Abstract
Schizotypy refers to a set of personality traits thought to reflect the subclinical expression of the signs and symptoms of schizophrenia. Here, we review the cognitive and brain functional profile associated with high questionnaire scores in schizotypy. We discuss empirical evidence from the domains of perception, attention, memory, imagery and representation, language, and motor control. Perceptual deficits occur early and across various modalities. While the neural mechanisms underlying visual impairments may be linked to magnocellular dysfunction, further effects may be seen downstream in higher cognitive functions. Cognitive deficits are observed in inhibitory control, selective and sustained attention, incidental learning, and memory. In concordance with the cognitive nature of many of the aberrations of schizotypy, higher levels of schizotypy are associated with enhanced vividness and better performance on tasks of mental rotation. Language deficits seem most pronounced in higher-level processes. Finally, higher levels of schizotypy are associated with reduced performance on oculomotor tasks, resembling the impairments seen in schizophrenia. Some of these deficits are accompanied by reduced brain activation, akin to the pattern of hypoactivations in schizophrenia spectrum individuals. We conclude that schizotypy is a construct with apparent phenomenological overlap with schizophrenia and stable interindividual differences that covary with performance on a wide range of perceptual, cognitive, and motor tasks known to be impaired in schizophrenia. The importance of these findings lies not only in providing a fine-grained neurocognitive characterization of a personality constellation known to be associated with real-life impairments, but also in generating hypotheses concerning the aetiology of schizophrenia.
Key words: schizophrenia, personality, perception, attention, memory, imagery, language, movement
Introduction
Schizotypy refers to a constellation of personality traits thought to reflect the subclinical expression of schizophrenia in the general population.1–3 Schizotypy encompasses behaviors, cognitions, and emotions that resemble, at a less pronounced level of expression, the disturbances characteristic of schizophrenia. Psychometric self-report measures of schizotypy have been shown to yield 3 dimensions, viz. the positive, negative, and disorganized dimensions.2 Positive schizotypy describes perceptual aberrations akin to subsyndromal hallucinations as well as unusual ideas that resemble the delusions of schizophrenia. Negative schizotypy refers to a reduction in emotional, physical, and social functions such as the experience of pleasure or interest in social contacts. The disorganized dimension involves thought disorder as well as bizarre behavior.
Although schizotypy is a risk factor for schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum disorders,4,5 only a few people with high schizotypy scores become clinically ill, and there is considerable and stable interindividual variance in schizotypy across the entire spectrum from low to high scores. With schizotypy thus straddling the domains of personality psychology and psychiatry, we argue that the study of variation in schizotypy, even in samples with low-to-medium scores, is of importance for several reasons.
First, the apparent phenomenological similarity of schizotypy with schizophrenia has implications for our understanding of the clinical condition. Specifically, schizophrenia is known not to be a binary phenotype (present/absent), suggesting the operation of multiple aetiological factors.6,7 Assuming that the resemblance between schizotypy and schizophrenia is not trivial,8 it is likely that at least partly overlapping aetiological factors underlie both phenotypes,9 making the study of similarities between the 2 an important contribution to aetiological research into schizophrenia. Additionally, an equally important but often neglected approach is the direct comparison of performance in people with high levels of schizotypy and patients with schizophrenia. Such work is critically needed to identify domains where schizotypy differs from schizophrenia, pointing to protective or compensatory mechanisms.8 see also Herzig et al.10
Second, people with high levels of schizotypy display maladaptive behaviors, such as smoking11 and drug use,12 and they suffer lower social, educational, and professional levels of functioning, impoverished quality of life, and high levels of distress13,14 (also see Cohen et al15 as well as Mohr and Claridge16). Therefore, schizotypy is an important research topic in its own right because a characterization of cognitive and neural processes in schizotypy may improve our mechanistic understanding of these disturbances and aid the development of intervention strategies.
Indeed, the study of schizotypy is of importance to clinical and nonclinical research questions. A methodological advantage of this work is the absence of confounds that plague schizophrenia patient studies, such as long-term pharmacological treatment.
The additional and complementary consideration of schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) as well as genetic and clinical high-risk detection methods such as the identification of prodromal symptoms would also be of interest in relation to cognition. However, for reasons of emphasis of this special issue, we focus on psychometrically identified schizotypy. When discussing studies on particularly high-scoring schizotypal individuals, we acknowledge that part of this population might also obtain a diagnosis of SPD. For example, Raine17 observed SPD in 55% of participants with schizotypal personality questionnaire (SPQ) scores in the top 10% of the distribution.
In the following sections, we provide a concise review of the pattern of performance in schizotypy in key domains of perception, cognition, and motor control. Wherever possible we discuss evidence of the neural correlates of these performance patterns. We particularly draw here on methods widely used in schizophrenia research, viz. electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Of course, this review is by no means exhaustive and we refer the reader to other reviews for further information.7,8,18,19 Following our review of these key findings we will discuss implications of this work for the study of schizophrenia.
Cognition and Brain Function in Schizotypy
Perception
At the core of schizotypy are unusual perceptual experiences across all sensory modalities (visual, auditory, olfactory, and somatosensory). Visual abnormalities in relation to schizotypy are already evident at early stages of processing, beginning with abnormal P120,21, backward-masking,22 localization,23 and depth perception.24 Schizotypy has also been associated with pervasive problems of perceptual organization.25,26
The neural mechanism underlying visual impairments in schizotypy has been linked to magnocellular dysfunction.23 In addition, it has also been suggested to be linked to abnormal network coordination,27 especially synchronized oscillatory activity in schizotypy that measures the precision and degree to which neurons are recruited to a common network to perform specific visual and higher cognitive tasks.28 Interestingly, there are also associations of schizotypy with blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal in perceptual networks during rest. A study of young individuals found positive correlations of SPQ positive, negative, and disorganized dimensions with functional connectivity in a visual network and negative correlations of the SPQ disorganized dimension with an auditory network.29 These findings suggest that functional connectivity in perceptual regions is related to schizotypy even in the absence of an active task, perhaps reflecting processes of imagery or social cognition.
Relatedly, it is important to note that “early” visual processing dysfunction may cascade to all stages of information processing with detrimental consequences. For example, abnormal P1, backward-masking, localization and so forth prevent high-fidelity encoding of stimulus and can lead to working memory (WM) problems.20,30 However, healthy individuals with elevated schizotypal traits are able to access compensatory mechanisms to achieve intact performance across a wide range of tasks.20
Impaired early visual processing has been associated with overall elevated schizotypal traits,20 but also more specifically with the level of cognitive disorganization22,31 and with the positive syndrome.23
Sensory processing abnormalities are also consistently observed in the auditory domain. Auditory imprecision29 and reduced sensory gating are associated with increased schizotypy,33 parallel to the findings of auditory imprecision34 and abolished P50 in schizophrenia patients.35 However, auditory deficits in schizotypy tend to be subtle. For example, increased schizotypy is associated with reduced P3b36,37 and N237 amplitudes, but not with other event-related potentials such as N1, P2, and P3a.36 Similar to the case of visual abnormalities, early auditory impairments can lead to broader consequences.38
There is also evidence of deficits in olfactory and somatosensory modalities, although these have not been as extensively examined, and olfactory deficits appear less clear than in visual or auditory perception. Elevated schizotypal traits have been associated with olfactory identification39 and detection40 but not with olfactory acuity.39,41 With respect to somatosensory and tactile functions, high schizotypy has been linked to higher 2-point discrimination threshold, with implications for compromised parietal functioning.42
Taken together, perceptual anomalies in relation to schizotypy are transmodal and persistent, with broad consequences for all aspects of behavior, as precise and efficient perceptual information processing lies at the heart of intact interaction with the external world.20 Interestingly, despite evidence of early visual processing deficits, performance on a number of visuospatial tasks is essentially normal in high schizotypy,43 suggesting operation of compensatory strategies. These strategies can take the form of presenting the material for longer20 or the use of attention to enhance the temporal precision of the information processing44,45; however, this needs testing in schizotypy.
In addition to early perceptual impairments, there is also an abundance of research showing impairments in higher cognitive processing in schizotypy, which we review in the next sections.
Attention and Memory
Selective attention to a given stimulus involves both focusing awareness on the target and inhibiting internal representations of the competing distractor. Indeed, inhibitory failures have been identified as a core feature of schizophrenia since Bleuler.46 Individuals with schizophrenia and psychometrically identified individuals with high levels of schizotypy have also been shown to display sustained attention deficits. Individuals characterized by positive schizotypy47–50 and those characterized by negative schizotypy50 display poorer signal discriminability, as indexed by lower d' scores.
A more direct measure of inhibitory control is negative priming (NP). In a NP task, 2 stimuli are presented during the prime trial and the participant must focus on 1 and ignore the other. If on the subsequent trial, the previously ignored distractor is used as the new target, processing of the stimulus requiring the previously inhibited representation will be impaired, resulting in an increased reaction time; this is known as NP. Individuals with acute schizophrenia display reduced NP, indicating disinhibition.51–53 Importantly, it was shown that this deficit in schizophrenia is not due to methodological artefacts but instead represents a genuine inhibitory problem.51 With regards to schizotypy, elevated scores on the Perceptual Aberration scale54 were found to be associated with reduced NP on a spatial task.52 In another study,55 reduced NP on verbal tasks was observed in relation to higher scores on the Perceptual Aberration scale,54 Predisposition to Hallucinations scale,56 social anhedonia,57 and hypomania.58 Regression analyses in that study, however, showed that it was primarily a positive schizotypy factor that was related to NP.55
An additional paradigm that has been widely used in schizophrenia and schizotypy research is latent inhibition (LI). In LI, exposure to an irrelevant stimulus prevents conditioning with the stimulus being formed at a later time. A well-replicated finding is that unmedicated schizophrenia patients with positive symptoms display reduced LI relative to healthy controls.59 Converging evidence based on investigations using verbal tasks,57 auditory tasks,61 and visual search tasks62 indicate that participants with higher scores on measures of positive schizotypy show a reduced LI effect.60,61,63
Impairments in WM, the active storage, manipulation, and selection of responses to guide subsequent behavior, have emerged as one of the cardinal features of schizophrenia.64–66 Indeed, WM is considered a potential endophenotypic marker for the disorder. More subtle WM impairments have been reported in relation to elevated levels of positive67–69 as well as negative68,70,71 schizotypy. As mentioned earlier, WM deficits may reflect reduced down-stream accuracy of information processing that can already be indexed by early potentials.20,72 Furthermore, WM deficits in schizotypy may also explain some of the problems in daily functioning.
Other aspects of memory performance may also be adversely associated with elevated levels of self-reported schizotypal traits. In incidental learning, individuals are tested on the degree of the attention devoted to nonrelevant stimuli by measuring how much of the nonattended material they recall. Regardless of the nature of the information assessed (ie, spatial or verbal), individuals with higher scores on measures of positive schizotypy had higher rates of incidental learning than individuals with lower schizotypy scores.73,74 In addition, participants with elevated levels of positive schizotypy show an absence of the expected enhancement of emotional memory.75 Finally, while LaPorte et al76 found no relationship between levels of positive schizotypy and verbal memory performance, Gooding and Braun77 observed an inverse relationship between negative schizotypy and nonverbal memory performance.
Research has also shown that attentional deficits as a function of (mainly positive) schizotypy might be biased in space, pointing to imbalances in hemispheric asymmetry with a potentially stronger right over left hemisphere activation.78 Individuals high in positive schizotypy show an attentional bias for small-scale (line bisection) and large-scale (spontaneous full-body navigation) spatial attention that is directed away from the right hemispace, reminiscent of the bias reported in patients with schizophrenia.78 Neuroimaging evidence for these aberrant laterality patterns have been published recently.79,80
In summary, there is plenty of evidence showing that attention (selection and inhibition), WM and memory is reduced in schizotypy. There is evidence for both deficits in individuals with psychometrically identified schizotypy relative to individuals with low levels of schizotypal traits, as well as evidence for associations between specific deficits and specific schizotypal dimensions.
Imagery and Representation
Mental imagery may be construed as the process of active generation, inspection, and manipulation or transformation of internal representations.81 Mental imagery is also related to hallucinations but with a key difference in the perceived locus of agency.82 Enhanced subjective vividness of mental imagery has been reported in individuals with schizophrenia and elevated schizotypal traits in visual83 and auditory84 modalities. However, the link between vivid imagery experiences and hallucinations is weak or unclear.85,86
In addition to vividness of mental imagery, some aspects of mental imagery manipulation appear enhanced in schizotypy. Mental rotation paradigms, which require mentally rotating a stimulus into a particular orientation relative to its own reference frame (allocentric mental rotation) or mentally rotating oneself into a particular orientation relative to the surrounding environment (egocentric mental rotation), allow for the parametric investigation of manipulation of internal representations. Both egocentric and allocentric mental rotation abilities have been investigated in relation to schizotypy. Faster allocentric mental rotation was associated with increased schizotypy87 and more specifically, negative schizotypy in women.88 Findings are mixed with respect to self-other transformation involving egocentric mental rotation. Both impaired,89 and enhanced87,88,90 egocentric mental rotation performance have been reported in relation to elevated schizotypy. These findings have implications for social cognitive abilities (see Cohen et al15), notably theory-of-mind because one crucial aspect of mentalizing involves visuospatial perspective taking and simulation.91 Although enhanced egocentric perspective-taking ability may be associated with increased empathy,88 in particular in women91 and as a function of life experience,93 excessive perspective-taking could lead to reduced agency and dissociative experiences.
Language Production
Abnormalities of language are also seen in schizotypy, especially in higher-level language processing. Differences in production94,95 and interpretation96 of prosody have been reported. These deficits should not be surprising given that thought disorder is a defining feature of schizophrenia and schizotypy.27,97,98
Besides odd speech and thought disorder, individuals with elevated schizotypy show differences in correct production and interpretation of nonliteral language such as metaphors,99 irony,80,99,100 fauxpas,101 and proverbs,102 similar to schizophrenia and autism. While nonliteral language comprehension problems seem correlated with schizotypy across the spectrum,80,102 the association in nonclinical schizotypy is subtle103 or even controversial,100,104 but still detectable on the neural level.105 A number of mechanisms may contribute to these deficits,106 including semantic association differences,107–109 aberrant semantic priming mechanisms,106 and inadequate context integration.110,111
Most likely both cerebral hemispheres contribute to language differences associated with elevated schizotypy,80,105 with the right hemisphere thought to play an important role in loosening of associations and pragmatic language deficits in schizotypy.99,103 Left fronto-temporal language network abnormalities may also explain language differences in schizotypy. Siever and Davis112 proposed that lateral temporal lobe deficits exist in both schizophrenia and SPD but are compensated by greater frontal capacity in schizotypy. This model is supported by a recent fMRI study in psychometric schizotypy.105 During comprehension of ironic remarks, healthy individuals with elevated schizotypy scores showed decreased bilateral temporal, but increased left prefrontal activation.105 A defective left hemisphere language system would also be compatible with the observation of N400 abnormalities108 and other functional neuroimaging evidence indicating reduced language lateralization. Yet, the consistency of such findings113 and their psychopharmacological mediation114 remain debated (see also Mohr and Claridge,16 on the link between language and creativity).
Motor Control
Individual differences in schizotypy have also been shown to be associated with alterations in the control of movements. Increased frequencies of neurological soft signs, considered to be endophenotypes of schizophrenia,115 have been reported in relation to schizotypy.116,117 Abnormalities in gait118 and reduced precision of manual motor control119 have also been found. Given the considerable knowledge base concerning motor control in the healthy brain, these findings can be used to gain insight into the neural alterations that underlie schizotypy. Specifically, neurological soft signs have in structural and functional neuroimaging studies been shown to tap a circuitry of cerebello-thalamo-prefrontal abnormalities.120
Interestingly, studies of motor control may also have implications for our understanding of impairments in cognitive and social functioning in relation to schizotypy, given that movement perception and generation are often inextricably linked with communicative and cognitive processes. For example, a recent fMRI study observed that schizotypy is associated with neural alterations within the mirror neuron network during the imitation of actions: People with higher negative schizotypy scores (on the interpersonal subscale of the SPQ) tended to show greater BOLD signal in inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) during imitative action, suggesting they exerted greater effort to perform a simple motor imitation task.121 Given that the IFG is thought to code the action goal within the mirror neuron network, these findings point to the social component of motor control and inform both our understanding of brain functional changes in schizotypy and the role of clinically relevant personality traits in explaining inter-individual differences in such processes (for more detail on social processing in schizotypy, see Cohen et al15).
In addition to manual motor control, a considerable body of evidence points to relatively subtle yet replicable impairments in oculomotor function in relation to high schizotypy. These studies originated in the observations of impairments in schizophrenia on a range of eye movement paradigms, including most prominently the smooth pursuit and antisaccade tasks.122,123 Extending this work, there is now considerable evidence of associations of higher schizotypy with impaired performance on smooth pursuit (following a slowly moving target)124–127 and antisaccades (making a rapid eye movement in the direction opposite to a sudden-onset peripheral target).127–129
Pursuit deficits, which have been observed in both positive and negative schizotypy,125,126 involve a reduced ability to match eye velocity to target velocity. It has also been shown that the association between pursuit performance and schizotypy is observed particularly when pursuit eye movements are based on predictive processes, such as during target occlusion,130 similar to deficits previously described in schizophrenia.131 A recent fMRI study132 provided first evidence for the neural alterations underlying the pursuit deficit in schizotypy. It was observed that individuals selected for high total scores of the O-LIFE short scale133 showed lower BOLD signal than controls with low O-LIFE scores in occipital areas that are known to be associated with early sensory and attentional processing as well as motion perception (V3A, middle occipital gyrus, and fusiform gyrus). This finding is compatible with evidence of motion processing deficits during smooth pursuit in schizophrenia patients.122,134
Higher levels of both positive and negative schizotypy are also associated with an increased rate of direction errors on the antisaccade task.126–129,135 Recently, an fMRI study investigated the neural underpinnings of this deficit in relation to positive schizotypy. It was found that higher positive schizotypy scores136 were associated with reduced BOLD signal in posterior and subcortical areas such as putamen, thalamus, cerebellum, and visual cortex,137 similar to what is seen in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives.138–140 Interestingly, the reductions in activation in frontal areas that are also observed in some schizophrenia studies141,142 were not found. Neuroimaging evidence thus points to both shared networks of alteration in schizotypy and schizophrenia and differences.
Together these studies document subtle impairments in schizotypy, similar to the deficits in schizophrenia. Consideration of the neural level of explanation further extends the conclusions concerning overlap between schizophrenia and schizotypy: In neuroimaging studies, high schizotypy has been found to be associated with altered neural functioning, ie, reductions during antisaccades137 and pursuit143 and increases during motor imitation.121 Importantly, these studies also point to the sparing of some neural functions, such as the lack of frontal changes during antisaccades137 and pursuit143 that are observed in schizophrenia.142 While it must be cautioned that the latter data do not come from direct comparisons but rather from independent studies, it may be argued that the combined assessment of oculomotor and neural data allows the delineation of (neuro-cognitive) functions that may be spared in schizotypy, suggesting the operation of protective factors.
Implications
The reviewed literature paints a picture of relatively subtle yet widespread performance impairments in perceptual, cognitive, and motor functions, with notable exceptions of spared or improved functions such as imagery or allocentric mental rotation. A methodological strength of this work is the absence of secondary confounds that cloud experimental neurocognitive assessments in schizophrenia, such as pharmacological treatment or chronicity effects.
As mentioned earlier, a thorough description of the pattern of neurocognitive alterations in high schizotypy will help address with more confidence the question of the existence and nature of a possible continuum (or overlap) between schizotypy and schizophrenia.7 At the phenotypic level alone, this issue has not been resolved.144 Comparisons between high schizotypy and schizophrenia drawing upon neurocognitive data, however, may help resolve this question by yielding different possible scenarios. For example, are all the neurocognitive impairments of schizophrenia also seen in schizotypy, but at lower levels? Alternatively, are only some of the impairments of schizophrenia seen in schizotypy? A detailed examination of how the pattern of impairments in schizotypy compares to the impairments in schizophrenia will give us clues about the nature of the relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia. Knowledge of these different kinds of patterns could significantly inform our understanding of the distribution of schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes across the population, without resorting to unspecified notions of a “continuum” between schizotypy and schizophrenia. Critically, direct comparisons across the entire spectrum, ie, low schizotypes, high schizotypes, and schizophrenia patients, are needed to address this question using the same tasks and paradigms. Research into the nature of the similarities and differences between schizophrenia and nonclinical schizotypy may also inform future development and validation of diagnoses for early clinical detection, such as the attenuated psychosis syndrome (APS).145
Second, recent work suggests that the study of cognition and brain function in schizotypy may inform the development of experimental medicine model systems of schizophrenia with implications for drug development. The incomplete effectiveness of antipsychotic and procognitive drugs represent an area of clear unmet need in schizophrenia and the development of new compounds is likely to benefit from model systems with well validated biomarkers.146,147 Applying the high schizotypy approach in an exploration of the validity of this model system it was recently shown that (1) WM impairments in high schizotypy were partly alleviated by amisulpride148 and (2) risperidone impaired antisaccade performance in medium schizotypal controls, whereas high schizotypals showed a numeric trend towards improvement.149 These recent data suggest that it may be possible to apply the schizotypy approach in drug development.19
The methodological advantages of a schizotypy model system include the ready availability of participants that are recruited using relatively inexpensive, reliable and objective psychometric questionnaires. Importantly, schizotypy differs from other models of schizophrenia, such as ketamine or sleep deprivation, in that it may represent a better model of the neurodevelopmental aspects of schizophrenia, similar perhaps to the isolation rearing rodent model.150
Conclusions
In conclusion, this review has identified a number of perceptual, cognitive, and motor functions that deteriorate in relation to higher schizotypy. However, while there is generally consistent evidence of impairments in certain perceptual-motor abnormalities, there is considerable cross-study variability in higher cognitive deficits such as WM or top-down attentional control.43 Therefore, it is intriguing to speculate that compensatory mechanisms may be at play at higher levels of cognitive function. These may be detected as neurally inefficient activations in neuroimaging studies.151 It is also important to point out that the objective cognitive deficits described here may be relatively subtle and occur on the background of much more severe subjective cognitive complaints.43
Further research is needed, especially to identify not only domains of impairment in high compared with low schizotypy, but also to characterize areas of function that may be spared in high schizotypy compared to schizophrenia. Such work will not only further elucidate the cognitive processing pattern of individuals with high expressions of this personality constellation, but will also inform aetiological theories of schizophrenia.
References
- 1. Claridge G. Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford, UK: OUP; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Raine A. Schizotypal personality: neurodevelopmental and psychosocial trajectories. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2006; 2:291–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Lenzenweger MF. Schizotypy and Schizophrenia: The View from Experimental Psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Gooding DC, Tallent KA, Matts CW. Clinical status of at-risk individuals 5 years later: further validation of the psychometric high-risk strategy. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:170–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Kwapil TR, Eckblad M, Zinser MC. Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:171–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. David AS. Why we need more debate on whether psychotic symptoms lie on a continuum with normality. Psychol Med. 2010;40:1935–1942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:317–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Ettinger U, Meyhöfer I, Steffens M, Wagner M, Koutsouleris N. Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Fanous AH, Neale MC, Gardner CO, et al. Significant correlation in linkage signals from genome-wide scans of schizophrenia and schizotypy. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:958–965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Herzig DA, Sullivan S, Lewis G, et al. Hemispheric Language Asymmetry in First Episode Psychosis and Schizotypy: The Role of Cannabis Consumption and Cognitive Disorganization. Schizophr Bull. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Williams JH, Wellman NA, Allan LM, et al. Tobacco smoking correlates with schizotypal and borderline personality traits. Pers Indiv Differ. 1996;20:267–270. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Williams JH, Wellman NA, Rawlins JN. Cannabis use correlates with schizotypy in healthy people. Addiction. 1996;91:869–877. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Barrantes-Vidal N, Lewandowski KE, Kwapil TR. Psychopathology, social adjustment and personality correlates of schizotypy clusters in a large nonclinical sample. Schizophr Res. 2010;122:219–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Cohen AS, Davis TE., III Quality of life across the schizotypy spectrum: findings from a large nonclinical adult sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2009;50:408–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Cohen AS, Park S, Mohr C, Ettinger U, Chan RCK, 2015. Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophr Bull. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Mohr C, Claridge G, 2015. Schizotypy—do not worry, it is not all worrisome. Schizophr Bull. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17:555–564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Giakoumaki SG. Cognitive and prepulse inhibition deficits in psychometrically high schizotypal subjects in the general population: relevance to schizophrenia research. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18:643–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Mohr C, Ettinger U. An overview of the association between schizotypy and dopamine. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Koychev I, El-Deredy W, Haenschel C, Deakin JF. Visual information processing deficits as biomarkers of vulnerability to schizophrenia: an event-related potential study in schizotypy. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2205–2214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bedwell JS, Chan CC, Trachik BJ, Rassovsky Y. Changes in the visual-evoked P1 potential as a function of schizotypy and background color in healthy young adults. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:542–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Cappe C, Herzog MH, Herzig DA, Brand A, Mohr C. Cognitive disorganisation in schizotypy is associated with deterioration in visual backward masking. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:652–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Richardson AJ, Gruzelier J. Visual processing, lateralization and syndromes of schizotypy. Int J Psychophysiol. 1994;18:227–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Barbato M, Collinson SL, Casagrande M. Altered depth perception is associated with presence of schizotypal personality traits. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2012;17:115–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Uhlhaas PJ, Silverstein SM. Perceptual organization in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: empirical research and theoretical implications. Psychol Bull. 2005;131:618–632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Luh KE, Gooding DC. Perceptual biases in psychosis-prone individuals. J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108:283–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Uhlhaas PJ, Silverstein SM, Phillips WA, Lovell PG. Evidence for impaired visual context processing in schizotypy with thought disorder. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:249–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Koychev I, Deakin JF, Haenschel C, El-Deredy W. Abnormal neural oscillations in schizotypy during a visual working memory task: support for a deficient top-down network? Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2866–2873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lagioia A, Van De Ville D, Debbané M, Lazeyras F, Eliez S. Adolescent resting state networks and their associations with schizotypal trait expression. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Mayer JS, Fukuda K, Vogel EK, Park S. Impaired contingent attentional capture predicts reduced working memory capacity in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012;7:e48586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Uhlhaas PJ, Phillips WA, Mitchell G, Silverstein SM. Perceptual grouping in disorganized schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2006;145:105–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bates TC. The panmodal sensory imprecision hypothesis of schizophrenia: reduced auditory precision in schizotypy. Pers Indiv Differ. 2005;38:437–449. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Croft RJ, Lee A, Bertolot J, Gruzelier JH. Associations of P50 suppression and desensitization with perceptual and cognitive features of “unreality” in schizotypy. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:441–446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Rabinowicz EF, Silipo G, Goldman R, Javitt DC. Auditory sensory dysfunction in schizophrenia: imprecision or distractibility? Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1149–1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Potter D, Summerfelt A, Gold J, Buchanan RW. Review of clinical correlates of P50 sensory gating abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:692–700. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Klein C, Berg P, Rockstroh B, Andresen B. Topography of the auditory P300 in schizotypal personality. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:1612–1621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Nuchpongsai P, Arakaki H, Langman P, Ogura C. N2 and P3b components of the event-related potential in students at risk for psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 1999;88:131–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Javitt DC. When doors of perception close: bottom-up models of disrupted cognition in schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2009;5:249–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Park S, Schoppe S. Olfactory identification deficit in relation to schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 1997;26:191–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Mohr C, Röhrenbach CM, Laska M, Brugger P. Unilateral olfactory perception and magical ideation. Schizophr Res. 2001;47:255–264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Mohr C, Hübener F, Laska M. Deviant olfactory experiences, magical ideation, and olfactory sensitivity: a study with healthy German and Japanese subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2002;111:21–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Lenzenweger MF. Two-point discrimination thresholds and schizotypy: illuminating a somatosensory dysfunction. Schizophr Res. 2000;42:111–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Chun CA, Minor KS, Cohen AS. Neurocognition in psychometrically defined college Schizotypy samples: we are not measuring the “right stuff.” J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19:324–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Gold JM, Fuller RL, Robinson BM, McMahon RP, Braun EL, Luck SJ. Intact attentional control of working memory encoding in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:658–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Haenschel C, Linden D. Exploring intermediate phenotypes with EEG: working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res. 2011;216:481–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York: International Universities Press; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Chen WJ, Hsiao CK, Lin CC. Schizotypy in community samples: the three-factor structure and correlation with sustained attention. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:649–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Lenzenweger MF, Cornblatt BA, Putnick M. Schizotypy and sustained attention. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100:84–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Obiols JE, Garcia-Domingo M, de Trincheria I, Domenech E. Psychometric schizotypy and sustained attention in young males. Pers Indiv Differ. 1993;14:381–384. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Gooding DC, Matts CW, Rollmann EA. Sustained attention deficits in relation to psychometrically identified schizotypy: evaluating a potential endophenotypic marker. Schizophr Res. 2006;82:27–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. MacQueen GM, Galway T, Goldberg JO, Tipper SP. Impaired distractor inhibition in patients with schizophrenia on a negative priming task. Psychol Med. 2003;33:121–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Park S, Lenzenweger MF, Püschel J, Holzman PS. Attentional inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a spatial negative priming study. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 1996;1:125–149. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Minas RK, Park S. Attentional window in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality: insight from negative priming studies. Appl Prev Psychol. 2007;12:140–148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Raulin ML. Body-image aberration in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1978;87:399–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Peters ER, Pickering AD, Hemsley DR. ‘Cognitive inhibition’ and positive symptomatology in schizotypy. Br J Clin Psychol. 1994;33(pt 1):33–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Launay G, Slade P. The measurement of hallucinatory predisposition in male and female prisoners. Pers Indiv Differ. 1981;2:221–234. [Google Scholar]
- 57. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP, Raulin ML. Scales for physical and social anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1976;85:374–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Eckblad M, Chapman LJ. Development and validation of a scale for hypomanic personality. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986;95:214–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Kumari V, Ettinger U. Latent inhibition in schizophrenia and schizotypy: a review of the empirical literature. In: Lubow RE, ed. Latent Inhibition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2010:419–447. [Google Scholar]
- 60. Gray NS, Fernandez M, Williams J, Ruddle RA, Snowden RJ. Which schizotypal dimensions abolish latent inhibition? Br J Clin Psychol. 2002;41:271–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Gray NS, Snowden RJ, Peoples M, Hemsley DR, Gray JA. A demonstration of within-subjects latent inhibition in the human: limitations and advantages. Behav Brain Res. 2003;138:1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Kaplan O, Lubow RE. Ignoring irrelevant stimuli in latent inhibition and Stroop paradigms: the effects of schizotypy and gender. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186:40–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Evans LH, Gray NS, Snowden RJ. A new continuous within-participants latent inhibition task: examining associations with schizotypy dimensions, smoking status and gender. Biol Psychol. 2007;74:365–373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Forbes NF, Carrick LA, McIntosh AM, Lawrie SM. Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2009;39:889–905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Park S, Holzman PS. Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:975–982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Lee J, Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:599–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Park S, Holzman PS, Lenzenweger MF. Individual differences in spatial working memory in relation to schizotypy. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104:355–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Tallent KA, Gooding DC. Working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizotypic individuals: a replication and extension. Psychiatry Res. 1999;89:161–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Schmidt-Hansen M, Honey RC. Working memory and multidimensional schizotypy: dissociable influences of the different dimensions. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2009;26:655–670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Park S, McTigue K. Working memory and the syndromes of schizotypal personality. Schizophr Res. 1997;26:213–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Gooding DC, Tallent KA. Spatial, object, and affective working memory in social anhedonia: an exploratory study. Schizophr Res. 2003;63:247–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Haenschel C, Bittner RA, Haertling F, et al. Contribution of impaired early-stage visual processing to working memory dysfunction in adolescents with schizophrenia: a study with event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1229–1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Jones SH, Gray JA, Hemsley DR. The Kamin blocking effect, incidental learning and schizotypy (a reanalysis). Pers Indiv Differ. 1991;13:57–60. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Burch GSJ, Hemsley DR, Corr PJ, Gwyer P. The relationship between incidental learning and multi-dimensional schizotypy as measured by the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences (O-LIFE). Pers Indiv Differ. 2006;40:394. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Hoshi R, Scoales M, Mason O, Kamboj SK. Schizotypy and emotional memory. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42:504–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. LaPorte DJ, Kirkpatrick B, Thaker GK. Psychosis-proneness and verbal memory in a college student population. Schizophr Res. 1994;12:237–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Gooding DC, Braun JG. Visuoconstructive performance, implicit hemispatial inattention, and schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:261–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Liouta E, Smith AD, Mohr C. Schizotypy and pseudoneglect: a critical update on theories of hemispheric asymmetries. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2008;13:112–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Folley BS, Park S. Verbal creativity and schizotypal personality in relation to prefrontal hemispheric laterality: a behavioral and near-infrared optical imaging study. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:271–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Rapp AM, Langohr K, Mutschler DE, Klingberg S, Wild B, Erb M. Isn’t it ironic? Neural correlates of irony comprehension in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2013;8:e74224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Ganis G. The Case for Mental Imagery. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 82. Linden DE, Thornton K, Kuswanto CN, Johnston SJ, van de Ven V, Jackson MC. The brain’s voices: comparing nonclinical auditory hallucinations and imagery. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:330–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Oertel V, Rotarska-Jagiela A, van de Ven V, et al. Mental imagery vividness as a trait marker across the schizophrenia spectrum. Psychiatry Res. 2009;167:1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Beaman CP, Williams TI. Individual differences in mental control predict involuntary musical imagery. Music Sci. 2013;17:398–409. [Google Scholar]
- 85. Aleman A, Böcker KBE, De Haan E.H.F. Disposition towards hallucinations and subjective versus objective vividness of imagery in normal subjects. Pers Indiv Differ. 1999;27:707–714. [Google Scholar]
- 86. van de Ven V, Merckelbach H. The role of schizotypy, mental imagery, and fantasy proneness in hallucinatory reports of undergraduate students. Pers Indiv Differ. 2003;35:889–896. [Google Scholar]
- 87. Steinisch M, Sulpizio V, Iorio AA, et al. A virtual environment for egocentric and allocentric mental transformations: a study on a nonclinical population of adults with distinct levels of schizotypy. Biomed Tech (Berl). 2011;56:291–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Thakkar KN, Park S. Empathy, schizotypy, and visuospatial transformations. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2010;15:477–500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Mohr C, Blanke O, Brugger P. Perceptual aberrations impair mental own-body transformations. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:528–534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Langdon R, Coltheart M. Visual perspective-taking and schizotypy: evidence for a simulation-based account of mentalizing in normal adults. Cognition. 2001;82:1–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Park S, Matthews N, Gibson C. Imitation, simulation, and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:698–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Mohr C, Rowe AC, Blanke O. The influence of sex and empathy on putting oneself in the shoes of others. Br J Psychol. 2010;101:277–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Mohr C, Rowe AC, Kurokawa I, Dendy L, Theodoridou A. Bodily perspective taking goes social: the role of personal, interpersonal, and intercultural factors. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2013;43:1369–1381. [Google Scholar]
- 94. Cohen AS, Hong SL. Understanding constricted affect in schizotypy through computerized prosodic analysis. J Pers Disord. 2011;25:478–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Dickey CC, Vu MA, Voglmaier MM, Niznikiewicz MA, McCarley RW, Panych LP. Prosodic abnormalities in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res. 2012;142:20–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Dickey CC, Morocz IA, Minney D, et al. Factors in sensory processing of prosody in schizotypal personality disorder: an fMRI experiment. Schizophr Res. 2010;121:75–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Gooding DC, Tallent KA, Hegyi JV. Cognitive slippage in schizotypic individuals. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189:750–756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Coleman MJ, Levy DL, Lenzenweger MF, Holzman PS. Thought disorder, perceptual aberrations, and schizotypy. J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105:469–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Langdon R, Coltheart M. Recognition of metaphor and irony in young adults: the impact of schizotypal personality traits. Psychiatry Res. 2004;125:9–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Jahshan CS, Sergi MJ. Theory of mind, neurocognition, and functional status in schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 2007;89:278–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Morrison SC, Brown LA, Cohen AS. A multidimensional assessment of social cognition in psychometrically defined schizotypy. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:1014–1019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Rapp AM, Langohr K, Mutschler DE, Wild B. Irony and proverb comprehension in schizophrenia: do female patients “dislike” ironic remarks? Schizophr Res Treat. 2014;2014:841086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Nunn J, Peters E. Schizotypy and patterns of lateral asymmetry on hemisphere-specific language tasks. Psychiatry Res. 2001;103:179–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Humphrey MK, Bryson FM, Grimshaw GM. Metaphor processing in high and low schizotypal individuals. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178:290–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Rapp AM, Mutschler DE, Wild B, et al. Neural correlates of irony comprehension: the role of schizotypal personality traits. Brain Lang. 2010;113:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Kiang M. Schizotypy and language: a review. J Neurolinguist. 2010;23:193–203. [Google Scholar]
- 107. Manschreck TC, Merrill AM, Jabbar G, Chun J, Delisi LE. Frequency of normative word associations in the speech of individuals at familial high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;140:99–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Kostova M, Bohec AL, Blanchet A. Event-related brain potential study of expectancy and semantic matching in schizotypy. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;92:67–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Mohr C, Landis T, Brugger P. Lateralized semantic priming: modulation by levodopa, semantic distance, and participants’ magical beliefs. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2006;2:71–84. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Nelson B, Whitford TJ, Lavoie S, Sass LA. What are the neurocognitive correlates of basic self-disturbance in schizophrenia? Integrating phenomenology and neurocognition: part 2 (aberrant salience). Schizophr Res. 2014;152:20–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Nelson B, Whitford TJ, Lavoie S, Sass LA. What are the neurocognitive correlates of basic self-disturbance in schizophrenia?: integrating phenomenology and neurocognition. Part 1 (Source monitoring deficits). Schizophr Res. 2014;152:12–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Siever LJ, Davis KL. The pathophysiology of schizophrenia disorders: perspectives from the spectrum. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:398–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Schofield K, Mohr C. Schizotypy and hemispheric asymmetry: results from two Chapman scales, the O-LIFE questionnaire, and two laterality measures. Laterality. 2014;19:178–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Mohr C, Krummenacher P, Landis T, Sandor PS, Fathi M, Brugger P. Psychometric schizotypy modulates levodopa effects on lateralized lexical decision performance. J Psychiatr Res. 2005;39:241–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Chan RC, Gottesman II. Neurological soft signs as candidate endophenotypes for schizophrenia: a shooting star or a Northern star? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:957–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Obiols JE, Serrano F, Caparrós B, Subirá S, Barrantes N. Neurological soft signs in adolescents with poor performance on the continous performance test: markers of liability for schizophrenia spectrum disorders? Psychiatry Res. 1999;86:217–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Barkus E, Stirling J, Hopkins R, Lewis S. The presence of neurological soft signs along the psychosis proneness continuum. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32:573–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Mohr C, Landis T, Sandor PS, Fathi M, Brugger P. Nonstereotyped responding in positive schizotypy after a single dose of levodopa. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1741–1751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Lenzenweger MF, Maher BA. Psychometric schizotypy and motor performance. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:546–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Zhao Q, Li Z, Huang J, et al. Neurological soft signs are not “soft” in brain structure and functional networks: evidence from ALE meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40:626–641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Thakkar KN, Peterman JS, Park S. Altered brain activation during action imitation and observation in schizophrenia: a translational approach to investigating social dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:539–548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Levy DL, Sereno AB, Gooding DC, O’Driscoll GA. Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization and pathophysiology. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:311–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Gooding DC, Basso MA. The tell-tale tasks: a review of saccadic research in psychiatric patient populations. Brain Cogn. 2008;68:371–390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Kendler KS, Ochs AL, Gorman AM, Hewitt JK, Ross DE, Mirsky AF. The structure of schizotypy: a pilot multitrait twin study. Psychiatry Res. 1991;36:19–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Gooding DC, Miller MD, Kwapil TR. Smooth pursuit eye tracking and visual fixation in psychosis-prone individuals. Psychiatry Res. 2000;93:41–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Holahan AL, O’Driscoll GA. Antisaccade and smooth pursuit performance in positive- and negative-symptom schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 2005;76:43–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. O’Driscoll GA, Lenzenweger MF, Holzman PS. Antisaccades and smooth pursuit eye tracking and schizotypy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:837–843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Ettinger U, Kumari V, Crawford TJ, et al. Saccadic eye movements, schizotypy, and the role of neuroticism. Biol Psychol. 2005;68:61–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Gooding DC. Antisaccade task performance in questionnaire-identified schizotypes. Schizophr Res. 1999;35:157–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Kattoulas E, Evdokimidis I, Stefanis NC, Avramopoulos D, Stefanis CN, Smyrnis N. Predictive smooth eye pursuit in a population of young men: II. Effects of schizotypy, anxiety and depression. Exp Brain Res. 2011;215:219–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Thaker GK, Ross DE, Buchanan RW, Adami HM, Medoff DR. Smooth pursuit eye movements to extra-retinal motion signals: deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1999;88:209–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. Meyhofer I, Steffens M, Kasparbauer A, Grant P, Weber B, Ettinger U. Neural mechanisms of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizotypy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Mason O, Linney Y, Claridge G. Short scales for measuring schizotypy. Schizophr Res. 2005;78:293–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Lencer R, Nagel M, Sprenger A, Heide W, Binkofski F. Reduced neuronal activity in the V5 complex underlies smooth-pursuit deficit in schizophrenia: evidence from an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2005;24:1256–1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Gooding DC, Shea HB, Matts CW. Saccadic performance in questionnaire-identified schizotypes over time. Psychiatry Res. 2005;133:173–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Rust J. The Rust Inventory of Schizotypal Cognitions (RISC). Schizophr Bull. 1988;14:317–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Aichert DS, Williams SC, Möller HJ, Kumari V, Ettinger U. Functional neural correlates of psychometric schizotypy: an fMRI study of antisaccades. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:345–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Raemaekers M, Jansma JM, Cahn W, et al. Neuronal substrate of the saccadic inhibition deficit in schizophrenia investigated with 3-dimensional event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:313–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139. Raemaekers M, Ramsey NF, Vink M, van den Heuvel MP, Kahn RS. Brain activation during antisaccades in unaffected relatives of schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:530–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Camchong J, Dyckman KA, Austin BP, Clementz BA, McDowell JE. Common neural circuitry supporting volitional saccades and its disruption in schizophrenia patients and relatives. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:1042–1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Crawford TJ, Puri BK, Nijran KS, Jones B, Kennard C, Lewis SW. Abnormal saccadic distractibility in patients with schizophrenia: a 99mTc-HMPAO SPET study. Psychol Med. 1996;26:265–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. McDowell JE, Brown GG, Paulus M, et al. Neural correlates of refixation saccades and antisaccades in normal and schizophrenia subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:216–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Meyhöfer I, Steffens M, Kasparbauer A, Grant P, Weber B, Ettinger U. Neural mechanisms of smooth pursuit eye movements in schizotypy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:340–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Rawlings D, Williams B, Haslam N, Claridge G. Taxometric analysis supports a dimensional latent trait structure for schizotypy. Pers Indiv Diff. 2008;44:1640–1651. [Google Scholar]
- 145. Tsuang MT, Van Os J, Tandon R, et al. Attenuated psychosis syndrome in DSM-5. Schizophr Res. 2013;150:31–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. Koychev I, Barkus E, Ettinger U, et al. Evaluation of state and trait biomarkers in healthy volunteers for the development of novel drug treatments in schizophrenia. J Psychopharmacol. 2011;25:1207–1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. Dourish CT, Dawson GR. Precompetitive consortium approach to validation of the next generation of biomarkers in schizophrenia. Biomark Med. 2014;8:5–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Koychev I, McMullen K, Lees J, et al. A validation of cognitive biomarkers for the early identification of cognitive enhancing agents in schizotypy: a three-center double-blind placebo-controlled study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2012;22:469–481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149. Schmechtig A, Lees J, Grayson L, et al. Effects of risperidone, amisulpride and nicotine on eye movement control and their modulation by schizotypy. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;227:331–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150. Geyer MA, Wilkinson LS, Humby T, Robbins TW. Isolation rearing of rats produces a deficit in prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle similar to that in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34:361–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151. Choi JS, Park JY, Jung MH, et al. Phase-specific brain change of spatial working memory processing in genetic and ultra-high risk groups of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:1189–1199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]