Abstract
Mounting evidence suggests that enriched mental, physical, and socially stimulating activities are beneficial for counteracting age-related decreases in brain function and cognition in older adults. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to demonstrate the functional plasticity of brain activity in response to a combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention and investigated the contribution of the intervention-related brain changes to individual performance in healthy older adults. The intervention was composed of a 6-week program of combined activities including cognitive training, Tai Chi exercise, and group counseling. The results showed improved cognitive performance and reorganized regional homogeneity of spontaneous fluctuations in the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signals in the superior and middle temporal gyri, and the posterior lobe of the cerebellum, in the participants who attended the intervention. Intriguingly, the intervention-induced changes in the coherence of local spontaneous activity correlated with the improvements in individual cognitive performance. Taken together with our previous findings of enhanced resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe regions following a combined intervention program in older adults, we conclude that the functional plasticity of the aging brain is a rather complex process, and an effective cognitive-psychological-physical intervention is helpful for maintaining a healthy brain and comprehensive cognition during old age.
1. Introduction
Normal aging is associated with cognitive decline in various domains, such as executive control, working memory, and episodic memory, and has been linked with structural morphology and functional changes in the brain [1, 2]. There has been accumulating evidence that older adults exhibit structural volumetric decreases, thinning of white matter tracts, and changes in functional activation patterns in several brain regions, specifically the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and medial temporal lobe (MTL) [3–7]. Fortunately, the aging brain exhibits experience-dependent plasticity, and increasing studies find that cognitive, physical, or social activity is greatly beneficial for the elderly to promote cognitive performance and optimize brain structure and function [8–10].
Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that cognitive training can counteract age-related brain structural and functional losses. For instance, memory training can increase the cortical thickness [11] and induce greater activation in brain regions associated with self-initiated semantic strategy use [12] in healthy older adults. Moreover, memory training has been shown to enhance hippocampal activity during memory retrieval in patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [13] and attenuate the differences in brain activation patterns between MCI patients and healthy controls [14]. Executive function training can induce functional alterations in cognitive control-related brain regions [15, 16]. Apart from cognitive training, physical exercise training has also been found to induce significant alterations in structural morphology and cerebral function [17–20].
Given that multiple factors and conditions have shown beneficial effects on the aging brain [2], it is expected that a combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention including cognitive, physical, and social activities should be a more efficient approach to improve brain function in old age [21–23]. Following this assumption, a previous study from our group explored the effects of combined intervention including cognitive training, physical exercise, and group counseling on the functional plasticity in resting-state interregional connectivity in healthy older adults [24]. The results showed that the combined intervention enhanced the functional connectivity between the MTL and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and the interregional connectivity changes were correlated with individual cognitive performance. This study confirmed that the combined intervention can improve the functional connectivity between the MTL and mPFC in the older adults. Nevertheless, these findings do not answer the question of whether or not the combined intervention could induce regionally brain functional plastic changes.
In recent years, the resting-state fMRI (RS-fMRI) has increasingly become a widely used method to investigate the brain functional plasticity [25]. RS-fMRI can be used to explore the intrinsic functional architecture of the brain without the need for participants to perform a specific task [26–28]. Previous studies usually use the resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) analysis to examine the intervention effects on functional organization changes between distinct brain regions [24, 29]. However, the RSFC method can only be used to measure the interregional connectivity between spatially remote brain regions and strongly rests upon the regions of interest (ROI) defined with prior knowledge [30]. Regional homogeneity (ReHo) analysis is a unique RS-fMRI method for evaluating the local temporal synchronizations of spontaneous low frequency BOLD signals, which measures the similarity between the time series of a given voxel and its nearest neighbors [31]. It has been shown that the variations of ReHo values have neurobiological and structural basis [32]. As a data-driven approach, the ReHo method has high test-retest reliability [33, 34] and can detect regional changes that are induced by different conditions across the whole brain without requiring any prior knowledge [31]. It is widely applied in exploring brain function of healthy people and can predict individual performance in cognitive tasks [35–37]. Moreover, ReHo has been used to monitor disease progression in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) and MCI [38, 39]. Thus, the ReHo could be a potentially important complimentary method to examine the regional plasticity in aging brain.
In the present study, we aimed to further explore the regionally functional plasticity by using the ReHo method to do an exploratory analysis in the whole brain. The intervention group attended a combined intervention program consisting of cognitive training, physical exercise, and group counseling which has been introduced in Li et al. [24]. The cognitive training component focused on mnemonic and executive function training. Tai Chi, as a typical form of physical exercise, has been demonstrated to effectively improve the cognitive function [40], and optimize the regionally functional homogeneity of the intrinsic brain architecture in older adults [41]. In addition, group counseling was adopted to promote the psychological well-being of older adults, given that positive mood states could influence individual cognition and brain function [42]. We expected that this combined intervention program could alter the local functional homogeneity of the brain regions, as reflected by the changed ReHo values in the participants who attended the intervention, and we further speculated that these changes in local functional homogeneity would reflect improved individual cognitive performance.
2. Methods
In this study, healthy older adults' brain activity and neuropsychological performance were assessed before and after a 6-week combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention. The effects of combined intervention on the functional plasticity in resting-state interregional connectivity have previously been reported for the participants in Li et al. [24]. This paper focused on the intervention effects on functional changes in the patterns of local spontaneous brain activity.
2.1. Participants
Forty-five healthy older adults were recruited from two communities near the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. After baseline evaluation, one community was randomly allocated to the intervention group (n = 26), and the other community formed the control group (n = 19). Participants were blind to the group allocation and study design. Participants were included in the study according to the same screen criteria used in Li et al. [24]. Of these, 11 participants were excluded from further analysis based on these criteria or for other reasons, and thus data for 34 participants were finally analyzed, with 17 in the intervention group (mean age: 68.59 years) and 17 in the control group (mean age: 71.65 years) [24].
The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants, and they were paid 200 Yuan for participation. The study was registered in the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR) (http://www.chictr.org/): ChiCTR-PNRC-13003813.
2.2. Outcome Measures
A battery of neuropsychological tests was used to evaluate the intervention effects on cognitive function, health status, social support, and subjective well-being. The tests of cognitive function included the Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Beijing Version (MoCA-BJ) [43], Paired Associative Learning Test (PALT) [44], Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward [45], Trail Making Test (TMT) [46], Stroop Test [47], and Category Fluency Test (CFT) [48], which were used to assess the global cognition, episodic memory, working memory, executive function, and language ability, respectively. Health status was measured using the Medical Outcomes Study Short Form-36 (MOS SF-36) [49]. The level of social support was measured using the Social Support Rating Scale (SSRS) [50]. Subjective well-being was measured using the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) [51] and the Index of Well-Being (IWB) [52]. The test examiners were blind to the group allocation of participants (control or intervention).
2.3. Procedure
Figure 1 displays the procedure for the study. All participants were subjected to a battery of neuropsychological tests and MRI scanning individually before and after the intervention. During the intervention process, the intervention group received a 6-week combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention, including cognitive intervention, Tai Chi exercise, and group counseling, while the control group attended two 120 min lectures related to health and aging. The 18 one-hour cognitive training sessions were administrated three times per week and consisted of mnemonic training (MT; nine sessions) and executive function training (EFT; nine sessions). MT was designed to teach older adults elaborate encoding and retrieval strategies, such as interactive imagery, sentence generation, and the method of loci. EFT was designed to train older adults three components of executive function: inhibition, switching, and updating. The 18 one-hour physical exercise sessions required participants to learn the Yang Style 24-form Tai Chi three times per week. Group counseling sessions aimed at promoting the psychological well-being of older adults through reminiscence and were conducted as six weekly sessions of 90 min each. Please refer to Li et al. [24] for details about the intervention program.
2.4. Statistical Analysis of Neuropsychological Data
The demographic and clinical characteristics of participants in both intervention and control groups were examined by using chi-square, t, or nonparametric (Mann-Whitney) tests. The repeated measures two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the within-subject factor of intervention (pre, post) and the between-subject factor of group (control, trained) was conducted on the performance for each test to examine the intervention effect. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 19.0 (IBM Corporation, Somers, NY).
2.5. Image Data Acquisition
All images were collected using a 3-Tesla Siemens Trio scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at the Beijing MRI Center for Brain Research. Each participant was scanned before and after the intervention using the same fMRI protocols. During each scan, participants lay supine with head snugly fixed by a belt and foam pads to minimize head motion. They were instructed to lie quietly, keep their eyes closed, and not think of anything in particular. For each participant, resting-state fMRI data was acquired using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters: time repetition (TR) = 2000 ms; time echo (TE) = 30 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view (FOV) = 200 × 200 mm2; thickness = 3.0 mm; gap = 0.6 mm; acquisition matrix = 64 × 64; in-plane resolution = 3.125 × 3.125; 33 axial slices; 200 volumes. A high-resolution, three-dimensional T1-weighted structural image was also collected for each subject with the following sequence: 176 slices; acquisition matrix = 256 × 256; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm³; TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.2 ms; flip angle = 9°.
2.6. Image Processing and Analysis
Image processing was performed using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) and Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF) V2.0 Basic Edition [53].
The first five images for each subject were discarded to allow for equilibration of the magnetic field and the acclimatization to the scanning environment. The 195 remaining images were first corrected for intravolume acquisition time differences between slices and intervolume geometrical displacement due to head motion. Participants included in this study were restricted to head motion of less than 2.0 mm in any direction and 2.0° of angular motion during the resting-state scan. The functional images were then normalized to the standard space of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) and resampled to a voxel size of 3 × 3 × 3 mm³. Following this, detrending and temporal band pass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz) of the fMRI data was carried out to reduce the effects of low-frequency drift and physiological high-frequency noise.
Following Zang et al. [31], the ReHo value in the brain was measured using Kendall's coefficient of concordance (KCC) between the time series of a given voxel and its nearest 26 neighbors. Specifically, we first calculated the KCC for each voxel across the whole brain to derive the ReHo map for each subject. For standardization purposes, each ReHo map was then divided by the mean ReHo value of the entire brain. Finally the ReHo maps were spatially smoothed with a 4 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.
For statistical analysis, we first used a one-sample t-test to compare the ReHo maps for intervention and control groups both before and after intervention and then performed a whole-brain voxel-wise Group (control, trained) × Intervention (pre, post) ANOVA on the ReHo maps to detect regions showing intervention-related changes. Clusters were considered significant at a level of P < 0.01 for the combined voxel-extend threshold of an uncorrected voxel and cluster extent >486 mm3, as determined using the Monte Carlo simulation with AlphaSim correction to P < 0.01. In further studies, the regions showing a significant Group × Intervention interaction were defined as regions of interest (ROIs). We extracted the mean ReHo value in each ROI and used paired sample t-tests (P < 0.05) to examine the effects of intervention on regional ReHo in each of the ROIs for the two groups.
Finally, to examine whether intervention-related changes in brain activity are associated with improvements in cognition, the correlations between intervention-related ReHo changes in the ROIs and changes in cognition variables were investigated (P < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Between-group comparisons were conducted with Fisher's r to Z transformation to directly compare two correlation coefficients.
3. Results
3.1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
Table 1 displays the demographic and clinical characteristics of the participants in the intervention and control groups. The two groups did not differ significantly in age, years of education, gender, or on the MoCA-BJ, Center for Epidemiologic Depression Scale (CES-D) [54], and ADL (Activities of Daily Living) scores [55].
Table 1.
Intervention group (n = 17) |
Control group (n = 17) |
P value | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 68.59 (5.65) | 71.65 (4.00) | 0.08a |
Gender (male/female) | 9/8 | 11/6 | 0.50b |
Education (years) | 13.29 (3.06) | 14.65 (3.26) | 0.22a |
MoCA | 26.29 (2.69) | 25.18 (2.53) | 0.33c |
CESD | 7.00 (5.53) | 6.82 (6.06) | 0.85c |
ADL | 14.12 (0.49) | 14.18 (0.73) | 0.97c |
Note: aThe P value was obtained using a two-sample two-tailed t test. bThe P value was obtained using a two-tailed Pearson chi-square test. cThe P value was obtained using nonparametric (Mann-Whitney) test.
3.2. Effects of Combined Cognitive-Psychological-Physical Intervention on Behavioral Performance
ANOVA analyses revealed that, after the intervention, significant improvements in the PALT and vitality (VT, a dimension of MOS SF-36) and greater improvements in SSRS were found in the intervention group, but no significant or smaller improvements in these tests were found in the control group. In addition, the results also showed that the performance on TMT and SWLS did not change for the intervention group but decreased for the control group after the intervention (refer to Li et al. [24] for detailed behavioral results).
3.3. ReHo Maps and the Intervention-Related Changes
Figure 2 demonstrates the ReHo maps before and after intervention for both groups. The ReHo maps displayed very similar spatial patterns to the default-mode network. The whole-brain voxel-wise Group × Intervention ANOVA analysis showed that there were four regions with significant interactions for Group × Intervention (AlphaSim correction, P < 0.01), including the left superior temporal gyrus (STG; peak MNI location: −27, 15, −33; 23 voxels); left posterior lobe of cerebellum (PCL; peak MNI location: −36, −60, −24; 22 voxels); left middle temporal gyrus (MTG; peak MNI location: −42, −63, 0; 27 voxels); and right MTG (peak MNI location: 36, −60, 3; 73 voxels) (Figure 3(a)). The paired t-tests of the mean ReHo for spontaneous brain activity in each of the four regions of interests (ROIs) demonstrated that combined intervention significantly increased the ReHo in the left STG (P = 0.019) and left PCL (P = 0.038) but decreased the ReHo in the left MTG (P = 0.032) in the intervention group. By contrast, the control group showed significantly decreased ReHo in the left STG (P = 0.005) and PCL (P = 0.009) and increased ReHo in the left MTG (P = 0.068) and right MTG (P < 0.001) (Figure 3(b)).
3.4. Correlations between Intervention-Related Changes in Brain Activity and Cognitive Performance
Correlations were calculated between the ReHo measures of four ROIs and neuropsychological test scores on cognition (TMT, PALT, and CFT). We performed the correlations analyses between the intervention-related changes in ReHo of spontaneous brain activity ((postintervention − preintervention)/preintervention) and changes in cognitive measures (postintervention − preintervention). In the intervention group, the results showed that intervention-related ReHo changes in the left STG were significantly positively correlated with changes in the CFT in the participants who attended the intervention activities (r = 0.699, P = 0.002) at a Bonferroni correction threshold of 0.0042 (0.05/12). On the other hand, no significant correlation between the ReHo changes in the left STG and gains of the CFT was found in the control group, r = 0.086, P = 0.742 (Figure 4(a)). A further analysis directly comparing the correlations between the two groups revealed that the two correlation coefficients were significantly different from one another, Fisher's r to Z = 2.06, P = 0.039. When a more liberal threshold of P < 0.05 without correction was applied, we found that the intervention-related ReHo changes in the right MTG were negatively correlated with the intervention-related increase in the total PALT scores in the intervention group, r = −0.544, P = 0.024. In the control group, no significant correlation was observed between the ReHo changes in the right MTG and gains of the PALT, r = 0.142, P = 0.586 (Figure 4(b)). Subsequent analysis revealed that there was significant difference between the two correlation coefficients, Fisher's r to Z = −1.99, P = 0.047.
4. Discussion
We explored the effects of a combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention on brain functional plasticity in healthy older adults using RS-fMRI. The results revealed improved cognitive performance and alterations in the ReHo of local spontaneous brain activity in the superior and middle temporal gyri and the posterior lobe of the cerebellum for the intervention group. These intervention-related ReHo changes were correlated with individual improvements in cognitive performance. The present study provides evidence that combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention can induce functional plastic changes in the lateral temporal lobe and cerebellum in healthy older adults.
Based on the whole-brain voxel-wise analysis, the fMRI results revealed that combined intervention significantly altered the coherence of local spontaneous brain activity in the left superior temporal gyrus (STG) and middle temporal gyrus (MTG) in the intervention group. Although previous studies have primarily emphasized the effects of aging on the frontal cortex and MTL, many studies have implied that the lateral temporal lobe is also vulnerable. Decreases in the density of gray matter in the left superior temporal region have been reported [56] and the bilateral temporal cortices show obvious atrophy [57, 58] and decreased metabolism [59] in the elderly. The left STG has previously been thought to play an important role in speech comprehension; however, this region is also involved in speech production [60]. Studies of conduction aphasia where there is damage in the left STG have confirmed the role of the STG in speech production, by revealing good comprehension but phonemic paraphasias and naming difficulties [61]. A recent study has demonstrated plasticity in this brain region [62]. The researchers found that the cortical thickness of the left STG increased in those who participate in foreign language training, and this structural change was positively correlated with posttest proficiency in language use.
It is well known that spontaneous resting state brain activity may serve an important role in brain function [27]. Furthermore, previous studies have proved that ReHo measures are associated with individual differences in cognitive performance [35–37]. Consequently, it is reasonable to speculate that the present finding of enhanced coherence of local spontaneous brain activity in the left STG for the intervention group suggests that combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention can induce brain functional plasticity in the superior temporal cortex and lead to functional improvements in this brain region associated with speech production.
Correlation analysis further supports this proposal by showing a positive correlation between intervention-related changes in coherence in the left STG and changes in the CFT for the intervention group. The CFT is a typical neuropsychological test used to examine speech production, and performance of the CFT declines with normal aging [63, 64]. The CFT greatly depends upon the function of the temporal lobe [65], and patients with semantic dementia and temporal lobe atrophy show poor category fluency [66, 67]. The results of an fMRI study revealed greater activation in the left STG during the category fluency tasks, further confirming the important role of this region in language production [68]. In the current study, our correlation results are consistent with an important functional role for the left STG in CFT performance and provide evidence that the patterns of local spontaneous resting brain activity in the left STG could reflect individual CFT performance in older adults.
Interestingly, the present results showed a concomitant increase and decrease in the ReHo of spontaneous brain activity in the left STG and left MTG for the intervention group. Although the exact reasons are still unclear, we believe that the present results to some extent echo heterogeneous functional characteristics in subregions of the lateral temporal lobe. The MTG is thought to be involved in mapping between the phonological forms of words and their meanings and serves as a sound-to-meaning interface [60, 69, 70], and meta-analyses of neuroimaging literature have confirmed the importance of its role in semantic processing [71, 72]. The MTG has extensive structural and functional connectivity with frontal, parietal, and temporal regions in the resting brain, and this is thought to play a central role in language comprehension [73]. Therefore, the present finding of decreased coherence of local spontaneous resting activity in left MTG suggests functional plastic changes in the middle temporal cortex and may reflect greater efficiency of information processing in the intervention group for this brain region involved in language processing.
Although the ReHo of the right MTG did not change significantly after intervention in the intervention group, within a more liberal threshold (P < 0.05), our results showed a significant correlation between coherence changes in the right MTG and the PALT for the intervention group. The PALT is a neuropsychological test used to examine the episodic memory using word pairs [44]. Previous task-based fMRI studies have found that activity of the right MTG during encoding is correlated with subsequent memory performance, suggesting that brain activity in regions in charge of language comprehension is predictive of episodic memory with narrative materials [74, 75]. In the present study, our results further confirm the important role of the MTG in the processing of language materials and provide evidence that intervention-induced changes in patterns of local spontaneous brain activity in the right MTG may also predict individual episodic memory in older adults.
The combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention also induced enhanced coherence of local spontaneous activity in the left posterior lobe of the cerebellum (PCL) for the intervention group. The traditional views are that the cerebellum is mainly responsible for motor coordination and motor learning, but in recent years, investigators have paid more attention to the higher-order functions of the cerebellum, such as working memory, executive functions, and emotional control [76–78]. Damage to the cerebellum, especially in the lateral hemisphere of the posterior cerebellum, will induce cerebellar cognitive affective syndromes (CCAS), which are characterized by impairments in executive, visual-spatial, linguistic abilities, and affective disorders [79]. Functional topography studies within the cerebellum have further indicated that anterior portions of the cerebellum mainly support motor function, whereas the posterior regions of the cerebellum are mainly involved in cognitive and emotional processing [80–82].
A substantial body of evidence has demonstrated that the cerebellum also shows age-related decreases in structural morphology and function [83]. For instance, a recent study found age-related decreases in resting-state cerebello-cortical functional connectivity, and lower connectivity was associated with poorer cognitive performance in older adults [84]. Our intervention program contains cognitive training (mnemonic and executive function training) and group counseling, so the present findings of increased local coherence of spontaneous brain activity in the PCL suggest that intervention-induced functional optimization of this region contributes to higher levels of cognitive processing and emotional control in the intervention group.
Taken together, the present study confirms that the combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention induces regionally brain functional reorganization. However, it should be noted that we cannot ascertain the exact contribution of each specific training component to the brain functional plasticity. Interestingly, though our previous study observed enhanced resting-state functional connectivity between the MTL and PFC [24], the present findings did not show significant intervention-induced changes of ReHo values in these two brain regions. These results indicated that the functional plasticity of the aging brain is a complex process. Further studies are required to elucidate the underlying causes and mechanism. In addition, we observed both a positive correlation in the left STG and a negative correlation in the right MTG between intervention-related changes in ReHo and cognitive performance in the present study. The relationship linking changes of ReHo values and individual behavioral performance is extremely complicated. The higher ReHo level does not necessarily result in better performance and vice versa. Evidences can be found in previous correlation studies between ReHo and cognitive function [35, 39]. The underlying meaning of increased or reduced ReHo values remains unclear. It seems that both positive and negative correlations could make the inference that the changes of ReHo may reflect individual behavioral performance.
Several limitations should be noted. Firstly, the control group only attended two 120 min lectures and is therefore not a completely active control group. Nevertheless, as noted in Takeuchi et al. [85], the lack of an active control group is actually a commonly used approach in neuroimaging studies involving training. The positive effects induced by intervention cannot simply be attributed to active stimulation [86]. In the present study, the intervention group showed alterations in patterns of local spontaneous brain activity in the brain regions that are vulnerable to aging, and moreover, the ReHo changes were correlated with the gains of individual cognitive performance. We believe that these beneficial effects at least partially reflect the function of the combined intervention. Secondly, it is unfortunate that the participants could not be randomized to the intervention and control groups. This was necessary because the participants were enrolled from two communities, and if there were participants from the same community in each group, it would be possible for them to communicate the intervention contents and thus confound the intervention effects. Thirdly, we found that combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention could improve the functional organization of the resting brain, and although it could be supposed that this corresponds to an overall increase in cellular activity and metabolic rate in the relevant brain regions [86], the underlying mechanisms for this functional plasticity are still obscure. Lastly, we did not assess the long-term effects of the combined intervention. Future research is needed to determine whether, and to what extent, these beneficial effects can be maintained over time.
5. Conclusion
In summary, the present study extended our previous findings by showing that combined intervention could optimize the intrinsic functional brain architecture in the temporal cortex and cerebellum in the normal elderly. Moreover, the changes in ReHo of local spontaneous resting state activity could predict improvements in individual cognitive performance. These results further suggest the effectiveness of the intervention in curbing the loss of brain function in older adults.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31271108, 31200847, 31070916, and 31470998), the Knowledge Innovation Project of the Chinese Academy of Sciences (KSCX2-EW-J-8), the CAS/SAFEA International Partnership Program for Creative Research Team (Y2CX131003), the Scientific Foundation of Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (Y1CX251005 and 111000C038), and the Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (KLMH2014ZK02).
Conflict of Interests
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.
References
- 1.Cabeza R., Nyberg L. Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging. New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nyberg L., Lövdén M., Riklund K., Lindenberger U., Bäckman L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(5):292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17(1):85–100. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.17.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Giorgio A., Santelli L., Tomassini V., et al. Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood. NeuroImage. 2010;51(3):943–951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Grady C. L. Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:127–144. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Raz N., Lindenberger U., Rodrigue K. M., et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15(11):1676–1689. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Raz N., Rodrigue K. M., Head D., Kennedy K. M., Acker J. D. Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology. 2004;62(3):433–438. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000106466.09835.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Arenaza-Urquijo E. M., Landeau B., La Joie R., et al. Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. NeuroImage. 2013;83:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hillman C. H., Erickson K. I., Kramer A. F. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9(1):58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kramer A. F., Bherer L., Colcombe S. J., Dong W., Greenough W. T. Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. Journals of Gerontology—Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2004;59(9):940–957. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.9.m940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Engvig A., Fjell A. M., Westlye L. T., et al. Effects of memory training on cortical thickness in the elderly. NeuroImage. 2010;52(4):1667–1676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kirchhoff B. A., Anderson B. A., Barch D. M., Jacoby L. L. Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. Cerebral Cortex. 2012;22(4):788–799. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hampstead B. M., Stringer A. Y., Stilla R. F., Giddens M., Sathian K. Mnemonic strategy training partially restores hippocampal activity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Hippocampus. 2012;22(8):1652–1658. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Belleville S., Clément F., Mellah S., Gilbert B., Fontaine F., Gauthier S. Training-related brain plasticity in subjects at risk of developing Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2011;134(6):1623–1634. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Anguera J. A., Boccanfuso J., Rintoul J. L., et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature. 2013;501(7465):97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Osaka M., Otsuka Y., Osaka N. Verbal to visual code switching improves working memory in older adults: an fMRI study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6, article 24 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Erickson K. I., Leckie R. L., Weinstein A. M. Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(supplement 2):S20–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Erickson K. I., Voss M. W., Prakash R. S., et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108(7):3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Voss M. W., Heo S., Prakash R. S., et al. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(11):2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Voss M. W., Prakash R. S., Erickson K. I., et al. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2010;2, article 32 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bamidis P. D., Vivas A. B., Styliadis C., et al. A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;44:206–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hötting K., Röder B. Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37(9, part B):2243–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kraft E. Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging: possible biological links and implications for multimodal interventions. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 2012;19(1-2):248–263. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.645010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Li R., Zhu X., Yin S., et al. Multimodal intervention in older adults improves resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2014;6, article 39 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Guerra-Carrillo B., Mackey A. P., Bunge S. A. Resting-state fMRI: a window into human brain plasticity. The Neuroscientist. 2014;20(5):522–533. doi: 10.1177/1073858414524442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Biswal B. B. Resting state fMRI: a personal history. NeuroImage. 2012;62(2):938–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fox M. D., Raichle M. E. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8(9):700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Raichle M. E. Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14(4):180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pieramico V., Esposito R., Sensi F., et al. Combination training in aging individuals modifies functional connectivity and cognition, and is potentially affected by dopamine-related genes. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(8) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043901.e43901 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Smith S. M., Miller K. L., Salimi-Khorshidi G., et al. Network modelling methods for FMRI. NeuroImage. 2011;54(2):875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zang Y., Jiang T., Lu Y., He Y., Tian L. Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. NeuroImage. 2004;22(1):394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jiang L., Xu T., He Y., et al. Toward neurobiological characterization of functional homogeneity in the human cortex: regional variation, morphological association and functional covariance network organization. Brain Structure and Function. 2014 doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0795-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zuo X.-N., Xu T., Jiang L. L., et al. Toward reliable characterization of functional homogeneity in the human brain: preprocessing, scan duration, imaging resolution and computational space. NeuroImage. 2013;65:374–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zuo X.-N., Xing X.-X. Test-retest reliabilities of resting-state FMRI measurements in human brain functional connectomics: a systems neuroscience perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;45:100–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tian L., Ren J., Zang Y. Regional homogeneity of resting state fMRI signals predicts Stop signal task performance. NeuroImage. 2012;60(1):539–544. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wang L., Song M., Jiang T., Zhang Y., Yu C. Regional homogeneity of the resting-state brain activity correlates with individual intelligence. Neuroscience Letters. 2011;488(3):275–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang T., Chen Z., Zhao G., et al. Linking inter-individual differences in the conflict adaptation effect to spontaneous brain activity. NeuroImage. 2014;90(15):146–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Zhang Z., Liu Y., Jiang T., et al. Altered spontaneous activity in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment revealed by Regional Homogeneity. NeuroImage. 2012;59(2):1429–1440. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.He Y., Wang L., Zang Y., et al. Regional coherence changes in the early stages of Alzheimer's disease: a combined structural and resting-state functional MRI study. NeuroImage. 2007;35(2):488–500. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chang Y.-K., Nien Y.-H., Tsai C.-L., Etnier J. L. Physical activity and cognition in older adults: the potential of Tai Chi Chuan. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 2010;18(4):451–472. doi: 10.1123/japa.18.4.451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wei G.-X., Dong H.-M., Yang Z., Luo J., Zuo X.-N. Tai Chi Chuan optimizes the functional organization of the intrinsic human brain architecture in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2014;6, article 74 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Subramaniam K., Vinogradov S. Improving the neural mechanisms of cognition through the pursuit of happiness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7, article 452 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Yu J., Li J., Huang X. The Beijing version of the montreal cognitive assessment as a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment: a community-based study. BMC Psychiatry. 2012;12(1, article 156) doi: 10.1186/1471-244x-12-156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Xu S., Wu Z. The construction of ‘The Clinical Memory Test’. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 1986;18:100–108. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gong Y. X. Manual of Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Chinese Version. Changsha, China: Chinese Map Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Reitan R. M. Trail Making Test: Manual for Administration and Scoring. Tucson, Ariz, USA: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Stroop J. R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18(6):643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ware J. E. SF-36 health survey update. Spine. 2000;25(24):3130–3139. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Xiao S. Y. The theoretical basis and research applications of ‘Social Support Rating Scale’. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1994;4:98–100. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Diener E., Emmons R. A., Larsen R. J., Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1985;49(1):71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Campbell A., Converse P. E., Rodgers W. L. The Quality of American Life: Perceptions, Evaluations, and Aatisfactions. Russell Sage Foundation; 1976. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yan C. G., Zang Y. F. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for ‘pipeline’ data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Frontiers in System Neuroscience. 2010;4, article 13 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Roberts R. E., Vernon S. W. The center for epidemiologic studies depression scale: its use in a community sample. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1983;140(1):41–46. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lawton M. P., Brody E. M. Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_part_1.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Sowell E. R., Peterson B. S., Thompson P. M., Welcome S. E., Henkenius A. L., Toga A. W. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6(3):309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fjell A. M., McEvoy L., Holland D., Dale A. M., Walhovd K. B., Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Brain changes in older adults at very low risk for Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(19):8237–8242. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5506-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Fjell A. M., Walhovd K. B., Fennema-Notestine C., et al. One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(48):15223–15231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.de Santi S., de Leon M. J., Convit A., et al. Age-related changes in brain: II. Positron emission tomography of frontal and temporal lobe glucose metabolism in normal subjects. Psychiatric Quarterly. 1995;66(4):357–370. doi: 10.1007/bf02238755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hickok G., Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8(5):393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Damasio A. R. Aphasia. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1992;326(8):531–539. doi: 10.1056/nejm199202203260806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mårtensson J., Eriksson J., Bodammer N. C., et al. Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage. 2012;63(1):240–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Clark L. J., Gatz M., Zheng L., Chen Y.-L., McCleary C., MacK W. J. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer's disease. The American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and Other Dementias. 2009;24(6):461–468. doi: 10.1177/1533317509345154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Mathuranath P. S., George A., Cherian P. J., Alexander A., Sarma S. G., Sarma P. S. Effects of age, education and gender on verbal fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(8):1057–1064. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.8.1057.16736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Clark D. G., Wadley V. G., Kapur P., et al. Lexical factors and cerebral regions influencing verbal fluency performance in MCI. Neuropsychologia. 2014;54(1):98–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Libon D. J., McMillan C., Gunawardena D., et al. Neurocognitive contributions to verbal fluency deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology. 2009;73(7):535–542. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b2a4f5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Marczinski C. A., Kertesz A. Category and letter fluency in semantic dementia, primary progressive aphasia, and Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language. 2006;97(3):258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Meinzer M., Flaisch T., Wilser L., et al. Neural signatures of semantic and phonemic fluency in young and old adults. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21(10):2007–2018. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hickok G., Poeppel D. Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition. 2004;92(1-2):67–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Indefrey P., Levelt W. J. M. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition. 2004;92(1-2):101–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Binder J. R., Desai R. H., Graves W. W., Conant L. L. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19(12):2767–2796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Vigneau M., Beaucousin V., Hervé P. Y., et al. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage. 2006;30(4):1414–1432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Turken A. U., Dronkers N. F. The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011;(2011, article no. 1) doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ezzyat Y., Davachi L. What constitutes an episode in episodic memory? Psychological Science. 2011;22(2):243–252. doi: 10.1177/0956797610393742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hasson U., Nusbaum H. C., Small S. L. Brain networks subserving the extraction of sentence information and its encoding to memory. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17(12):2899–2913. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Balsters J. H., Whelan C. D., Robertson I. H., Ramnani N. Cerebellum and cognition: evidence for the encoding of higher order rules. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23(6):1433–1443. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Schmahmann J. D. The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychology Review. 2010;20(3):236–260. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9142-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Stoodley C. J. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum. 2012;11(2):352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Schmahmann J. D. Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2004;16(3):367–378. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.3.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Hautzel H., Mottaghy F. M., Specht K., Müller H.-W., Krause B. J. Evidence of a modality-dependent role of the cerebellum in working memory? An fMRI study comparing verbal and abstract n-back tasks. NeuroImage. 2009;47(4):2073–2082. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Stoodley C. J., Schmahmann J. D. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2009;44(2):489–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Stoodley C. J., Valera E. M., Schmahmann J. D. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2012;59(2):1560–1570. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Bernard J. A., Seidler R. D. Moving forward: age effects on the cerebellum underlie cognitive and motor declines. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;42:193–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Bernard J. A., Peltier S. J., Wiggins J. L., et al. Disrupted cortico-cerebellar connectivity in older adults. NeuroImage. 2013;83:103–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Takeuchi H., Taki Y., Nouchi R., et al. Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest. Cortex. 2013;49(8):2106–2125. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Chapman S. B., Aslan S., Spence J. S., et al. Neural mechanisms of brain plasticity with complex cognitive training in healthy seniors. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25(2):396–405. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]