Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Sep 3.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Sleep Med. 2014 Sep 15;13(5):412–423. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2014.940104

Sleep Hygiene Practices and Bedtime Resistance in Low-Income Preschoolers: Does Temperament Matter?

Katherine E Wilson 1, Julie C Lumeng 2,3, Niko Kaciroti 2,4, Sophie Yu-Pu Chen 4, Monique K LeBourgeois 5, Ronald D Chervin 1, Alison L Miller 2,6
PMCID: PMC4362749  NIHMSID: NIHMS610167  PMID: 25221914

Abstract

Objective

To test the association of home sleep hygiene practices with bedtime resistance among preschoolers, and to determine whether this association differs by child temperament.

Methods

Parents of Head Start preschoolers (n=374, 56% non-Hispanic white) completed questionnaires, including the 5-item Going to Bed subscale of the Children’s Sleep-Wake Scale (GTB; higher score reflects less bedtime resistance), 22-item Children's Sleep Hygiene Scale (CSHS; higher score reflects better sleep hygiene practices) and Child Behavior Questionnaire-Short Form anger, activity, and impulsivity subscales (our measure of difficult temperament). Monte Carlo simulation adjusted for child age, gender, race/ethnicity, and maternal education and tested the association of CSHS with GTB in children with more difficult vs. less difficult temperaments.

Results

Children with more difficult vs. less difficult temperaments experienced worse sleep hygiene practices (p<.0001) and had more bedtime resistance (p<.0001). Among children with more difficult temperaments, better sleep hygiene was linearly associated with less bedtime resistance (GTB β = 1.28, 95% CI 0.77, 1.78). Among children with less difficult temperaments, the association followed a piecewise linear tread: sleep hygiene was not associated with bedtime resistance when CSHS scores were < 4.1 (GTB β = 0.15, 95% CI -4.87, 3.13), but for CSHS scores ≥ 4.1, an increase in CSHS was associated with lower bedtime resistance (GTB β = 1.33, 95% CI 1.00, 1.79).

Conclusions

Consistency in sleep hygiene practices are associated with less bedtime resistance and may be especially helpful in reducing bedtime resistance among children with more difficult temperaments.

Keywords: Child behavior, Sleep, Parenting

INTRODUCTION

Sleep disturbances in early childhood can have significant consequences for children’s overall quality of life1, daytime behavior15, and school performance46 and have been associated with an increased risk of sleep problems later in childhood.7,8 Bedtime resistance typically manifests as bedtime delay through behaviors such as bedtime refusal, stalling, or "curtain calls" and has been associated with sleep problems such as nocturnal arousals, sleepwalking and difficulty waking in children.9 Bedtime resistance behaviors are part of a well described type of behavioral insomnia of childhood (limit setting type). Behavioral insomnia of childhood is commonly reported in young children, with an estimated prevalence of 13–50%.811

Good sleep hygiene practices, such as use of a regular bedtime and avoiding stimulating activities (electronic use and caffeine intake), are an important part of healthy sleep in children; poor sleep hygiene may play an important role in bedtime problems. For example, in preschool children, poor sleep hygiene practices such as falling asleep with a parent in the room, having a TV in the room, and drinking caffeine were associated with shorter sleep duration.12 Another component of good sleep hygiene practice commonly recommended in the clinical setting is having a regular bedtime routine.13 Yet, the association between bedtime routine and sleep duration in preschoolers is less clear. In a national sample of over 4000 predominantly low-income preschool-aged children, having a bedtime routine that includes activities, such as reading a book, praying, or playing a game, at age 3 years was associated with longer sleep duration at age 5 years.14 In contrast, in a national sample of 385 preschool-aged children, having a regular bedtime routine was not associated with longer sleep duration.12 Therefore, optimizing sleep hygiene practices may be helpful for parents who are struggling with bedtime problems at home.

A number of studies have evaluated specific sleep hygiene practices with regard to bedtime resistance behaviors. Television viewing before bed is a common example of a poor sleep hygiene practice that has been associated with increased bedtime resistance in children.15 Similarly, children who share a bed with a parent or fall asleep somewhere other than their own bed are also more likely to have bedtime resistance.9 On the other hand, the consistent use of a good sleep hygiene practice such as a regular bedtime routine has been associated with less bedtime resistance in elementary school children.9 One randomized controlled trial examined the effect of having a bedtime routine on bedtime behaviors that could be considered problematic or resistant (e.g., "number of times child calls for parent," "number of times out of crib/bed," "how difficult was bedtime," and “number of night wakings") in 199 children, ages 18 to 36 months. Families were instructed to institute a nightly 3-step bedtime routine that included a bath, applying lotion, and doing quiet activities (e.g., cuddling, singing a lullaby), with lights out within 30 minutes of the end of the bath. It was not reported how adherent families were to the prescribed intervention, but following the 2-week intervention period, children in the intervention arm had a number of sleep benefits including a longer sleep duration, fewer parent-reported “sleep problems”, “less difficult” parent-reported bedtimes, fewer times out of bed and fewer times calling the parent.16

Despite sleep hygiene being commonly recommended in clinical practice13 and increased parent reported problematic bedtime behaviors at the preschool age, few studies have examined the association between sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance in preschool children. A better understanding of this association could help clinicians with recommendations for families struggling with bedtime resistance behaviors. Furthermore, identifying child characteristics that may alter the association between sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance may contribute to the development of more effective interventions for bedtime resistance, or the ability to target such interventions more appropriately, but to our knowledge, these characteristics have not previously been examined.

Child temperament is particularly important to examine, as sleep problems are more common among children with a more difficult temperament.17,18 Temperament, and particularly the construct of difficult temperament, is defined differently across studies, with some measures asking parents to rate directly the difficultness of their infants across domains, and others focused on behavioral qualities of the child. In either case, difficult temperament is typically conceptualized as a constellation of traits, characterized by high reactivity, particularly negative emotionality, and a lack of behavioral control.19 Studies of temperament and sleep in young children have shown that toddlers with lower rhythmicity, adaptability, and approach; preschoolers and school-aged children with more emotionality20; and school-aged children with lower task-orientation and higher reactivity6 have all been described as having more sleep problems. Five studies have evaluated child temperamental qualities in relation to sleep problems, finding associations with varying dimensions of temperament, including child emotionality (but not shyness, sociability, or activity)20, resistance to control21, difficult temperament (defined as more emotionally negative, less adaptable and more persistent/unstoppable)22, difficult temperament based on 9 different temperament dimensions in the Toddler Temperament Scale23, and higher reactivity and less task-orientation.6 Three of these five studies used non-US samples6,22,23, one focused solely on school-aged children6, none focused on lower-income families, and several may have been underpowered by limited sample sizes.20,23 Additionally, one study looked at the association between bed sharing and child temperament in preschool children and found that bedsharers were less adaptable, less rhythmic, and more intense as compared to children who were solitary sleepers.24 Thus, although it appears that more difficult temperament is associated with sleep difficulties, less is known about associations between sleep hygiene practices and temperament. It is also unknown whether associations between good sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance differ among children with more difficult versus less difficult temperaments.

In this context, the present study sought to assess the association between sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance in preschoolers, and to examine the relationship between sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance among children with more difficult versus less difficult temperaments. Our study focuses on low-income families as they have been found to have more bedtime problems25 and are less likely to have a regular bedtime routine.26 We hypothesized that 1) worse sleep hygiene practices would be associated with greater bedtime resistance; 2) children with more difficult temperaments would demonstrate greater bedtime resistance; 3) children with more difficult temperaments would experience worse sleep hygiene practices; and 4) the association of sleep hygiene and bedtime resistance would differ based on child temperament.

METHODS

Participants

Participants (n = 374) were preschool children ages 3 to 5 years enrolled in Head Start (federally-funded preschool for children in poverty) and their parents, who were recruited as part of a larger study about stress and eating behaviors in children. This was a convenience sample of typically-developing preschool-age children with no known history of sleep problems who were selected on the basis of Head Start participation. Inclusion criteria were: parent and child spoke English fluently, no parent with a 4-year college degree or more, child born at 35 weeks gestation or later with no significant perinatal complications, and no history of food allergies, serious medical problems, or significant developmental delay. After written informed consent of the parent or legal guardian, study questionnaires were administered by a research assistant in the family’s home. The study protocol was approved by the University of Michigan Institutional Review Board.

Measures

The Children’s Sleep Wake Scale (CSWS) is a 40-item parent-report measure that assesses five behavioral dimensions of sleep quality in 2- to 8-year-old children using a 6-point scale (1 = never; 6 = always).27 This scale has been previously used to evaluate sleep quality in children.28,29 The Going to Bed Subscale (GTB) is the mean of 5 items on the CSWS describing behaviors that indicate bedtime resistance (e.g., repeated requests to stay up, crying/complaining about going to bed). Thus, in the current study we defined bedtime resistance, our primary outcome measure, as the GTB subscale (means and standard deviations for individual items contributing to the GTB subscale are listed in Table 1; sample Cronbach’s α = 0.88). A higher score indicated less bedtime resistance. We used the Children’s Sleep Hygiene Scale30 (CSHS) total mean score as our primary predictor. The CSHS is a 22-item parent-report measure that has been used in previous research31,32 and assesses the frequency of use of good sleep hygiene practices (e.g., regular bedtimes and wake times; restriction of caffeine and highly arousing activities prior to bedtime; predictable and comfortable sleeping environment; sample Cronbach’s α = 0.72). Mothers responded to each item on a 6-point scale (1 = never; 6 = always); a higher score indicates that the family is engaging in better sleep hygiene practices.

TABLE 1.

Question items contributing to primary outcome1

Mean (SD)
Going to Bed Subscale (GTB)
Your child makes repeated requests (asks for another drink, hug, etc.) at bedtime2 3.69 (1.57)
Your child wants to stay up and do other things at bedtime (read, play, watch TV)2 3.63 (1.52)
Your child is ready to go to bed at bedtime 2.94 (1.44)
Your child complains or cries about going to bed2 3.19 (1.56)
Your child puts off or delays going to bed2 3.36 (1.62)
1

Individual items ranged from 1=never to 6=always.

2

Items are presented as the raw responses in the table and were reverse coded to create scores.

To assess child temperament, parents completed 7 subscales (Activity Level, Anger, Attentional Focusing, High Intensity Pleasure, Impulsivity, Inhibitory Control, Shyness) of the Child Behavior Questionnaire-Short Form,3335 by rating items on a scale from 1 = extremely untrue to 7 = extremely true. Subscale scores are calculated as the means of responses to relevant items. Of these scales, we chose the aspects of child behavior that are conceptually and theoretically implicated as “difficult” in the temperament literature to characterize difficult temperament in the current study. Thus, the anger (6 items, e.g. “Has temper tantrums when s(he) doesn’t get what s(he) wants”, sample Cronbach’s α = 0.78), activity level (7 items, e.g. “Seems always in a big hurry to get from one place to another”, Cronbach’s α = 0.66), and impulsivity (6 items, e.g. “Usually rushes into an activity without thinking about it”, Cronbach’s α = 0.58) subscales were averaged to create a ‘difficult temperament’ score (Cronbach’s α = 0.78), with potential scores ranging from 1 to 7 and a higher score indicating a more difficult temperament. As in other studies that have conceptualized difficult temperament based on parent-reported temperament scales,3436 these 3 subscales were chosen to reflect increased reactivity and negative affect, hallmarks of difficult temperament.22,35,37 Although temperament is theoretically best conceptualized as a continuum, we dichotomized our temperament variable for the purposes of the current study, in order to test whether children with more difficult temperaments would have more bedtime resistance and experience worse sleep hygiene practices, and to determine whether the sleep hygiene-bedtime resistance association was similar for children who varied in their temperamental difficulty. The mean score in our sample (M = 4.97, SD = 0.76) was comparable to previously-published scales using this measure.38 As we were interested in the role of difficult temperament in particular, we dichotomized the difficult temperament score based on an extreme-group approach and categorized children who were rated 1 standard deviation or above the sample mean as having a “more difficult” temperament, and children with scores below 1 standard deviation as having a “less difficult" temperament. We used 1 versus 2 standard deviations as the cutoff in our approach in order to have an adequate number of children in each group for analysis.

Demographic information included child age, gender, and race/ethnicity (categorized as non-Hispanic white versus not); and parent education level (categorized as high school diploma or equivalent versus more than high school education).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Correlations were used to assess the association of CSHS and GTB. T-tests for continuous measures and Chi- square tests for categorical measures were used to examine associations of child temperament (more difficult versus easier) with covariates as well as CSHS and GTB. Cohen’s d coefficients were calculated and effect sizes were defined as a small (≤0.2), moderate effect (≥0.5) or large effect (≥0.8). To examine the association between CSHS and GTB among children with more difficult versus less difficult temperaments, allowing for the possibility that the relationships were non-linear, we implemented a Bayesian change point model, adjusting for demographic covariates, age, gender, race/ethnicity and parent education level. Such a model is appropriate when there is a threshold at which the strength of the association changes. We implemented Proc MCMC in SAS to fit this model, using Markov chain Monte Carlo simulations techniques which are used to jointly estimate the threshold, if it exists, as well as the strength of the associations (slopes) above and below the threshold. Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) were used to decide whether a non-linear change point model fit the data better than a linear model.

RESULTS

Participant characteristics are shown in Table 2, for the entire study sample and by temperament (easier versus more difficult). Children had a mean age of 4.1 (SD = 0.54) years. Half of the children were male and 56% were non-Hispanic white, 16% were Black, 17% were Biracial or another race, and 11% were Hispanic. About half (48%) of the parents had less than a high school diploma or the equivalent. GTB and CSHS were positively correlated (r = .55, p<.001). The mean GTB Score was 3.41 (SD = 1.27), with an observed range of 1.00 to 6.00. The mean CSHS score was 4.82 (SD = 0.50), with an observed range of 3.2 to 5.9. T-tests for continuous measures and Chi- square tests for categorical measures revealed that none of the demographic covariates (age, gender, race/ethnicity, parent education level) differed by child temperament. However, as seen in Table 2, children with more difficult temperaments experienced worse sleep hygiene practices and had more bedtime resistance compared to children with less difficult temperaments (p<0.001). Therefore, there was a moderate effect size for sleep hygiene (Cohen’s d = 0.63) and a large effect size for bedtime resistance (Cohen’s d = 0.98).

TABLE 2.

Sample demographics (n = 374)

Total Less Difficult
Temperament
n= 313
More
Difficult
Temperament
n= 61
p-
value1
Age (years), Mean (SD) 4.10 (0.54) 4.12 (0.53) 4.03 (0.60) 0.270
Gender, n (%)
  Male 186 (49.7) 153 (48.9) 33 (54.1) 0.457
  Female 188 (50.3) 160 (51.1) 28 (45.9)
Race/Ethnicity, n (%)
  Non-Hispanic white 209 (55.9) 171 (54.6) 38 (62.3) 0.271
  Hispanic or not white 165 (44.1) 142 (45.4) 23 (37.7)
Parent Education level, n (%)
  ≤ High school diploma/equivalent 179 (47.9) 145 (46.3) 34 (55.7) 0.179
  > high School diploma/equivalent 195 (52.1) 168 (53.7) 27 (44.3)
CSHS, Mean (SD) 4.82 (0.50) 4.87 (0.48) 4.55 (0.53) <.0001
GTB, Mean (SD) 3.41 (1.27) 3.59 (1.20) 2.42 (1.20) <.0001
1

Pearson’s chi-square test was used for categorical variables, and the t test was used for continuous variables.

CSHS: Children’s Sleep Hygiene Scale. A higher score indicates better sleep hygiene practices.

GTB: Going to Bed Subscale. A higher score indicates less bedtime resistance.

The Bayesian change point model showing the association between sleep hygiene and bedtime resistance among children with easier versus more difficult temperaments, controlling for child age, child gender, child race/ethnicity, and parent education is shown in Figure 1. The BIC indicated that the nature of the association between sleep hygiene consistency and bedtime resistance differed by child temperament, with a linear association between sleep hygiene and bedtime resistance present among children with a more difficult temperament, and a change point (cp) of 4.11 on the CSHS identified for the less difficult temperament group. For children with a more difficult temperament, each one-point increase in CSHS resulted in a linear increase of GTB (i.e., less bedtime resistance; β = 1.28, 95% CI 0.77, 1.78, n = 61). For children with less difficult temperaments, increases in CSHS up to 4.1 were not associated with statistically significant changes in GTB scores (β = 0.15, 95% CI -4.87, 3.13, n = 16); whereas increases above 4.1 were associated with increases in GTB scores (β = 1.33, 95% CI 1.00, 1.79, n = 297). Even with the highest CSHS scores, children with more difficult temperaments, on average, had a lower GTB score than the children with less difficult temperaments.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Markov chain Monte Carlo simulations depicting relationship between sleep hygiene practices as measured by total score on the Children Sleep Hygiene Scale (CSHS) and Bedtime Resistance as measured by the Going to Bed subscale (GTB), based on child temperament.

CONCLUSIONS

Five main findings emerged from this study. First, we found that sleep hygiene was positively associated with bedtime resistance in that better sleep hygiene practices correlated with less bedtime resistance. Second, children with more difficult temperaments had substantially more bedtime resistance than children with less difficult temperaments. Third, children with a more difficult temperament, on average, had worse sleep hygiene practices than children with a less difficult temperament. Fourth, better sleep hygiene practices were linearly associated with lower bedtime resistance among children with more difficult temperaments. In contrast, among children with less difficult temperaments, better sleep hygiene practices were only associated with lower bedtime resistance above the change point of 4.11 on the CSHS, and not associated below this point. Finally, even when children with more difficult temperaments had the best sleep hygiene practices their bedtime resistance remained greater than the bedtime resistance reported in children with less difficult temperaments who had worse sleep hygiene practices.

Our finding that children with more difficult temperaments have more bedtime resistance adds to a somewhat diverse literature in this area. Prior work has identified associations of child behavioral39 or psychiatric problems40 with sleep problems. Much of the prior work in this area has focused broadly on sleep problems21,40, evaluated nocturnal arousals alone23, or combined bedtime resistance with other sleep problems20,22 so that the association with bedtime resistance in particular cannot be clearly delineated. Further, less is known in general about the association between parents’ use of sleep hygiene practices and child temperament among preschool-age children, even though this information may help guide clinical practice. Thus, our findings are novel and suggest low-income US preschool children with more difficult temperaments are more likely to have more bedtime resistance and also to have worse sleep hygiene practices.

Similarly, to our knowledge, our finding that the relationship between sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance differs based on child temperament has not been previously described. We found that among children with more difficult temperaments, better sleep hygiene practices were linearly associated with lower bedtime resistance. In contrast, among children with less difficult temperaments, only the best sleep hygiene practices (a score of 4.1 or more on the CSHS, reflecting a mean response approximating “quite often”) were associated with lower bedtime resistance. However, children with less difficult temperaments as compared to peers with more difficult temperaments still had lower bedtime resistance, regardless of sleep hygiene practices. An additional explanation for our findings is that it may be more challenging for parents to implement good sleep hygiene practices with children who have more difficult temperaments, particularly when these children are likely to have more bedtime resistance behaviors.

This study does have several limitations. First, the same parent completed all measures, which may have resulted in reporter bias. Future work might consider using other, more objective approaches, such as ecological momentary assessments, for data collection. Second, our sample was not pre-selected on any clinical characteristics, but all participants were low-income, preschool-aged, English-speaking children who were attending Head Start. Thus, study results may not be generalizable to children who do not share these demographic characteristics. Additionally, our measures of sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance may not fully capture all variations in sleep hygiene practices or bedtime resistance behaviors. Therefore, it is possible that our findings describe associations between these two measures and may not reflect associations between other sleep hygiene practices and bedtime resistance behaviors not measured in this study. It is also possible that our measures of bedtime resistance and sleep hygiene practices assessed similar child behaviors to some degree. However, bedtime resistance reflects more child-driven attempts to delay bedtime, whereas sleep hygiene practices reflect more parent-driven routines and strategies, as well as general home environment conditions to promote sleep. Thus, examining the associations between these measures is valuable. It is also possible that our findings of reduced bedtime resistance only at high levels of sleep hygiene for children with less-difficult temperaments were related to there being few children in the lower (CSHS < 4.1) group. Examining this association in a larger sample could help clarify this finding. Finally, it is important to note that this cross-sectional study cannot demonstrate causality; longitudinal and experimental work could address this issue by testing whether better sleep hygiene practices with children identified as having more difficult temperaments earlier in development could prevent bedtime resistance for these children at preschool age.

Nevertheless, our findings do have several clinical implications. First, pediatric providers now have added evidence to recommend use of good sleep hygiene practices to reduce bedtime resistance in preschool-aged children. Second, children with more difficult temperaments are prone to have bedtime resistance, but also may be likely to respond to use of good sleep hygiene practices. Parents of children with more difficult temperaments may find that intensive efforts to improve sleep hygiene practices only achieve levels of bedtime resistance that children with less difficult temperaments experience with worse sleep hygiene practices. We hope our finding that better sleep hygiene practices are associated with less bedtime resistance among children with more difficult temperaments will be encouraging to parents and providers. While more research is needed to address the needs of these children, children with more difficult temperaments for whom bedtime resistance is still problematic, despite relatively good sleep hygiene practices, may benefit from more targeted behavioral treatment if bedtime resistance continues to be problematic despite optimization of sleep hygiene practices.

Acknowledgments

Funding Source: All phases of this study were funded by an NIH grant (1RC1 DK086376-01) and salary support for Katherine Wilson was funded by an NIH grant (5T32 NS007222-30)

COI: Ronald D. Chervin has received research grants from the NIH, Fox Foundation, and University of Michigan. He has received support for an educational program from Philips Respironics and Fisher Paykel. He serves on boards of directors for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, American Sleep Medicine Foundation, American Board of Sleep Medicine, Associated Professional Sleep Societies, and International Pediatric Sleep Association. He has consulted for Proctor and Gamble, Zansors, and MC3. He serves as a section editor for UpToDate and a book editor for Cambridge University Press. He serves as a volunteer on the advisory board of the non-profit Sweet Dreamzzz. He is named in patents, patents pending, and copyrighted material related to sleep disorder diagnosis and assessment, and owned by the University of Michigan.

References

  • 1.Hiscock H, Canterford L, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M. Adverse associations of sleep problems in Australian preschoolers: national population study. Pediatrics. 2007 Jan;119(1):86–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Yokomaku A, Misao K, Omoto F, et al. A study of the association between sleep habits and problematic behaviors in preschool children. Chronobiol Int. 2008 Jul;25(4):549–564. doi: 10.1080/07420520802261705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lavigne JV, Arend R, Rosenbaum D, et al. Sleep and behavior problems among preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1999 Jun;20(3):164–169. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199906000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 2002 Mar-Apr;73(2):405–417. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Shang CY, Gau SS, Soong WT. Association between childhood sleep problems and perinatal factors, parental mental distress and behavioral problems. J Sleep Res. 2006 Mar;15(1):63–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bruni O, Ferini-Strambi L, Russo PM, et al. Sleep disturbances and teacher ratings of school achievement and temperament in children. Sleep Med. 2006 Jan;7(1):43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2005.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Stein MA, Mendelsohn J, Obermeyer WH, Amromin J, Benca R. Sleep and behavior problems in school-aged children. Pediatrics. 2001 Apr;107(4):E60. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.4.e60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kataria S, Swanson MS, Trevathan GE. Persistence of sleep disturbances in preschool children. J Pediatr. 1987 Apr;110(4):642–646. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(87)80571-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Blader JC, Koplewicz HS, Abikoff H, Foley C. Sleep problems of elementary school children. A community survey. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1997 May;151(5):473–480. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1997.02170420043007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M, Nobile C. Sleep habits and sleep disturbance in elementary school-aged children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2000 Feb;21(1):27–36. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200002000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Beltramini AU, Hertzig ME. Sleep and bedtime behavior in preschool-aged children. Pediatrics. 1983 Feb;71(2):153–158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mindell J. Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: findings from the 2004 National Sleep Foundation. Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Medicine. 2009;10:771–779. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mindell JAOJ. A clinical guide to pediatric sleep: diagnosis and management of sleep problems. Second ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hale L, Berger LM, LeBourgeois MK, Brooks-Gunn J. A longitudinal study of preschoolers' language-based bedtime routines, sleep duration, and well-being. J Fam Psychol. 2011 Jun;25(3):423–433. doi: 10.1037/a0023564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Owens J, Maxim R, McGuinn M, Nobile C, Msall M, Alario A. Television-viewing Habits and Sleep Disturbance in School Children. Pediatrics. 1999 Sep 1;104(3):e27. doi: 10.1542/peds.104.3.e27. 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mindell JA, Telofski LS, Wiegand B, Kurtz ES. A nightly bedtime routine: impact on sleep in young children and maternal mood. Sleep. 2009 May;32(5):599–606. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.5.599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Owens-Stively J, Frank N, Smith A, Hagino O, Spirito A, Arrigan M, Alario A. Child temperament, parenting discipine style, and daytime behavior in childhood sleep disorders. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1997;18(5):314–321. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199710000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sadeh A, Lavie P, Scher A. Sleep and Temperament: Maternal Perceptions of Temperament of Sleep-Disturbed Toddlers. Early Education and Development. 1994 Oct 01;5(4):311–322. 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rothbart M, Bates JE. In: Temperament. 5 ed. Eisenberg N, editor. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 1998. ed. Handbook of child psychology. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Owens-Stively J, Frank N, Smith A, et al. Child temperament, parenting discipline style, and daytime behavior in childhood sleep disorders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1997 Oct;18(5):314–321. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199710000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Goodnight JA, Bates JE, Staples AD, Pettit GS, Dodge KA. Temperamental resistance to control increases the association between sleep problems and externalizing behavior development. J Fam Psychol. 2007 Mar;21(1):39–48. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Reid GJ, Hong RY, Wade TJ. The relation between common sleep problems and emotional and behavioral problems among 2- and 3-year-olds in the context of known risk factors for psychopathology. J Sleep Res. 2009 Mar;18(1):49–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00692.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Atkinson E, Vetere A, Grayson K. Sleep disruption in young children. The influence of temperament on the sleep patterns of pre-school children. Child Care Health Dev. 1995 Jul;21(4):233–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.1995.tb00754.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hayes MJPK, Sallienen B, Davare AA. Bedsharing, temperament, and sleep disturbance in early childhood. Sleep. 2001;24:657–662. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.6.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McLaughlin Crabtree V, Beal Korhonen J, Montgomery-Downs HE, Faye Jones V, O'Brien LM, Gozal D. Cultural influences on the bedtime behaviors of young children. Sleep Med. 2005 Jul;6(4):319–324. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2005.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hale L, Berger LM, LeBourgeois MK, Brooks-Gunn J. Social and demographic predictors of preschoolers' bedtime routines. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009 Oct;30(5):394–402. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ba0e64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.LeBourgeois MHM, Harsh J . Validation of the Children's Sleep-Wake Scale (CSWS) Sleep. 2001;24:A219. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.LeBourgeois MK, Avis K, Mixon M, Olmi J, Harsh J. Snoring, sleep quality, and sleepiness across attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes. Sleep. 2004;27(3):520–525. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.LeBourgeois MK, Wright KP, LeBourgeois HB, Jenni O. Dissonance between parent-selected bedtimes and young children's circadian physiology influences nighttime settling difficulties. Mind, Brain, and Education. 2013;7(4):234–242. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Harsh J, Easley A, LeBourgeois MK. A measure of sleep hygiene. Sleep. 2002;25:A316. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Meltzer LJ, Biggs S, Reynolds A, Avis KT, Crabtree VM, Bevans KB. The Children's Report of Sleep Patterns--Sleepiness Scale: a self-report measure for school-aged children. Sleep Med. 2012 Apr;13(4):385–389. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Witcher LA, Gozal D, Molfese DM, Salathe SM, Spruyt K, Crabtree VM. Sleep hygiene and problem behaviors in snoring and non-snoring school-age children. Sleep Med. 2012 Aug;13(7):802–809. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of Temperament at Three to Seven Years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development. 2001;72(5):1394–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2006 Jul 01;87(1):102–112. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8701_09. 2006/ [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rudasill KM, Reio TG, Jr, Stipanovic N, Taylor JE. A longitudinal study of student-teacher relationship quality, difficult temperament, and risky behavior from childhood to early adolescence. J Sch Psychol. 2010 Oct;48(5):389–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Goodnight JA, Bates JE, Staples AD, Pettit GS, Dodge KA. Temperamental resistance to control increases the association between sleep problems and externalizing behavior development. Journal of Family Psychology. 2007;21(1):39–48. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tschann JM, Kaiser P, Chesney MA, Alkon A, Boyce WT. Resilience and vulnerability among preschool children: family functioning, temperament, and behavior problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996 Feb;35(2):184–192. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199602000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001 Sep-Oct;72(5):1394–1408. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bruni O, Lo Reto F, Miano S, Ottaviano S. Daytime behavioral correlates of awakenings and bedtime resistance in preschool children. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;53:358–361. doi: 10.1016/s1567-424x(09)70181-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Paavonen EJ, Almqvist F, Tamminen T, et al. Poor sleep and psychiatric symptoms at school: an epidemiological study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002 Feb;11(1):10–17. doi: 10.1007/s007870200002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES