Highlights
-
•
Cardiac vagal control (CVC) was measured during an extended standardized baseline.
-
•
Subjective and polysomnographic variables of sleep quality were assessed.
-
•
Higher CVC was found to be associated with better subjective and objective sleep quality.
Keywords: Parasympathetic nervous system, Heart rate variability, Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, Sleep quality, Polysomnography
Abstract
Cardiac vagal control (CVC) has been linked to both physical and mental health. One critical aspect of health, that has not received much attention, is sleep. We hypothesized that adults with higher CVC – operationalized by high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV) – will exhibit better sleep quality assessed both subjectively (i.e., with Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index) and objectively (i.e., with polysomnography). HF-HRV was measured in 29 healthy young women during an extended neutral film clip. Participants then underwent full polysomnography to obtain objective measures of sleep quality and HF-HRV during a night of sleep. As expected, higher resting HF-HRV was associated with higher subjective and objective sleep quality (i.e., shorter sleep latency and fewer arousals). HF-HRV during sleep (overall or separated by sleep phases) showed less consistent relationships with sleep quality. These findings indicate that high waking CVC may be a key predictor of healthy sleep.
1. Introduction
The parasympathetic nervous system is the branch of the autonomic nervous system responsible for key restorative processes like “resting and digesting” (e.g., Sherwood, 2010). Effective functioning of this system has been linked with stronger phasic activity of vagus nerve efferent activity to the sino-atrial node of the heart, often termed cardiac vagal control (CVC). CVC can be measured noninvasively by quantification of respiratory sinus arrhythmia, i.e., the rhythmic oscillation in heart rate linked to breathing frequency (Grossman & Taylor, 2007). Greater CVC during resting baseline assessments at wake (CVCwake) has been associated with both better physical and mental health (Beauchaine, 2001; Porges, 2007; Thayer & Lane, 2007). One particularly critical aspect of physical and mental health is sleep (Buysse, 2014), raising the possibility that CVCwake would be positively linked with sleep quality. Yet, the links between CVCwake and sleep have not received much attention. Therefore, we tested in healthy adults whether individual differences in CVCwake are linked to sleep quality, as assessed with subjective (i.e., self-report questionnaire) and objective measures (i.e., full polysomnography).
1.1. CVC and health
Higher CVCwake has been reliably linked with a range of positive physical health outcomes, including better cardiovascular health (e.g., Giese-Davis et al., 2015; Thayer & Lane, 2007) as well as positive mental health outcomes, including greater subjective well-being (e.g., Beauchaine, 2001; Geisler, Vennewald, Kubiak, & Weber, 2010). While multiple theoretical models have been proposed to account for the positive link between CVCwake and health, most agree that CVC is a marker of processes involved in the regulation of arousal. In this view, the parasympathetic system exerts an inhibitory influence via the vagus nerve, which promotes calm states by actively reducing autonomic arousal (e.g., Berntson et al., 1997; Grossman & Taylor, 2007; Porges, 2007; Thayer & Lane, 2009). One state characterized by lower arousal is sleep, as sleep can only occur when arousal is substantially reduced (Dahl, 1996). Given that CVCwake is thought to be crucially involved in regulating arousal, further supported by links between reduced arousal and increases in CVCwake (e.g., during mediation; Delgado et al., 2010), and given that sleep relies on low arousal, one might expect that sleep would also show a relationship with CVCwake.
1.2. CVC and sleep
A variety of research has linked CVC and sleep. It has been shown that CVC increases in anticipation of sleep onset (e.g., Burgess, Trinder, Kim, & Luke, 1997) and remains enhanced during sleep (Stein & Pu, 2012; Trinder et al., 2001). Emerging evidence also supports a link between higher CVCwake and better subjective sleep quality in specific populations like children or clinical samples (e.g., El-Sheikh, Erath, & Bagley, 2013; Fang, Huang, Yang, & Tsai, 2008; Hovland et al., 2013; Yang et al., 2011) as well as in healthy adults in the context of daily stressors (Jackowska, Dockray, Endrighi, Hendrickx, & Steptoe, 2012; Kageyama et al., 1998). Only one study measured objective sleep quality and found that higher levels of CVCwake were associated with deeper sleep (i.e., delta power) in participants with alcohol-dependency (e.g., Irwin, Valladares, Motivala, Thayer, & Ehlers, 2006).
Research assessing links between CVCwake and sleep, however, may not speak to the links between CVC during sleep (CVCsleep) and sleep. Studies linking CVCsleep (sometimes but not often separated for each sleep stage) and sleep quality showed, for example, reduced levels of CVCsleep in patients with different sleep disorders, such as primary insomnia or in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (e.g., Burton, Rahman, Kadota, Lloyd, & Vollmer-Conna, 2010; Stein & Pu, 2012; Tobaldini et al., 2013). A few studies also found a link between higher levels of CVCsleep and better subjective sleep quality (e.g., Yang et al., 2011) in patient groups as well as in healthy individuals (Brosschot, Van Dijk, & Thayer, 2007; Patel et al., 2013).
While higher CVCwake and higher CVCsleep have both been linked with better sleep quality, additional research suggests that these links might be due to distinct processes. Some studies did not show a significant relationship between CVCwake and CVCsleep or found that CVCwake – but not CVCsleep – was associated with better sleep quality (Irwin et al., 2006; Jackowska et al., 2012). This might partly be due to variation in CVCsleep across different sleep stages (Stein & Pu, 2012; Tobaldini et al., 2013) and across different periods of the same sleep stage across the night (Snyder, Hobson, Morrison, & Goldfrank, 1964), leading to heterogeneity of CVCsleep during one night of sleep and relatively low reliability across nights. On the other hand, CVCwake has shown good reliability as a stable trait marker (Bertsch, Hagemann, Naumann, Schachinger, & Schulz, 2012) and has been reliably linked to subjective sleep quality.
In summary, evidence suggests that CVCwake and CVCsleep are associated with different processes and are distinct predictors of sleep quality. During wakefulness, CVC corresponds to how people process and flexibly respond to their external environment, which implies the flexible regulation of physiological arousal (Dahl, 1996; Porges, 2007; Thayer & Sternberg, 2006). During sleep, however, CVC is not likely to correspond to online processing and responding to the environment. Rather, CVCsleep may be related to maintaining a generally low arousal state to protect ongoing sleep. Importantly, existing evidence supports CVCwake as the more reliable predictor of sleep quality in comparison to CVCsleep.
1.3. The present study
The primary aim of the present study was to extend previous research on links between CVC and health by examining the relationship between CVCwake and sleep quality. The present research fills three important gaps. First, prior research has relied predominantly on clinical samples. To advance understanding of the basic relationship between CVCwake and sleep quality, we investigated this question in healthy adults. Second, research to date has assessed sleep quality almost exclusively with subjective measures. To thoroughly and validly measure sleep quality, we obtained both subjective and objective sleep measures. More specifically, subjective sleep quality was indexed by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989) and objective sleep quality by polysomnography (the gold standard for sleep assessments). Third, prior studies assessed CVCwake typically only during relatively short periods (e.g., Fang, Huang, Yang, & Tsai, 2008; Hovland et al., 2013; Kageyama et al., 1998) or waking periods directly before sleep while resting in bed, which can produce confounds with circadian influences (e.g., Irwin et al., 2006; Trinder et al., 2001). In addition, resting baseline assessment without anything to do or focus on may lead to uncontrolled differences in mental activity (Wilson et al., 2014) and reduced reliability. To avoid these limitations, we employed a minimally-demanding, standardized, and extended baseline (Jennings, Kamarck, Stewart, Eddy, & Johnson, 1992) in the form of a full-length, emotionally neutral film to assess trait-level CVCwake. This baseline assessment approach extends the window for CVCwake assessment to improve its trait-like quality, while concurrently minimizing individual differences in mental activity.
We hypothesized that participants with higher (vs. lower) CVCwake, operationalized by heart rate variability in the high-frequency spectral band (0.15–0.40 Hz, HF-HRV, Berntson et al., 1997) would exhibit higher subjective sleep quality (i.e., lower scores on the PSQI) and higher objective sleep quality (assessed by polysomnography). We also assessed HF-HRVsleep across the time spent sleeping and separately for each sleep stage. However, we included these indices primarily for exploratory purposes and, based on the extant literature, we did not expect strong and consistent relationships between HF-HRVsleep and either subjective or objective sleep quality. Finally, we aimed to assess whether associations between HF-HRVwake and sleep quality are specific to sleep quality per se, rather than sleep parameters more broadly construed (e.g., the duration of different sleep stages), which were included in complementary analyses.
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Participants were 29 healthy female undergraduates (University of Salzburg, all Caucasian) between 19 and 31 years of age (M = 23.6 years, SD = 3.3) with a body mass index (BMI) between 17.4 and 31.7 (M = 21.8, SD = 3.5). All participants were non- or only occasional smokers with no history of mental, neurological, or sleep disorders. We only accepted participants who were considered in the normal range of subjective sleep quality, as defined by values in the PSQI up to 7 (higher scores indicate poorer sleep) for the past month. The cut-off value for good subjective sleep quality in the PSQI is 5 (Buysse et al., 1989), but values up to 7 are acceptable because this still can be considered within a normal range of sleep quality in female student populations (Pranada, 2005) and we did not want to artificially reduce variance in this variable. The average reported sleep duration on the 3 days before the first night in the laboratory was 8.2 h (SD = 1.1 h).
2.2. Procedure
The investigation took place as part of a larger study in the Clinical Stress and Emotion Lab of the University of Salzburg. The whole study spanned 11 days. Participants completed daily sleep diaries to assure regular sleep cycles during the whole study. The study included four visits to the lab: the entrance examination, which took place at least 3 days prior to the first night and the 3 nights in the lab each separated by one night, as well as a final 3 days of sleep assessment by sleep diaries. The first night in the lab was used for adaptation and screening purposes (sleep disturbances like sleep apnea, insomnia, periodic leg movements, which did not occur in any of the participants) and was performed two nights prior to the experimental nights. Participants came to the lab around 9 pm and completed several questionnaires including assessment of subjective sleep quality (referring to the prior four weeks) as well as general medical and psychological health condition, e.g., physical fitness (on a 4-point Likert scale, 1 – not very active to 4 – physical activity at least three times a week) and BMI. After electrodes were attached they went to bed and were woken up after 8 h in bed.
The second and third nights in the laboratory (i.e., night 6 and 8 within the whole study period) were conducted as experimental nights with presentation of a full-length, aversive or emotionally neutral film (counterbalanced order across participants). Results concerning pre-to post-sleep changes in emotional reactivity to the aversive film in relation to REM sleep have been reported separately (Werner, Schabus, Blechert, Kolodyazhniy, & Wilhelm, 2015). The focus of the present investigation was only the neutral film and the subsequent night (either first or second experimental night). On that night, participants were seated on a chair placed 40 inches in front of a 24-inch full HD monitor and the electrodes and sensors for measuring sleep and cardiovascular activity were attached. During film viewing (see below), cardiovascular activity was recorded, whereas during subsequent sleep full polysomnography (PSG, also including cardiovascular activity) was recorded. Stimulus presentation of the film was controlled by E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA).
The study was approved by the local ethics committee. Participants signed written informed consent before the study and were compensated with course credit or payment of 100 Euro after the last session of the whole study period.
2.3. Measures
2.3.1. HF-HRV
Waking psychophysiological measurements during the neutral film were recorded using a 32-channel amplifier (TMSi, EJ Oldenzaal, The Netherlands) and the recording software package Polybench 1.22 (TMSi) with a sampling rate of 1024 Hz and 22 bit resolution. The recordings contained electrocardiogram (ECG) for which alcohol pads were used to clean the skin sites. ECG was recorded using disposable 55 mm diameter solid-gel snap electrodes; the electrodes were applied on the upper sternum and lowest rib on the left side. The ground electrode was applied at the middle forehead. A 0.05 Hz high-pass filter was applied during ECG measurement. After recording, ECG raw data were bandpass filtered between 0.5 and 40 Hz and further processed using the software Autonomic Nervous System Laboratory (ANSLAB) version 2.51 (Wilhelm, Grossman, & Roth, 1999); R-waves were determined automatically by ANSLAB and further manually checked. The preprocessed ECG was analyzed with power spectral analyses between .15 Hz and .40 Hz. More specifically, to obtain HF-HRVwake values, heart-period time series were linearly detrended and resampled at 4 Hz using cubic spline interpolation. Then, power-spectral densities for the whole duration of the neutral film (94 min) were computed using the Welch algorithm (Welch, 1967), which creates ensemble averages of successive periodograms. The averages were derived from spectra estimated for 120-s segments, overlapping by half. For each 120-s segment, we analyzed 512 points, which includes 480 sampled points with zero padding. We used Hanning-windowed segments that were subjected to fast Fourier transform. Estimates of power were adjusted to account for attenuation produced by the Hanning window and distribution characteristics were normalized by natural-logarithm transformation. We obtained overall HF-HRVsleep values for the time from falling asleep to awakening on the next day (as indicated by PSG analyses) in a similar way as during wakefulness. Furthermore, we obtained HF-HRVsleep for selected sleep stage epochs (≥2 min to increase reliability of spectral analyses and to exclude stage transition periods) and averaged HF-HRVsleep for each sleep stage separately (N1, N2, N3, REM). Although associations between CVC and HF-HRV might be influenced by variables like respiratory rate (e.g., Grossman & Taylor, 2007; Wilhelm, Grossman, & Coyle, 2004), we did not adjust for respiration, as this is less useful for individual difference analyses because participants might differ in respiratory function due to factors unrelated to CVC, such as basal metabolic rate and respiratory pacemaker function (Grossman & Kollai, 1993; Grossman & Taylor, 2007). However, we did control for other possible confounding factors like age, physical fitness, and BMI.
Like others, we used a neutral film to measure HF-HRVwake (Butler, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2006; Jennings et al., 1992; Kogan, Gruber, Shallcross, Ford, & Mauss, 2013). We chose the neutral documentary Living in a Monastery (Spiegel TV, 94 min1), which describes the daily routine of nuns in a convent without any positive or negative emotional content. The film was validated in a pilot study with 11 female participants (age: M = 21.8, SD = 2.0) who provided valence ratings (1 = positive; 9 = negative) and arousal ratings (1 = not arousing, 9 = very arousing). Participants rated the film neutral to slightly positive (M = 3.4, SD = 1.5) and low arousing (M = 2.5, SD = 1.5).
2.3.2. Subjective sleep quality
We used the PSQI (Buysse et al., 1989; German version by Riemann & Backhaus, 1996) to measure subjective sleep quality. The PSQI is a self-report questionnaire assessing sleep quality and sleep disturbances over the preceding 4-week time interval. Eighteen individual items generate seven “component” scores (each with values between 0 and 3): subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleeping medication, and daytime dysfunction. These scores are summed up to obtain one global measure of subjective sleep quality (0–21). Lower values in the PSQI indicate better subjective sleep quality.
2.3.3. Objective sleep quality
Night recordings contained full PSG. Electroencephalogram (EEG) and other channels during the night were recorded using a separate 32-channel amplifier (TMSi, EJ Oldenzaal, The Netherlands), which was located next to the bed and using the software package Polybench 1.22 (TMSi). The signals were digitized with a 256 Hz sampling rate and 22 bit resolution. During recording, 0.5 Hz low-pass and 70 Hz high-pass filters were applied. Seven Ag/AgCl electrodes (F3, F4, C3, Cz, C4, O1, O2) were attached with Genuine Grass electrode paste (Grass Technologies, Warwick, US) according to the international 10/20 system with common reference and were later re-referenced to the two additional electrodes on the contralateral mastoids (A1, A2) using Brain Vision Analyzer (Brain Products, Inc., Munich, Germany). Furthermore, four electrooculogram (EOG) channels, two electromyogram (EMG; left and right chin), one bipolar ECG, two bipolar respiratory (thorax and abdomen), one oxygen saturation, and one finger pulse plethysmography channel were recorded. In the screening/adaptation night we used two additional bipolar EMG (musculus tibialis on both sides) and one additional respiratory channel (air flow) to be able to rule out different sleep disorders. Sleep was visually checked and scored by The Siesta Group© (Somnolyzer 24 × 7; c.f., Anderer et al., 2004, 2005) according to the standard criteria of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (Iber, Ancoli-Israel, Chesson, & Quan, 2007).
Objective sleep quality was indexed by currently accepted measures: total sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency (EFF, defined as TST divided by the time spent in bed in percent), sleep latency (SL, defined as time from going to bed until falling asleep in minutes, measured as stage transition to N2), the wake time after sleep onset, and the number of awakenings during sleep (Blunden & Galland, 2014; Krystal & Edinger, 2008). We also used electroencephalography (EEG) to assess arousals during sleep, which are defined by sudden frequency shifts toward faster rhythms (e.g., theta, alpha, beta, but not spindle activity) that shortly interrupt sleep continuity for at least 3 s (Iber et al., 2007) and are theorized to indicate worse objective sleep quality (Blunden & Galland, 2014; Krystal, Edinger, Wohlgemuth, & Marsh, 2002; Terzano et al., 2003). Sleep architecture variables like the duration of different sleep stages (e.g., N1–N3, rapid eye movement – REM – in minutes) and variables related to specific sleep stages (e.g., REM latency) were included in complementary analyses.
2.4. Statistical analysis
We used Pearson correlations to quantify the relationship between HF-HRVwake and indices of sleep quality, i.e., subjective sleep quality, TST, EFF, SL, wake time after sleep onset, number of awakenings during TST as well as number of arousals. In exploratory analyses, we correlated HF-HRVwake with secondary sleep variables related to sleep architecture (sleep stage N1-N3, REM sleep, REM latency). We also examined the relationship of HF-HRVsleep (from falling asleep until awakening on the next morning) and for each sleep stage separately with HF-HRVwake and with primary and secondary sleep variables.
The α-level for correlational analyses was set to 0.05. We report effect sizes, Pearson's r for correlational analyses or non-parametric Spearman rho if normal distribution was not met (Shapiro–Wilk < .05). Therefore, we used non-parametric Spearman correlations for the following variables: TST, EFF, SL, wake time after sleep onset, REM latency and BMI. Spearman correlations were also used for ordinally coded variables, i.e., subjective sleep quality (PSQI) and physical fitness. We used partial Spearman correlations when controlling for physical fitness, BMI, and age as simultaneous covariates (Sheskin, 2003). Research often shows inverse links between BMI and physical fitness (e.g., Bovet, Auguste, & Burdette, 2007; Nikolaidis, 2013), which in turn can be linked to sleep quality and HF-HRV. BMI may thus be a reasonable interval-scaled proxy for physical fitness.
3. Results
3.1. HF-HRV during wakefulness & sleep quality
To investigate the link between HF-HRVwake and sleep quality, we correlated HF-HRVwake with the different indices of sleep quality.
Significant correlations were found for subjective sleep quality as well as for several primary objective variables of sleep quality: Participants with higher levels of HF-HRVwake showed shorter sleep latency and fewer arousals, indicating a less disturbed sleep pattern (see Table 1). TST, EFF, wake time after sleep onset and the number of awakenings were not significantly correlated with HF-HRVwake. HF-HRVwake did not correlate significantly with any of the secondary sleep variables, −.13 < rs < .18, ps > .338.
Table 1.
Variables | M | SD | HF-HRVwake | HF-HRVsleep |
---|---|---|---|---|
HF-HRVwake (ms2)a | 8.17 | 1.02 | − | |
HF-HRVsleep (ms2)a | 8.77 | 0.80 | .52** | − |
PSQI (sum score)b | 4.10 | 1.37 | −.39* | −.32+ |
Sleep latency (min)b | 25.71 | 19.06 | −.43* | −.17 |
Total sleep time (min)b | 445.81 | 22.97 | .06 | −.17 |
Sleep efficiency (%)b | 92.86 | 4.70 | .07 | −.18 |
Wake after sleep onset (min)b | 13.50 | 15.81 | .25 | .26 |
Awakenings (N)a | 10.03 | 6.29 | .19 | .24 |
Arousals (N)a | 177.62 | 67.47 | −.53** | −.03 |
Note: N = 29.
HF-HRV = high-frequency heart rate variability; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, with lower values indicating better sleep quality.
Correlation between: HF-HRV wake & PSQI: p = .039; HF-HRV wake & SL: p = .022; HF-HRV wake & Arousals: p = .003; HF-HRV sleep & PSQI: p = .086.
Pearson correlations.
Spearman rho correlations.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .10.
3.2. HF-HRV during sleep & sleep quality
Next, we correlated HF-HRVsleep (on average) with HF-HRVwake as well as with all sleep variables. HF-HRVsleep was significantly related to HF-HRVwake, was marginally (but not significantly) related to subjective sleep quality, and was not correlated with any primary (see Table 1) or secondary objective sleep variable, −.16 < rs < .26, ps > .166. We further correlated HF-HRVsleep for each sleep stage (N1, N2, N3 and REM) with subjective and objective sleep quality. Only higher HF-HRVsleep for N2 and N3 also revealed significant correlations with better subjective sleep quality. However, no significant correlations were found for HF-HRVsleep during sleep stage N1, N2 or N3 with primary variables of objective sleep quality. Higher HF-HRVsleep during REM sleep was related to more awakenings and marginally related to longer wake time after sleep onset (Table 2). Higher HF-HRVsleep during N3 was significantly correlated with one secondary sleep parameter, i.e., shorter duration of sleep stage N1, r = −.40, p = .032.
Table 2.
Variables | M | SD | HF-HRV N1 | HF-HRV N2 | HF-HRV N3 | HF-HRV REM |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HF-HRV N1 (ms2)a | 8.73 | 0.97 | – | |||
HF-HRV N2 (ms2)a | 8.57 | 0.92 | .85** | – | ||
HF-HRV N3 (ms2)a | 8.37 | 1.01 | .79** | .93** | – | |
HF-HRV REM (ms2)a | 8.53 | 0.97 | .81** | .83** | .69** | – |
PSQI (sum score)b | 4.10 | 1.37 | −.23 | −.37* | −.38* | −.26 |
Sleep latency (min)b | 25.71 | 19.06 | −.22 | −.17 | −.10 | −.13 |
Total sleep time (min)b | 445.81 | 22.97 | −.10 | −.11 | −.01 | −.23 |
Sleep efficiency (%)b | 92.86 | 4.70 | −.10 | −.12 | −.04 | −.23 |
Wake after sleep onset (min)b | 13.50 | 15.81 | .29 | .20 | −.01 | .36+ |
Awakenings (N)a | 10.03 | 6.29 | .25 | .21 | .09 | .37* |
Arousals (N)a | 177.62 | 67.47 | −.15 | −.13 | −.16 | .00 |
Note: N = 29. For HF-HRV N1 N = 28, because no N1 segment was longer than 2 min in one participant.
HF-HRV = high-frequency heart rate variability; PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, with lower values indicating better sleep quality; HF-HRV N1/N2/N3/REM: HF-HRV during different sleep stages.
Correlation between: HF-HRV N2 &PSQI: p = .035; HF-HRV N3 & PSQI: p = .029; HF-HRV REM & Wake after sleep onset: p = .055; HF-HRV REM & Awakenings: p = .049.
Pearson correlations.
Spearman rho correlations.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .10.
3.3. Controlling for potential confounds
Finally, we performed partial Spearman correlations with age, physical fitness, and BMI as simultaneous covariates to control for their possible confounding influence. All correlations between HF-HRVwake and subjective sleep quality (i.e., PSQI) as well as objective sleep quality variables (i.e., sleep latency and number of arousals) remained significant, −.54 ≤ rs ≤ −.39, ps ≤ .049. The marginal correlation between HF-HRVsleep (overall) and subjective sleep quality became non-significant, r = −.32, p = .108. The correlations between HF-HRVsleep during N2 and N3 with subjective sleep quality became marginal, N2: r = −.37, p = .062, N3: r = −.38, p = .058. This was also the case for the correlation between HF-HRVsleep during N3 and sleep stage N1, r = −.33, p = .096. The correlation between HF-HRV during REM and the number of awakenings became marginal, r = .38, p = .058, whereas the correlation between HF-HRVsleep during REM and wake time after sleep onset became significant, r = .41, p = .036.
4. Discussion
The primary focus of the present study was the relationship between cardiac vagal control as an individual-difference trait marker, assessed during an extended baseline (CVCwake), and both subjective and objective sleep quality in healthy adults. We provide evidence for an association between CVCwake and sleep quality. Specifically, higher HF-HRVwake was associated with better subjective and objective sleep quality (i.e., shorter sleep latency and fewer arousals during sleep) in the subsequent night, whereas this pattern of results was much weaker for HF-HRVsleep (assessed overall and during sleep stages). This suggests that the link with sleep quality is specific to HF-HRVwake.
These findings extend the research on clinical populations that demonstrate a link between higher CVCwake and better subjective sleep quality (e.g., Hovland et al., 2013; Irwin et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2011). By assessing healthy adults’ subjective and objective sleep quality, utilizing an optimized assessment design, and controlling for several possible confounds, we believe the present study provides strong evidence for the link between CVCwake – reliably marked by HF-HRVwake – and subjective and objective sleep quality.
Some links were found between HF-HRVsleep (overall, and during N2, N3 and REM sleep) and isolated parameters of sleep quality. Specifically (when controlling for possible confounds), higher HF-HRVsleep during sleep stage N2 and N3 marginally correlated with better subjective sleep quality, but not with primary variables of objective sleep quality. Higher HF-HRVsleep during REM was related to more wake time after sleep onset and marginally related to higher number of awakenings. However, the overall pattern of results was much weaker and less consistent for HF-HRVsleep measures (overall, and for different sleep stages). Since these analyses were mainly included for exploratory purposes without strong empirical or theoretical background they should be considered tentative and replication is important before drawing any conclusions. Nevertheless, the links between HF-HRVsleep (overall, and for N2 and N3) with subjective sleep quality, although mostly marginal, are consistent with earlier studies regarding this topic (Brosschot et al., 2007; Patel et al., 2013).
4.1. Theoretical implications
The present results converge with existing research to suggest that CVCwake is related to the flexible regulation of arousal, which has important implications for its role in supporting high-quality sleep. Beyond implications for the role of CVCwake in sleep, the present results have implications for how CVCwake is conceptualized more generally.
4.1.1. Implications for understanding the role of CVC in sleep quality
Our results are characterized by a pattern that supports CVCwake as a key marker of arousal-related processes. Specifically, and consistent with prior research using actigraphy (Elmore-Staton et al., 2012; Palesh et al., 2008), we assessed a variety of sleep variables and found that CVCwake was only related to variables assessing sleep quality (e.g., sleep latency, number of arousals during sleep) and not variables that are more related to sleep quantity or sleep architecture (e.g., total sleep time, amount of N1-N3, REM, REM latency) associated with specific cognitive processes during sleep like, for example, memory consolidation (e.g., Ackermann & Rasch, 2014; Diekelmann, Wilhelm, & Born, 2009).
Higher CVCwake was associated with shorter sleep latency. The transition from wake to sleep is thought to be accompanied by a simultaneous reduction in autonomic arousal (Trinder et al., 2001) and high CVCwake may accelerate autonomic regulation toward this transition. On the other hand, arousals during sleep are related to sleep disruptions due to interspersed short periods of higher EEG spectral frequencies more characteristic of wake states (Iber et al., 2007). Consequently, the link between higher CVCwake and better sleep quality seems to specifically reflect the ability to reduce autonomic arousal in the anticipation of sleep as well as central arousal during sleep. In other words, lower CVCwake related to higher autonomic arousal or perhaps the inhibition of de-arousal processes (see Espie, 2002; Winzeler et al., 2014) may impair sleep quality by lengthening sleep latency and disrupting sleep continuity, while variables more related to sleep architecture are left largely unchanged (e.g., Bonnet & Arand, 2003; Carskadon et al., 1976).
The specific relationship between CVCwake and sleep variables related to arousal might further explain why our results do not support a link between CVCwake and all variables we chose for measuring sleep quality. Krystal and Edinger (2008) address the problematic lack of an established definition for objective sleep quality and point out that sleep quality can refer to various polysomnographically measured sleep variables (see also Buysse, Ancoli-Israel, Edinger, Lichstein, & Morin, 2006). However, they emphasize other results showing that some individuals report sleep complaints while these objective sleep variables are comparable to individuals without sleep complaints (e.g., Carskadon et al., 1976; Coleman et al., 1982). Our results underscore the relationship between CVCwake and specific variables of sleep quality and suggest that total sleep time or the number of awakenings might be related to sleep processes other than arousal regulation (see Krystal & Edinger, 2008).
4.1.2. Implications for understanding CVC
Our results converge with several lines of evidence to suggest that it is CVCwake that is linked to the flexible regulation of arousal. We found that only CVCwake, but not CVCsleep was related to objective sleep quality, although all indices of CVC were highly correlated. The current results highlight the importance of CVC assessed during wakefulness (when the organism is required to interact with its environment) compared to CVC assessed during sleep (when the organism is unable to interact with its environment). Specifically, other studies that have assessed both CVCwake and CVCsleep have also found that only CVCwake is linked with sleep quality, at least when assessing non-healthy adult samples (e.g., Irwin et al., 2006). Using objective measures of sleep quality in healthy adults, the present results extend these findings and suggest that CVCwake may carry more predictive power than CVCsleep (overall or during specific sleep stages), when using sleep quality as an outcome of interest. This argument is even more salient when considering that CVCwake measurement was separated in time from the objective sleep measures, while CVCsleep measurement was obtained in parallel with objective sleep measures.
The link between CVCwake and the flexible regulation of autonomic arousal is also consistent with several models supporting CVCwake as important predictor for health-related outcomes more generally. First, conceptual models support CVCwake as a marker of arousal-related processes especially in the context of perceived stress (e.g., Porges, 2007). Evolutionarily, it is important to attend to the environment and to be able to respond with a “fight or flight” reaction. Whereas the actual reaction is related to decreased CVCwake (often called withdrawal of vagal activity or vagal reactivity) and enhanced autonomic arousal, the state of surveying the environment is related to increased CVCwake and reduced autonomic arousal. Importantly, this vigilance function is not necessary while the organism is asleep, a state that is restricted to times and places when the organism is relatively safe (Dahl, 1996). Second, another theory of CVCwake has emphasized its role as a marker of self-regulation and flexible adaptation to changing physiological and environmental demands (Thayer & Lane, 2000, 2009). This model of neurovisceral integration assumes that CVCwake is related to a system that continuously evaluates the environment for different signs of danger and safety and prepares the organism for appropriate reactions, and promoting general adaptability to the environment. Thus, CVC assessed while the organism is awake and able to interact effectively with the environment may be more predictive of that organism's underlying self-regulation abilities than CVC assessed during sleep.
Together, these lines of evidence converge to support a model in which CVCwake is a marker of processes related to the flexible regulation of autonomic arousal. We propose this as an underlying factor for why CVCwake is related to effective sleep regulation, but also attentional and emotional processes, as well as various physical and mental health conditions related to well-regulated autonomic arousal.
4.2. Limitations and future directions
The present research suggests that higher CVCwake is related to better subjective and objective sleep quality. We acknowledge four possible limitations that suggest important future directions. First, because sleep is known to vary across the lifespan and between genders (e.g., Carrier, Land, Buysse, Kupfer, & Monk, 2001; Dijk, Beersma, & Bloem, 1989; Tsai & Li, 2004), our research involved only young women. This might limit the generalizability of the present study, and future research should investigate the relationship between CVC and sleep quality also in healthy men and older populations. Second, the use of a neutral film to assess CVCwake could have elicited an orienting response. However, we believe an orienting response would be more prominent early in the film, as we would predict a habituation response over the course of the 94-min neutral documentary. Additionally, this similarly would have affected all participants and should not have created a selective bias in individual difference correlational analyses. In relation to that, one could argue that this response during the neutral film might reflect a state of enhanced vigilance or automatic processing of visual and auditory stimuli. However, as described in the introduction, others suggest that this baseline assessment is equivalent to a resting baseline (concerning cardiovascular measures), but reveals greater stability and is better able to create a neutral context (e.g., Jennings et al., 1992; Wilson et al., 2014). Third, as we used a correlational design, we are not able to draw causal conclusions. Higher levels of CVCwake may promote higher sleep quality. Prior research has demonstrated that experimental manipulation of CVCwake by biofeedback training improved health (e.g., Nolan et al., 2005). However, sleep clearly influences cardiovascular functioning (e.g., Gangwisch et al., 2006) and thus CVCwake and sleep quality are likely linked in a bidirectional fashion. Future research could use experimental designs or longitudinal assessments to examine more directly the causal direction between CVCwake and sleep quality. Finally, we investigated the relationship between CVCwake and sleep quality as one important marker of health. Other research has examined and reliably linked CVCwake with other components of physical and mental health such as cardiovascular disease, depression, or psychological well-being (e.g., Beauchaine, 2001; Rottenberg, Clift, Bolden, & Salomon, 2007; Thayer & Lane, 2007). There are two plausible models how CVCwake and sleep quality might be linked to health more broadly. On the one hand, sleep may mediate the association between CVCwake and different health outcomes. On the other hand, research linking both CVCwake and sleep to, e.g., psychological health outcomes suggest that high sleep quality and high CVCwake might interact to predict health (e.g., El-Sheikh et al., 2007). Future research should test these different models to better understand how CVCwake and sleep quality interact to shape psychological and physical health.
5. Concluding comments
To our knowledge, this is the first investigation to link individual differences in CVCwake to different variables of objective sleep quality in healthy adults. Results showed that higher CVCwake is related to better subjective and objective sleep quality, whereas the pattern of results is much weaker for CVCsleep. Thus, high levels of CVCwake may be one particularly important ingredient of healthy sleep.
Funding
The first author of this paper was financially supported by the Doctoral College “Imaging the Mind” of the Austrian Science Fund (FWF-W1233). The second author of this paper was financially supported by a National Institutes of Health Affective Science Training Fellowship (5T32MH020006).
Footnotes
This German documentary can be found on the webpage of the television channel SpiegelTV (http://www.spiegel.tv/filme/kloster-leben-verliebt-gott/).
Contributor Information
Gabriela G. Werner, Email: gabriela.werner@sbg.ac.at.
Brett Q. Ford, Email: brett.q.ford@berkeley.edu.
Iris B. Mauss, Email: imauss@berkeley.edu.
Manuel Schabus, Email: manuel.schabus@sbg.ac.at.
Jens Blechert, Email: jens.blechert@sbg.ac.at.
Frank H. Wilhelm, Email: frank.wilhelm@sbg.ac.at.
References
- Ackermann S., Rasch B. Differential effects of non-REM and REM sleep on memory consolidation? Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports. 2014;14:430. doi: 10.1007/s11910-013-0430-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderer P., Gruber G., Parapatics S., Woertz M., Miazhynskaia T., Klosch G. An E-health solution for automatic sleep classification according to Rechtschaffen and Kales: Validation study of the Somnolyzer 24 × 7 utilizing the Siesta database. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;51:115–133. doi: 10.1159/000085205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderer P., Saletu B., Saletu-Zyhlarz G.M., Gruber G., Parapatics S., Miazhynskaia T. Recent advances in the electrophysiological evaluation of sleep. In: Drinkenburg W., Ruigt G., Jobert M., editors. Essentials and applications of EEG research in preclinical and clinical pharmacology. Unipublish Verlag für Studium & PraxisOHG; Berlin: 2004. pp. 307–339. [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine T. Vagal tone, development, and Gray's motivational theory: Toward an integrated model of autonomic nervous system functioning in psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:183–214. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401002012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson G.G., Bigger J.T., Jr., Eckberg D.L., Grossman P., Kaufmann P.G., Malik M. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:623–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertsch K., Hagemann D., Naumann E., Schachinger H., Schulz A. Stability of heart rate variability indices reflecting parasympathetic activity. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:672–682. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01341.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blunden S., Galland B. The complexities of defining optimal sleep: Empirical and theoretical considerations with a special emphasis on children. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.01.002. (epub ahead of print) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnet M.H., Arand D.L. Clinical effects of sleep fragmentation versus sleep deprivation. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2003;7:297–310. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bovet P., Auguste R., Burdette H. Strong inverse association between physical fitness and overweight in adolescents: A large school-based survey. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2007;4:24. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-4-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brosschot J.F., Van Dijk E., Thayer J.F. Daily worry is related to low heart rate variability during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;63:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess H.J., Trinder J., Kim Y., Luke D. Sleep and circadian influences on cardiac autonomic nervous system activity. American Journal of Physiology. 1997;273:H1761–H1768. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1997.273.4.H1761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burton A.R., Rahman K., Kadota Y., Lloyd A., Vollmer-Conna U. Reduced heart rate variability predicts poor sleep quality in a case-control study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Experimental Brain Research. 2010;204:71–78. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2296-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler E.A., Wilhelm F.H., Gross J.J. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:612–622. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buysse D.J. Sleep health: Can we define it? Does it matter? Sleep. 2014;37:9–17. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buysse D.J., Ancoli-Israel S., Edinger J.D., Lichstein K.L., Morin C.M. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of insomnia. Sleep. 2006;29:1155–1173. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buysse D.J., Reynolds C.F., Monk T.H., Berman S.R., Kupfer D.J. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrier J., Land S., Buysse D.J., Kupfer D.J., Monk T.H. The effects of age and gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20–60 years old) Psychophysiology. 2001;38:232–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carskadon M.A., Dement W.C., Mitler M.M., Guilleminault C., Zarcone V.P., Spiegel R. Self-reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug-free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1976;133:1382–1388. doi: 10.1176/ajp.133.12.1382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman R.M., Roffwarg H.P., Kennedy S.J., Guilleminault C., Cinque J., Cohn M.A. Sleep–wake disorders based on a polysomnographic diagnosis. A national cooperative study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1982;247:997–1003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahl R.E. The regulation of sleep and arousal: Development and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:3–27. [Google Scholar]
- Delgado L.C., Guerra P., Perakakis P., Vera M.N., Reyes del Paso G., Vila J. Treating chronic worry: Psychological and physiological effects of a training programme based on mindfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48:873–882. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diekelmann S., Wilhelm I., Born J. The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2009;13:309–321. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijk D.J., Beersma D.G., Bloem G.M. Sex differences in the sleep EEG of young adults: Visual scoring and spectral analysis. Sleep. 1989;12:500–507. doi: 10.1093/sleep/12.6.500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Sheikh M., Erath S.A., Keller P.S. Children's sleep and adjustment: the moderating role of vagal regulation. Journal of Sleep Research. 2007;16:396–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00618.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elmore-Staton L., El-Sheikh M., Vaughn B., Arsiwalla D.D. Preschoolers' daytime respiratory sinus arrhythmia and nighttime sleep. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;107:414–417. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.07.005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El-Sheikh M., Erath S.A., Bagley E.J. Parasympathetic nervous system activity and children's sleep. Journal of Sleep Research. 2013;22:282–288. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Espie C.A. Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:215–243. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fang S.C., Huang C.J., Yang T.T., Tsai P.S. Heart rate variability and daytime functioning in insomniacs and normal sleepers: Preliminary results. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008;65:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gangwisch J.E., Heymsfield S.B., Boden-Albala B., Buijs R.M., Kreier F., Pickering T.G. Short sleep duration as a risk factor for hypertension: Analyses of the first National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Hypertension. 2006;47:833–839. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000217362.34748.e0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geisler F.C.M., Vennewald N., Kubiak T., Weber H. The impact of heart rate variability on subjective well-being is mediated by emotion regulation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;49:723–728. [Google Scholar]
- Giese-Davis J., Wilhelm F.H., Tamagawa R., Palesh O., Neri E., Taylor C.B. Higher vagal activity as related to survival in patients with advanced breast cancer: An analysis of autonomic dysregulation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2015 doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000167, (epub ahead of print). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman P., Kollai M. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, cardiac vagal tone, and respiration: Within- and between-individual relations. Psychophysiology. 1993;30:486–495. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb02072.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman P., Taylor E.W. Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:263–285. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hovland A., Pallesen S., Hammar A., Hansen A.L., Thayer J.F., Sivertsen B. Subjective sleep quality in relation to inhibition and heart rate variability in patients with panic disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;150:152–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iber C., Ancoli-Israel S., Chesson A.L., Quan S.F. Terminology and technical specifications. 1st ed. American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Westchester, Illinois: 2007. The AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events: Rules. [Google Scholar]
- Irwin M.R., Valladares E.M., Motivala S., Thayer J.F., Ehlers C.L. Association between nocturnal vagal tone and sleep depth, sleep quality, and fatigue in alcohol dependence. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:159–166. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195743.60952.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackowska M., Dockray S., Endrighi R., Hendrickx H., Steptoe A. Sleep problems and heart rate variability over the working day. Journal of Sleep Research. 2012;21:434–440. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.00996.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jennings J.R., Kamarck T., Stewart C., Eddy M., Johnson P. Alternate cardiovascular baseline assessment techniques: Vanilla or resting baseline. Psychophysiology. 1992;29:742–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb02052.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kageyama T., Nishikido N., Kobayashi T., Kurokawa Y., Kaneko T., Kabuto M. Self-reported sleep quality, job stress, and daytime autonomic activities assessed in terms of short-term heart rate variability among male white-collar workers. Industrial Health. 1998;36:263–272. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.36.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan A., Gruber J., Shallcross A.J., Ford B.Q., Mauss I.B. Too much of a good thing? Cardiac vagal tone's nonlinear relationship with well-being. Emotion. 2013;13:599–604. doi: 10.1037/a0032725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal A.D., Edinger J.D. Measuring sleep quality. Sleep Medicine. 2008;9(Suppl 1):S10–S17. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70011-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krystal A.D., Edinger J.D., Wohlgemuth W.K., Marsh G.R. NREM sleep EEG frequency spectral correlates of sleep complaints in primary insomnia subtypes. Sleep. 2002;25:630–640. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nikolaidis P.T. Body mass index and body fat percentage are associated with decreased physical fitness in adolescent and adult female volleyball players. Journal of Research in Medical Sciences. 2013;18:22–26. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolan R.P., Kamath M.V., Floras J.S., Stanley J., Pang C., Picton P. Heart rate variability biofeedback as a behavioral neurocardiac intervention to enhance vagal heart rate control. American Heart Journal. 2005;149:1137. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2005.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palesh O., Zeitzer J.M., Conrad A., Giese-Davis J., Mustian K.M., Popek V. Vagal regulation, cortisol, and sleep disruption in women with metastatic breast cancer. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2008;4:441–449. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel V., Giesebrecht S., Burton A.R., Cvejic E., Lemon J., Hadzi-Pavlovic D. Reliability revisited: Autonomic responses in the context of everyday well-being. International Journal of Cardiology. 2013;166:743–745. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2012.09.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porges S.W. The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:116–143. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pranada A.B. [Internet based investigation of the links between sleep quality and the medical studies course in medical students in Bochum]. Ruhr-University of Bochum; Germany: 2005. Internetbasierte Untersuchung zum Zusammenhang zwischen Schlafqualität, Lebensqualität und dem Verlauf des Studiums bei Bochumer Medizinstudenten. (doctoral thesis) Retrieved from http://www-brs.ub.ruhr-uni-bochum.de/netahtml/HSS/Diss/PranadaArthurBoniface/diss.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Riemann D., Backhaus J. [Treatment of sleep disorders]. Beltz Psychologie Verlags Union; Weilheim: 1996. Behandlung von Schlafstörungen, Ein psychologisches Gruppenprogramm. [Google Scholar]
- Rottenberg J., Clift A., Bolden S., Salomon K. RSA fluctuation in major depressive disorder. Psychophysiology. 2007;44:450–458. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00509.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherwood L. Cengage Learning; USA: 2010. Human physiology: From cells to systems. [Google Scholar]
- Sheskin D.J. Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures. Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2003. Spearman's rank-order correlation coefficient. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder F., Hobson J.A., Morrison D.F., Goldfrank F. Changes in respiration, heart rate, and systolic blood pressure in human sleep. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1964;19:417–422. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1964.19.3.417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein P.K., Pu Y. Heart rate variability, sleep and sleep disorders. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2012;16:47–66. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terzano M.G., Parrino L., Spaggiari M.C., Palomba V., Rossi M., Smerieri A. CAP variables and arousals as sleep electroencephalogram markers for primary insomnia. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2003;114:1715–1723. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00136-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J.F., Lane R.D. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;61:201–216. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00338-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J.F., Lane R.D. The role of vagal function in the risk for cardiovascular disease and mortality. Biological Psychology. 2007;74:224–242. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J.F., Lane R.D. Claude Bernard and the heart–brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33:81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thayer J.F., Sternberg E. Beyond heart rate variability: Vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Science. 2006;1088:361–372. doi: 10.1196/annals.1366.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tobaldini E., Nobili L., Strada S., Casali K.R., Braghiroli A., Montano N. Heart rate variability in normal and pathological sleep. Frontiers in Physiology. 2013;4:294. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trinder J., Kleiman J., Carrington M., Smith S., Breen S., Tan N. Autonomic activity during human sleep as a function of time and sleep stage. Journal of Sleep Research. 2001;10:253–264. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsai L.L., Li S.P. Sleep patterns in college students: Gender and grade differences. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;56:231–237. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00507-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welch P.D. The use of fast Fourier transform for the estimation of power spectra: A method based on time averaging over short, modified periodograms. IEEE Transactions on Audio and Electroacoustics. 1967;15:70–73. [Google Scholar]
- Werner G.G., Schabus M., Blechert J., Kolodyazhniy V., Wilhelm F.H. Pre- to post-sleep change in psychophysiological reactivity to emotional films: Late-night REM sleep is associated with attenuated emotional processing. Psychophysiology. 2015 doi: 10.1111/psyp.12404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12404 (epub ahead of print) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm F.H., Grossman P., Coyle M.A. Improving estimation of cardiac vagal tone during spontaneous breathing using a paced breathing calibration. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation. 2004;40:317–324. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm F.H., Grossman P., Roth W.T. Analysis of cardiovascular regulation. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation. 1999;35:135–140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson T.D., Reinhard D.A., Westgate E.C., Gilbert D.T., Ellerbeck N., Hahn C. Social psychology. Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science. 2014;345:75–77. doi: 10.1126/science.1250830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winzeler K., Voellmin A., Schafer V., Meyer A.H., Cajochen C., Wilhelm F.H. Daily stress, presleep arousal, and sleep in healthy young women: A daily life computerized sleep diary and actigraphy study. Sleep Medicine. 2014;15:359–366. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang A.C., Tsai S.J., Yang C.H., Kuo C.H., Chen T.J., Hong C.J. Reduced physiologic complexity is associated with poor sleep in patients with major depression and primary insomnia. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;131:179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]