Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Mar 20.
Published in final edited form as: J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;40(2):331–341. doi: 10.3233/JAD-130245

The KEEPS-Cognitive and Affective Study: Baseline Associations between Vascular Risk Factors and Cognition

Whitney Wharton A,B,C, Carey E Gleason A,B,C, N Maritza Dowling A,C,D, Cynthia M Carlsson A,B,C, Eliot A Brinton E, M Nanette Santoro F, Genevieve Neal-Perry G, Hugh Taylor H, Frederick Naftolin I, Rogerio Lobo J, George Merriam K, JoAnn E Manson L, Marcelle Cedars M, Virginia M Miller N, Dennis M Black O, Matthew Budoff P, Howard N Hodis Q, Mitchell Harman R, Sanjay Asthana A,B,C
PMCID: PMC4367860  NIHMSID: NIHMS666001  PMID: 24430001

Abstract

Background

Midlife vascular risk factors influence later cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). The decrease in serum estradiol levels during menopause has been associated with cognitive impairment and increased vascular risk, such as high blood pressure (BP), which independently contribute to cognitive dysfunction and AD.

Methods

We describe the extent to which vascular risk factors relate to cognition in healthy, middle–aged, recently postmenopausal women enrolled in the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Cognitive and Affective Study (KEEPS-Cog) at baseline. KEEPS-Cog is a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled, parallel group design, clinical trial, investigating the efficacy of low-dose, transdermal 17β-estradiol and oral conjugated equine estrogen on cognition.

Results

The KEEPS-Cog cohort (N=662) is healthy and free of cognitive dysfunction. Higher systolic BP was related to poorer performance in auditory working memory and attention (unadjusted p=0.004; adjusted p=0.10). This relationship was not associated with endogenous hormone levels.

Conclusions

Lower BP early in menopause may positively affect cognitive domains known to be associated with AD.

Keywords: Clinical Trial, Estrogen, Blood Pressure, Vascular Risk, Hormone Therapy, Estradiol, Cognition, Attention, Memory

Introduction

Declining serum estrogen levels during the menopausal transition have been linked to increased vascular risk factors and subtle cognitive decline (1, 2). Results from basic science, observational studies and clinical trials suggest that hormone therapy (HT) administered soon after menopause may reduce these deleterious vascular and cognitive effects (3, 4). Specifically, HT’s salutary vascular effects have been linked to protection of arterial wall function and lowering blood pressure (BP) (5, 6). HT administration may also have beneficial cognitive effects, both via direct actions on estrogen receptors in the brain, and indirectly, through HT’s beneficial effects on the vasculature.

Increases in vascular risk factors, including hypertension and insulin resistance during midlife are associated with an increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in later life (for a comprehensive review see (7); (8, 9)). Similarly, controlled vascular risk factors such as reductions in BP are associated with protection against AD (10, 11). This indicates that long-standing uncontrolled BP and other vascular risk factors may contribute to AD pathology, possibly through decreased cerebral blood flow (CBF) and accumulation of β-amyloid (Aβ), a key pathological feature of preclinical AD (12, 13). Some studies show that precipitous decrease in serum estradiol levels during the menopausal transition is associated with some cognitive impairment, particularly in the domains of attention and memory (14). Moreover, the decline in serum estradiol levels increases the risk of hypertension and hypercholesterolemia, factors that independently contribute to cognitive dysfunction and AD in later life (15).

While there is a clear relationship between cognition and clinically diagnosable hypertension and hypercholesterolemia, the point at which vascular risk factors influence cognitive task performance is less understood. Moreover, while the menopausal decline in serum estradiol levels has been linked to subtle reductions in cognition and increased vascular risk factors, the extent to which serum estradiol levels influence vascular risk factors and cognition independently or in tandem is unclear.

The majority of research linking vascular factors and cognition has been conducted in older populations and in samples with established vascular disease (16). Recently, studies have shown that vascular risk factors can influence cognitive task performance in younger populations, whose vascular disease risk factors are within clinically ‘normal’ limits (17). For instance, our group (18) and others (19) found a relationship between cognitive task performance and blood pressure (BP) extending into the normotensive range (i.e., around 120/80 mmHg) in younger samples of healthy men and women (i.e., 18–25 years). These changes in cognition are similar to changes observed in older populations (20).

The Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Cognitive and Affective Study (KEEPS-Cog) is a 4-year, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel group, clinical trial, designed to investigate the differential efficacy of low-dose estrogen formulations on cognition in recently postmenopausal women. Women enrolled in the KEEPS-Cog sub-study serve as an excellent cohort to investigate the relationship among vascular risk factors, cognitive task performance and endogenous sex hormone levels in recently postmenopausal women, prior to randomization to study medication. Continued follow-up of this cohort will clarify the relationship between midlife HT use, cardiovascular function and cognitive performance.

The purpose of this study was to define the relationships among vascular risk factors, cognitive task performance and endogenous sex hormone levels (e.g. estradiol, estrone, progesterone and testosterone) at baseline in healthy, middle–aged, recently postmenopausal women enrolled in the KEEPS-Cog sub-study at baseline. Additionally, we sought to explore the extent to which this relationship might be associated with serum endogenous sex hormone levels.

Methods

The KEEPS-cog is a sub-study of the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA). Details of the trial design, inclusion and exclusion criteria, treatment assignment, and participant characteristics have been published previously (21, 22). The University of Wisconsin in Madison was the coordinating site for the KEEPS-Cog trial. The trial consisted of four visits over four years (baseline and months 18, 36 and 48). KEEPS-Cog cognitive and affective testing sessions coincided with visits for the parent KEEPS trial. The current manuscript includes measures ascertained at the baseline study visit only, prior to randomization to treatment and thus hormone levels reflect only endogenous levels.

Participants

KEEPS-Cog participants consisted of 662 recently menopausal women (42–59 years old), between 6 months and 3 years of their last menses. Participants included women without known or suspected cognitive or cardiovascular disease, and meeting the inclusion criteria for KEEPS (KEEPS-Cog NCT000154180) enrolled at one of nine clinical testing sites. KEEPS is a multicenter, randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled trial, designed to test the hypothesis that low-dose HT initiated in recently postmenopausal women will reduce the progression of subclinical atherosclerosis as measured by carotid artery intima-media thickness (CIMT) and coronary artery calcification (CAC) over four years (21, 22).

Potential participants in the KEEPS Cog study underwent a depression and cognitive dysfunction screening, a medical examination, blood tests and an electrocardiogram at baseline. Women scoring 18 or greater on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), or reporting suicidal ideation as assessed by the Beck Depression Inventory, or scoring 22/30 or lower on the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) were excluded from the KEEPS-Cog study. All women underwent a high-resolution B-mode ultrasound examination for the assessment of CIMT (23) and computed tomography for the assessment of CAC (24). Women were also excluded if they had a history of clinically defined cardiovascular disease; were current heavy smokers (more than ten cigarettes/day by self-report); their CAC score was ≥50 Agatston units (AU); body mass index was >35 kg/m2; or if they had dyslipidemia (low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol >190 mg/dL), hypertriglyceridemia (>400 mg/dL), serum 17β-estradiol >40 mg/dL, uncontrolled hypertension (systolic BP >150 mmHg or diastolic BP >95 mmHg), or fasting blood glucose (FBG) >126 mg/dL (21). Women using lipid-lowering medications at baseline were excluded. KEEPS-Cog was approved by Institutional Review Boards at each of the nine clinical testing sites and the University of Wisconsin (UW) in Madison. All participants provided written informed consent.

1.1. Cognitive task measures

Cognitive tasks included the Modified Mini-Mental State Exam (25), Prospective Memory Test (26), NYU Paragraph Recall (27), Stroop Color Word Interference Test (28), Letter-Number Sequencing (29), Digit Symbol (30), Trail Making Test (Trails A & B) (31), the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) (32), the Benton Visual Retention Test (33), Digit Span (29) and Verbal Fluency (31). We combined components of the individual cognitive tests into a four-factor structure for analyses (described below).

Laboratory values and vascular risk factors

Blood pressure was recorded in the morning by a registered nurse familiar with BP measurement methodology. Participants were seated for five minutes prior to when BP was taken. A conventional mercury sphygmomanometer, appropriate sized BP cuff and a stethoscope with a bell were used. Two BP determinations were obtained from the same arm, ten minutes apart. The average of the two BP readings was used for analyses. Venous blood samples were obtained from the arm opposite of and after measurements of BP. Low density lipoprotein-cholesterol (LDL-C), high density lipoprotein-cholesterol (HDL-C) triglycerides and blood glucose (FBG) were measured by Kronos Science Laboratories (Phoenix AZ).

Hormone analyses

Serum estradiol, estrone, testosterone and progesterone were measured at the University of Wisconsins Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) - funded Institute for Clinical and Translational Research (ICTR) laboratory in the Assay Services Core. Baseline samples were batched and assays were conducted at one time. Ultrapure water (500 μl) with 40 μl of internal standards (deuterated steroids: testosterone, estradiol, estrone, progesterone; CDN Isotopes in Pointe-Claire, Quebec, Canada) were added to 400 μl of serum and extracted with 2 ml of methyl ether. The ether layer was dried and re-suspended in ethanol and 500 μl water and 1 ml of dichloromethane. The dichloromethane portion was dried and re-suspended in 25 μl acetonitrile/water (50:50). To derivatize the estrogens, 25 μl dansyl chloride was added, heated for 3 minutes and samples were prepared for injection of 30 μl into the LC/MS. A 150×2.10 mm column (2.6 μ, C18, Kinetex, Phenomenx, Torrance, California) was used for the HPLC separation on our Agilent 1100 series system. Positive ion identification was used for testosterone (m/z 289), progesterone (m/z 315), estradiol (m/z 506) and estrone (m/z 504) measurements. Separation was performed by using a gradient in mobile phase B = 95% acetonitrile (ACN)/5% water (H2O) and A 95% H20/5% ACN where %B begins at 44%, increases to 60% at 10 minutes, to 90% at 16 minutes and back to 70% at 20 minutes, to 44% at 23 minutes. Flow rate was 100 μl/minutes. Coefficients of variation for sex hormones assayed in the present study are as follows: testosterone (18.8%); progesterone (25.3%); estradiol (12.9%); and estrone (22.4%).

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were controlled for age, education, race, study site and apolipoprotein E ε4 (APOE ε4) status based on APOE’s reported independent effects on cardiovascular risk and cognitive task performance (34, 35).

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to examine cognitive function at baseline. Compilation of factor scores was comprised of components of the 12 cognitive tests utilized in the KEEPS-Cog battery. Using standard criteria for model fit (36) a four-factor solution provided an acceptable fit (χ2=360.58 with a p-value<0.001; comparative fit index - CFI=0.92, root mean squared error of approximation-RMSEA=0.05). The four cognitive domains used in the analyses included 1) Verbal Learning 2) Auditory Attention & Working Memory, 3) Verbal Attention & Executive Function and 4) Speeded Language & Flexibility. The factor scores served as outcome measures in the mixed regression models. The model was estimated using the R package.

To explore the relationship between vascular risk factors and cognitive factor scores, we employed a linear mixed effect modeling approach. Vascular risk factors entered into the model included systolic BP, LDL-C, HDL-C, FBG and body mass index (BMI) as well as CIMT and CAC. We controlled for gender, age, race, education level and APOE 4 status. All p values were set at 0.05 and were adjusted for multiple comparisons using Benhamini-Hochberg’s procedures (37)

In order to investigate a potential impact of sex endogenous hormone levels on the systolic BP - cognition relationship, we next employed another linear effects model. In this model, we included endogenous sex hormone levels including estradiol, estrone, testosterone and progesterone. As was the case with the first model, we controlled for gender, age race, education level and APOE 4 status, p values were set at 0.05 and we adjusted for multiple comparisons (37).

Procedures

Administration of the cognitive battery was conducted by trained psychometricians at each of the nine testing sites. Before the KEEPS-cog sub-study initiated, all psychometricians took part in a two-day group training covering KEEPS-Cog rationale, methodology, test administration and scoring and a full day of interactive, mock cognitive and affective testing. Each site was provided with a cognitive testing manual and instructions for test administration and scoring and a DVD of a mock testing session. In the instance of psychometrician turnover, the KEEPS-Cog coordinator, a trained cognitive neuroscientist at the UW Madison, traveled to the testing site and trained the new psychometrician on testing administration and scoring techniques. Additionally, we conducted training sessions every two years, and quarterly conference calls were held between the UW Madison and all sites to ensure continuity of the project. To further ensure the quality of the data, 10% of participant data at each testing site was audited to ensure correct test scoring and data entry. Quality assurance checks exceeded 98% accuracy for all sites for all cognitive variables.

The KEEPS-Cog testing session lasted approximately 1.5 hours. The cognitive testing area was free of excessive noise and approved by the KEEPS-Cog study personnel at the UW Madison. Because the KEEPS-Cog visit coincided with the parent KEEPS visits that required a fasting blood draw, all participants were tested in the morning and were given a light breakfast before cognitive testing began. All participants were tested by the same psychometrician at their respective site. Order of neuropsychological test administration was consistent across sites and tests were grouped to minimize between test interference. Tests were scored and entered into a centralized database within 3 days of the visit.

Results

Participants

Of the 662 women meeting inclusion criteria for KEEPS and consenting to participate in the KEEPS-Cog, 91 were excluded from the present analyses due to missingness of cognitive data or unwillingness to take part in genetic testing for APOE genotyping.

Demographic information for participants enrolled in the KEEPS-Cog sub-study is listed in Tale 1. Participants (N = 571) were middle aged (mean 52.7 years) and well-educated, with 73.8% reporting at least some college education. The majority of participants identified themselves as White (78.1%). Those identifying as Black or Hispanic were 7.1% and 6.2%, respectively. Percentage of Non-White, non-Hispanic, participants from each of the nine clinical testing sites ranged from 5.6% to 17.7% of the total sample.

Table 2 shows KEEPS-Cog participants’ vascular risk factors. By design, the middle-aged sample is very healthy and at low risk for vascular disease based on BP measures, BMI, and HDL-C, LDL-C and FBG levels. Percent of women enrolled in KEEPS-Cog with an ApoE 4 allele is 14.3%. There is no significant difference on any measure between KEEPS-Cog participants and participants enrolled in the parent KEEPS study.

Table 2.

Description of vascular risk factors among women in the KEEPS-Cog study.

Vascular Risk Factor Mean SD
 Age (years) 52.66 2.59
 Height (ft) 5.45 2.40
 Weight (lbs) 155.60 26.60
 Waist Circumference (in) 33.32 6.82
 Average systolic (mm/Hg) 118.58 15.25
 Average diastolic (mm/Hg) 74.61 9.24
 BMI (kg/m2) 26.32 4.31
 FBG (mg/dL) 89.17 9.77
 Trig (mg/dL) 91.75 51.41
 LDL-C (mg/dL) 128.99 29.73
 HDL-C (mg/dL) 64.66 17.13
 Current Tobacco Use (%users) 6.4%
APOE ε4 (% E4 positive women) 14.3%

BMI = body mass index, FBG = fasting blood glucose, Trig = triglycerides LDL-C = low density lipoprotein-cholesterol, HDL-C = high density lipoprotein-cholesterol

Results of the fixed effects models are shown in Table 3. Results reveal a significant association between systolic BP and the auditory attention and working memory factor score (unadjusted, p = .004) after controlling for age, education and APOE ε4 status. This relationship, however, fades after adjusting for multiple comparisons (adjusted, p = 0.10). Additional analyses showed no relationship between BP and the other three factor scores.

Table 3.

Results of the mixed effects model describing the relationship between the auditory attention and working memory factor score and vascular risk factors and subclinical atherosclerosis measures of CIMT and CAC. Age, education, race and ApoE 4 status were entered as covariates.

Attention/Working Memory t-value p-value
 Systolic Blood Pressure −2.93 0.003
 CIMT 0.533 0.594
 CAC −0.778 0.436
 LDL-C (mg/dL) −.0.225 0.821
 HDL-C (mg/dL) −0.345 0.730
 FBG (mg/dL) 0.807 0.420
 BMI 0.737 0.460

CIMT = carotid artery intima–media thickness, CAC = coronary arterial calcification, LDL-C = low density lipoprotein-cholesterol, HDL-C = high density lipoprotein-cholesterol, FBG = fasting blood glucose, BMI = body mass index.

To ensure the relationship between systolic BP and cognition was not attributed to endogenous sex hormone levels, we entered estradiol, estrone, progesterone and testosterone into a fixed effects model. Analyses revealed that the relationship between the auditory attention and working memory factor score and systolic BP was not altered after including baseline sex hormones (all p values > 0.28) (See Table 4).

Table 4.

Results of the mixed effects model describing the relationship between the auditory attention and working memory factor score and sex hormone levels.

Serum Hormone Levels Mean (SD) t-value p-value
Estradiol, pg/mL 21.7 (30.9) 0.81 0.42
Estrone, pg/mL 23.8 (16.7) −1.03 0.29
Testosterone, pg/mL 217.8 (128.3) −0.60 0.54
Progesterone, pg/mL 355.7 (288.8) 1.00 0.31

Discussion

Our results show that the KEEPS-Cog cohort was healthy and free of major medical conditions and comorbidities at baseline. As such, our results are less prone to confounding related to concomitant vascular and cognitive interference. Our main result was a significant relationship between auditory attention/working memory and systolic BP such that participants with higher systolic BP performed worse on tests within this domain.

Present results are consistent with other studies showing an inverse relation between BP and cognition (i.e., higher BP is associated with poorer cognitive performance) (38). A number of studies have demonstrated that midlife hypertension has a negative impact on cognitive performance (39, 40), is an established risk factor for AD (9, 41), and has been linked to increased cognitive impairment among AD patients (42). Hypertension has been linked to deficits in attention (43), and memory (44), the factor score found to be related to BP in the present study. The auditory attention/working memory factor score is comprised of digit span forward, digit span backward and the letter number sequencing task. Prior research has shown that the individual components of the attention/working memory factor score, particularly digit span, have been linked to BP dysregulation during midlife (45, 46).

Our data revealed that the relationship between BP and cognition was not influenced by endogenous sex hormone levels (p values > .28) for estradiol, estrone, testosterone and progesterone. This result is consistent with prior findings linking hypertension to cognitive impairment and mild cognitive impairment (MCI), independent of the effects of endogenous estrogen levels or HT administration (47, 48). These results suggest that subtle increases in vascular risk factors influence cognition, independent of estrogen’s effects on cognition and disease incidence. It is likely that both increased midlife vascular risk factors and the loss of estradiol during the menopausal transition serve as independent risk factors that work synergistically to increase the risk for cognitive decline and incident AD in later life.

Midlife hypertension has been associated with decreasing estradiol levels during menopause and cognitive impairment. Hypertension during midlife has also been associated with an increased risk of AD in later life, while reductions in BP are associated with protection against the disease (7, 13). This indicates that long-standing, uncontrolled BP may contribute to AD neuropathology, possibly through decreased cerebral blood flow (CBF) and accumulation of β-amyloid (Aβ), a key pathological feature of preclinical AD (13). For instance, plasma Aβ42 has been shown to be significantly and positively correlated with systolic and diastolic BP and pulse pressure (49), and the Honolulu Heart Program/Honolulu-Asia Aging Study reported that midlife systolic BP variation is associated with increased Aβ in the hippocampus (50). Moreover, studies involving BP medications suggest that some antihypertensives reduce the risk for AD and improve cognition in patients with AD via improved CBF (5153). Although collectively, these studies suggest that cognition is impaired in the presence of prolonged uncontrolled hypertension and the mechanism driving this relationship may be directly related to AD neuropathology. Future studies investigating the relationship between midlife cognition and BP would likely benefit from the inclusion of neuroimaging and measures of potential soluble or cellular biomarkers in order to assess the potential mechanisms driving this relationship.

It should be noted that our prior work (18) as well as other studies (19) have shown that even subclinical vascular dysfunction, (e.g., slightly high or low BP), may pose a significant additional risk factor for cognitive decline and AD, compounded by genetics and family history. While our participants were normotensive, we observed a relationship between higher systolic BP and poorer cognitive task performance in a healthy, middle-aged cohort. The healthy KEEPS-Cog cohort is ideally positioned to address this important issue- as the participants do not have clinical evidence of vascular disease at baseline.

A potential limitation of the current data is that the KEEPS-Cog study cohort primarily identifies as White, though the study as a whole is more representative than the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) or the Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study (WHIMS) (22, 54). Black and Hispanic populations are more likely to be afflicted with increased risk for hypertension across the lifespan than White participants.

An important distinction between the KEEPS-Cog sub-study and the WHIMS, is that our participants were younger, more educated, and arguably most important for the current project, have healthy cardiovascular profiles. Compared to WHIMS participants, women in the KEEPS-Cog have lower systolic BP levels, are more likely to be never smokers, have lower lipid levels and lower BMI. While the WHI and WHIMS studies have contributed knowledge surrounding standard-dose HT, vascular risk factors and AD, the KEEPS and KEEPS-Cog sub-studies are ideally positioned to examine the effects of low-dose HT when initiated soon after menopause. Additionally, the KEEPS-Cog sub-study provides the opportunity to determine early postmenopausal initiation of low-dose HT in conjunction with subclinical vascular risk factors during midlife, and their contribution to cognitive task performance and subsequent AD in later life. Thus, KEEPS is positioned to answer questions regarding the long term use of differential low-dose estrogen formulations administered soon after menopause, the impact on vascular risk and how these factors may work independently and synergistically to affect cognition over time.

Table 1.

Demographic Information of KEEPS-Cog Participants.

Education N %
 Some High School 3 .5
 High School Diploma or GED 46 7.0
 Some College/Vocational School 122 18.7
 College Graduate 263 40.3
 Some Graduate or professional school 30 4.6
 Graduate or Professional degree 189 28.9
Site N %
 Brigham and Women 81 12.3
 Columbia 87 13.2
 Mayo 117 17.7
 Albert Einstein 69 10.4
 University of California 51 7.7
 Utah 90 13.6
 Washington 37 5.6
 Yale 67 10.1
 Kronos 62 9.4
Race N %
 No Answer 36 5.4
 Asian Indian 4 .6
 Black 47 7.1
 White 516 78.1
 Chinese 6 .9
 Philipino 2 .3
 Hispanic 41 6.2
 Japanese 1 .2
 Korean 1 .2
 Other 7 1.1

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) R01 AG029624, P50AG033514, R01AG031790, the by grant 1UL1RR025011 from the Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program of the NIH National Center for Research Resources and the Wisconsin National Primate Research Center base grant, NIH NCRR000167.

Funding/Support: KEEPS is funded by grants from the Aurora Foundation to the Kronos Longevity Research Institute, National Institutes of Health (NIH) HL90639 to VMM, Mayo CTSA 1 UL1 RR024150, the Mayo Foundation, Brigham and Women’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School CTSA, CTSA UL1 RR024139 and UCSF CTSA UL1 RR024131 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and NIH Roadmap for Medical Research GMERRIAM. The manuscript’s contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of NCATS or NIH. Information on NCRR is available at http://www.ncrr.nih.gov. Bayer HealthCare Pharmaceuticals, Inc. supplied the CLIMARA® estradiol and placebo patches and Abbott Laboratories (formerly Solvay Pharmaceuticals) provided the micronized progesterone (PROMETRIUM®) and placebo capsules.

Abbreviations

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

BP

blood pressure

KEEPS

Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study

KEEPS-Cog

Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Cognitive and Affective Study

HT

hormone therapy

CBF

cerebral blood flow

CIMT

carotid artery intima-media thickness

AU

Agatston units

HDL-C

Agatston units

LDL-C

low-density lipoprotein-cholesterol

UW

University of Wisconsin

CVLT-II

California Verbal Learning Test

FBG

Fasting blood glucose

ICTR

Institute for Clinical and Translational Research

ACN

acetonitrile

BMI

body mass index

APOE

apolipoprotein E

CFA

confirmatory factor analysis

β-amyloid

WHI

Women’s Health Initiative

WHIMS

Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study

Footnotes

Role of the Sponsors: The Aurora Foundation did not have input into the design or conduct of the study or the review or approval of this article.

ClinicalTrials.gov number is NCT00154180.

IRB numbers for KEEPS institutions:

The central KEEPS and Phoenix KEEPS (IRB protocol by the Western IRB): STUDY NUM: 1058663 and WIRB PRO NUM: 20040792KEEPS (main study & cognitive substudy) #10-02980 and MDBHAS #11-05383

Brigham and Women’s Hospital (Partners): #2004-P-002144 BWH

Mayo Clinic: 2241-04

Columbia: IRB#: AAAA-8062

Yale: 0409027022

University of Utah: 13257

Einstein/Montefiore: 04-08-213

Univ of Wisconsin: H-2005-0059

UCSF: KEEPS (main study & cognitive substudy) #10-02980

University of Washington IRB #26702; VAPSHCS IRB #01048

Albert Einstein College of Medicine: Genevieve Neal-Perry, Ruth Freeman, Hussein Amin, Barbara Isaac, Maureen Magnani, Rachel Wildman

Brigham and Women’s Hospital/Harvard Medical School: JoAnn Manson, Maria Bueche, Marie Gerhard-Herman, Kate Kalan, Jan Lieson, Kathryn M. Rexrode, Barbara Richmond, Frank Rybicki, Brian Walsh

Columbia College of Physicians and Surgeons: Rogerio Lobo, Luz Sanabria, Maria Soto, Michelle P. Warren, Ralf C. Zimmerman

Kronos Longevity Research Institute: S. Mitchell Harman, Mary Dunn, Panayiotis D. Tsitouras, Viola Zepeda

Mayo Clinic: Virginia M. Miller, Philip A. Araoz, Rebecca Beck, Dalene Bott-Kitslaar, Sharon L. Mulvagh, Lynne T. Shuster, Teresa G. Zais

University of California, Los Angeles, CAC Reading Center: Matthew Budoff, Chris Dailing, Yanlin Gao, Angel Solano

University of California, San Francisco Medical Center: Marcelle I. Cedars, Nancy Jancar, Jean Perry, Rebecca S. Wong, Robyn Pearl, Judy Yee, Brett Elicker, Gretchen A.W. Gooding; UCSF Statistical Reading Center: Dennis Black, Lisa Palermo

University of Southern California, Atherosclerosis Research Unit/Core Imaging and Reading Center: Howard N. Hodis, Yanjie Li, Mingzhu Yan

University of Utah School of Medicine: Eliot Brinton, Paul N. Hopkins, M. Nazeem Nanjee, Kirtly Jones, Timothy Beals, Stacey Larrinaga-Shum

VA Puget Sound Health Care System and University of Washington School of Medicine: George Merriam, Pamela Asberry, SueAnn Brickle, Colleen Carney, Molly Carr, Monica Kletke, Lynna C. Smith

Yale University, School of Medicine: Hugh Taylor, Kathryn Czarkowski, Lubna Pal, Linda McDonald, Mary Jane Minkin, Diane Wall, Erin Wolff (now at NIH/NICHD).

Others: Frederick Naftolin (New York University), Nanette Santoro (University of Colorado)

Additional Contributions: We gratefully acknowledge the dedicated efforts of all the investigators and staff at the KEEPS clinical centers, the KEEPS Data Coordinating Center at KLRI, and the NIH Institutes supporting ancillary studies. The KEEPS investigators would like to thank the Aurora Foundation for study support and Bayer HealthCare Pharmaceuticals, Inc. and Abbott Laboratories for providing study medications. Above all, we recognize and thank the KEEPS participants for their dedication and commitment to the KEEPS research program.

References

  • 1.Berent-Spillson A, Persad CC, Love T, et al. Hormonal Environment Affects Cognition Independent of Age during the Menopause Transition. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism. 2012 doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-1365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Weber M, Mapstone M. Memory complaints and memory performance in the menopausal transition. Menopause (New York, NY. 2009;16(4):694–700. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e318196a0c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Bagger YZ, Tanko LB, Alexandersen P, et al. Early postmenopausal hormone therapy may prevent cognitive impairment later in life. Menopause (New York, NY. 2005;12(1):12–7. doi: 10.1097/00042192-200512010-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sherwin BB. Estrogen and cognitive functioning in women: lessons we have learned. Behavioral neuroscience. 2012;126(1):123–7. doi: 10.1037/a0025539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hashimoto M, Akishita M, Eto M, et al. Modulation of endothelium-dependent flow-mediated dilatation of the brachial artery by sex and menstrual cycle. Circulation. 1995;92(12):3431–5. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.92.12.3431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ichikawa J, Sumino H, Ichikawa S, Ozaki M. Different effects of transdermal and oral hormone replacement therapy on the renin-angiotensin system, plasma bradykinin level, and blood pressure of normotensive postmenopausal women. American journal of hypertension. 2006;19(7):744–9. doi: 10.1016/j.amjhyper.2005.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Craft S. The role of metabolic disorders in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia: two roads converged. Archives of neurology. 2009;66(3):300–5. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kivipelto MHE, Laakso MP, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele, elevated midlife total cholesterol level, and high midlife systolic blood pressure are independent risk factors for late-life Alzheimer disease. Ann Intern Med. 2002;137:149–155. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-137-3-200208060-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Launer LJ, Ross GW, Petrovitch H, et al. Midlife blood pressure and dementia: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Neurobiology of aging. 2000;21(1):49–55. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00096-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Khachaturian AS, Zandi PP, Lyketsos CG, et al. Antihypertensive medication use and incident Alzheimer disease: the Cache County Study. Archives of neurology. 2006;63(5):686–92. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.5.noc60013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ohrui T, Matsui T, Yamaya M, et al. Angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors and incidence of Alzheimer’s disease in Japan. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2004;52(4):649–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52178_7.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.in’t Veld BA, Ruitenberg A, Hofman A, et al. Antihypertensive drugs and incidence of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Neurobiology of aging. 2001;22(3):407–12. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00241-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7(3):280–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Weber MT, Mapstone M, Staskiewicz J, Maki PM. Reconciling subjective memory complaints with objective memory performance in the menopausal transition. Menopause (New York, NY. 2012;19(7):735–41. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e318241fd22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Maric-Bilkan C, Manigrasso MB. Sex differences in hypertension: contribution of the Renin-Angiotensin system. Gender medicine. 2012;9(4):287–91. doi: 10.1016/j.genm.2012.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jefferson AL. Cardiac output as a potential risk factor for abnormal brain aging. J Alzheimers Dis. 20(3):813–21. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Laakso MP, et al. Midlife vascular risk factors and Alzheimer’s disease in later life: longitudinal, population based study. BMJ (Clinical research ed. 2001;322(7300):1447–51. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7300.1447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wharton W, Hirshman E, Merritt P, et al. Lower blood pressure correlates with poorer performance on visuospatial attention tasks in younger individuals. Biological psychology. 2006;73(3):227–34. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Knecht S, Wersching H, Lohmann H, et al. High-normal blood pressure is associated with poor cognitive performance. Hypertension. 2008;51(3):663–8. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.105577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Elias PK, Elias MF, Robbins MA, Budge MM. Blood pressure-related cognitive decline: does age make a difference? Hypertension. 2004;44(5):631–6. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000145858.07252.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Harman SM, Brinton EA, Cedars M, et al. KEEPS: The Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study. Climacteric. 2005;8(1):3–12. doi: 10.1080/13697130500042417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Miller VM, Black DM, Brinton EA, et al. Using basic science to design a clinical trial: baseline characteristics of women enrolled in the Kronos Early Estrogen Prevention Study (KEEPS) J Cardiovasc Transl Res. 2009;2(3):228–39. doi: 10.1007/s12265-009-9104-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hodis HN, Mack WJ, Lobo RA, et al. Estrogen in the prevention of atherosclerosis. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Annals of internal medicine. 2001;135(11):939–53. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-135-11-200112040-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Budoff MJ, Chen GP, Hunter CJ, et al. Effects of hormone replacement on progression of coronary calcium as measured by electron beam tomography. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 2005;14(5):410–7. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2005.14.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 1987;48(8):314–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Wilson JR, Cockburn J, Baddeley A. The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. Thames Valley Test Company; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kluger A, Ferris SH, Golomb J, et al. Neuropsychological prediction of decline to dementia in nondemented elderly. Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology. 1999;12(4):168–79. doi: 10.1177/089198879901200402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Golden C. The Stroop Color and Word Test: A manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago: Stoetling; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scales. 3. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wechsler D. WAIS-R Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III. 3. New York, N.Y: Psychological Corporation; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California Verbal Learning Test-II. 2. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Benton A. Revised Visual Retention Test Manual. New York: Psychological Corp; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Niu W, Qi Y, Qian Y, et al. The relationship between apolipoprotein E epsilon2/epsilon3/epsilon4 polymorphisms and hypertension: a meta-analysis of six studies comprising 1812 cases and 1762 controls. Hypertens Res. 2009;32(12):1060–6. doi: 10.1038/hr.2009.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261(5123):921–3. doi: 10.1126/science.8346443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen KA, Long JS, editors. Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park: Sage Publications; 1993. pp. 136–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hochberg Y. A Sharper Bonferroni Procedure for Multiple Significance Testing. Biometrika. 1988;75:800–3. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brady CB, Spiro A, 3rd, Gaziano JM. Effects of age and hypertension status on cognition: the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(6):770–7. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, et al. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Jama. 1995;274(23):1846–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Elias MF, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB, et al. Untreated blood pressure level is inversely related to cognitive functioning: the Framingham Study. American journal of epidemiology. 1993;138(6):353–64. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Skoog I, Lernfelt B, Landahl S, et al. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and dementia. Lancet. 1996;347(9009):1141–5. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90608-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Goldstein FC, Ashley AV, Freedman LJ, et al. Hypertension and cognitive performance in African Americans with Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2005;64(5):899–901. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152888.26576.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Madden DJ, Blumenthal JA. Interaction of hypertension and age in visual selective attention performance. Health Psychol. 1998;17(1):76–83. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.17.1.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Elias PK, Elias MF, D’Agostino RB, et al. NIDDM and blood pressure as risk factors for poor cognitive performance. The Framingham Study. Diabetes care. 1997;20(9):1388–95. doi: 10.2337/diacare.20.9.1388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kovacs KR, Szekeres CC, Bajko Z, et al. Cerebro- and cardiovascular reactivity and neuropsychological performance in hypertensive patients. J Neurol Sci. 299(1–2):120–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2010.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Blumenthal JA, Madden DJ, Pierce TW, et al. Hypertension affects neurobehavioral functioning. Psychosomatic medicine. 1993;55(1):44–50. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199301000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Peng N, Clark JT, Prasain J, et al. Antihypertensive and cognitive effects of grape polyphenols in estrogen-depleted, female, spontaneously hypertensive rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2005;289(3):R771–5. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00147.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lin J, Kroenke CH, Epel E, et al. Greater endogenous estrogen exposure is associated with longer telomeres in postmenopausal women at risk for cognitive decline. Brain research. 2011;1379:224–31. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Fujiwara Y, Takahashi M, Tanaka M, et al. Relationships between plasma beta-amyloid peptide 1-42 and atherosclerotic risk factors in community-based older populations. Gerontology. 2003;49(6):374–9. doi: 10.1159/000073765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Korf ES, White LR, Scheltens P, Launer LJ. Midlife blood pressure and the risk of hippocampal atrophy: the Honolulu Asia Aging Study. Hypertension. 2004;44(1):29–34. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000132475.32317.bb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Staessen JA, Fagard R, Thijs L, et al. Randomised double-blind comparison of placebo and active treatment for older patients with isolated systolic hypertension. The Systolic Hypertension in Europe (Syst-Eur) Trial Investigators. Lancet. 1997;350(9080):757–64. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(97)05381-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wang J, Ho L, Chen L, et al. Valsartan lowers brain beta-amyloid protein levels and improves spatial learning in a mouse model of Alzheimer disease. The Journal of clinical investigation. 2007;117(11):3393–402. doi: 10.1172/JCI31547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lojkowska W, Ryglewicz D, Jedrzejczak T, et al. The effect of cholinesterase inhibitors on the regional blood flow in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. J Neurol Sci. 2003;216(1):119–26. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(03)00229-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Shumaker SA, Legault C, Kuller L, et al. Conjugated equine estrogens and incidence of probable dementia and mild cognitive impairment in postmenopausal women: Women’s Health Initiative Memory Study. Jama. 2004;291(24):2947–58. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES