Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2015 Mar;1337(1):26–31. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12629

Neuronal oscillations as a mechanistic substrate of auditory temporal prediction

Benjamin Morillon 1,2, Charles E Schroeder 1,3
PMCID: PMC4363099  NIHMSID: NIHMS642473  PMID: 25773613

Abstract

Neuronal oscillations are comprised of rhythmic fluctuations of excitability that are synchronized in ensembles of neurons and thus function as temporal filters that dynamically organize sensory processing. When perception relies on anticipatory mechanisms, ongoing oscillations also provide a neurophysiological substrate for temporal prediction. In this article we review evidence for this account with a focus on auditory perception. We argue that such “oscillatory temporal predictions” can selectively amplify neuronal sensitivity to inputs that occur in a predicted, task-relevant rhythm and optimize temporal selection. We elaborate this argument for a prototypic example, speech processing, where information is present at multiple time scales, with delta, theta, and low-gamma oscillations being specifically and simultaneously engaged, enabling multiplexing. We then consider the origin of temporal predictions, specifically the idea that the motor system is involved in the generation of such prior information. Finally, we place temporal predictions in the general context of internal models, discussing how they interact with feature-based or spatial predictions. We propose that complementary predictions interact synergistically according to a dominance hierarchy, shaping perception in the form of a multidimensional filter mechanism.

Keywords: perception, sensorimotor, expectation, neurophysiology, active sensing

Neuronal oscillations underpin temporal predictions

Seminal theories in neuroscience consider sensory processing as an inference problem: given the noisy sensory and neural signals, the brain estimates which stimuli caused these observations.1 Accordingly, neuronal representations start to be viewed as continuous top-down prediction signals, that is, internal models of causal dynamics in the world shaped by one’s perceptual, behavioral, and emotional experiences.2 Such theories have two far-reaching corollaries: (1) predictions are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic activity;3 and (2) sensory-evoked neuronal activity corresponds to the perturbation of ongoing circuit dynamics by input signals, initiating the generation of an updated representation of the world.47

However, it remains unclear how predictions are represented in the brain. One problem resides in the fact that an event can be anticipated with regard to its features (content), location (space), and/or moment (time) of occurrence.810 Focusing on this latter dimension, dynamic attending theory suggests that during perception attention is modulated dynamically to optimize sensory processing at expected points in time.1113 Critically, this framework capitalizes on the fact that many stimuli and actions are rhythmically organized (e.g., speech, music, walking). By extracting such temporal regularities, the brain is able to predict the occurrence of subsequent events of interest and to optimize their processing.

Behavioral studies have shown that temporal expectations indeed optimize perception by dynamically modulating the gain of sensory information.1416 The signal-to-noise ratio of the predicted sensory signal is increased, which leads to improved detection performance. This effect is associated with a reduction of the large-scale sensory evoked neuronal response, as observed in both visual17,18 and auditory modalities.1921 At the neurophysiological level, low-frequency neuronal oscillations are the hypothesized substrate of temporal predictions.5 Neuroelectric oscillations correspond to temporal fluctuations of the excitability state of neuronal ensembles22 and make it possible to selectively amplify neuronal input responses occurring at predicted moments.23,24 This mechanism results in a selective neuronal tracking of attended sensory streams, as exemplified by the cocktail party effect, where listeners have to “tune in” to one conversation in a noisy scene.25

Speech processing at multiple time scales

Speech, together with music, is arguably the most interesting stimulus to study the role of neuronal oscillations in perception. Speech perception arises from the dynamic sampling of acoustic information at multiple time scales simultaneously, referred to as multiplexing. Indeed, although syllabic modulations (1–7 Hz) are critical to speech comprehension,26 acoustic modulations above 12 Hz also contribute to speech identification, as they are associated with some phonetic feature dimensions.27,28 In recent years neuronal entrainment of cortical oscillatory dynamics to the stimulus acoustic envelope has been described, suggesting a prominent role of auditory delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), and low-gamma (25–45 Hz) frequencies in speech parsing.29 In addition to syllabic-based sampling at theta rate, fine-grained sampling (~ 30 Hz) preserves the detailed temporal structure of sounds necessary for phonemic contrasts extraction and articulatory routines execution;30 slower sampling (<4 Hz) is suggested to underline more steady cues such as formants at the basis of vowel recognition but also vocal analysis (e.g., speaker identification, emotional prosody).

Most studies of speech perception emphasize that auditory responses phase-lock to speech syllabic modulations in the theta range.31,32 However, in left auditory regions neuronal entrainment is also observed at low-gamma rate,33,34 which indicates that auditory information is sampled at multiple time scales simultaneously.35 Such neuronal tracking is dynamic, resulting from the close interplay between stimulus-driven responses and the top-down influence of internal predictions. This latter assumption is exemplified by two complementary sets of evidence. First, neuronal entrainment strengthens with learning and comprehension.34,3638 Second, in auditory cortex the modulation spectrum of spontaneous oscillatory activity predicts the interhemispheric differences observed during speech processing, where low-gamma activity dominates in the left hemisphere.33,39,40 This strongly highlights the role of internal models as crucial in shaping the sensory neuronal response.41

Finally, in addition to theta and low-gamma oscillations, speech processing is also associated with neuronal entrainment in the delta range. Delta activity is likely to be involved in multiple facets of auditory processing. On one hand a right-lateralized neuronal entrainment is observed at delta rate during speech processing, co-occurring with theta entrainment but carrying complementary information.34,42 This result is compatible with a role of delta activity in paralinguistic processing, capturing prosodic information occurring at a phrasal rate.43 On the other hand, some reports indicate a left-lateralized involvement of delta oscillations during auditory processing, hypothesized to reflect motor-driven top-down modulations.40,44

Motor origin of temporal predictions

The motor system sits at the top of the cortical hierarchy involved in the perception–action cycle.45 Capitalizing on predictive coding or Bayesian inference frameworks, recent theoretical attempts suggest that the motor system may be conceptualized as a purely predictive system, generating top-down predictions that shape perception.5,4648 In particular, these models focus on the temporal dimension, emphasizing the temporal essence of the motor system. More than a century ago, rhythm and movement were hypothesized to be intrinsically linked.49 Since then the motor system has been shown to be a key structure in the precise estimation of short durations,50,51 and neuroimaging studies on beat and rhythm processing invariably report the involvement of the motor system.46,5254 Thus, the motor system might be involved in the generation of putative beats and engaged in the analysis of temporal sequences, neuronal motor activity corresponding to a temporal prediction signal that sharpens sensory representations.

Interestingly, neuroimaging studies of temporal attention have shown that orienting attention in time is specifically associated with activation of left parietal and premotor cortex.55 Moreover, it was recently observed that delta oscillations in primary motor cortex covary with the occurrence of temporally predictable task-relevant visual cues.56 A similar result was also observed during an auditory task, the accurate estimation of short duration depending on delta oscillatory fluctuations.44 This pattern of findings suggests that the (left lateralized) motor system may drive temporal expectations through slow oscillatory dynamics.

Active sensing in the auditory domain

The involvement of sensorimotor loops in perception is often referred to as active sensing.47,57 This framework proposes that perception is shaped by the motor system in two distinct ways. First, the motor system directs sensing organs (e.g., finger squeezes, ocular saccades, olfactory sniffs) toward relevant stimuli, thereby structuring the content of bottom-up sensory information inflow. Second, it modulates the processing of sensory information via top-down internal corollary discharge signals, that is, copies of movement commands sent to associated sensory structures.58 This latter mechanism corresponds to an active sampling of sensory information by marking the timing (and location) of relevant sensory events according to current motor plans. This predictively modulates sensory processing according to the temporal (and spatial) patterns of motor activity patterns, thus providing “when” (and “where”) predictions at a minimum.59

Active sensing was never described in the auditory domain because of its relative bottom-up disconnection from the motor system. Indeed, whereas bottom-up and top-down motor influences are contingent in most sensory modalities, audition is the exception because we cannot selectively move our ears. However, sensitivity to temporal information is at its best in the auditory modality, and music exemplifies the strong relationship existing between rhythm and auditory and motor systems.53,60 This suggests that (1) the motor system could exert a specific top-down predictive influence on auditory processing, and (2) top-down motor influences, often conflated with bottom-up ones (the directing of sensing organs), could play a distinct and fundamental role.

Motor contributions to the temporal precision of auditory attention

We recently developed a mechanistic behavioral account of auditory active sensing.61 This work was based on four theoretically driven parameters. First, we focused on the auditory modality because of its relative bottom-up disconnection from the motor system. Second, we measured the ability of human participants to extract relevant auditory information interleaved with distracting information, in a cocktail party–like fashion, and embedded in rhythmic streams. Thus, performance critically depended on the implementation of temporal expectation. Third, we used a perceptual decision-making task to study the dynamics of evidence accumulation and its modulation by temporal expectation. This led us to the following experiment. Participants were asked to categorize sequences of eight pure tones as higher or lower pitched, on average, than a reference frequency. In order to drive rhythmic fluctuations in attention, the tones (targets) were delivered in phase with a reference beat, presented at the beginning of each trial, and in antiphase with irrelevant yet physically indistinguishable tones (distractors). Fourth, participants were instructed either to use their motor system overtly, by pressing in rhythm with the reference beat, or to keep the rhythm covertly, in both cases as a way to optimize the allocation of temporal attention (predictions) in a purely top-down fashion.

Our findings show that producing a rhythmic movement engages a top-down modulation that sharpens the temporal selection of auditory information. It improves the segregation between relevant and distracting information, facilitating perception of relevant items and suppressing perception of irrelevant ones. We additionally observed that the impact of overt motor tracking on perception parametrically scales with the temporal predictability of the auditory sequence. Finally, motor contributions depend on the temporal alignment between motor and attention fluctuations. This indicates that this rhythmic top-down process requires an exquisite temporal alignment among motor, attention, and auditory systems during the sampling of sensory information. Altogether, our results suggest that motor-driven temporal predictions optimize perception, and a most likely substrate for this effect is a slow oscillation timed by rhythmic motor activity.

Previous behavioral experiments on the top-down role of the motor system in auditory perception showed that self-initiated head movements can reset the perceptual organization of an auditory scene62 or influence the way an auditory rhythmic pattern is perceived.63 Here we went several important steps further by firmly isolating top-down motor influences on auditory stimulus processing and by showing that the engagement of the motor system improves (1) the precision of temporal attention and, thus, (2) the quality of sensory selection.61 Our work confirms the operation of active sensing in the auditory domain, which emphasizes the prominent role of motor activity in sensory processing through its close interaction with attention.

Hierarchical organization of predictive filters in auditory perception

A first necessary step toward a Cartesian approach64 is to study temporal expectations independently of other dimensions (such as location or feature). However, the drawback in most paradigms is that these attributes of noninterest are kept constant across trials and so are highly predictable. These strong (e.g., spatial or feature-based) expectations might in turn impact perception and bias results or limit interpretations.65 Indeed, in primary auditory cortex the combination of temporal and spectral expectations results in both the amplification and sharpening of neuronal responses, in essence acting as a spectrotemporal filter mechanism.23 Such a combination is not restricted to what and when dimensions in audition but can also integrate spatial66 and intensity67 information at higher levels. These data suggest that predictions constantly fuse into a dynamically evolving model of upcoming events, which enhances the representation of predicted stimuli.

However, predictions might not combine linearly. For example, although temporal expectations alone influence behavior during a visual task,68 their impact is increased by concurrent spatial expectations and is erased if spatial expectations are invalid.68,69 This indicates that the impact of temporal expectations on perception is optimized by the co-occurrence of spatial expectations. Because the visual cortex is retinotopically (i.e., spatially) organized, temporal predictions might not be able to efficiently modulate the first cortical stages of visual processing unless they are part of a spatiotemporal predictive filter that taps onto retinotopically specific regions. This suggests that the influence expectations exert on perception might rely on a hierarchy of predictive filters that trails the structure of the concerned sensory system. Spatial predictions would be necessary in the retinotopic visual system, whereas spectral predictions would be key in the tonotopic auditory system.

Overall, current research suggests that perception is a rhythmic process70 and features neuronal oscillations and the motor system as key operators in sensory processing.47 Recent evidence in developmental neurosciences accordingly pinpoints temporal sampling deficits as a potential cause of developmental dyslexia.71,72 It moreover suggests that music, which is exquisitely rhythmic and linked to motor activity,53 might be used as a rehabilitation strategy: by boosting temporal prediction mechanisms, musical rhythmic training could ultimately benefit language processing and language acquisition.73

Acknowledgments

Supported by the Fyssen foundation (B.M.) and by NIH (DC011490) (C.E.S.). We thank our reviewers for their helpful comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Helmholtz H von. Handbook of Physiological Optics. Leipzig: Leopold Voss; 1856. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Friston K. A theory of cortical responses. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2005;360:815–836. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Sadaghiani S, Kleinschmidt A. Functional interactions between intrinsic brain activity and behavior. NeuroImage. 2013;80:379–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ringach DL. Spontaneous and driven cortical activity: implications for computation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2009;19:439–444. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arnal L, Giraud AL. Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:390–398. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fiser J, Berkes P, Orbán G, Lengyel M. Statistically optimal perception and learning: from behavior to neural representations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Winkler I, Denham SL, Nelken I. Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regularity representations and perceptual objects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009;13:532–540. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nobre A, Rohenkohl G, Stokes M, Nobre M. Nervous anticipation: top-down biasing across space and time. In: Posner MI, editor. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford; 2012. pp. 159–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Olson IR, Chun MM. Temporal contextual cuing of visual attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2001;27:1299–1313. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.27.5.1299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kingstone A. Combining expectancies. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A. 1992;44:69–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Large EW, Jones MR. The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol. Rev. 1999;106:119–159. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jones MR. Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychol. Rev. 1976;83:323–355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jones MR, Boltz M. Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol. Rev. 1989;96:459–491. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.96.3.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Jones MR, Moynihan H, MacKenzie N, Puente J. Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychol. Sci. 2002;13:313–319. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rohenkohl G, Cravo AM, Wyart V, Nobre AC. Temporal expectation improves the quality of sensory information. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:8424–8428. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0804-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jaramillo S, Zador AM. The auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:246–251. doi: 10.1038/nn.2688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Praamstra P, Kourtis D, Kwok HF, Oostenveld R. Neurophysiology of implicit timing in serial choice reaction-time performance. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:5448–5455. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0440-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Alink A, Schwiedrzik CM, Kohler A, et al. Stimulus predictability reduces responses in primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:2960–2966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3730-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lange K. Brain correlates of early auditory processing are attenuated by expectations for time and pitch. Brain Cogn. 2009;69:127–137. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Costa-Faidella J, Baldeweg T, Grimm S, Escera C. Interactions between "what" and "when" in the auditory system: temporal predictability enhances repetition suppression. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:18590–18597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2599-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yaron A, Hershenhoren I, Nelken I. Sensitivity to complex statistical regularities in rat auditory cortex. Neuron. 2012;76:603–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schroeder CE, Lakatos P. Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lakatos P, et al. The spectrotemporal filter mechanism of auditory selective attention. Neuron. 2013;77:750–761. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cravo AM, Rohenkohl G, Wyart V, Nobre AC. Temporal expectation enhances contrast sensitivity by phase entrainment of low-frequency oscillations in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:4002–4010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4675-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Zion Golumbic EM, et al. Mechanisms underlying selective neuronal tracking of attended speech at a "cocktail party". Neuron. 2013;77:980–991. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elliott TM, Theunissen FE. The modulation transfer function for speech intelligibility. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2009;5:e1000302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Christiansen TU, Greenberg S. Frequency selective filtering of the modulation spectrum and its impact on consonant identification. Linguistic Theory Raw Sound. 2009:119–133. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Drullman R, Festen JM, Plomp R. Effect of reducing slow temporal modulations on speech reception. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1994;95:2670–2680. doi: 10.1121/1.409836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Giraud AL, Poeppel D. Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:511–517. doi: 10.1038/nn.3063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bouchard KE, Mesgarani N, Johnson K, Chang EF. Functional organization of human sensorimotor cortex for speech articulation. Nature. 2013 doi: 10.1038/nature11911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Abrams DA, Nicol T, Zecker S, Kraus N. Right-hemisphere auditory cortex is dominant for coding syllable patterns in speech. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:3958–3965. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0187-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Luo H, Poeppel D. Phase patterns of neuronal responses reliably discriminate speech in human auditory cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:1001–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Morillon B, Liégeois-Chauvel C, Arnal L, et al. Asymmetric function of theta and gamma activity in syllable processing: an intra-cortical study. Front. Psychol. 2012;3:248. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gross J, et al. Speech rhythms and multiplexed oscillatory sensory coding in the human brain. PLoS Biol. 2013;11:e1001752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Xiang J, Poeppel D, Simon JZ. Physiological evidence for auditory modulation filterbanks: cortical responses to concurrent modulations. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013;133:EL7–E12. doi: 10.1121/1.4769400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ahissar E, et al. Speech comprehension is correlated with temporal response patterns recorded from auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2001;98:13367–13372. doi: 10.1073/pnas.201400998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Peelle JE, Gross J, Davis MH. Phase-locked responses to speech in human auditory cortex are enhanced during comprehension. Cereb. Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Luo H, Tian X, Song K, et al. Neural response phase tracks how listeners learn new acoustic representations. Curr. Biol. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Giraud AL, et al. Endogenous cortical rhythms determine cerebral specialization for speech perception and production. Neuron. 2007;56:1127–1134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Morillon B, et al. Neurophysiological origin of human brain asymmetry for speech and language. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2010;107:18688–18693. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007189107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Arieli A, Sterkin A, Grinvald A, Aertsen A. Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science. 1996;273:1868–1871. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cogan GB, Poeppel D. A mutual information analysis of neural coding of speech by low-frequency MEG phase information. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:554–563. doi: 10.1152/jn.00075.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schroeder CE, Lakatos P, Kajikawa Y, et al. Neuronal oscillations and visual amplification of speech. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008;12:106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Arnal LH, Doelling KB, Poeppel D. Delta-beta coupled oscillations underlie temporal prediction accuracy. Cereb. Cortex. 2014 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fuster JM. Prefrontal cortex and the bridging of temporal gaps in the perception-action cycle. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1990;608:318–329. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1990.tb48901.x. discussion 330–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Grahn JA, Rowe JB. Finding and feeling the musical beat: striatal dissociations between detection and prediction of regularity. Cereb. Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Schroeder CE, Wilson DA, Radman T, et al. Dynamics of active sensing and perceptual selection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2010;20:172–176. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schubotz RI. Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007;11:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bolton TL. Rhythm. Am. J. Psychol. 1894 [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Morillon B, Kell CA, Giraud AL. Three stages and four neural systems in time estimation. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:14803–14811. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3222-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Coull JT, Vidal F, Nazarian B, Macar F. Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science. 2004;303:1506–1508. doi: 10.1126/science.1091573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Grahn JA, Rowe JB. Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:7540–7548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Zatorre RJ, Chen JL, Penhune VB. When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Teki S, Grube M, Kumar S, Griffiths TD. Distinct neural substrates of duration-based and beat-based auditory timing. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:3805–3812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5561-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Coull JT, Nobre AC. Where and when to pay attention: the neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:7426–7435. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-18-07426.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Saleh M, Reimer J, Penn R, et al. Fast and slow oscillations in human primary motor cortex predict oncoming behaviorally relevant cues. Neuron. 2010;65:461–471. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Wachowiak M. All in a sniff: olfaction as a model for active sensing. Neuron. 2011;71:962–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Crapse TB, Sommer MA. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:587–600. doi: 10.1038/nrn2457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sommer MA, Wurtz RH. Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:317–338. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Repp BH, Penel A. Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychol. Res. 2004;68:252–270. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0143-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Morillon B, Schroeder CE, Wyart V. Motor contributions to the temporal precision of auditory attention. Nat. Commun. 2014;5:5255. doi: 10.1038/ncomms6255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kondo HM, Pressnitzer D, Toshima I, Kashino M. Effects of self-motion on auditory scene analysis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2012;109:6775–6780. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112852109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Phillips-Silver J, Trainor LJ. Feeling the beat: movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science. 2005;308:1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1110922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Descartes R. Discours de la méthode (Pour bien conduire sa raison et chercher la vérité dans les sciences) 1637 [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Morillon B, Barbot A. Attention in the temporal domain: a phase-coding mechanism that controls the gain of sensory processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:e480. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Rimmele J, Jolsvai H, Sussman E. Auditory target detection is affected by implicit temporal and spatial expectations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23:1136–1147. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Polley DB, Steinberg EE, Merzenich MM. Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influences. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:4970–4982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3771-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Doherty JR, Rao A, Mesulam MM, Nobre AC. Synergistic effect of combined temporal and spatial expectations on visual attention. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:8259–8266. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1821-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Rohenkohl G, Gould IC, Pessoa J, Nobre AC. Combining spatial and temporal expectations to improve visual perception. J. Vis. 2014;14 doi: 10.1167/14.4.8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.VanRullen R, Zoefel B, Ilhan B. On the cyclic nature of perception in vision versus audition. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2014;369 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0214. 20130214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Goswami U. A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lehongre K, Ramus F, Villiermet N, et al. Altered low-γ sampling in auditory cortex accounts for the three main facets of dyslexia. Neuron. 2011;72:1080–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schön D, Tillmann B. Short- and long-term rhythmic interventions: perspectives for language rehabilitation. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12635. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES