A, Sambeth A, Huotilainen M. Learning-induced neural plasticity of speech processing before birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:15145–15150. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1302159110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polley DB, Heiser MA, Blake DT, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM. Associative learning shapes the neural code for stimulus magnitude in primary auditory cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101:16351–16356. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407586101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polley DB, Steinberg EE, Merzenich MM. Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4970–4982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3771-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pons F. The effects of distributional learning on rats' sensitivity to phonetic information. 2006;32:97–101. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranasinghe KG, Carraway RS, Borland MS, Moreno NA, Hanacik EA, Miller RS, Kilgard MP. Speech discrimination after early exposure to pulsed-noise or speech. Hearing research. 2012;289:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM. Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci. 1993;13:87–103. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-01-00087.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed A, Riley J, Carraway R, Carrasco A, Perez C, Jakkamsetti V, Kilgard MP. Cortical map plasticity improves learning but is not necessary for improved performance. Neuron. 2011;70:121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosen S. Temporal information in speech: acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspects. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1992;336:367–373. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russ BE, Lee YS, Cohen YE. Neural and behavioral correlates of auditory categorization. Hearing research. 2007;229:204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science. 1996;274:1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR, Johnson EK, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults. Cognition. 1999;70:27–52. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00075-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanes DH, Bao S. Tuning up the developing auditory CNS. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009;19:188–199. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnupp JW, Hall TM, Kokelaar RF, Ahmed B. Plasticity of temporal pattern codes for vocalization stimuli in primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:4785–4795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4330-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreiner CE, Polley DB. Auditory map plasticity: diversity in causes and consequences. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;24:143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simoncelli EP. Gazzaniga M, editor. Optimal estimation in sensory systems. The Cognitive Neurosciences. 2009:525–535. [Google Scholar]
- Singh NC, Theunissen FE. Modulation spectra of natural sounds and ethological theories of auditory processing. J Acoust Soc Am. 2003;114:3394–3411. doi: 10.1121/1.1624067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinschneider M, Fishman YI, Arezzo JC. Representation of the voice onset time (VOT) speech parameter in population responses within primary auditory cortex of the awake monkey. J Acoust Soc Am. 2003;114:307–321. doi: 10.1121/1.1582449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talwar SK, Gerstein GL. Reorganization in awake rat auditory cortex by local microstimulation and its effect on frequency-discrimination behavior. Journal of neurophysiology. 2001;86:1555–1572. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RF, Bao S, Chen L, Cipriano BD, Grethe JS, Kim JJ, Thompson JK, Tracy JA, Weninger MS, Krupa DJ. Associative learning. International review of neurobiology. 1997;41:151–189. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60351-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toro JM, Trobalon JB. Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent. Percept Psychophys. 2005;67:867–875. doi: 10.3758/bf03193539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsunada J, Lee JH, Cohen YE. Representation of speech categories in the primate auditory cortex. Journal of neurophysiology. 2011;105:2634–2646. doi: 10.1152/jn.00037.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vouloumanos A, Werker JF. Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental science. 2007;10:159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Kadia SC. Differential representation of species-specific primate vocalizations in the auditory cortices of marmoset and cat. Journal of neurophysiology. 2001;86:2616–2620. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.5.2616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang LI, Bao S, Merzenich MM. Persistent and specific influences of early acoustic environments on primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1123–1130. doi: 10.1038/nn745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou X, Nagarajan N, Mossop BJ, Merzenich MM. Influences of un-modulated acoustic inputs on functional maturation and critical-period plasticity of the primary auditory cortex. Neuroscience. 2008;154:390–396. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.01.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]