Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2015 Feb 16;4:e03614. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03614

Different types of theta rhythmicity are induced by social and fearful stimuli in a network associated with social memory

Alex Tendler 1, Shlomo Wagner 1,*
Editor: Peggy Mason2
PMCID: PMC4353977  PMID: 25686218

Abstract

Rhythmic activity in the theta range is thought to promote neuronal communication between brain regions. In this study, we performed chronic telemetric recordings in socially behaving rats to monitor electrophysiological activity in limbic brain regions linked to social behavior. Social encounters were associated with increased rhythmicity in the high theta range (7–10 Hz) that was proportional to the stimulus degree of novelty. This modulation of theta rhythmicity, which was specific for social stimuli, appeared to reflect a brain-state of social arousal. In contrast, the same network responded to a fearful stimulus by enhancement of rhythmicity in the low theta range (3–7 Hz). Moreover, theta rhythmicity showed different pattern of coherence between the distinct brain regions in response to social and fearful stimuli. We suggest that the two types of stimuli induce distinct arousal states that elicit different patterns of theta rhythmicity, which cause the same brain areas to communicate in different modes.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.001

Research organism: rat

eLife digest

For the brain to function correctly, the activities of multiple regions must be coordinated. This coordination is thought to be carried out by waves of electrical activity in the brain. One of the most prominent signals within these waves is called the theta rhythm.

The theta rhythm is thought to help coordinate neural activity between the regions of the brain that are involved in learning and memory. However, theta rhythms also appear when subjects encounter emotional stimuli, which suggests that they might have a role in social cognition. Consistent with this idea, theta rhythms are reduced in individuals with autism spectrum disorders, but the exact nature of the relationship between theta rhythms and social behavior has remained unclear.

Tendler and Wagner have now addressed this question directly by implanting electrodes into five brain regions that are active when rats engage in social interactions. Exposing a rat to a social stimulus, such as an unfamiliar visitor rat, caused the intensity of theta rhythms to increase in this network. This change was temporary, with the theta rhythms gradually returning to normal as the novelty of the visitor wore off.

An increase in the intensity of theta rhythms also occurred in the same network when the rats encountered a fearful stimulus, such as a tone that had previously signaled the delivery of a mild electric shock. Notably, however, the fearful stimulus led to an increase in low frequency theta rhythms, whereas the social stimulus led to an increase in high frequency theta rhythms.

These results suggest that social and fearful stimuli give rise to two different forms of alertness or arousal, which are reflected by the two types of theta rhythms in this network within the brain. Tendler and Wagner also suggest that the distinct frequencies of theta rhythms might be used to support different forms of communication between various regions of the brain, depending on the emotional value of the stimuli (for example, are they social or fearful stimuli?) encountered by the animal. This means that emotional states might be dictating cognitive processes such as learning and memory.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.002

Introduction

Oscillatory brain activity, mostly categorized to the theta (3–12 Hz), beta (12–30 Hz), and gamma (30–80 Hz) bands, is thought to coordinate neural activity in vast neuronal assemblies dispersed over different brain regions (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004). This type of coordination may underlie high level cognitive functions, such as speech and social communication (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006; Uhlhaas et al., 2009) that are impaired in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Geschwind, 2009). Increasing evidence suggest that individuals with ASD show deficits in long-range neuronal communication associated with low-frequency rhythms, such as the theta rhythm (Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Rippon et al., 2007; Wass, 2011). Nonetheless, a clear connection between rhythmic brain activity and social behavior has not yet been established.

Mammalian social organization depends on the ability to recognize and remember individual conspecifics (Wiley, 2013). This social recognition memory (SRM) can be assessed in rodents using their innate tendency to investigate novel conspecifics more persistently than familiar ones (Gheusi et al., 1994). In the SRM habituation–dishabituation test, social memory is assessed by the gradual reduction in the amount of time the animal spends investigating a particular social stimulus during consecutive encounters (Ferguson et al., 2002). This short-term memory was shown to be mediated mainly by chemical cues (semiochemicals) perceived via the main and accessory olfactory systems (Dulac and Torello, 2003). Upon binding of semiochemicals to the receptors expressed by the sensory neurons of the main olfactory epithelium and the vomeronasal organ, sensory information is conveyed to the main (MOB) and accessory (AOB) olfactory bulbs, respectively (Dulac and Wagner, 2006). Both bulbs then project, directly and indirectly, to the medial amygdala (MeA) (Pro-Sistiaga et al., 2007; Kang et al., 2011) that is thought to transfer the information to the hippocampus through the lateral septum (LS) (Bielsky and Young, 2004). The MOB projects also to several cortical areas comprising the primary olfactory cortex, of which the piriform cortex (Pir) is best characterized (Wilson and Sullivan, 2011) (Figure 1).

Figure 1. A simplistic scheme of sensory information flow in the network of brain regions thought to underlie social recognition memory.

Figure 1.

Social olfactory cues are detected by sensory neurons in the main olfactory epithelium (MOE) and vomeronasal organ (VNO). These neurons project to the main (MOB) and accessory (AOB) olfactory bulbs, which transmit information, either directly or indirectly (via the cortical nucleus of the amygdala—CoA) to the medial amygdala (MeA). The MOB also innervates the piriform cortex (Pir). The MeA projects to the lateral septum (LS), which innervates the hippocampus (Hip).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.003

Here we hypothesized that social behavior is associated with an elevation of rhythmic activity in the network of brain areas that process social stimuli. To examine this hypothesis, we recorded electrophysiological activity from the brains of freely behaving adult male rats performing the SRM paradigm (Video 1). A telemetric system was used to record from wire electrodes chronically implanted in the five aforementioned brain regions: MOB, AOB, MeA, LS, and Pir (Dulac and Wagner, 2006). We found that social encounters were associated with enhancement of brain rhythmic activity, specifically at 7–10 Hz range, in all brain regions. This enhancement that was proportional to the degree of novelty of the social stimulus appeared to reflect an internal brain-state associated with social arousal. In contrast, a fear-conditioned tone, which is associated with fear arousal, induced rhythmicity in the low theta range (3–7 Hz) in the same network of brain regions. Moreover, social and fearful stimuli elicited different patterns of change in coherence between the distinct brain regions. We hypothesize that these two types of stimuli induce distinct arousal states in the animal, which are reflected by the different kinds of theta rhythmicity. We further suggest that the distinct types of theta rhythmicity support different modes of communication between the various brain areas. These in turn may modify cognitive processes such as memory acquisition and recall depending on the value and saliency of the stimulus by enhancing synchronous neuronal activity between remote neuronal assemblies.

Video 1. Social encounter between two adult male rats in the experimental arena.

Download video file (5.1MB, wmv)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.004

The recorded subject carries a black transmitter equipped with a flashing led light on its head. Frame number is shown in the right low corner. The graph below the video shows the LFP recorded in the AOB (blue), MOB (red), and MeA (green). The bottom graph shows raster plots of spikes detected from the recorded multi-unit activity signal.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.004

Results

Brain theta rhythmicity is modulated by the novelty of the social stimulus

Electrophysiological recordings were carried out in the brains of freely behaving adult male rats performing the SRM habituation–dishabituation paradigm (Figure 2A). We first analyzed the dynamics of the local field potential (LFP) in the course of the behavioral paradigm. A highly rhythmic LFP was recorded in all brain areas during social encounters (Figure 2B). Power spectral density (PSD) analysis of the LFP showed a prominent peak at ∼8 Hz, typical for the high theta band (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004), in all areas (Figure 2C). The value of this peak, termed theta power (TP), was very low in the absence of a social stimulus (Base, Figure 2D–E) but increased profoundly during the first encounter (Enc. 1). It then gradually decreased during further encounters with the same stimulus (Enc. 2–4), but increased again when another novel stimulus was introduced (Enc. 5). These changes in theta power during SRM testing closely followed the changes in investigation time (IT) (Figure 2F), with both parameters appearing to correlate with the degree of stimulus novelty.

Figure 2. Theta rhythmicity in the rat brain is enhanced during social encounters, in correlation with the novelty of the social stimulus.

Figure 2.

(A) A scheme of the habituation–dishabituation SRM paradigm. (B) Examples of LFP traces recorded in the MOB, LS, and MeA during a social encounter. (C) Power spectral density (PSD) analyses of a 5-min LFP recording from all five brain areas during a social encounter. Gray bar represents the 7–9 Hz band. (D) Superimposed PSD analyses of LFP recordings from the MeA of one animal during the various stages of the SRM test. (E) As in D, zooming on the 4–10 Hz range. (F) The ∼8 Hz PSD peak (TP) and social investigation time (IT) for the same experiment as in D, plotted as a function of the encounter number. Encounter 0 represents no stimulus (Base).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.005

We next analyzed the effect of social and non-social stimuli on the dynamics of investigation time and theta power in all recorded brain areas. As exemplified in Figure 3A (lower panel), exposure of an animal to either type of stimulus caused similar dynamics of the investigation time. However, there was a vast difference with regards to the theta power response to the social and non-social stimuli: whereas significant theta power modulation that was similar across all brain regions was observed with social stimuli, whether awake or anesthetized, object and odor stimuli did not cause such an effect (Figure 3A, upper panels).

Figure 3. Theta rhythmicity is modulated by the novelty of social, but not other tested stimuli.

Figure 3.

(A) TP for all brain areas (upper) as well as IT (lower) during the SRM test of one animal, using awake and anesthetized social stimuli as well as object and smell stimuli, all except smell tested with the same animal. (B) Mean TP for the various brain regions averaged (±SEM) and plotted as a function of the test stage, for social (blue, n = 8) and object (red, n = 6) stimuli. A significant difference was found between the various encounters in all brain regions for social stimuli (p < 0.005, one-way repeated measures ANOVA, Figure 3—source data 1A), while no difference was found for object recognition (p > 0.05, Figure 3—source data 1B). Post hoc paired t-test showed significant differences between Enc. 1 and Enc. 4 as well as between Enc. 4 and Enc. 5 (dashed lines) in all brain regions for social stimuli (*pcorr < 0.05, Figure 3—source data 2). (C) As in B, for the IT of the social and object paradigms. Unlike the TP, both paradigms showed similarly significant modulation of the IT (Figure 3—source data 1–2).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.006

Figure 3—source data 1. Theta power (TP) modulation between encounters.
One-way ANOVA (repeated measures) test was used to determine whether there is a significant difference between the mean ΔTP of all five encounters during either social (1a) or object (1b) recognition. The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests. Sphericity was assessed by Mauchly's test.
elife03614s001.docx (24.5KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.007
Figure 3—source data 2. Statistical assessment of habituation and dishabituation.
Paired t-tests were used for the social (2a) and object (2b) recognition paradigms, to examine if the differences between Enc.1 and Enc. 4 (habituation), as well as between Enc. 4 and Enc. 5 (dishabituation) are statistically significant. Tests were one-sided and corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni's correction.
elife03614s002.docx (25.2KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.008

Combined analyses of the modulation of both theta power and investigation time in animals exposed to social and object stimuli are presented in Figure 3B. Social stimuli caused a marked increase of mean theta power during Enc. 1 compared to Base, with the MOB and AOB showing the largest changes (6.2 dB/Hz) and other areas showing more moderate ones (4.0–5.1 dB/Hz). In all regions tested, the theta power decreased gradually during the habituation phase (Enc. 1–4) but returned the values obtained in Enc. 1 after dishabituation (Enc. 5) (p < 0.005 one-way repeated measures ANOVA, *pcorr < 0.05 post-hoc paired t-test, Figure 3—source data 1–2). In contrast, object stimuli elicited a much weaker initial change from Base to Enc. 1 (1.1–2.7 dB/Hz) in all brain regions. Furthermore, the theta power showed modulation during the object paradigm similarly to the social paradigm only in the MOB, and even this change was not statistically significant (p > 0.05, Figure 3—source data 1). In a sharp contrast to the theta power, comparison of the investigation time of the social and object paradigms showed a highly similar course and magnitude of habituation and dishabituation that were statistically significant in both cases (Figure 3C, Figure 3—source data 1–2). Taken together, these results show that in almost all recorded brain areas, theta power is modulated by the degree of novelty of social but not object stimuli.

The modulation of theta rhythmicity during social encounters is driven by an internal brain state of arousal

The lack of theta power modulation despite the clear investigation time modulation induced by object stimuli rejects the possibility that the theta rhythmicity is caused by the investigative behavior. We therefore reasoned that rather, theta power modulation may reflect processes that are either directly driven by the sensory input (Bottom-Up processes) or induced by an internal state of the brain that is modulated by the saliency of the social stimulus (Top-Down processes). In order to distinguish between these two possibilities, we continued our recordings for 5 min after the stimulus was removed from the arena (Post 1–5). As depicted in Figure 4A, the theta rhythmicity did not cease with the removal of the social stimulus following Enc. 1, but remained at a high level during most of the Post 1 period (for spectrograms of the full experiment, see Figure 4—figure supplements 1–5). Plotting the mean instantaneous theta power as a function of time, revealed that this was true for all encounters with a social stimulus. In contrast, encounters with object stimuli were followed by a sharp drop in the theta power to a low level almost immediately following stimulus removal (Figure 4B, for all other brain areas see Figure 4—figure supplements 6–7). This significant reduction in mean theta power between the Enc. and Post periods of the object paradigm was characteristic of all brain areas (Figure 4C, *p < 0.05 paired t-test, Figure 4—source data 1). In contrast, high theta power levels were found in both these periods in the social paradigm (p > 0.05). Moreover, all encounters with social stimuli showed a steep but gradual increase in theta power during the first 15 s in which the stimulus was being transferred into the arena (Figure 4A,D, gray bars). This rise in theta power probably reflects the subject's anticipation for a social meeting, as there was no similar increase with object stimuli (Figure 4D). Altogether, these data suggest that the changes in theta power during the SRM test reflect a graded internal brain state of arousal that is proportional to the novelty of the social stimulus and slowly fades away after its removal.

Figure 4. Modulation of the theta rhythmicity by social stimulus novelty reflects an internal state in the brain.

(A) Color-coded spectrograms of the LFP recorded in the MOB (upper), LS (middle), and MeA (lower) for 5 min before (Base), during (Enc. 1), and after (Post 1) the first encounter of the SRM test. All spectrograms are averages of five animals (4 animals for LS). Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus transfer to the arena. (B) Upper—instantaneous ΔTP (change from mean Base) in the LS averaged over four rats (±SEM) during the Enc. and Post periods of all trials (1-5), for social (left, n = 5) and object (right, n = 4) paradigms. The 15-min breaks between last Post and next Enc. periods are labeled with gray bars. Lower—mean (±SEM) values for the corresponding periods shown above. (C) Comparison of mean ΔTP averaged over all trials (1-5) for each brain area, between the Enc. and Post periods of the social and object paradigms (*p < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 4—source data 1). (D) Left—the instantaneous ΔTP shown in B, expanded to show the initial 50 s of all encounters. Gray area represents the 15 s needed for stimulus transfer to the experimental arena. Right—The same for object stimuli.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.009

Figure 4—source data 1. Comparison of ΔTP between Enc. and Post periods.
Paired t-tests were used to compare between the mean ΔTP across Enc. vs the mean ΔTP across Post periods. The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.
elife03614s003.docx (22KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.010

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Mean LFP spectrograms across the SRM paradigm for the AOB.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the LFP recorded in the AOB during the SRM test. Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus delivery to the arena. Mean of five animals.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Mean LFP spectrograms across the SRM paradigm for the MOB.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the LFP recorded in the MOB during the SRM test. Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus delivery to the arena. Mean of five animals.
Figure 4—figure supplement 3. Mean LFP spectrograms across the SRM paradigm for the MEA.

Figure 4—figure supplement 3.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the LFP recorded in the MEA during the SRM test. Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus delivery to the arena. Mean of five animals.
Figure 4—figure supplement 4. Mean LFP spectrograms across the SRM paradigm for the LS.

Figure 4—figure supplement 4.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the LFP recorded in the LS during the SRM test. Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus delivery to the arena. Mean of four animals.
Figure 4—figure supplement 5. Mean LFP spectrograms across the SRM paradigm for the Pir.

Figure 4—figure supplement 5.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the LFP recorded in the Pir during the SRM test. Gray bar marks the 15 s needed for stimulus delivery to the arena. Mean of five animals.
Figure 4—figure supplement 6. Comparison of mean instantaneous TP between social and object stimuli, for the AOB and MOB.

Figure 4—figure supplement 6.

Upper panels—instantaneous ΔTP (change from mean Base) in each brain area averaged over all animals (mean ± SEM) during the Enc. and Post periods of all trials (1–5), for social (left, n = 5 rats) and object (right, n = 4 rats) paradigms. The 15-min breaks between last Post and next Enc. periods are labeled with gray bars. Lower panels—mean (±SEM) values for the corresponding periods shown above.
Figure 4—figure supplement 7. Comparison of mean instantaneous TP between social and object stimuli, for the MeA and Pir.

Figure 4—figure supplement 7.

Upper panels—instantaneous ΔTP (change from mean Base) in each brain area averaged over all animals (mean±SEM) during the Enc. and Post periods of all trials (1–5), for social (left, n = 5 rats) and object (right, n = 4 rats) paradigms. The 15 min breaks between last Post and next Enc. periods are labeled with gray bars. Lower panels—mean (±SEM) values for the corresponding periods shown above.

The theta rhythmicity during social behavior emerges from multiple sources with dynamic coherence between brain areas

The theta rhythmicity recorded in the network may reflect a single rhythm originating from one source. In that case, the various brain regions are expected to display high correlation and similar dynamics of coherence in their rhythmicity. Alternatively, if it represents a combination of multiple independent rhythms arising from several sources, we expect low correlation and differential dynamics of coherence between various brain regions. To discriminate between these possibilities, we first examined the cross-correlation of the LFP, filtered in the theta range, between the MeA and the other brain areas. Despite the fact that both areas are directly connected to the MeA, the strongest correlation appeared with the LS and the weakest with the MOB (Figure 5A–D). Moreover, whereas the correlation between the MeA and LS was significantly higher during Enc. 1 (blue) compared to Base (red), the MOB showed consistently low correlation with the MeA during both periods. The presence of a social stimulus thus appears to differentially affect the correlation of theta rhythmicity between distinct brain areas.

Figure 5. Differential and dynamic correlation of theta rhythmicity between specific brain regions.

Figure 5.

(A) Upper—superimposed LFP traces (filtered 5–11 Hz) from the MeA (black) and LS (colored) of one animal during Base (left, red) and Enc. 1 (right, blue). Lower—cross-correlations between both regions for each of the 300 s recorded during the same periods, with peaks labeled by colored dots. (B) Same as A for the MeA and MOB. (C) Middle—distribution of the cross-correlation peaks for the data in A. Borders—histograms of the cross-correlation peaks in the correlation (right) and lag (bottom) axes. Mean ± SD are marked to the left (correlation) or above (lag) the histograms. (D) Same as C for the data in B.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.018

We next analyzed the coherence of the LFP signal among all brain areas during the Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods of the SRM paradigm. As depicted in Figure 6A, the coherence between the MeA and the LS showed several prominent peaks, especially in the theta and gamma bands. Yet, while no change was recorded in the gamma band, the theta coherence showed a significant increase between the Base and Enc. 1. Furthermore, similarly to theta rhythmicity itself (Figure 4), the high coherence at theta range persisted during the Post 1 period despite the lack of a social stimulus (Figure 6A,C). In contrast, the coherence in theta band between the MeA and MOB remained low throughout all periods (Figure 6B,C). Analyses across all regions revealed a hierarchy in the theta coherence between the MeA and all other areas, ranging from a low level with the MOB and AOB, medium coherence with the Pir and high coherence with the LS (Figure 6D). This notion of functional hierarchy between brain regions is strengthened by the fact that despite their largest physical distance, the highest level of theta coherence was found between the MeAs in the two hemispheres (Figure 6—figure supplements 1,3). Furthermore, the theta coherence between the MeA and the higher brain centers (Pir, LS) significantly increased during Enc. 1 and Post 1 (*pcorr < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 6—source data 1), while no change was recorded between the MeA and both areas of the olfactory bulb (MOB, AOB, pcorr > 0.05). This suggests the existence of at least two independent theta rhythms, one that governs the olfactory bulb and another that dominates higher brain structures. This conclusion is further supported by the findings that the MOB shows opposite relationships with all other brain areas; high coherence with the AOB and low coherence with the higher areas (Figure 6E, Figure 6—figure supplements 2,3). Moreover, a significant enhancement in theta coherence with the AOB was observed during Enc. 1 and Post1 (*pcorr < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 6—source data 1), while all other regions showed no change (pcorr > 0.05, paired t-test). Interestingly, similar enhancement of theta coherence between the AOB and MOB was recorded with object stimuli, while these stimuli did not cause any enhancement of the coherence between the MeA and LS or Pir (Figure 6F,G, Figure 6—source data 1). Together, these data support multiple sources of theta rhythmicity in the network.

Figure 6. Theta coherence between specific brain regions increases during social encounter.

(A) Mean (n = 10 animals) coherence (0–100 Hz) of the LFP signals recorded in the MeA and LS during Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods. (B) Same animals, coherence analysis between the MeA and MOB. (C) Spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the coherence analyses shown in A (between MeA and LS, upper panel) and B (between MeA and MOB, lower panel). (D) Mean coherence at 8 Hz between the MeA and all other areas (MOB, AOB n = 11; LS, Pir n = 10) during the Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods of social encounter (*pcorr < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 6—source data 1A). (E) Same as D, for coherence of the MOB with all other areas (*pcorr < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 6—source data 1B). (F) Same as D, for object stimuli (Figure 6—source data 1C). (G) Same as E, for object stimuli (*pcorr < 0.05, paired t-test, Figure 6—source data 1D).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.019

Figure 6—source data 1. Assessment of change in theta Coherence from Base to either Enc. 1 or Post 1.
The change from Base to Enc. 1 (upper) and from Base to Post 1 (lower), in theta coherence during social recognition between the MeA and all other areas (1a) and between the MOB and all areas (1b), as well as during object recognition between the MeA and all other areas (1c), and between the MOB and all areas (1d), was statistically validated using paired t-tests, corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.
elife03614s004.docx (26.1KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.020

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Mean spectrograms of coherence between the MeA and all other areas during trial 1 of the SRM paradigm.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the mean LFP coherence (MOB, AOB—n = 11; LS, Pir—n = 10, cMeA—contralateral MeA—n = 3) between the MOB and all other brain areas, during the first trial of SRM test, each depicting continuous 15 min divided to the Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods.
Figure 6—figure supplement 2. Mean spectrograms of coherence between the MOB and all other areas during trial 1 of the SRM paradigm.

Figure 6—figure supplement 2.

Color-coded spectrograms (0–20 Hz) of the mean LFP coherence (MeA, AOB—n = 11; LS, Pir—n = 10, cMeA—contralateral MeA—n = 3) between the MOB and all other brain areas, during the first trial of SRM test, each depicting continuous 15 min divided to the Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods.
Figure 6—figure supplement 3. Mean theta coherence during trial 1 of the SRM paradigm.

Figure 6—figure supplement 3.

(A) Mean coherence at 8 Hz between the MeA and all other areas (MOB, AOB n = 11; LS, Pir n = 10, cMeA—contralateral MeA—n = 3) during the Base, Enc. 1, and Post 1 periods of the SRM paradigm. (B) Same as A, for coherence of the MOB with all other areas.

Distinct types of theta rhythmicity are induced in the same brain regions by social and fearful stimuli

Theta rhythmicity was previously found to be elicited in several brain regions during states of arousal, mainly in response to fearful stimuli (Knyazev, 2007). This phenomenon was best studied in the context of fear learning in a network of brain regions comprising the basolateral complex of the amygdala (lateral and basolateral amygdala), hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex (Pape and Pare, 2010). In this network, a recall of a fearful memory, induced by a fear-conditioned stimulus, elicits robust theta rhythmicity that shows high coherence between these brain regions (Paré and Collins, 2000; Pare et al., 2002; Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Pape et al., 2005; Popa et al., 2010). Here, we examined whether the brain state-induced theta rhythmicity during the SRM paradigm is similar to the fear-induced rhythmicity. To address this question, we compared the theta rhythmicity induced by a social encounter to that of a fear stimulus within the social network that we investigated. To that end, a new cohort of six animals was implanted with wire electrodes as before, with an additional electrode in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), which was recently shown to be involved in social motivation (Dölen et al., 2013; Gunaydin et al., 2014). These animals were fear-conditioned by coupling a 40-s long tone to an electrical foot shock for five consecutive times separated by 180-s intervals (Figure 7—figure supplement 1A). A day later the electrical activity was recorded in two consecutive sessions, each following a 30 min of habituation to the arena. The first session was recorded during a recall of fear memory (FC experiment), and the second during a 5-min long encounter with a novel social stimulus (SR experiment). During the FC experiments (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B), introduction of the fear-conditioned tone caused animals to begin moving intensively, followed by immobility (freezing) towards the end of the tone, in anticipation of the foot shock. The freezing response was especially significant at the end of the first tone (Figure 7—figure supplement 1C). Thus, the fear-conditioned tone caused a robust arousal state that was associated with intense movement of the conditioned animals. We then compared the theta rhythmicity between the FC and SR experiments. A PSD analysis of the LFP signals recorded in the LS during 5 min prior to stimulus introduction (Base) yielded a similar profile in both cases (Figure 7A, red). However, the PSD was very different between the two types of stimuli during the first 15 s following stimulus introduction (Stimulus) (Figure 7A, blue). Whereas the fear stimulus caused a marked peak at the low theta range (3–7 Hz), the social stimulus resulted in a peak at the high theta range (7–10 Hz). This change is clearly observed when subtracting the Base PSD from the Stimulus profile (Figure 7B). These differences appeared in all recorded brain regions (Figure 7C) and Statistical analysis showed a highly significant interaction between the type of experiment (FC or SR) and theta band (Figure 7D) (**p < 0.01, two-way repeated measures ANOVA, Figure 7—source data 1). Thus, we conclude that fearful and social stimuli cause changes in very different ranges of theta rhythmicity in the same limbic network of brain regions. We suggest that these different types of theta rhythmicity reflect distinct arousal states; the low theta reflects aversive arousal that is associated with fear while the high theta reflects appetitive arousal associated with a social encounter.

Figure 7. Distinct types of theta rhythmicity are induced by social and fearful stimuli.

(A) PSD analyses (0–20 Hz) of LFP signal recorded in the LS of one animal, 5 min prior to stimulus introduction (Base, red) and 15 s following it (Stimulus, blue) during fear memory recall (left, FC) and social encounter (right, SR). (B) The change between Stimulus and Base PSD analyses (Stimulus minus Base) shown in A, for FC and SR, superimposed. (C) Mean change in PSD profile for all brain areas of the same six animals during the FC (continuous lines) and SR (dashed lines) experiments. (D) Mean (±SEM) values of the peak change in PSD in the low (4–8 Hz, red and blue) and high (8–12 Hz, pink and light blue) theta ranges for the FC (red and pink) and SR (blue and light blue) experiments (**p < 0.01, experiment X theta range interaction, two-way repeated measures ANOVA, Figure 7—source data 1).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.024

Figure 7—source data 1. Comparison of change in theta power in low and high theta bands between social and fearful stimuli.
Comparison of the change in theta power between social recognition (SR) and fear conditioning (FC) at high and low theta ranges, statistically validated using two-way repeated measures ANOVA (p—experiment X theta range interaction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.
elife03614s005.docx (21KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.025

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Arousal-driven locomotion during recall of fear memory.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) A schematic drawing of the fear conditioning session, comprising five events of 40-sec tone (gray bar) followed by brief electrical foot shock (red bar). (B) Locomotion activity of one animal during the recall of fear memory, 1 day after fear conditioning, plotted as a function of the experimental stage. Gray bars represent the 40-sec long tone. Tone start is marked on the X-axis by T1…5 and tone end by S1…5. (C) Mean locomotion (n = 6 animals) during fear recall around the first tone, as a function of time. Tone started 15 sec from the beginning of the experiment and is marked by a gray bar. Note the intense locomotion of the animals during most of the tone, as opposed to their freezing at the end of it, when expecting the electrical foot shock. Black dashed line represents the 15-sec period during which theta activity was calculated. Error bars represent SEM.

Distinct changes in coherence are induced in the network by social and fearful stimuli

We next examined how the coherence between the various brain regions changes in response to the two types of arousing stimuli. Figure 8A depicts the coherence between the MeA and LS during Base and Stimulus periods of FC and SR experiments, respectively. The change in coherence of the two stimuli is presented in Figure 8B and reveals a positive peak at the high theta range for the social encounter, and at the low theta range for the fear memory recall. A quantitative analysis of all coherence changes within the network in both ranges showed that this tendency generally holds for all pairs of brain regions (Figure 8C). Accordingly, most pairs showed a statistically significant interaction between the type of experiment (FC or SR) and theta band (high or low) (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, two-way repeated measures ANOVA, Figure 8—source data 1). Nevertheless, the magnitude of changes was different between distinct pairs. For example, the changes in the coherence between the LS and NAcc were much smaller than those recorded between the Pir and NAcc and did not show any statistical significance. Moreover, the increases of coherence between the AOB-MOB and MOB-Pir pairs were much bigger in SR compared to the FC experiment. We conclude that the distinct arousal states are characterized by distinct patterns of coherence changes within that same network of brain regions (Figure 9).

Figure 8. Distinct changes in theta coherence between various brain regions are induced by social and fearful stimuli.

Figure 8.

(A) Coherence analyses (0–20 Hz) of LFP signal recorded in the LS and MeA of one animal, 5 min prior to stimulus introduction (Base, red) and 15 s following it (Stimulus, blue) during fear memory recall (left, FC) and social encounter (right, SR). (B) The change between Stimulus and Base coherence analyses (Stimulus minus Base) shown in A, for FC and SR, superimposed. (C) Mean (±SEM) values of the peak change in coherence between all possible couples of brain areas in the low (4–8 Hz, red and blue) and high (8–12 Hz, pink and light blue) theta ranges for the FC (red and pink) and SR (blue and light blue) experiments (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, experiment X theta range interaction, two-way repeated measures ANOVA, Figure 8—source data 1).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.027

Figure 8—source data 1. Comparison of change in coherence in low and high theta bands between social and fearful stimuli.
Comparison of the change in coherence between social recognition (SR) and fear conditioning (FC) at high and low theta ranges (right), statistically validated using two-way repeated measures ANOVA (p—experiment X theta range interaction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.
elife03614s006.docx (21.8KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.028

Figure 9. Different patterns of coherence change characterize the distinct arousal states.

Figure 9.

Graphical color-coded presentation of the mean changes in coherence for the FC and SR experiments.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.029

Discussion

This study demonstrates that an encounter with a social stimulus causes increased LFP rhythmicity in the high theta range (7–10 Hz), in a network of limbic brain areas associated with social memory. Strikingly, the change in theta rhythmicity is directly proportional to the novelty of the social partner, and may thus be considered a neuronal correlate of short-term social memory (Liebe et al., 2012). Since the modulation of theta rhythmicity is observed even when anesthetized stimuli are used, we infer that it does not depend on the behavior of the social stimulus. Despite the similarity in investigative behavior, such modulation of theta rhythmicity is not observed with object stimuli, suggesting that it is social specific. Since the augmented theta rhythmicity and the associated increase in theta coherence persist beyond the removal of the social stimulus itself, we conclude that these parameters do not mirror sensory inputs but rather reflect a state of arousal that slowly fades away. This is in agreement with the fact that the increase in theta power occurs prior to the actual introduction of the social stimulus in the arena, suggesting increased arousal due to the anticipated social encounter. Finally, since the change in theta rhythmicity during the SRM test correlates with the novelty of the social stimulus, we posit that it reflects a graded level of arousal, which is proportional to the stimulus saliency.

One of the questions that arise from the study is whether the social encounter-induced state of arousal is elicited by the ‘social’ quality of the stimulus or whether it simply results from the complexity of the stimulus. Notably, the social stimulus is much more complex than the single object or odor stimuli that we used as controls. It emits a complex mixture of odors and semiochemicals, and in addition to the main and accessory olfactory systems, it also stimulates the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems. It is not likely that the full complexity of the social stimulus may be mimicked by the use of any artificial mixture of odors, hence the possibility that the arousal state results from the complexity of the stimulus cannot be excluded. On the other hand, at least as regards to fear-associated arousal, it is well documented (Takahashi et al., 2008) that a very simple cue is sufficient to evoke a state of arousal, such that is observed by the freezing of rodents in response to the pure odorant 2,3,5-Trimethyl-3-thiazoline (TMT), a component of fox odor (Fendt et al., 2003), or to a pure tone in a fear conditioning paradigm (Rogan et al., 1997). This suggests that the factor that determines the state of arousal is not the complexity of the stimulus but rather the information it embodies with regards to the natural environment of the animal.

Many studies, both in animals and humans, have linked brain theta rhythmicity to the processing of emotional cues (Sainsbury and Montoya, 1984; Sainsbury et al., 1987a; Sainsbury et al., 1987b; Aftanas et al., 2001; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2009; Knyazev et al., 2009; Maratos et al., 2009; Luo et al., 2013). In animals theta rhythmicity was mostly studied in the hippocampus (Buzsáki, 2002), where it was classified into two types, Type 1 and Type 2. The atropine-insensitive Type 1 theta rhythmicity shows higher frequency (8–12 Hz) and is thought to be associated mainly with voluntary movement. In contrast, atropine-sensitive Type 2 rhythmicity is characterized by lower frequency (4–8 Hz) and is thought to be linked to arousal during states of immobility (Bland, 1986; Sainsbury, 1998). Notably, Type 2 rhythmicity was mostly studied using states of fear and aversive stimuli and was shown to be induced by neutral stimuli if conditioned by fear or introduced in the presence of predators (Sainsbury and Montoya, 1984; Sainsbury et al., 1987a; Sainsbury et al., 1987b). The relationship of the two types of hippocampal theta rhythmicity and similar rhythms recorded from other brain regions, such as in our case, should be cautiously examined for several reasons. First, recent studies showed that in the hippocampus itself there are differences in the profile of theta rhythmicity between the earlier studied dorsal hippocampus and the more recently studied ventral hippocampus (Adhikari et al., 2010), the latter of which shows theta rhythmicity with stronger association to the one recoded in the mPFC (Jacinto et al., 2013), and may be dissociated from the dorsal hippocampus under certain conditions such as decision making (Schmidt et al., 2013). Second, even for the dorsal hippocampus the dichotomy between the two types of theta rhythmicity is far from being perfect with Type 2 rhythmicity reported to reach 12 Hz at some states and Type 1 rhythmicity reported to disappear during certain movements (Sainsbury, 1998). Interestingly, researchers reported that in cats the correlation between movement and Type 1 rhythmicity was good at the beginning of the experiments, when a lot of exploratory and object manipulation behavior was observed, but deteriorated towards the end of the experiments, when the animals were still moving but were uninterested in the task (Sainsbury, 1998). This might suggest that in the hippocampus too, high frequency Type 1 theta may be associated with sensory information processing during ‘positive’ arousal states associated with motivational voluntary movements, such as exploration, while low frequency Type 2 theta may be linked to ‘negative’ arousal states, such as those caused by fear, which is usually associated with freezing.

Regardless of the nature of hippocampal theta oscillations, theta rhythmicity associated with emotional states was reported in several other brain areas (Bland et al., 1993; Bland and Oddie, 2001; Pignatelli et al., 2012). Of particular interest is the finding that theta rhythmicity in a limbic network that includes the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral complex of the amygdala (lateral and basolateral amygdala) is associated with fear memories. Importantly, the consolidation and recall of long-term fear memory was found to be associated with elevated coherence of the theta rhythmicity in this network (Paré and Collins, 2000; Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Pape et al., 2005; Popa et al., 2010; Lesting et al., 2013), while its extinction was associated with a decline in coherence, in a brain-region dependent manner (Narayanan et al., 2007). Moreover, interfering with theta coherence through local electrical micro-stimulation affected fear-memory recall and extinction depending on theta phase (Lesting et al., 2013). Thus, coordinated arousal-induced theta rhythmicity within this network seems to be involved in consolidation and recall of aversive memories (Popa et al., 2010; Lesting et al., 2013). Here, we demonstrated for the first time that similar phenomena occur in a distinct network of limbic areas that are linked to social memory, in the course of social encounters. Importantly, a comparison of the theta activity between social and fearful stimuli revealed that although both cause a state of arousal, the patterns of theta rhythmicity and coherence within the same network are completely different. First, in agreement with previous studies (Paré and Collins, 2000; Seidenbecher et al., 2003; Pape et al., 2005; Popa et al., 2010; Lesting et al., 2013), the recall of fear memory causes rhythmicity in the low theta range, while a social encounter elicits rhythmic activity in the high theta range. This suggests the existence of two types of arousal: fear-associated arousal and social-related arousal. Second, each of these conditions caused a distinct pattern of coherence changes between the same regions of the network. Given these results, we hypothesize that the distinct types of theta rhythmicity promote different communication protocols (Kepecs et al., 2006) for the coordination of neural activity in the network, which depends on the emotional state of the animal. Our results are in agreement with the hypothesis that theta rhythmicity facilitates cognitive processes such as memory formation that are associated with emotionally salient stimuli (Pelletier and Paré, 2004).

The source and distribution of theta rhythms in the mammalian brain are not fully understood (Pignatelli et al., 2012). This issue was extensively studied in the hippocampus (Buzsáki, 2002), which was shown to have the capacity to self-generate theta rhythmicity (Goutagny et al., 2009). Yet, as described above theta rhythmicity also exists in various cortical and limbic areas, where it shows dynamic coherence with the hippocampal theta rhythm. One area shown to display robust theta rhythmicity is the olfactory bulb, where it follows the rhythm of respiration (‘sniff cycle’) (Rojas-Libano et al., 2014). Sniffing, similarly to whisking, is a sensory sampling activity, the rate of which dynamically changes throughout the theta band and is strongly influenced by internal arousal and motivational state of the animal (Clarke and Trowill, 1971; Chang, 1992). Specifically, high-frequency sniffing (8–12 Hz) develops in anticipation of reward delivery (Freeman et al., 1983; Monod et al., 1989; Kepecs et al., 2007; Wesson et al., 2008). The olfactory bulb theta rhythm and sniffing are not usually coherent with the hippocampal rhythm. However, in some odor-based learning tasks these rhythms do become transiently coherent (Macrides et al., 1982; Kay, 2005; Martin et al., 2007), a process that was suggested to be mediated by cholinergic neurons in the medial septum (Tsanov et al., 2014). Interestingly, whisking was shown to get occasionally phase locked with the sniff cycle (Cao et al., 2012; Ranade et al., 2013) or with the hippocampal theta rhythm (Komisaruk, 1970) during exploratory behavior. Thus, various generators of theta rhythmicity in the brain, such as those reflected by sniffing, whisking, or the hippocampal theta rhythm may become dynamically coupled by the brain neuromodulatory systems. While we did not monitor sniffing in our experiments, several recent studies reported changes in sniffing during both social interactions (Assini et al., 2013; Wesson, 2013) and fear conditioning (Shionoya et al., 2013). These studies showed that the sniff cycle adopt high-range theta rhythmicity during social interactions and low-range rhythmicity during fear conditioning. These differences are probably reflected by the distinct rates of theta rhythmicity that we record in the MOB and AOB during these conditions. This may explain our observation of high coherence between MOB-AOB and the low coherence each of them display with all other regions. Moreover, while the coherence between the MOB-AOB is increased during exploration of both social and object stimuli, the coherence between the LS–MeA increases only during social interactions. Thus, the theta rhythmicity displayed by the AOB and MOB probably emerges from a distinct generator, most likely the sniff cycle, that is separate from the one causing rhythmicity in higher brain areas. Furthermore, the significant differences in correlation and coherence dynamics between the various limbic areas suggest the involvement of distinct generators as well. For example, neither paradigm showed significant coherence changes between the LS-NAcc, as opposed to a significant increase in coherence between the LS-MeA or LS-Pir during social interactions. It should be noted that these differences cannot not be accounted for by local diffusion of LFP signals, since the LS is much closer to the NAcc than to the MeA or Pir. Direct synaptic connections cannot explain these differences either as the MeA shows very low coherence with the AOB, despite the strong bidirectional connections between them, but rather displays the highest coherence with the contralateral MeA, despite the lack of direct synaptic pathway (Canteras et al., 1995). Therefore, the differential coherence changes between distinct pairs of brain regions during the various conditions are most likely mediated by either a common input to these regions or via brain-region specific neuromudulatory systems. However, the arousal-driven modulation of theta rhythmicity which seems to be common to all brain regions is probably mediated by a general, brain-wide neuromodulatory mechanism such as neurohormonal activity (Lee and Dan, 2012; Marder, 2012).

An ever growing body of evidence implies rhythmic brain activity in various cognitive processes, particularly in memory acquisition and recall (Fell and Axmacher, 2011; Buzsáki and Watson, 2012; Cannon et al., 2014). Specifically, slow frequency rhythms such as the theta rhythm, are hypothesized to mediate communication between brain regions and to promote the temporal binding of neural assemblies in these areas into coherent networks subserving specific cognitive processes (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004; Jutras and Buffalo, 2010; Benchenane et al., 2011; Fell and Axmacher, 2011). During the last decade, several prominent theories implied a disordered or weak communication among brain regions as a major deficit underlying ASD etiology and symptoms (Brock et al., 2002; Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Kana et al., 2011; Wass, 2011). Indeed, multiple recent studies found reduction in the power and coherence of slow brain rhythms, such as the alpha and theta rhythms, in ASD individuals (Murias et al., 2007; Coben et al., 2008; Isler et al., 2010; Barttfeld et al., 2013; Doesburg et al., 2013; Machado et al., 2013; Kikuchi et al., 2015). In agreement with these findings, our results suggest that arousal-driven theta rhythmicity may help bind correlated neuronal assemblies in distinct brain areas participating in cognitive and emotional processes underlying social behavior. A disruption of the correlated neuronal activity associated with the theta rhythmicity is likely to impair these processes (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2006; Geschwind and Levitt, 2007; Buzsáki and Watson, 2012) resulting in atypical social behaviors.

Materials and methods

Animals

Sprague-Dawley (SD) male rats (5–6 weeks of age, 250–300 gr) served as subjects while SD or Wistar Hola/Hannover male rats (5–6 weeks of age, 250–300 gr) served as stimuli. All rats were purchased from Harlan Laboratories (Jerusalem, Israel) and housed in groups (2–5 per cage) in the SPF rat facility of the University of Haifa under veterinary supervision, food and water available ad libidum, lights on between 7:00 and 19:00. Experiments were performed in a strict accordance with the guidelines of the University of Haifa and approved by its Animal Care and Use Committee.

Electrodes

We used home-made electrodes for implantation. Stimulating electrodes were prepared by twisting together two stainless steel wires (A-M Systems, Sequim, WA, USA) with bare diameter of 0.005” (Coated-0.008”). Recording electrodes were prepared from Tungsten wire (A-M Systems) with bare diameter of 0.008” (Coated-0.011”) soldered to stainless steel wire. For reference/ground wire, we used stainless steel wires attached to a small screw.

Surgery and electrodes implantation

The rats were anesthetized with subcutaneously injected Ketamine (10% 0.09 cc/100 gr) and Medetomidine (0.1% 0.055 cc/100 gr). Anesthesia level was monitored by testing toe pinch reflexes and held constant throughout surgery with consecutive injections. The body temperature of the rat was kept constant at approximately 37°C, using a closed-loop temperature controller connected to a rectal temperature probe and a heating-pad placed under the rat (FHC, Bowdoin, MA, USA).

Anesthetized rats were fixed in a stereotaxic apparatus (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA), with the head flat, the skin was gently removed, and holes were drilled in the skull for implantation of electrodes and for reference/ground screw connection. Stimulating electrodes were placed in the left AOB (A/P = +3.0 mm, L/M = +1.0 mm, D/V = −4.0 mm at 50°) and MOB (A/P = +7.08 mm, L/M = +1.0 mm, D/V = −5.5 mm). Recording electrodes were placed in antero-ventral area of the MeA (A/P = −2.4 mm, L/M = +3.18 mm, D/V = −8.5 mm), LS (A/P = −0.24 mm, L/M = +0.4 mm, D/V = −4.4 mm), and Pir (A/P = +3.2 mm, L/M = +3.5 mm, D/V = −5.5 mm), as well as in the NAcc (A/P = +1.2 mm, L/M = +1.4 mm, D/V = −5.8 mm) in later experiments. Each electrode location was verified by its typical field potential signal, evoked in the MeA and LS by AOB stimulation (Gur et al., 2014) and in the Pir by MOB stimulation (Cohen et al., 2013). Following verification implanted electrodes (one at a time) were fixed by dental cement (Stoelting). When all electrodes were in place, the free ends of the stainless steel wires (including one wire for each stimulation electrode) were wired up to a connector which was then connected to the skull by dental cement, followed by skin is suturing. To avoid a need of soldering, procedure that could damage brain tissue due to excessive heat, we used gold pins inserted to the connector holes under pressure which destroyed the wires isolation to create a contact between the wires and the pins. After surgery, Amoxicillin (15%, 0.07 cc/100 gr) was injected daily (for 3 days) to prevent contamination. Rats allowed recovery for at least 7 days before experiments.

The experimental setup

All experiments were video-recorded from above the arena (see Video 1) by a CCD camera (Prosilica GC1290 GigE, Allied Vision Technology, Taschenweg, Germany). Electrophysiological recordings where made using an 8-channel wireless recording system (W8, Multi Channel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany). Recoded signals (sampled at 1 kHz, low-pass filtered at 0–300Hz) were synchronized with the video recordings by start signal sent through a digital to USB converter (NI USB-6008, National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) controlled by a self-written LabVIEW program (National Instruments).

The experimental arena comprised a three-layer box (inner dimensions: width—26 cm, length—28 cm, height—40 cm) with door on its front side. The inner layer was made of material (cloth) stretched on cuboid metal carcass to soften mechanical bumps of the recording system. The outer layer was made of adhesive black tape to prevent light entrance. A stainless steel net serves as a faraday cage in between these layers and the Multi-Channel wireless receiver was placed between it and the inner layer. During the experiment, the arena was illuminated by dim red light. We used a double floor made of two plastic slices that can be separately removed.

Experiments

Overall, we recorded from 22 animals, of them, 11 were tested with the social paradigm, 6 with the object paradigm (1 animal was tested with both) and 6 animals were tested with both fear conditioning and social encounter. Social recognition memory using anesthetized stimuli was performed in two animals and smell recognition was tested in three animals. The sample size is not always the same for all brain regions since in some of the recorded animals we lost the signals from specific electrodes due to various causes.

At the beginning of each experiment, the tested rat was taken out of its home cage and the wireless transmitter was fastened to the connector on its head by a male-to-male Interconnect header (Mill-Max Mfg. Oyster Bay, NY, USA) with 18 pins. Following 0.5–1 hr of habituation in the experimental arena, the rat was subjected to social, object, smell recognition test (Figure 2A), or fear conditioning test (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Each encounter initiated by pressing ‘start’ button on LabVIEW virtual instrument that sends synchronizing start signal to the camera and the wireless system. Then, during a period of 15 s, the stimulus was removed from its cage and delivered into the experimental arena. At the end of each encounter following stimulus removal, the upper floor slice is taken out and thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol and water to remove any odors left by the stimulus. It was then put back below the other slice 5 min after stimulus removal.

Stimuli

Rat stimuli were individually placed in clean covered plastic box and held in the experiment room throughout the experiment. The two stimulus animals used for each paradigm were always from different rat strains. Anesthetized animal stimuli were subcutaneously injected Ketamine (10% 0.09 cc/100 gr) and Medetomidine (0.1% 0.055 cc/100 gr) 10 min prior to experiment. As object stimuli, we used clean metal office stapler and hole-puncher. For smell recognition, we used small metal-net balls filled with cloth soaked with artificial food smells of citrus and vanilla. The metal-net ball was attached to the cage floor by hot melt adhesive. It should be noted that obviously, both object and smell stimuli are much poorer sources of chemosignals that social stimuli.

Fear conditioning

Fear conditioning took place in a Plexiglas rodent conditioning chamber with a metal grid floor dimly illuminated by a single house light and enclosed within a sound attenuating chamber (Coulbourn Instruments, Lehigh Valley, PA, USA). Rats were habituated to the chamber for 1 hr before fear conditioning. During fear conditioning rats were presented with five pairings of a tone (CS; 40 s, 5 kHz, 75 dB) that co-terminated with a foot-shock (US; 0.5 s, 1.3 mA). The inter-trial interval was 180 s. The fear recall experiments were conducted a day later in the experimental arena described above, using the same procedure without the electrical foot shocks.

Histology

After completion of the experiments, the rats were anesthetized and killed with an overdose of Isoflurane (Abbott Laboratories, Chicago, IL, USA). The brains are removed and placed in PFA (4% in PBS) over night, followed by sectioning to 200-µm slices using vibrating slicer (Vibroslice, Campden Instruments, Lafayette, IN, USA). The locations of the implanted electrode tips were identified using binocular and compared to the Pexinos-Watson rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007).

Data analysis

All analyses were done using self-written MATLAB programs (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). In all cases, when LFP signals were filtered we used band-pass filter between 5 and 11 Hz (high theta band) using MATLAB ‘fir1’ function.

PSD estimation

We estimated Power Spectrum Density (PSD) of LFP signal using multi-tapper approach based on standard Welch's method (‘pwelch’ function) using 1-s long dpss (discrete prolate spheroidal sequences) window with 50% overlap. The peak of the PSD curve was considered to be the maximum theta power value for each encounter (Figure 2).

ΔTheta power (ΔTP) calculation

For each brain region, the theta power obtained during Enc. 0 (Base) was subtracted from the TP values of each encounter.

Spectrogram

For each brain region, spectrograms were computed for each rat per trial using standard ‘spectrogram’ function with 1-s long dpss window with 50% overlap.

LFP cross-correlation

We used standard ‘xcorr’ function with ‘coeff’ option for cross-correlation between different brain regions of filtered LFP signals for each second. The mean peak cross-correlation value across all 300 s of each encounter was considered to be the cross-correlation value of the encounter (Figure 4A).

Coherence

The coherence between two signals x and y is defined as:

Cohxy(f)=Sxy(f)Sxx(f)Syy(f).

We computed the cross-spectrum Sxy(f) and the auto-spectra of each signal Sxx(f) and Syy(f) using the multitaper method (Thomson, 1982), implemented in Chronux 2.0 (Mitra and Bokil, 2008), an open-source, data analysis toolbox available at http://chronux.org. Coherograms were computed using a moving window of 2 s shifted in 200 ms increments, 5 tapers, and time-bandwidth of 3. (params.tapers=[TW=3 K=5]; movingwin=[2 0.2];). As spectrograms, coherograms, for each brain region, were computed for each rat per trial. For each brain region, mean coherograms were obtained by averaging coherograms computed per trial across all rats.

Statistics

Statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB, except for repeated measures ANOVA analyses that were conducted using SPSS (IBM) statistical software. Each brain region was separately analyzed. Parametric t-test and ANOVA tests were used if data were found to be normally distributed (Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests). Bonferroni's corrections were performed for multiple comparisons using t-test. One-sided t-tests were used when a change in specific direction was expected before the experiment.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr Liza Barki-Harrington for a helpful reading of this manuscript. We thank Dr Ido Izhaki and Ms Rotem Gur for generous help with the statistical analyses. This research was supported by the Legacy Heritage Bio-Medical Program of the Israel Science Foundation (grant #1901/08), by the Israel Science Foundation (grant #1350/12) and by a Teva fellowship to AT.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Israel Science Foundation (ISF) 1901/08, 1350/12 to Shlomo Wagner.

  • Teva Pharmaceutical Industries Graduate student fellowship to Alex Tendler.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

AT, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

SW, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols of the University of Haifa. The protocol was approved by the Committee on the Ethics of Animal Experiments of the University of Haifa (Permit Number: 194-10). All surgery was performed under Ketamine and Medetomidine, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

References

  1. Adhikari A, Topiwala MA, Gordon JA. Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety. Neuron. 2010;65:257–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aftanas LI, Varlamov AA, Pavlov SV, Makhnev VP, Reva NV. Affective picture processing: event-related synchronization within individually defined human theta band is modulated by valence dimension. Neuroscience Letters. 2001;303:115–118. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01703-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Assini R, Sirotin YB, Laplagne DA. Rapid triggering of vocalizations following social interactions. Current Biology. 2013;23:R996–R997. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Balconi M, Pozzoli U. Arousal effect on emotional face comprehension: frequency band changes in different time intervals. Physiology & Behavior. 2009;97:455–462. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barttfeld P, Amoruso L, Ais J, Cukier S, Bavassi L, Tomio A, Manes F, Ibanez A, Sigman M. Organization of brain networks governed by long-range connections index autistic traits in the general population. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. 2013;5:16. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benchenane K, Tiesinga PH, Battaglia FP. Oscillations in the prefrontal cortex: a gateway to memory and attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2011;21:475–485. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bielsky IF, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and social recognition in mammals. Peptides. 2004;25:1565–1574. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2004.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bland BH. The physiology and pharmacology of hippocampal formation theta rhythms. Progress in Neurobiology. 1986;26:1–54. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(86)90019-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bland BH, Colom LV. Extrinsic and intrinsic properties underlying oscillation and synchrony in limbic cortex. Progress in Neurobiology. 1993;41:157–208. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90007-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bland BH, Oddie SD. Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: the case for its role in sensorimotor integration. Behavioural Brain Research. 2001;127:119–136. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00358-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brock J, Brown CC, Boucher J, Rippon G. The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14(2):209–224. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402002018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Buzsáki G. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron. 2002;33:325–340. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buzsáki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buzsáki G, Watson BO. Brain rhythms and neural syntax: implications for efficient coding of cognitive content and neuropsychiatric disease. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience. 2012;14:345–367. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.4/gbuzsaki. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cannon J, McCarthy MM, Lee S, Lee J, Borgers C, Whittington MA, Kopell N. Neurosystems: brain rhythms and cognitive processing. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;39:705–719. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Canteras NS, Simerly RB, Swanson LW. Organization of projections from the medial nucleus of the amygdala: a PHAL study in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;360:213–245. doi: 10.1002/cne.903600203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cao Y, Roy S, Sachdev RN, Heck DH. Dynamic correlation between whisking and breathing rhythms in mice. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:1653–1659. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4395-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chang FC. Modification of medullary respiratory-related discharge patterns by behaviors and states of arousal. Brain Research. 1992;571:281–292. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90666-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Clarke S, Trowill JA. Sniffing and motivated behavior in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1971;6:49–52. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(71)90013-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coben R, Clarke AR, Hudspeth W, Barry RJ. EEG power and coherence in autistic spectrum disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2008;119:1002–1009. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cohen Y, Wilson DA, Barkai E. Differential modifications of synaptic weights during odor rule learning: dynamics of interaction between the piriform cortex with lower and higher brain areas. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;25:180–191. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Doesburg SM, Vidal J, Taylor MJ. Reduced theta connectivity during set-shifting in children with autism. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:785. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dölen G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC. Social reward requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature. 2013;501:179–184. doi: 10.1038/nature12518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dulac C, Torello AT. Molecular detection of pheromone signals in mammals: from genes to behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:551–562. doi: 10.1038/nrn1140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dulac C, Wagner S. Genetic analysis of brain circuits underlying pheromone signaling. Annual Review of Genetics. 2006;40:449–467. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genet.39.073003.093937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fell J, Axmacher N. The role of phase synchronization in memory processes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:105–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn2979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fendt M, Endres T, Apfelbach R. Temporary inactivation of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis but not of the amygdala blocks freezing induced by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:23–28. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-01-00023.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ferguson JN, Young LJ, Insel TR. The neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 2002;23:200–224. doi: 10.1006/frne.2002.0229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Freeman WJ, Viana Di Prisco G, Davis GW, Whitney TM. Conditioning of relative frequency of sniffing by rabbits to odors. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1983;97:12–23. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.97.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Geschwind DH. Advances in autism. Annual Review of Medicine. 2009;60:367–380. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.60.053107.121225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Geschwind DH, Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2007;17:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gheusi G, Bluthé RM, Goodall G, Dantzer R. Social and individual recognition in rodents: Methodological aspects and neurobiological bases. Behavioural Processes. 1994;33:59–88. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(94)90060-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goutagny R, Jackson J, Williams S. Self-generated theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:1491–1493. doi: 10.1038/nn.2440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gunaydin LA, Grosenick L, Finkelstein JC, Kauvar IV, Fenno LE, Adhikari A, Lammel S, Mirzabekov JJ, Airan RD, Zalocusky KA, Tye KM, Anikeeva P, Malenka RC, Deisseroth K. Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell. 2014;157:1535–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gur R, Tendler A, Wagner S. Long-term social recognition memory is mediated by oxytocin-dependent synaptic plasticity in the medial amygdala. Biological Psychiatry. 2014;76:377–386. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Isler JR, Martien KM, Grieve PG, Stark RI, Herbert MR. Reduced functional connectivity in visual evoked potentials in children with autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2010;121:2035–2043. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jacinto LR, Reis JS, Dias NS, Cerqueira JJ, Correia JH, Sousa N. Stress affects theta activity in limbic networks and impairs novelty-induced exploration and familiarization. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;7:127. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jutras MJ, Buffalo EA. Synchronous neural activity and memory formation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2010;20:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kana RK, Libero LE, Moore MS. Disrupted cortical connectivity theory as an explanatory model for autism spectrum disorders. Physics of Life Reviews. 2011;8:410–437. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kang N, Baum MJ, Cherry JA. Different profiles of main and accessory olfactory bulb mitral/tufted cell projections revealed in mice using an anterograde tracer and a whole-mount, flattened cortex preparation. Chemical Senses. 2011;36:251–260. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kay LM. Theta oscillations and sensorimotor performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2005;102:3863–3868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407920102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kepecs A, Uchida N, Mainen ZF. The sniff as a unit of olfactory processing. Chemical Senses. 2006;31:167–179. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kepecs A, Uchida N, Mainen ZF. Rapid and precise control of sniffing during olfactory discrimination in rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:205–213. doi: 10.1152/jn.00071.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kikuchi M, Yoshimura Y, Hiraishi H, Munesue T, Hashimoto T, Tsubokawa T, Takahashi T, Suzuki M, Higashida H, Minabe Y. Reduced long-range functional connectivity in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2015;10:248–254. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Knyazev GG. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2007;31:377–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Knyazev GG, Slobodskoj-Plusnin JY, Bocharov AV. Event-related delta and theta synchronization during explicit and implicit emotion processing. Neuroscience. 2009;164:1588–1600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Komisaruk BR. Synchrony between limbic system theta activity and rhythmical behavior in rats. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology. 1970;70:482–492. doi: 10.1037/h0028709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lee SH, Dan Y. Neuromodulation of brain states. Neuron. 2012;76:209–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lesting J, Daldrup T, Narayanan V, Himpe C, Seidenbecher T, Pape HC. Directional theta coherence in prefrontal cortical to amygdalo-hippocampal pathways signals fear extinction. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e77707. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Liebe S, Hoerzer GM, Logothetis NK, Rainer G. Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:456–462, S1–S2. doi: 10.1038/nn.3038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Luo Q, Cheng X, Holroyd T, Xu D, Carver F, Blair RJ. Theta band activity in response to emotional expressions and its relationship with gamma band activity as revealed by MEG and advanced beamformer source imaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:940. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Machado C, Estévez M, Leisman G, Melillo R, Rodriguez R, Defina P, Hernández A, Pérez-Nellar J, Naranjo R, Chinchilla M, Garófalo N, Vargas J, Beltrán C. QEEG spectral and coherence assessment of autistic children in three different experimental conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2013;45:406–424. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1909-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Macrides F, Eichenbaum HB, Forbes WB. Temporal relationship between sniffing and the limbic theta rhythm during odor discrimination reversal learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1982;2:1705–1717. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-12-01705.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Maratos FA, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Rippon G, Senior C. Coarse threat images reveal theta oscillations in the amygdala: a magnetoencephalography study. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;9:133–143. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.2.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Marder E. Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron. 2012;76:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Martin C, Beshel J, Kay LM. An olfacto-hippocampal network is dynamically involved in odor-discrimination learning. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;98:2196–2205. doi: 10.1152/jn.00524.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mitra P, Bokil H. Observed brain dynamics. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press; 2008. xxii, 381 p. p. [Google Scholar]
  58. Monod B, Mouly AM, Vigouroux M, Holley A. An investigation of some temporal aspects of olfactory coding with the model of multi-site electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research. 1989;33:51–63. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(89)80018-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Murias M, Webb SJ, Greenson J, Dawson G. Resting state cortical connectivity reflected in EEG coherence in individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62:270–273. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Narayanan RT, Seidenbecher T, Kluge C, Bergado J, Stork O, Pape HC. Dissociated theta phase synchronization in amygdalo- hippocampal circuits during various stages of fear memory. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25:1823–1831. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05437.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pape HC, Narayanan RT, Smid J, Stork O, Seidenbecher T. Theta activity in neurons and networks of the amygdala related to long-term fear memory. Hippocampus. 2005;15:874–880. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pape HC, Pare D. Plastic synaptic networks of the amygdala for the acquisition, expression, and extinction of conditioned fear. Physiological Reviews. 2010;90:419–463. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00037.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Paré D, Collins DR. Neuronal correlates of fear in the lateral amygdala: multiple extracellular recordings in conscious cats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:2701–2710. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02701.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Paré D, Collins DR, Pelletier JG. Amygdala oscillations and the consolidation of emotional memories. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2002;6:306–314. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01924-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 6th edition. Amsterdam, Boston: Academic Press/Elsevier; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  66. Pelletier JG, Paré D. Role of amygdala oscillations in the consolidation of emotional memories. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55:559–562. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pignatelli M, Beyeler A, Leinekugel X. Neural circuits underlying the generation of theta oscillations. Journal of Physiology. 2012;106:81–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2011.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Popa D, Duvarci S, Popescu AT, Léna C, Pare D. Coherent amygdalocortical theta promotes fear memory consolidation during paradoxical sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of USA. 2010;107:6516–6519. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913016107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pro-Sistiaga P, Mohedano-Moriano A, Ubeda-Banon I, Del Mar Arroyo-Jimenez M, Marcos P, Artacho-Perula E, Artacho-Pérula E, Crespo C, Insausti R, Martinez-Marcos A. Convergence of olfactory and vomeronasal projections in the rat basal telencephalon. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;504:346–362. doi: 10.1002/cne.21455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ranade S, Hangya B, Kepecs A. Multiple modes of phase locking between sniffing and whisking during active exploration. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:8250–8256. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3874-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rippon G, Brock J, Brown C, Boucher J. Disordered connectivity in the autistic brain: challenges for the ‘new psychophysiology’. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;63:164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rojas-Libano D, Frederick DE, Egana JI, Kay LM. The olfactory bulb theta rhythm follows all frequencies of diaphragmatic respiration in the freely behaving rat. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;8:214. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sainsbury RS. Hippocampal theta: a sensory-inhibition theory of function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998;22:237–241. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00011-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sainsbury RS, Harris JL, Rowland GL. Sensitization and hippocampal type 2 theta in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1987a;41:489–493. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90085-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sainsbury RS, Heynen A, Montoya CP. Behavioral correlates of hippocampal type 2 theta in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1987b;39:513–519. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90382-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sainsbury RS, Montoya CP. The relationship between type 2 theta and behavior. Physiology & Behavior. 1984;33:621–626. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90381-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Schmidt B, Hinman JR, Jacobson TK, Szkudlarek E, Argraves M, Escabi MA, Markus EJ. Dissociation between dorsal and ventral hippocampal theta oscillations during decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:6212–6224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2915-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Seidenbecher T, Laxmi TR, Stork O, Pape HC. Amygdalar and hippocampal theta rhythm synchronization during fear memory retrieval. Science. 2003;301:846–850. doi: 10.1126/science.1085818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shionoya K, Hegoburu C, Brown BL, Sullivan RM, Doyere V, Mouly AM. It's time to fear! Interval timing in odor fear conditioning in rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2013;7:128. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Takahashi LK, Chan MM, Pilar ML. Predator odor fear conditioning: current perspectives and new directions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32:1218–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Thomson DJ. Spectrum Estimation and Harmonic-Analysis. P Ieee. 1982;70:1055–1096. [Google Scholar]
  83. Tsanov M, Chah E, Reilly R, O'Mara SM. Respiratory cycle entrainment of septal neurons mediates the fast coupling of sniffing rate and hippocampal theta rhythm. The European Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;39:957–974. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Uhlhaas PJ, Pipa G, Lima B, Melloni L, Neuenschwander S, Nikolić D, Singer W. Neural synchrony in cortical networks: history, concept and current status. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2009;3:17. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.017.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Wass S. Distortions and disconnections: disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain and Cognition. 2011;75:18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wesson DW. Sniffing behavior communicates social hierarchy. Current Biology. 2013;23:575–580. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wesson DW, Donahou TN, Johnson MO, Wachowiak M. Sniffing behavior of mice during performance in odor-guided tasks. Chemical Senses. 2008;33:581–596. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjn029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wiley RH. Specificity and multiplicity in the recognition of individuals: implications for the evolution of social behaviour. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2013;88:179–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2012.00246.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wilson DA, Sullivan RM. Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron. 2011;72:506–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2015 Feb 16;4:e03614. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03614.030

Decision letter

Editor: Peggy Mason1

eLife posts the editorial decision letter and author response on a selection of the published articles (subject to the approval of the authors). An edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the substantive concerns or comments; minor concerns are not usually shown. Reviewers have the opportunity to discuss the decision before the letter is sent (see review process). Similarly, the author response typically shows only responses to the major concerns raised by the reviewers.

[Editors’ note: this article was originally rejected after discussions between the reviewers, but the authors were invited to resubmit after an appeal against the decision.]

Thank you for choosing to send your work entitled “Arousal-driven theta rhythmicity synchronizes neuronal activity in a limbic network during social behavior” for consideration at eLife. Your full submission has been evaluated by Eve Marder (Senior editor) and 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the decision was reached after discussions between the reviewers. We regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication. We should stress that it is eLife's policy to not ask for extensive revisions, and when it appears that significant work would be needed to bring a manuscript to the stage necessary for publication, we feel it is fairer to the authors to reject the paper and allow them the freedom to prepare it for publication elsewhere, with the advantage of the information found in these reviews.

This manuscript describes an ambitious and interesting study looking at theta oscillatory activity in olfactory, amygdala and basal forebrain areas in rats during social interactions of varying novelty. The authors interpret their results as showing that social memory is correlated with theta activity in the lateral septum and amygdala but not in lower tier olfactory areas. What this means is that theta power in LS and MeA decreases as the same stranger rat is repeatedly presented and then increases when a new rat is presented. Such theta power-tracking of novelty does not occur in AOB or MOB or Pir Ctx or in any area when rats are tested with metal objects or scented objects. In addition there was a theta index which measured the correspondence of MUA with theta power. The authors suggest that the theta response is centrally driven in an arousal dependent manner, and that socially driven theta oscillations in this circuit help bind diverse limbic areas important in this behavior. They further speculate that reported impairment in theta in the autistic spectrum population may contribute to impaired social function.

The reviewers were intrigued but there were several major points that detracted from their enthusiasm:

First and foremost, the social nature of the stimulus was not convincingly parsed from the arousing nature of the stimulus. A non-smelly metal object and an object with a volatile odor that activates MOB glomeruli are not controls for arousal.

Second, there are a number of methodological descriptions (eg theta index was not clearly understood by any of the reviewers) that were unclear and statistical tests which appear to be wrong. A full list is included below.

Third, the authors fail to cite a long literature showing that AOB /MOB theta is a respiratory rhythm which is independent of the olfactory respiratory rhythm. The piriform cortex is known to participate in both the respiratory rhythm and more central theta rhythms, which explains its midpoint position in the coherence strength spectrum among the areas studied here. The large change in AOB and MOB theta power during social stimulation speaks to the odor profile of a conspecific engaging robust sniffing behavior.

We have included the full set of reviews so that you can get a sense of the issues raised by each reviewer in their initial reviews.

Reviewer #1:

This paper is an interesting communication that makes a strong case for a physiological correlate for social memory. There are several interesting features to the findings. The social memory is correlated with activity in the lateral septum and amygdala and not in lower tier olfactory areas. While I am not completely convinced by the story, I believe that the data are sufficiently compelling that readers should evaluate the case for themselves.

1) One idea that is not tested is whether or not the social memory is for smell mediated by the VNO. The smell controls were typical volatile odorants that would be processed by the main olfactory epithelium (clove, vanilla). The fact that MOE-mediated odorants do not engage the same responses as individual animals is not that surprising. Observing the response to smells from the urine and feces of two different animals would be more compelling.

2) Theta power is not related to the stimulus animal being awake or anesthetized (but see below) nor is it related to the investigative behavior. This is interpreted as TP being related to neither stimulus nor behavior but rather to an internal state. Another interpretation would be that TP is related to an unrecorded behavior.

3) Conclusions from two animals, two recordings, regarding the issue of awake or anesthetized, is an uncomfortably low sample size. While these observations are worth noting in the Discussion, without more data, I would recommend not making this point part of the results.

4) Finally, how do the results distinguish between the favored top-down model and the possibility of the rhythm as an emergent property of two related areas?

Reviewer #2:

This paper addresses the theta rhythm as a coupling mechanism among areas involved in social communication. The data show modulation of theta power and coherence as well as multiunit firing increases and decreases and modulation by theta in response to several types of stimuli. Recordings are done in two primary olfactory areas (MOB, AOB), piriform cortex, medial amygdala and lateral septum. While several of these areas are activated in what is described as a social network, they all participate in many other functions: piriform cortex–olfaction; medial amygdala–emotional processing and olfaction; lateral septum–theta rhythmicity associated with learning and other hippocampal functions. The authors conclude that they have shown that the social areas are preferentially activated by social stimuli. I am not convinced that they have shown anything more than an arousing multimodal stimulus, which happens to be a conspecific and therefore very arousing, can enhance theta rhythms in the limbic areas. The controls are rather impoverished stimuli (clean office supplies, odor in a tea ball). I also have some serious concerns about the statistical methods as detailed below.

Methods and statistics:

1) Were spikes and fields recorded from the same electrodes? If so, was there any check to be sure that the LFP did not contaminate the MUA or vice versa?

2) The band pass for theta is very narrow. This can result in artifactual change in theta power and phase. Please use a much wider band or a low pass filter some larger distance from the end of the band of interest.

3) How do the authors know that the amygdala LFP does not come from the piriform cortex, which is very close by? The spectrograms look quite similar.

4) The method for analyzing MUA is incorrect. From what I can understand of the description (which is not very clear), it appears that all data were pooled across subjects, with those contributing more spikes weighted more, and the resulting pooled data analyzed. Each subject should be analyzed separately and a mean value derived for that subject. Then the statistical tests should be performed.

5) The description of the theta index is very difficult to understand. Is a 60 sec bin correct? That seems a couple of orders of magnitude too large, especially to resolve something on the order of 10s of msecs.

6) It seems that in every analysis a different statistical test is used. Please justify the choice of the various tests. Also, using one-sided t-tests and a large number of t-tests when an ANOVA is a more appropriate test needs to be justified. If many t-tests are used to compare each area separately, a Bonferroni or other correction is necessary to adjust for the multiple tests.

7) deltaTP should use a division of baseline power instead of a subtraction.

8) Figure 3 shows a mean across all brain areas (3A). This measure doesn't offer any information about the system and is an incorrect treatment of power. Power of a signal in any area depends on the circuits in that area. Theta may be big or small but what matters is its power relative to other frequencies in that area.

9) There are many differences between social stimuli and objects that have nothing to do with the social aspect. One is that the live animal stimulus moves in somewhat unpredictable manner or an anesthetized conspecific that is rather confusing. Another is that the odor of the stimulus is very complex, especially as compared to clean office supplies. Also, the waking conspecific can be a threatening stimulus (one aspect of social) and arousal from this emotional state can persist longer than whatever arousal is produced by office supplies. Is the emotional state the object of interest or the social aspect? None of the other stimuli were arousing. Is it possible that the authors have shown that emotional arousal increases theta power in some areas?

10) The authors fail to cite a long literature showing that AOB /MOB theta is a respiratory rhythm which is independent of the olfactory respiratory rhythm. The piriform cortex is known to participate in both the respiratory rhythm and more central theta rhythms, which explains its midpoint position in the coherence strength spectrum among the areas studied here. The large change in AOB and MOB theta power during social stimulation speaks to the odor profile of a conspecific engaging robust sniffing behavior. A scholarly discussion of the results in light of what is already known re respiratory and central theta rhythms is missing.

Reviewer #3:

This is a well designed and analyzed experiment describing theta oscillatory activity in olfactory, amygdala and basal forebrain areas in rats during social interactions. The results demonstrate that in response to social interactions with a conspecific, but not with metal objects or scented objects, theta oscillations centering on 8Hz are strongly elevated in all regions, and maintain that activity for at least several minutes after the encounter. Multi-unit activity is also elevated, though only during the encounter. As social interactions habituation over repeated exposure, so does the theta response. The authors suggest that the theta response is centrally driven by in an arousal dependent manner, and that socially driven theta oscillations in this circuit help bind diverse limbic areas important in this behavior. They suggest that reported impairment in theta in the autistic spectrum population may contribute to impaired social function.

Major comments:

1) Despite the authors’ reference to other work, the results would be substantially strengthened by at least one example of simultaneously recorded respiration, definitively showing that theta in this system under these conditions is independent of respiration.

2) Does TP return to baseline prior to the next social stimulus? These data do not seem to be presented. This makes it difficult to interpret TP responses to subsequent social stimuli.

3) There seems to be some disconnect between the various results and how they are interpreted. For example, TP is elevated during social encounters and then stays elevated, while MUA is elevated during social encounters and then returns immediately to baseline. In addition, TI is elevated during social encounters and then is maintained or decreases depending on region. Despite these dramatic differences in time course, the authors seem to imply a single central mechanism based on arousal. This needs to be more fully discussed.

4) The authors suggest that theta oscillations help link large networks, and this may be important in social recognition/cognition. However, the coherence data suggest that while all regions examined express elevated theta, for the most part these oscillations are not coherent across regions. How does this support the authors' hypothesis?

[Editors’ note: what now follows is the decision letter after the authors submitted for further consideration.]

Thank you for sending your work entitled “Distinct types of theta rhythmicity are induced by social and fearful stimuli in a network associated with social memory” for consideration at eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Eve Marder (Senior editor), Peggy Mason (Reviewing editor), and one additional reviewer.

The Reviewing editor and the other reviewer discussed their comments before we reached this decision, and the Reviewing editor has assembled the following comments to help you prepare a revised submission.

The addition of the fear conditioning group is a very nice addition to this study and really addresses the arousal issue that so concerned the reviewers in the previous version. There remain matters that we would like you to address in a revision:

1) You have convincingly shown that the effective stimulus has to be either social or it has to be at least more complex than 1 odor (formally a minimum of 2 odors or one odor plus some other cue). Can you acknowledge this possibility? Please give alternate explanations such as an “oligo” mixture of odors full consideration. A measured and sober analysis of potential alternate interpretations will strengthen this paper. So please take this opportunity for revision to produce an even-handed assessment of other possible explanations of the data.

2) The authors may want to consider that the two amygdalae are connected by fibers that pass through the anterior commissure and they are similar biophysically and produce the same sorts of rhythms via the same mechanisms, so this connecting is expected to give rise to strong coherence, despite the distance. Thus strong coherence between the amygdalae is not surprising (despite the distance).

3) In the literature, high frequency theta is assigned to locomotion and sensory activity, while low frequency theta is assigned to immobility or jumping to avoid something (type I and type II theta). In addition to citing the literature concerning low frequency (type II) theta and fear conditioning, could you also cite and discuss the literature demonstrating that high frequency theta accompanies sensory processing and locomotion.

eLife. 2015 Feb 16;4:e03614. doi: 10.7554/eLife.03614.031

Author response


[Editors’ note: the author responses to the first round of peer review follow.]

We strongly believe that rather than requiring additional experiments, we

can easily address all the concerns raised within a very short time frame. We would therefore greatly appreciate an opportunity to re-submit this manuscript to eLife.

The main criticism was for the lack of non-social arousing control. We directly addressed this criticism by examining the theta rhythmicity in the network during fear conditioning, a situation previously shown to be accompanied by arousaldriven theta rhythmicity in other brain regions. These results, presented in the second part of the manuscript, significantly changed the paper that now mainly deals with the LFP signals. Therefore, we omitted the MUA data, the importance of which became marginalized in the revised manuscript.

Reviewer #1:

This paper is an interesting communication that makes a strong case for a physiological correlate for social memory. There are several interesting features to the findings. The social memory is correlated with activity in the lateral septum and amygdala and not in lower tier olfactory areas. While I am not completely convinced by the story, I believe that the data are sufficiently compelling that readers should evaluate the case for themselves.

1) One idea that is not tested is whether or not the social memory is for smell mediated by the VNO. The smell controls were typical volatile odorants that would be processed by the main olfactory epithelium (clove, vanilla). The fact that MOE-mediated odorants do not engage the same responses as individual animals is not that surprising. Observing the response to smells from the urine and feces of two different animals would be more compelling.

In our manuscript we do not try to tell between the main and accessory olfactory systems in terms of function. Our vision, shared by many other scientists, is that both systems act synergistically to process social chemosensory cues during social interactions and that it would be very difficult to distinguish between their contributions without using genetic tools. Thus, we recorded from areas associated with both systems. Indeed, our results point to the distinction between the olfactory bulb and higher brain centers as more relevant to theta rhythmicity than the difference between the main and accessory systems.

2) Theta power is not related to the stimulus animal being awake or anesthetized (but see below) nor is it related to the investigative behavior. This is interpreted as TP being related to neither stimulus nor behavior but rather to an internal state. Another interpretation would be that TP is related to an unrecorded behavior.

The reason why we do not think that any external factor is driving the theta rhythmicity is the variable coherence of this rhythmicity between the various brain regions. This variability cannot be explained if one rhythmic behavior drives all theta rhythmicity in the brain. Moreover, we are continuing a long trail of studies showing that theta rhythmicity in many brain areas is associated with arousal state and that the behavioral parameters such as sniffing and whisking are driven by the internal theta rhythmicity rather than driving it. This issue is widely discussed in the revised manuscript.

3) Conclusions from two animals, two recordings, regarding the issue of awake or anesthetized is an uncomfortably low sample size. While these observations are worth noting in the Discussion, without more data, I would recommend not making this point part of the results.

We accept the criticism and omitted the conclusion (regarding the stimulus behavior) from the Results section.

4) Finally, how do the results distinguish between the favored top-down model and the possibility of the rhythm as an emergent property of two related areas?

The top-down process is supported mainly by the fact that theta rhythmicity is similarly modulated by stimulus novelty in all brain areas, despite the highly variable coherence between them. This suggests that several sources of theta rhythmicity in the brain are influenced by a top-down process which is related to the stimulus novelty. Practically, such influence may be mediated by a common input from a higher brain center or a neuromodulatory system with a brain-wide influence. This issue is discussed in the revised manuscript.

Reviewer #2:

This paper addresses the theta rhythm as a coupling mechanism among areas involved in social communication. The data show modulation of theta power and coherence as well as multiunit firing increases and decreases and modulation by theta in response to several types of stimuli. Recordings are done in two primary olfactory areas (MOB, AOB), piriform cortex, medial amygdala and lateral septum. While several of these areas are activated in what is described as a social network, they all participate in many other functions: piriform cortexolfaction; medial amygdalaemotional processing and olfaction; lateral septumtheta rhythmicity associated with learning and other hippocampal functions. The authors conclude that they have shown that the social areas are preferentially activated by social stimuli. I am not convinced that they have shown anything more than an arousing multimodal stimulus, which happens to be a conspecific and therefore very arousing, can enhance theta rhythms in the limbic areas. The controls are rather impoverished stimuli (clean office supplies, odor in a tea ball). I also have some serious concerns about the statistical methods as detailed below.

Methods and statistics:

1) Were spikes and fields recorded from the same electrodes? If so, was there any check to be sure that the LFP did not contaminate the MUA or vice versa?

We are using methodology which is well accepted in the field and was used by many very well published studies, some of which are cited in the manuscript. Generally, the possibility of contamination of the LFP by the MUA is relevant for high-frequency rhythms such as the gamma rhythm (100Hz), where only one order of magnitude separate between the signal widths. In the case of theta rhythmicity (10Hz), where two order of magnitude, separate between the signals, we do not expect any contamination.

2) The band pass for theta is very narrow. This can result in artifactual change in theta power and phase. Please use a much wider band or a low pass filter some larger distance from the end of the band of interest.

The band-pass filter does not cause any distortion of the TP. This can be seen in Figure 2C–F, where unfiltered data is analyzed suing the same methodology and we get the same results. The filtering is only making the peak location clearer.

3) How do the authors know that the amygdala LFP does not come from the piriform cortex, which is very close by? The spectrograms look quite similar.

The LFP signals of the MeA and Pir are not especially similar. It can be seen very well in Figure 4–figure supplement 7, where the instantaneous TP of both structures are shown together. Also, the MeA signals show only moderate coherence with the Pir LFP, relative to the LS or contralateral MeA, both of which are remote from the MeA. This issue is discussed in the revised manuscript.

4) The method for analyzing MUA is incorrect. From what I can understand of the description (which is not very clear), it appears that all data were pooled across subjects, with those contributing more spikes weighted more, and the resulting pooled data analyzed. Each subject should be analyzed separately and a mean value derived for that subject. Then the statistical tests should be performed.

Not relevant anymore since the MUA data are not included in the revised manuscript.

5) The description of the theta index is very difficult to understand. Is a 60 sec bin correct? That seems a couple of orders of magnitude too large, especially to resolve something on the order of 10s of msecs.

Not relevant anymore since the MUA data are not included in the revised manuscript.

6) It seems that in every analysis a different statistical test is used. Please justify the choice of the various tests. Also, using one-sided t-tests and a large number of t-tests when an ANOVA is a more appropriate test needs to be justified. If many t-tests are used to compare each area separately, a Bonferroni or other correction is necessary to adjust for the multiple tests.

Generally, each brain region was analyzed separately. All multiple comparisons were corrected using Bonferroni's corrections and this is explicitly written in the relevant figure legends, as well as in a section of “statistics” added to the Methods in the revised manuscript. As explained, one-sided t-tests were used when a change in specific direction was expected before the experiment.

Figure 3: We used repeated measures 1-way ANOVA to examine the possibility of significant differences between encounters and then post-hoc t-tests (corrected for multiple comparisons) to examine the habituation and dishabituation phases. The t-tests were used to minimize the number of post-hoc comparisons within the model.

Figure 4: We used t-test rather than ANOVA since the only desired comparison was between the Encounter and Post periods for each experiment.

Figure 6: t-tests, corrected for multiple comparisons, were used to compare

between the Encounter and Base periods and between the Post and Base periods. We were not interested in comparisons between the encounter and post periods.

Figure 7, 8: 2-way ANOVA test was used to assess the interaction between the type of experiment (FC or SR) and theta band (low or high) in which changes occur.

7) deltaTP should use a division of baseline power instead of a subtraction.

Since the TP is a product of a logarithmic function, the use of subtraction is appropriate. Mathematically, subtraction after log is the same as dividing the parameters before log.

8) Figure 3 shows a mean across all brain areas (3A). This measure doesn't offer any information about the system and is an incorrect treatment of power. Power of a signal in any area depends on the circuits in that area. Theta may be big or small but what matters is its power relative to other frequencies in that area.

The mean was removed from the figure.

9) There are many differences between social stimuli and objects that have nothing to do with the social aspect. One is that the live animal stimulus moves in somewhat unpredictable manner or an anesthetized conspecific that is rather confusing. Another is that the odor of the stimulus is very complex, especially as compared to clean office supplies. Also, the waking conspecific can be a threatening stimulus (one aspect of social) and arousal from this emotional state can persist longer than whatever arousal is produced by office supplies. Is the emotional state the object of interest or the social aspect? None of the other stimuli were arousing. Is it possible that the authors have shown that emotional arousal increases theta power in some areas?

The point with the objects is exactly that they are not arousing and still elicit a similar investigative behavior, as well as habituation and dishabituation, as the social stimuli. Thus, the objects control for the possibility that the theta is not induced by arousal. We now added the fear conditioning experiments, showing that social and fear stimuli, both causing arousal states, yield very different changes in theta rhythmicity and theta coherence in the same network. Thus, the theta rhythmicity differentially reflects distinct arousal states caused by social and fearful stimuli.

10) The authors fail to cite a long literature showing that AOB /MOB theta is a respiratory rhythm which is independent of the olfactory respiratory rhythm. The piriform cortex is known to participate in both the respiratory rhythm and more central theta rhythms, which explains its midpoint position in the coherence strength spectrum among the areas studied here. The large change in AOB and MOB theta power during social stimulation speaks to the odor profile of a conspecific engaging robust sniffing behavior. A scholarly discussion of the results in light of what is already known re respiratory and central theta rhythms is missing.

A scholarly discussion was added to the Discussion section, where we address all these points.

Reviewer #3:

This is a well designed and analyzed experiment describing theta oscillatory activity in olfactory, amygdala and basal forebrain areas in rats during social interactions. The results demonstrate that in response to social interactions with a conspecific, but not with metal objects or scented objects, theta oscillations centering on 8Hz are strongly elevated in all regions, and maintain that activity for at least several minutes after the encounter. Multi-unit activity is also elevated, though only during the encounter. As social interactions habituation over repeated exposure, so does the theta response. The authors suggest that the theta response is centrally driven by in an arousal dependent manner, and that socially driven theta oscillations in this circuit help bind diverse limbic areas important in this behavior. They suggest that reported impairment in theta in the autistic spectrum population may contribute to impaired social function.

Major comments:

1) Despite the authors’ reference to other work, the results would be substantially strengthened by at least one example of simultaneously recorded respiration, definitively showing that theta in this system under these conditions is independent of respiration.

We added a detailed discussion regarding the possible involvement of the sniffing cycle in the theta rhythmicity. Briefly, there is no reason to record the sniffing in our case, first because it was previously done with similar paradigms and second because the sniffing cycle itself is known to be modulated by the arousal state and even by expectation to reward. Therefore, it is almost impossible to disconnect the sniffing from the loop. Moreover, the sniffing cycle is probably involved in the entrainment of the OB theta and this may be the reason for the high coherence between the AOB and MOB. However, it cannot be generating the theta of the higher brain centers because of the low coherence between the OB and these centers. This was shown by several papers regarding the hippocampal theta rhythm.

2) Does TP return to baseline prior to the next social stimulus? These data do not seem to be presented. This makes it difficult to interpret TP responses to subsequent social stimuli.

These data appear in Figure 4–figure supplements 1–6, where the mean spectrograms of the whole experiment are depicted for each brain region. Generally, this is brain region specific and TP goes back to baseline in the AOB and MOB but not in the other regions, which is another point showing that they are generated by distinct sources of theta rhythmicity.

3) There seems to be some disconnect between the various results and how they are interpreted. For example, TP is elevated during social encounters and then stays elevated, while MUA is elevated during social encounters and then returns immediately to baseline. In addition, TI is elevated during social encounters and then is maintained or decreases depending on region. Despite these dramatic differences in time course, the authors seem to imply a single central mechanism based on arousal. This needs to be more fully discussed.

Generally, only the TP is a reflection of the arousal state. In any case, since the MUA data are omitted from the revised manuscript, this is not relevant anymore.

4) The authors suggest that theta oscillations help link large networks, and this may be important in social recognition/cognition. However, the coherence data suggest that while all regions examined express elevated theta, for the most part these oscillations are not coherent across regions. How does this support the authors' hypothesis?

The hypothesis that brain oscillations in general, and theta rhythmicity in particular, coordinate between remote neural assemblies to promote cognitive processes in not new, and may be the most accepted in the field (see for example Jutras and Buffalo, 2010). How exactly theta rhythmicity coordinate neural activity is a general and complex issue that was dealt in may review papers, some of which are cited by us (for example Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004) and is out of the scope of this paper. Our coherence data indeed suggest that such coordination is dynamic and may change its pattern in different contexts. In times when the regions recorded by us are not synchronized among themselves, they may be synchronized with other relevant regions. We suggest that such dynamics may be crucial for the enhancement of certain cognitive processes, such as memorization of specific stimuli, in distinct neuronal networks according to their value (social or fearful) and saliency (novel or familiar).

[Editors’ note: the author responses to the re-review follow.]

The addition of the fear conditioning group is a very nice addition to this study and really addresses the arousal issue that so concerned the reviewers in the previous version. There remain matters that we would like you to address in a revision:

1) You have convincingly shown that the effective stimulus has to be either social or it has to be at least more complex than 1 odor (formally a minimum of 2 odors or one odor plus some other cue). Can you acknowledge this possibility? Please give alternate explanations such as an ‘”oligo” mixture of odors full consideration. A measured and sober analysis of potential alternate interpretations will strengthen this paper. So please take this opportunity for revision to produce an even-handed assessment of other possible explanations of the data.

We added a paragraph dealing with this issue to the Discussion section (second paragraph).

2) The authors may want to consider that the two amygdalae are connected by fibers that pass through the anterior commissure and they are similar biophysically and produce the same sorts of rhythms via the same mechanisms, so this connecting is expected to give rise to strong coherence, despite the distance. Thus strong coherence between the amygdalae is not surprising (despite the distance).

The lack of a direct synaptic connection between the MeAs of the two hemispheres is well documented, especially in the comprehensive work of Canteras, Simerly and Swanson (The Journal of Comparative Neurology 360:213-245, 1995, see paragraphs regarding contralateral projections at pp. 225, 227: “Virtually no anterograde labeling was observed in the contralateral amygdala”) specifically for rats, and is also evident in the Mouse Connectivity section of the Allen Brain Atlas. We added a reference to the work of Canteras et al. in the relevant place towards the end of the Discussion section.

3) In the literature, high frequency theta is assigned to locomotion and sensory activity, while low frequency theta is assigned to immobility or jumping to avoid something (type I and type II theta). In addition to citing the literature concerning low frequency (type II) theta and fear conditioning, could you also cite and discuss the literature demonstrating that high frequency theta accompanies sensory processing and locomotion.

We added a full discussion of the two types of theta recorded in the hippocampus and the relationship of them to other theta rhythms recorded in the brain to the Discussion section (third paragraph).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 3—source data 1. Theta power (TP) modulation between encounters.

    One-way ANOVA (repeated measures) test was used to determine whether there is a significant difference between the mean ΔTP of all five encounters during either social (1a) or object (1b) recognition. The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests. Sphericity was assessed by Mauchly's test.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.007

    elife03614s001.docx (24.5KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.007
    Figure 3—source data 2. Statistical assessment of habituation and dishabituation.

    Paired t-tests were used for the social (2a) and object (2b) recognition paradigms, to examine if the differences between Enc.1 and Enc. 4 (habituation), as well as between Enc. 4 and Enc. 5 (dishabituation) are statistically significant. Tests were one-sided and corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni's correction.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.008

    elife03614s002.docx (25.2KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.008
    Figure 4—source data 1. Comparison of ΔTP between Enc. and Post periods.

    Paired t-tests were used to compare between the mean ΔTP across Enc. vs the mean ΔTP across Post periods. The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.010

    elife03614s003.docx (22KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.010
    Figure 6—source data 1. Assessment of change in theta Coherence from Base to either Enc. 1 or Post 1.

    The change from Base to Enc. 1 (upper) and from Base to Post 1 (lower), in theta coherence during social recognition between the MeA and all other areas (1a) and between the MOB and all areas (1b), as well as during object recognition between the MeA and all other areas (1c), and between the MOB and all areas (1d), was statistically validated using paired t-tests, corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.020

    elife03614s004.docx (26.1KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.020
    Figure 7—source data 1. Comparison of change in theta power in low and high theta bands between social and fearful stimuli.

    Comparison of the change in theta power between social recognition (SR) and fear conditioning (FC) at high and low theta ranges, statistically validated using two-way repeated measures ANOVA (p—experiment X theta range interaction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.025

    elife03614s005.docx (21KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.025
    Figure 8—source data 1. Comparison of change in coherence in low and high theta bands between social and fearful stimuli.

    Comparison of the change in coherence between social recognition (SR) and fear conditioning (FC) at high and low theta ranges (right), statistically validated using two-way repeated measures ANOVA (p—experiment X theta range interaction). The assumption of normality was assessed by Lilliefors and Shapiro–Wilk tests.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03614.028

    elife03614s006.docx (21.8KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.03614.028

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES