Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Mar 6.
Published in final edited form as: J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;44(1):309–318. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140636

Subjective memory complaint only relates to verbal episodic memory performance in mild cognitive impairment

Katherine A Gifford 1,*, Dandan Liu 1,2, Stephen M Damon 1, William G Chapman IV 3, Raymond R Romano III 1, Lauren R Samuels 2, Zengqi Lu 2, Angela L Jefferson 1, for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
PMCID: PMC4351870  NIHMSID: NIHMS659524  PMID: 25281602

Abstract

Background

A cognitive concern from the patient, informant, or clinician is required for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI); however, the cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of complaint are poorly understood.

Objective

We assessed how self-complaint relates to cognitive and neuroimaging measures in older adults with MCI.

Method

MCI participants were drawn from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative and dichotomized into two groups based on the presence of self-reported memory complaint (no complaint n=191, 77±7 years; complaint n=206, 73±8 years). Cognitive outcomes included episodic memory, executive functioning, information processing speed, and language. Imaging outcomes included regional lobar volumes (frontal, parietal, temporal, cingulate) and specific medial temporal lobe structures (hippocampal volume, entorhinal cortex thickness, parahippocampal gyrus thickness).

Results

Linear regressions, adjusting for age, gender, race, education, Mini-Mental State Examination score, mood, and apolipoprotein E-4 status, found that cognitive complaint related to immediate (β=−1.07, p<0.001) and delayed episodic memory performances assessed on a serial list learning task (β=−1.06, p=0.001) but no other cognitive measures or neuroimaging markers.

Conclusions

Self-reported memory concern was unrelated to structural neuroimaging markers of atrophy and measures of information processing speed, executive functioning, or language. In contrast, subjective memory complaint related to objective verbal episodic learning performance. Future research is warranted to better understand the relation between cognitive complaint and surrogate markers of abnormal brain aging, including Alzheimer’s disease, across the cognitive aging spectrum.

Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, cognitive complaint, episodic memory, magnetic resonance imaging, mild cognitive impairment, serial list-learning

Introduction

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is widely considered a prodromal phase of dementia because many individuals diagnosed with MCI convert to Alzheimer’s disease (AD) [1]. Current MCI diagnostic criteria require quantitative evidence of neuropsychological impairment, relative preservation of functional abilities, and a concern regarding a change in cognition observed by the patient, someone close to the patient (i.e., an informant), or a clinician [2]. Extensive research has investigated the clinical meaningfulness of neuropsychological impairment [36], functional abilities [79], and neuroimaging markers in MCI [1012]. Despite a growing body of data on the clinical correlates of cognitive complaint among cognitively normal older adults [13], there remains an underrepresentation of literature examining the cognitive and neuroimaging correlates of cognitive complaint in older adults with MCI. Understanding cognitive complaint in MCI is important because it is an essential part of the MCI diagnostic criteria, and cognitive complaint may be an early and predictive marker of unhealthy brain aging [13].

Evidence relating self-reported subjective cognitive complaint in MCI to objective cognitive performance is mixed. Cross-sectional studies suggest that subjective cognitive complaint in MCI is related to poorer verbal episodic memory performances [14,15], but this finding is not consistent across the literature [16]. For example, leveraging very large MCI cohorts, other groups have shown that subjective cognitive complaint is not associated with decline in global [17,18] or domain-specific cognition, such as verbal episodic memory, attention, executive functioning, or information processing speed [18]. Overall, it remains unclear how subjective cognitive complaint relates to cognition.

Cognitive complaint may correspond to structural brain changes reflecting AD pathophysiology, but there are limited studies examining neuroimaging correlates of subjective complaint that exclusively focus on MCI. For example, multiple groups have focused on medial temporal lobe volumes and compared MCI with other groups, such as cognitively normal elders with and without complaint [19,20]. This prior work suggests that the presence of cognitive complaint is related to smaller structural volumes within the hippocampus [19], entorhinal cortex [20], and parahippocampal gyrus [19].

The frontal or parietal cortex may also be linked to self-perceived memory changes as suggested in the existing literature on anosagnosia (i.e., awareness of cognitive abilities and impairment) [for review; 21]. Functional imaging studies suggest that MCI individuals with poor awareness of their own cognitive ability have altered metabolism in the medial frontal [22], parietotemporal [23], and posteriomedial [24], and posterior cingulate [22] regions. Volumetric brain analyses suggest that more unawareness of one’s cognitive ability is related to smaller medial frontal cortex in MCI [25]. It is plausible that subjective cognitive complaint might also relate to structural differences in these neuroanatomical regions.

The current study cross-sectionally examines whether endorsement of a specific subjective memory question (i.e., “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?”) in MCI corresponds to objective cognitive impairment or structural brain changes. Leveraging the geographically representative and comprehensive Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database, we hypothesize that older adults with MCI who endorse a subjective memory complaint question will have poorer verbal episodic learning and memory performances (i.e., total learning, learning slope, delayed recall, delayed recognition) because rapid forgetting is most commonly the first clinical manifestation of AD [26]. To comprehensively assess the relation of subjective cognitive complaint and cognition, additional measures were included that tap key cognitive domains affected in the prodromal phase of AD, including executive functioning [6], language [27], and processing speed [28]. Our second hypothesis is that older adults with MCI who endorse a subjective memory complaint question will have smaller medial temporal lobe volumes (i.e., hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, and entorhinal cortex) because these structures are affected earliest by AD neuropathology [29]. We also examine structural variables reported in the anosagnosia literature [for review; 21], including cingulate, frontal lobe, and parietal lobe volumes. While all MCI participants enrolled in ADNI have some form of complaint (self, informant, or clinician), the current study aims to provide a comprehensive assessment of the biological underpinnings and cognitive correlates of subjective memory complaint in MCI using a single but common question.

Methods

Participant characteristics

Participants were drawn from the multisite, longitudinal Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI; http://adni.loni.usc.edu/), launched in 2003 to examine neuroimaging biomarkers in the progression of MCI and AD. At the time of participant enrollment, ADNI-1 exclusion criteria included neurological disease other than AD (e.g., Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis), history of brain lesion (e.g., infection, infarction) or head trauma, and history of psychoactive medication use. For a list of full inclusion and exclusion criteria, please refer to http://www.adni-info.org. We accessed publicly available data from the original ADNI cohort (ADNI1) on 4/1/2013, and the current study was limited to participants with MCI at baseline, available baseline structural 1.5T neuroimaging data, and baseline completion of the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [30], which resulted in a total sample size of 397 participants. Analysis of ADNI’s publicly available database was approved by our local Institutional Review Board prior to data access or analysis.

Diagnostic Determination

MCI was defined by ADNI as (a) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [31] score>23; (b) Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) [32] global score ≤0.5 (reflecting mild severity of impairment); (c) relatively spared activities of daily living; (d) objective cognitive impairment as measured by education-adjusted scores on Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) Logical Memory Delayed Recall [33]; (e) expressed concern regarding cognitive change by participant, informant, or study clinician; and (e) not meeting diagnostic criteria for AD (please refer to http://www.adni-info.org) [34].

Cognitive Complaint Status

All ADNI MCI participants were required to have a cognitive complaint at study entry defined as a cognitive concern reported by the participant, the informant, or the study clinician. An additional complaint definition is implemented in the current study. That is, before analyses, MCI participants were categorized into two complaint groups using their response to the GDS question “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?” [17,35]. A “yes” response was coded as a complaint, and a “no” response was coded as no complaint. Thus, while all ADNI MCI participants have some form of complaint (self, informant, or clinician), the current study investigates the relation between a specific subjective cognitive complaint question and cognitive and neuroimaging outcomes.

Neuropsychological Assessment

All participants completed a common neuropsychological protocol assessing multiple cognitive systems as described below:

  1. Episodic Memory: The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [36] is a verbal episodic memory test that includes five learning trials for a list of 15 nouns (Trials 1-5 Total Learning), followed by immediate recall of a 15-item distractor list and short-delay free recall of the original list (Immediate Recall). After a 30-minute filled delay, participants are asked to recall the original list (Delayed Recall) followed by a yes/no recognition test for the original 15-item list (Delayed Recognition). We included learning slope as an outcome (a regression-based slope, which statistically models the linear best fit over all learning trials) [37] because a flat learning slope is characteristic of a classic amnestic profile [38]. The WMS-R Logical Memory [33] is a verbal episodic memory test using a paragraph-long story to assess Immediate Recall and Delayed Recall.

  2. Executive Functioning: Working memory was assessed using Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) Digit Span Backward [39], and sequencing was assessed using Trail Making Test Part B [40].

  3. Information Processing Speed: Information processing speed was assessed by WAIS-III Digit Symbol Coding [39] and Trail Making Test Part A [40].

  4. Language: Lexical retrieval was assessed using the 30-item Boston Naming Test [41], and category fluency was assessed using Animal Naming [42] and Vegetable Naming [43].

Neuroimaging Protocol

The ADNI neuroimaging protocol has been reported in great detail elsewhere [44,45]. Images for the current study included original uncorrected 1.5T T1-weighted high-resolution three-dimensional structural data. Most neuroimaging measures of interest were derived using FreeSurfer Version 5.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) [46,47]. Briefly, participant data was run through the reconstruction process (recon-all) for skull stripping, intensity normalization, and segmentation by tissue type (i.e., cerebrospinal fluid, gray matter, and white matter). White and gray matter regions were segmented using spatial intensity gradients and intensity of gray/white borders [48]. Contiguous ROIs were detected based on intensity similarity and spatial gradient (contour). Bias fields were modeled as a three-dimensional second order polynomial. The cortical surface of the brain was then inflated and registered to a spherical atlas to parcellate gyral and sulcal structures [49]. After recon-all, all data were manually inspected and edited (SD, WC) to correct for registration, topological, and segmentation defects, which included inspection of white and gray surfaces in accordance with the FreeSurfer training manual (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/Edits). During these manual edits, the segmentation of the hippocampus was reviewed and edited as necessary. After these manual edits were complete, images were re-processed through FreeSurfer to update the transformation template and segmentation information. After surface generation, all surfaces were smoothed at 30mm full-width/half-maximum Gaussian kernel to reduce the effects of noise on the results. Variables of interest for the current study were generated as follows:

  1. Cortical thickness analysis: Both intensity and continuity information were used to produce representations of cortical thickness, calculated as the closest difference from the gray/white matter boundary to the gray matter/CSF boundary at each surface vertex [48]. The generated values relied on spatial intensity gradients not restricted to the voxel resolution, so they were not affected by absolute signal intensity and were able to detect submillimeter features. Such cortical thickness procedures have been validated with histological [50] and manual measurements [51]. Average gray matter thickness was calculated for all cortical ROIs. For the current study, ROIs from FreeSurfer [52] included the parahippocampual gyrus and entorhinal cortex.

  2. Volumetric analysis: For volumetric analyses, images underwent automated Talairach transformation and segmentation [53]. Regional volume was calculated based upon the number of voxels occupied within the region of interest. FreeSurfer’s lobe mapping was used to calculate lobar volumes (i.e., frontal, temporal, parietal, cingulate) laterally by each hemisphere and bilaterally (total volumes).

  3. Intracranial Volume: FreeSurfer computed estimated total intracranial volume (etICV) by completing three iterations of likelihood maximizations of the hidden Markov field model, then summing the gray and white matter voxels [54]. All regional and lobar volumes were corrected by intracranial volume (ICV), computed as ROI volume/etICV*100. The ICV-corrected volumes were then used in all volume-based analyses.

Statistical Analysis

Prior to hypothesis testing, between-group comparisons were conducted for demographic variables (i.e., age, education, gender, race), global cognitive functioning (i.e., MMSE), depressed mood (i.e., GDS total score excluding the question “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?”), cognitive performances, and neuroimaging indices, using Wilcoxon rank-sum test for continuous outcomes and Pearson’s chi-squared for categorical outcomes. Effect sizes were calculated according to Cohen’s d formula and interpreted according to published guidelines [55].

Hypothesis testing was conducted using linear regression for each cognitive and neuroimaging outcome with subjective cognitive complaint defined as the independent variable using no complaint as the referent. Each model adjusted for age, race, gender, education, MMSE, GDS (excluding the cognitive complaint question), and apolipoprotein-E (APOE) ε4 status (i.e., positive=ε2/ε4, ε3/ε4, or ε4/ε4 versus negative=ε2/ε2, ε2/ε3, or ε3/ε3). Ordinal least square method was used for parameter estimation. For demographic comparisons and primary outcome models, significance was set a priori at p<0.0022 based on a strict Bonferroni correction factor (i.e., α=0.05/22 comparisons). Secondary analyses were conducted using lateral lobar volumes to assess for possible lateralization effects [for review; 56]. Analyses were conducted using R 2.14.1 with ols function from rms package (http://cran.r-project.org) and MATLAB (2012a; The MathWorks; Natick, MA).

Results

Participant Characteristics

The subjective memory complaint group (n=206) differed from the non-complaint group (n=191) on age (F(1, 395)=23, p<0.001). However, the two groups were statistically comparable for race (x2=1, p=0.31), gender (x2=0.03, p=0.86), education (F(1, 395)=1.2, p=0.27), MMSE (F(1, 395)=0.05, p=0.82), GDS (F(1,395)=1.4, p=0.23), and APOE status (x2=2.6, p=0.11). The complaint group had lower performances on RAVLT Immediate Recall (F(1, 395)=7.5, p=0.006), RAVLT Delayed Recall (F(1, 395)=8.9, p=0.003), and RAVLT Delayed Recognition (F(1, 395)=7.6, p=0.006) but did not differ on all other cognitive measures (p-values>0.08). The groups did not differ on any neuroimaging variable (p-values>0.15). Refer to Table 1 for details and effect sizes.

Table 1.

Participant Characteristics

No Complaint Complaint p-value** Effect Size§
Sample size, n 191 206 -- --
Age, y 77 (7) 73 (8) <0.001 0.26
Sex, % Female 35 36 0.86 --
Race, % White 92 95 0.31 --
Education, y 16 (3) 16 (3) 0.27 0.00
APOE-ε4, % Positive 49 57 0.11 --
Mini-Mental State Examination 27.0 (1.8) 27.0 (1.8) 0.82 0.00
Geriatric Depression Scale 1.0 (1.3) 1.1 (1.2) 0.23 −0.08
Conversion to Dementia,ŧ% 13 17 0.39 --

Neuropsychological Outcomes No Complaint Complaint p-value** Effect Size§

Digit Symbol Coding 36 (11) 37 (11) 0.42 −0.09
Trail Making Test - Part A* 46 (25) 44 (21) 0.29 0.09
Trail Making Test - Part B* 135 (77) 128 (70) 0.48 0.10
Digit Span Backward 6.2 (2.2) 6.2 (1.9) 0.40 0.00
Category Fluency - Animals 15.7 (5.0) 16.0 (4.8) 0.37 −0.06
Category Fluency - Vegetables 10.9 (3.6) 10.6 (3.3) 0.29 0.09
Boston Naming Test 25.5 (4.2) 25.5 (4.0) 0.79 0.00
Logical Memory - Immediate Recall 7.4 (3.2) 6.8 (3.1) 0.09 0.19
Logical Memory - Delayed Recall 4.1 (2.7) 3.6 (2.6) 0.08 0.19
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
 Trials 1-5 Total Learning 31.7 (9.6) 29.8 (8.4) 0.04 0.11
 Learning Slope 0.9 (0.6) 0.8 (0.5) 0.11 0.09
 Immediate Recall 4.3 (3.4) 3.3 (2.8) 0.006 0.16
 Delayed Recall 3.4 (3.6) 2.3 (2.9) 0.003 0.17
 Delayed Recognition 10.2 (3.7) 9.3 (3.5) 0.006 0.14

Neuroimaging Outcomes No Complaint Complaint p-value** Effect Size§

Hippocampal Volume 0.41 (0.08) 0.41 (0.07) 0.93 0.00
Parahippocampal Gyrus Thickness 0.24 (0.04) 0.24 (0.04) 0.15 0.00
Entorhinal Cortex Thickness 0.20 (0.05) 0.21 (0.05) 0.29 −0.10
Cingulate Volume 0.48 (0.04) 0.47 (0.04) 0.75 0.25
Frontal Lobe Volume 4.14 (0.27) 4.15 (0.25) 0.71 −0.04
Temporal Lobe Volume 2.43 (0.17) 2.45 (0.16) 0.15 −0.12
Parietal Lobe Volume 3.04 (0.21) 3.06 (0.20) 0.54 −0.10

Note: Data presented as mean (standard deviation);

*

higher raw scores represent worse performance;

ŧ

mean follow-up interval=2.8±1.2 years;

all volumetric neuroimaging variables corrected for intracranial volume;

§

Cohen’s d;

**

based on Wilcoxon rank-sum tests for continuous variables and Pearson’s chi-square for categorical variables

Subjective Memory Complaint and Cognitive Indices

Linear regressions adjusting for baseline characteristics indicated that MCI participants with a subjective memory complaint performed worse than participants with no complaint on RAVLT Immediate (β=−1.07, p<0.001) and RAVLT Delayed Recall (β=−1.06, p=0.001). The association between subjective complaint and RAVLT Trials 1-5 Total Learning (p=0.006) did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison. No other associations between complaint status and cognition were observed (p-values>0.03) using the Bonferroni-adjusted significance threshold. Refer to Table 2 for details.

Table 2.

Cognitive Complaint Associations with Neuropsychological and Neuroimaging Variables

Neuropsychological Outcomes β Coefficient 95% CI p-value
Digit Symbol Coding 0.80 −1.33, 2.93 0.46
Trail Making Test - Part A −2.15 −6.69, 2.39 0.35
Trail Making Test - Part B −7.35 −21.66, 6.70 0.34
Digit Span Backward 0.06 −0.34, 0.47 0.76
Category Fluency - Animals −0.13 −1.08, 0.81 0.78
Category Fluency - Vegetables −0.52 −1.18, 0.15 0.13
Boston Naming Test −0.32 −1.10, 0.45 0.41
Logical Memory - Immediate Recall −0.69 −1.28, −0.11 0.02
Logical Memory - Delayed Recall −0.47 −0.96, 0.02 0.06
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
 Trials 1-5 Total Learning −2.36 −4.03, −0.69 0.006
 Learning Slope −0.12 −0.23, −0.01 0.03
 Immediate Recall −1.07 −1.68, −0.46 <0.001*
 Delayed Recall −1.06 −1.70, −0.42 0.001*
 Delayed Recognition −0.81 −1.53, −0.10 0.03

Neuroimaging Outcomes β Coefficient 95% CI p-value

Hippocampal Volume −0.01 −0.03, 0.00 0.12
Parahippocampal Gyrus Thickness 0.00 −0.00, 0.01 0.42
Entorhinal Cortex Thickness 0.00 −0.01, 0.01 0.79
Cingulate Volume −0.00 −0.01, 0.01 0.81
Frontal Lobe Volume 0.02 −0.03, 0.08 0.49
Temporal Lobe Volume 0.03 −0.01, 0.06 0.16
Parietal Lobe Volume 0.03 −0.01, 0.08 0.17

Note: CI, confidence interval;

*

Bonferroni corrected p<0.0022;

higher raw scores represent worse performance; referent is no cognitive complaint group;

all volumetric neuroimaging variables corrected for intracranial volume; all models adjusted for age, gender, race, education, Mini-Mental State Examination score, Geriatric Depression Scale score, and APOE status.

Memory Complaint and Neuroimaging Markers

After adjusting for baseline clinical characteristics, subjective memory complaint did not relate to any neuroimaging outcome examined (all p-values>0.12, see Table 2 for details). Secondary analyses yielded no association between subjective memory complaint and lateralized (hemispheric) lobar volumes (data not shown).

Discussion

Leveraging a multicenter cohort, the current study examined the cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of a subjective memory complaint in individuals with a clinical diagnosis of MCI. Our cross-sectional findings suggest that a subjective memory complaint was related to aspects of verbal episodic memory, specifically lower (or worse) immediate and delayed recall on a serial list-learning task. Our findings are consistent with prior research suggesting that MCI individuals with a subjective memory complaint have poorer episodic memory performances, defined with a composite measure (i.e., serial list-learning, story learning, and serial figure learning) in comparison to MCI individuals without a complaint [14].

In contrast, no association was observed between complaint and story learning, consistent with prior work in which we found no cross-sectional or longitudinal differences in story learning performance between MCI elders with and without a self-reported cognitive complaint [18]. The discordant finding between the list and story learning paradigms used in the current study may due to several factors. Foremost, the two measures may assess different aspects of learning and memory. Compared to story learning, serial list-learning has been shown to be more sensitive to episodic memory changes in MCI [57,58] and more sensitive to detection of early AD pathology [59]. Second, we used a correction factor, which may be so strict that it creates a Type II error. In the absence of any correction factor, cognitive complaint would have statistically related to poorer story learning performance. Third, discrepant findings within and across the literature could relate to different complaint assessment methods. Subjective cognitive complaint can be assessed by one [20] or multiple questions [60] that query for changes compared to one’s own past abilities [61], to one’s peers [35], or based upon a functional ability [62]. Different complaint questions may relate differently to objective cognitive performance [63] without comparable clinical significance. Taken cumulatively, it is plausible that the implementation of a more sensitive measure of episodic memory or a strict correction factor, or the method of assessing subjective cognitive complaint in the current study yielded differences not captured in other recent work.

Subjective cognitive complaint was not predictive of any non-memory cognitive performances, including executive functioning, information processing speed, or language skills. This finding is consistent with existing research suggesting that a self-complaint of cognitive change among individuals with MCI is not related to cross-sectional or longitudinal changes in other areas of cognition [18]. The lack of association with non-memory domains could be due to the method by which we defined complaint. In the current study, subjective cognitive complaint is specific to memory (i.e., “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?”), whereas prior work has emphasized general “cognitive decline” [18]. The current findings may speak to more precise concordance between memory-specific concerns and objective memory performances in individuals with MCI.

Based on prior research in cognitively normal elders and the known distribution of pathology early in the AD course, we hypothesized that cognitive complaint would be related to greater atrophy in the hippocampus [19], parahippocampal gyrus [19], and entorhinal cortex [20]. However, our statistical models did not yield any significant associations between memory complaint and the neuroimaging markers of interest, including brain regions commonly affected early in AD (i.e., hippocampal, parahippocampal, and entorhinal cortex) and areas implicated in anosagnosia (i.e., frontal, parietal, and cingulate volumes) [for review; 21,56]. Recent evidence suggests that objective episodic learning and memory impairments (as measured by RAVLT) precede structural imaging evidence of hippocampal atrophy [64]. Given the association between subjective memory concern and objective list-learning performance, it is plausible that a subjective memory complaint is an early clinical marker of AD pathogenesis. However, alternative explanations should be considered. This null finding could suggest that MCI individuals who report a memory change have medial temporal and global atrophy comparable to MCI individuals who deny any memory changes, making detection of any between-group differences difficult. Morphological brain changes in the medial temporal lobe are known to be present in MCI [1,65], but ADNI MCI cohort members may have more medial temporal atrophy than typically seen in MCI given ADNI’s higher conversion rate to dementia (i.e., 16.5% over 12 months) [34] as compared to epidemiological studies (i.e., 2–7%) [66]. It is also possible that our method for segmenting hippocampal volume introduced unwanted variance, making it difficult to detect differences in this anatomical region [67]. Alternatively, we defined cognitive complaint by one memory-focused question, which may be insufficiently sensitive to neuroanatomical changes, especially those areas implicated in anosagnosia.

It is important to note that as part of the diagnostic classification at ADNI enrollment, all MCI participants had some form of cognitive complaint (i.e., self-report, informant-report, or clinician-report). Due to possible inconsistency in site-specific methods for defining complaint and the unavailability of item-level self-report complaint data in the ADNI enrollment dataset, we leveraged responses to one self-report question (i.e., Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?), which is collected as part of the GDS. Our single-item method provided an opportunity to define complaint consistently across all MCI cohort members enrolled across the ADNI sites. The current findings provide new information about how this specific self-perceived memory question relates to cognitive and neuroimaging markers of unhealthy brain aging. Furthermore, results suggesting that endorsement of the item was related to poorer objective episodic memory performance augment past work examining the validity of this particular memory complaint question [35].

The ADNI cohort offers a number of strengths, including nationwide representation of participants, standardized diagnostic criteria, standardized neuroimaging protocol, and standardized neuropsychological protocol. A strength of the current study is that methodologically, we considered memory complaints in tandem with neuropsychological and neuroimaging outcomes. Lastly, restriction of participant inclusion to MCI allowed for a greater understanding of how complaint relates to cognitive and neuroimaging markers of cognitive aging in a population at very high risk for converting to dementia.

The present study has several noteworthy limitations. First, ADNI participants are predominantly White and well-educated (i.e., with a mean education of 16 years), which may limit the generalizability of findings to the population at large. Criteria for MCI diagnosis in ADNI requires a memory complaint, thus all participants in the current study have some form of self-, informant-, or clinician-concern regarding cognitive changes. Although the ADNI MRI protocol was optimized for comparability across different scanning platforms, variability in hardware and software configurations may have contributed unknown variance to the data. While our analytical plan was hypothesis-driven, the current study did not analyze all possible brain structures, so we may have overlooked an important association between memory complaint and neuroanatomical changes not captured in the regions selected. Furthermore, the lobar regions (i.e., frontal, parietal, and cingulate) included in our analyses were large and possibly lacked sufficient sensitivity to detect associations between subjective complaint and these broader cortical areas. Our analyses were cross-sectional, so we are unable to make temporal or causal associations between memory complaint and outcomes, but a longitudinal analysis may help resolve inconsistencies in relations between cognitive complaint and neuroanatomical changes.

The current study provides new information about the cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of memory complaint by suggesting that a memory complaint in MCI is related to worse immediate and delayed recall performances. The current findings enhance this prior literature by investigating detailed verbal episodic learning and memory performances (i.e., total learning, learning slope, immediate recall, delayed recall, recognition), rather than a global composite measure in older adults with MCI. Results indicate that subjective memory complaint correlates not only with delayed recall but also with immediate recall.

The findings highlight that this memory complaint question may have clinical implications for individuals with MCI and that endorsement of the question “Do you feel you have more problems with your memory than most?” is preferentially related to objective memory performance as compared to other cognitive performances (i.e., executive functioning, information processing speed, language). Further research is needed to better understand the clinical relevance of cognitive complaint in MCI and to extend these analyses to cognitively normal older adults to examine the role of memory complaint as an early marker of unhealthy brain aging. Additionally, expanding outcomes to include cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of AD or amyloid and tau imaging will further enhance understanding of the clinical significance of cognitive complaint. Such information could provide clinicians and researchers with an important and easy-to-use tool for identifying individuals at risk for unhealthy brain aging. Early recognition of older adults with abnormal cognitive changes is critical for minimizing the public health burden of dementia and AD.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Alzheimer’s Association NIRG-13-283276 (KAG); K12-HD043483 (KAG); K23-AG030962 (Paul B. Beeson Career Development Award in Aging, ALJ); K24-AG046373 (ALJ); Alzheimer’s Association IIRG-08-88733 (ALJ); R01-AG034962 (ALJ); R01-HL111516 (ALJ); and the Vanderbilt Memory & Alzheimer’s Center.

Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, by the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: Abbott; Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Amorfix Life Sciences Ltd.; AstraZeneca; Bayer HealthCare; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen Idec, Inc.; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; Eisai, Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; GE Healthcare; Innogenetics, N.V.; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development, LLC.; Medpace, Inc.; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer, Inc.; Servier; Synarc, Inc.; and Takeda Pharmaceutical Company. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Rev October 16, 2012 at the University of California, San Diego. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory of NeuroImaging at the University of California, Los Angeles. This research was also supported by NIH grants P30 AG010129 and K01 AG030514.

Footnotes

Authors’ disclosures available online (http://www.j-alz.com/disclosures/view.php?id=2525).

References

  • 1.Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L, Winblad B. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1985–1992. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Albert MS, Dekosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, Gamst A, Holtzman DM, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC, Snyder PJ, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Phelps CH. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Backman L, Jones S, Berger AK, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology. 2005;19:520–531. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tabert MH, Manly JJ, Liu X, Pelton GH, Rosenblum S, Jacobs M, Zamora D, Goodkind M, Bell K, Stern Y, Devanand DP. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fleisher AS, Sowell BB, Taylor C, Gamst AC, Petersen RC, Thal LJ. Clinical predictors of progression to Alzheimer disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68:1588–1595. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000258542.58725.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Albert MS, Moss M, Tanzi R, Jones K. Preclinical prediction of AD using neuropsychological tests. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2001;7:631–639. doi: 10.1017/s1355617701755105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jefferson AL, Byerly LK, Vanderhill S, Lambe S, Wong S, Ozonoff A, Karlawish JH. Characterization of activities of daily living in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:375–383. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318162f197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Griffith HR, Belue K, Sicola A, Krzywanski S, Zamrini E, Harrell L, Marson DC. Impaired financial abilities in mild cognitive impairment: a direct assessment approach. Neurology. 2003;60:449–457. doi: 10.1212/wnl.60.3.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tabert MH, Albert SM, Borukhova-Milov L, Camacho Y, Pelton G, Liu X, Stern Y, Devanand DP. Functional deficits in patients with mild cognitive impairment: prediction of AD. Neurology. 2002;58:758–764. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.5.758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Du AT, Schuff N, Amend D, Laakso MP, Hsu YY, Jagust WJ, Yaffe K, Kramer JH, Reed B, Norman D, Chui HC, Weiner MW. Magnetic resonance imaging of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71:441–447. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.4.441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Bates JF, Atiya M, Killiany RJ, Greve DN, Dale AM, Stern CE, Blacker D, Albert MS, Sperling RA. Medial temporal lobe function and structure in mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 2004;56:27–35. doi: 10.1002/ana.20163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Devanand DP, Pradhaban G, Liu X, Khandji A, De Santi S, Segal S, Rusinek H, Pelton GH, Honig LS, Mayeux R, Stern Y, Tabert MH, de Leon MJ. Hippocampal and entorhinal atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: prediction of Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2007;68:828–836. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000256697.20968.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jessen F, Wiese B, Bachmann C, Eifflaender-Gorfer S, Haller F, Kolsch H, Luck T, Mosch E, van den Bussche H, Wagner M, Wollny A, Zimmermann T, Pentzek M, Riedel-Heller SG, Romberg HP, Weyerer S, Kaduszkiewicz H, Maier W, Bickel H. Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:414–422. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cook S, Marsiske M. Subjective memory beliefs and cognitive performance in normal and mildly impaired older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2006;10:413–423. doi: 10.1080/13607860600638487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Benito-Leon J, Mitchell AJ, Vega S, Bermejo-Pareja F. A population-based study of cognitive function in older people with subjective memory complaints. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22:159–170. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lenehan ME, Klekociuk SZ, Summers MJ. Absence of a relationship between subjective memory complaint and objective memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment (MCI): is it time to abandon subjective memory complaint as an MCI diagnostic criterion? Int Psychogeriatr. 2012;24:1505–1514. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212000695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Purser JL, Fillenbaum GG, Wallace RB. Memory complaint is not necessary for diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and does not predict 10-year trajectories of functional disability, word recall, or short portable mental status questionnaire limitations. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54:335–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.00589.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gifford KA, Liu D, Carmona H, Lu Z, Romano R, Tripodis Y, Martin B, Kowall N, Jefferson AL. Inclusion of an informant yields strong associations between cognitive complaint and longitudinal cognitive outcomes in non-demented elders. J Alzheimers Dis. doi: 10.3233/JAD-131925. (In press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Saykin AJ, Wishart HA, Rabin LA, Santulli RB, Flashman LA, West JD, McHugh TL, Mamourian AC. Older adults with cognitive complaints show brain atrophy similar to that of amnestic MCI. Neurology. 2006;67:834–842. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000234032.77541.a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jessen F, Feyen L, Freymann K, Tepest R, Maier W, Heun R, Schild HH, Scheef L. Volume reduction of the entorhinal cortex in subjective memory impairment. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1751–1756. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zamboni G, Wilcock G. Lack of awareness of symptoms in people with dementia: the structural and functional basis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:783–792. doi: 10.1002/gps.2620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ries ML, Jabbar BM, Schmitz TW, Trivedi MA, Gleason CE, Carlsson CM, Rowley HA, Asthana S, Johnson SC. Anosognosia in mild cognitive impairment: Relationship to activation of cortical midline structures involved in self-appraisal. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13:450–461. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hanyu H, Sakurai H, Hirao K, Shimizu S, Iwamoto T. Unawareness of memory deficits depending on cerebral perfusion pattern in mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:470–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01074.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Nobili F, Mazzei D, Dessi B, Morbelli S, Brugnolo A, Barbieri P, Girtler N, Sambuceti G, Rodriguez G, Pagani M. Unawareness of memory deficit in amnestic MCI: FDG-PET findings. J Alzheimers Dis. 2010;22:993–1003. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rosen HJ, Alcantar O, Rothlind J, Sturm V, Kramer JH, Weiner M, Miller BL. Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive self-appraisal in neurodegenerative disease. Neuroimage. 2010;49:3358–3364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Albert MS. Cognitive and neurobiologic markers of early Alzheimer disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:13547–13551. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mickes L, Wixted JT, Fennema-Notestine C, Galasko D, Bondi MW, Thal LJ, Salmon DP. Progressive impairment on neuropsychological tasks in a longitudinal study of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:696–705. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.6.696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Twamley EW, Ropacki SA, Bondi MW. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging changes in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12:707–735. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706060863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Braak H, Braak E. Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol Aging. 1995;16:271–278. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(95)00021-6. discussion 278–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, Leirer VO. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1983;17:37–49. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, Texas: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Petersen RC, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, Donohue MC, Gamst AC, Harvey DJ, Jack CR, Jr, Jagust WJ, Shaw LM, Toga AW, Trojanowski JQ, Weiner MW. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): Clinical characterization. Neurology. 2010;74:201–209. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lam LC, Lui VW, Tam CW, Chiu HF. Subjective memory complaints in Chinese subjects with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;20:876–882. doi: 10.1002/gps.1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rey A. L’examen clinique en psychologie. Presses Universitaires de France; Paris: 1964. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tulving E. Intratrial and Intertrial Retention: Notes Towards a Theory of Free Recall Verbal Learning. Psychol Rev. 1964;71:219–237. doi: 10.1037/h0043186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bigler ED, Rosa L, Schultz F, Hall S, Harris J. Rey-Auditory Verbal Learning and Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Design performance in Alzheimer’s disease and closed head injury. J Clin Psychol. 1989;45:277–280. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198903)45:2<277::aid-jclp2270450216>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd edition (WAIS-III) Manual. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8:271–276. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. Lea & Febiger; Philadelphia: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Lea & Febiger; Philadelphia, PA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology. 1997;11:138–146. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.11.1.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Jack CR, Jr, Bernstein MA, Fox NC, Thompson P, Alexander G, Harvey D, Borowski B, Britson PJ, JLW, Ward C, Dale AM, Felmlee JP, Gunter JL, Hill DL, Killiany R, Schuff N, Fox-Bosetti S, Lin C, Studholme C, DeCarli CS, Krueger G, Ward HA, Metzger GJ, Scott KT, Mallozzi R, Blezek D, Levy J, Debbins JP, Fleisher AS, Albert M, Green R, Bartzokis G, Glover G, Mugler J, Weiner MW. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): MRI methods. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008;27:685–691. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Weiner MW, Aisen PS, Jack CR, Jr, Jagust WJ, Trojanowski JQ, Shaw L, Saykin AJ, Morris JC, Cairns N, Beckett LA, Toga A, Green R, Walter S, Soares H, Snyder P, Siemers E, Potter W, Cole PE, Schmidt M. The Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative: Progress report and future plans. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6:202–211. e207. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:272–284. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4&#x0003c;272::AID-HBM10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rosas HD, Liu AK, Hersch S, Glessner M, Ferrante RJ, Salat DH, van der Kouwe A, Jenkins BG, Dale AM, Fischl B. Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington’s disease. Neurology. 2002;58:695–701. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.5.695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, Busa E, Morris JC, Dale AM, Fischl B. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Destrieux C, Fischl B, Dale A, Halgren E. A sulcal depth-based anatomical parcellation of the cerebral cortex. NeuroImage. 2009;47:S151. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM. Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Buckner RL, Head D, Parker J, Fotenos AF, Marcus D, Morris JC, Snyder AZ. A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: Reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage. 2004;23:724–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Cosentino S, Stern Y. Metacognitive theory and assessment in dementia: do we recognize our areas of weakness? J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:910–919. doi: 10.1017/s1355617705050964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tremont G, Miele A, Smith MM, Westervelt HJ. Comparison of verbal memory impairment rates in mild cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32:630–636. doi: 10.1080/13803390903401328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Baek MJ, Kim HJ, Kim S. Comparison between the story recall test and the word-list learning test in Korean patients with mild cognitive impairment and early stage of Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2012;34:396–404. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.645020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Tierney M, Nores A, Snow W, Fisher R, Zorzitto M, Reid D. Use of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test in differentiating normal aging from Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s dementia. Psychol Assess. 1994;6:129–134. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wang Y, West JD, Flashman LA, Wishart HA, Santulli RB, Rabin LA, Pare N, Arfanakis K, Saykin AJ. Selective changes in white matter integrity in MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2012;1822:423–430. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Jessen F, Wiese B, Cvetanovska G, Fuchs A, Kaduszkiewicz H, Kolsch H, Luck T, Mosch E, Pentzek M, Riedel-Heller SG, Werle J, Weyerer S, Zimmermann T, Maier W, Bickel H. Patterns of subjective memory impairment in the elderly: association with memory performance. Psychol Med. 2007;37:1753–1762. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Farias ST, Mungas D, Reed BR, Cahn-Weiner D, Jagust W, Baynes K, Decarli C. The measurement of everyday cognition (ECog): scale development and psychometric properties. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:531–544. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.4.531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Amariglio RE, Townsend MK, Grodstein F, Sperling RA, Rentz DM. Specific subjective memory complaints in older persons may indicate poor cognitive function. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:1612–1617. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03543.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Jedynak BM, Lang A, Liu B, Katz E, Zhang Y, Wyman BT, Raunig D, Jedynak CP, Caffo B, Prince JL. A computational neurodegenerative disease progression score: method and results with the Alzheimer’s disease Neuroimaging Initiative cohort. Neuroimage. 2012;63:1478–1486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pennanen C, Kivipelto M, Tuomainen S, Hartikainen P, Hanninen T, Laakso MP, Hallikainen M, Vanhanen M, Nissinen A, Helkala EL, Vainio P, Vanninen R, Partanen K, Soininen H. Hippocampus and entorhinal cortex in mild cognitive impairment and early AD. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:303–310. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00084-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mitchell AJ, Shiri-Feshki M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia--meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2009;119:252–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gronenschild EH, Habets P, Jacobs HI, Mengelers R, Rozendaal N, van Os J, Marcelis M. The effects of FreeSurfer version, workstation type, and Macintosh operating system version on anatomical volume and cortical thickness measurements. PLoS One. 2012;7:e38234. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES