Highlights
-
•
Neural population codes are organized at multiple spatial scales.
-
•
Microscopic organization of neural codes reveals a key role of neural heterogeneity.
-
•
Microscopic and population dynamics interact to make processing state-dependent.
-
•
Additional computational analyses of neural activity across resolutions are needed.
Keywords: neural code, cross-correlation, relative timing, sensory processing, oscillation, multiscale processing, neuroimaging, state-dependent coding
Abstract
Behavior relies on the distributed and coordinated activity of neural populations. Population activity can be measured using multi-neuron recordings and neuroimaging. Neural recordings reveal how the heterogeneity, sparseness, timing, and correlation of population activity shape information processing in local networks, whereas neuroimaging shows how long-range coupling and brain states impact on local activity and perception. To obtain an integrated perspective on neural information processing we need to combine knowledge from both levels of investigation. We review recent progress of how neural recordings, neuroimaging, and computational approaches begin to elucidate how interactions between local neural population activity and large-scale dynamics shape the structure and coding capacity of local information representations, make them state-dependent, and control distributed populations that collectively shape behavior.
Neural population codes at multiple scales
In complex animals, information about behaviorally important variables such as sensory signals or motor actions is carried by the activity of populations of neurons [1–4]. Our understanding of neural information processing is founded on the conceptual assumption that, if two or more sensory stimuli can be discriminated, or two or more behavioral responses are different, their associated patterns of neural activity must be readily discriminable. Several key ingredients shape the capacity of a neural population code (see Glossary) to form such discriminable representations: the diversity of neural response properties, their spatial and temporal response profiles, the cross-neural correlations, and the state-dependence of cortical activity (Box 1).
Box 1. Neural response features contributing to a population code.
Understanding a population code requires investigating its statistical properties along all relevant dimensions and linking them to the external events that are encoded (for example, sensory inputs). Informative response features can spread across the dimension of time (temporal variations in the responses of individual neurons or of the population), space (stimulus tuning differences in the firing of different neurons), or along non-separable combinations of both dimensions.
Current experimental methods emphasize different dimensions. Multi-neuron recordings resolving individual neurons at high temporal resolution provide information about both spatial and temporal dimensions and their non-separable intersection [6]. Imaging techniques such as two-photon imaging resolve individual neurons in space but often lack the fine temporal resolution to inform about spike timing [107]. Measures of mass activity at high temporal resolution (LFP, ECoG, EEG, MEG) cannot resolve individual neurons (and sometimes even brain regions), but capture temporal activity patterns that are coordinated across multiple neurons [108].
Diversity of single-neuron firing rates. Neurons with heterogeneous stimulus preferences can each add complementary stimulus information as they each differ in terms of stimulus preference or tuning width. In the example, neuron A has narrow tuning (it responds only to the diamond), whereas neurons B and C prefer other stimuli and have a wider tuning than neuron A. Neuron D is uninformative and responds equally to all stimuli.
Relative timing. In a population, informative response patterns can include the relative timing between neurons. For example, the star and square can be distinguished by the relative timing between neurons B and C (emphasized by the dashed line connecting their spikes), while both neurons elicit the same number of spikes for these stimuli.
Network state modulation. Neural responses depend not only on sensory inputs but also on large-scale brain states that vary on timescales slower than the transient responses to individual stimuli. In the example, these are represented by a slow wave of a mass signal (e.g., an LFP filtered into a low-frequency component; the bottom row). In this cartoon, firing rates during one state (reflected by the peak of the cycle) are stronger than during another state (around the trough of the cycle), but the individual stimulus preferences or relative timing are preserved. Such state variables likely play a key – though not yet fully understood – role in population coding [60,78,79,81].
Periods of silence. Neural populations also encode information by the silence (i.e., absence of firing) of some neurons [109]. In the schematic of Figure I, when neuron A is silent, it is reporting information about the absence of the diamond.
Recent progress in understanding the contribution and the interplay of each of these key ingredients arises from both technical and conceptual developments. Experimental methods now allow measuring and manipulating up to hundreds of neurons simultaneously in behaving animals and permit a direct link between population codes and behavior [5–8]. Multi-scale studies combining invasive recordings with measurements of neuroimaging signals are becoming more frequent, allowing us to combine insights across methodologies [9,10]. Finally, advances in computational methods for single-trial analysis of multivariate data allow us to fully exploit the avenues opened by high-density brain measurements [11–14].
We review here how these developments facilitate the convergence of knowledge gained from invasive multi-neuron recordings, neuroimaging data, and mathematical modeling, and begin to reveal the organization and computations of neural population codes at multiple scales of organization. For simplicity, we mostly focus on the encoding of sensory variables, but the concepts are relevant for the generic encoding of any variable (for example, motor variables).
The diverse response selectivity of sensory neurons
The computational properties of population codes are usually quantified using measures of the information they carry about sensory stimuli [15]. The most widely used quantifications of neural information are based upon Shannon information (quantifying the accuracy of discrimination among different stimuli in a set) or Fisher information (quantifying discrimination of small stimulus changes, or the accuracy of decoding an individual stimulus). Because most concepts reviewed here apply to both types of information, we only distinguish between them when necessary.
How a neural population represents information is partly determined by the diverse selectivity of individual neurons [16]. Individual neurons can carry information about sensory stimuli using a firing-rate code [17], for example by elevating their firing rate when presented with ‘preferred’ stimuli, and decreasing their rate when presented with other stimuli (Box 1). Nevertheless, even neighboring neurons may have heterogeneous stimulus selectivity [18,19]. For example, different neurons may have different stimulus tuning curves exhibiting a preference for different stimulus features or exhibiting different stimulus tuning widths. The heterogeneity of stimulus tuning generally implies that individual neurons may carry complementary information to that provided by others. As a result, the ability of a heterogeneous population to discriminate among stimuli in a set should, under most conditions, increase with population size. If individual neurons in a population each prefer well-separated stimulus features (such that their tuning curves do not overlap), the range of stimulus features encoded by that population would increase with population size. If, instead, neurons in a population have diverse but partly overlapping tuning curves, the complementary information carried by different neurons would lead to a better discrimination of stimulus features in the regions where the tuning curves overlap.
The scaling of information with population size depends on the structure of tuning preferences and of trial-to-trial response correlations, and its investigation can provide important insights. It can indicate how many neurons would be sufficient to achieve a desired level of sensory accuracy (assuming that the decoding mechanisms or later processing stages do not discard any of this information). Its extrapolation to infinite population size sets an upper bound on the information that can be achieved by a population with the considered response properties.
The scaling of information with population size is often studied by averaging information over subpopulations that are randomly sampled from the recorded neurons. This approach typically reveals a steady increase of information with population size (Figure 1A), and led to the hypothesis that increasing population size allows the encoding of arbitrarily high amounts of information [20,21]. However, recent work [22] has shown that this steady increase may actually be an artifact resulting from random subsampling. This artifact arises because of an often neglected aspect of neural heterogeneity [22]: only a small fraction of neurons in a given population carry significant sensory information in a specific context (Figure 1B). As a result of this heterogeneity in single-neuron properties, a small but highly informative subset of neurons is sufficient to carry essentially all the information present in the entire observed population (Figure 1A) [22].
The above results are part of an emerging picture that a small-dimensional subspace of the experimentally measured activity suffices to explain the population dynamics underling sensory processing [23] and motor behavior [24]. This picture is consistent with the observed sparseness of cortical activity [25] (at any moment only a small fraction of neurons are active) and is compatible with studies showing that perception and actions can be driven by small groups of neurons [26]. A sparse population code may be advantageous for metabolic efficiency and may facilitate dendritic computations requiring the separation of individual synaptic afferents [27,28].
Studies on the auditory system suggest that those neurons contributing the most to a population code are those that respond sparsely over time, and do so with precisely timed temporal spike patterns [22,29,30]. Importantly, the information carried by these precise spike patterns cannot be replaced by the information in the coarse-scale firing rates of other neurons in the population (Figure 1C), because information in spike patterns is complementary to that in firing rates and because the fraction of neurons carrying information by rates is limited [22]. This implies that spike timing remains crucial even for population codes [22,29,31]. The complementary nature of millisecond-scale spike patterns and firing rates has similarly been observed in the somatosensory [32] and visual system [33]. Thus, both the spatial and temporal dimensions are important for understanding neural population codes, raising questions as to how such precise timing or population sparseness can be assessed using neuroimaging.
Benefits of mixed selectivity for population coding
In higher association regions the heterogeneity of cortical neurons expresses itself as patterns of selectivity to multiple sensory and task-related variables, which can be mixed in complex, sometimes nonlinear ways [34,35]. A pioneering study unveiled the advantages of nonlinear mixed selectivity [35]. If responses were selective only to individual task or stimulus aspects, or to their linear combinations (linear mixed selectivity; Figure 1D), the dimensionality of the neural representation would be lower than the number of neurons in the population. In the example in Figure 1D–F, the neural representation of a set of stimuli of two linearly mixed neurons lies on a line. This implies that complex nonlinear operations are required for decoding the information content (Figure 1F). By contrast, a heterogeneous nonlinear mixed representation (Figure 1E) leads to a richer population representation that has higher dimensionality than its linear counterpart and that can be more easily decoded by downstream areas using linear combinations of neural activity [36] (Figure 1F). In sum, heterogeneous nonlinear mixtures of selectivity increase the effective dimensionality of a population code and help to extract diverse information using simple linear decoding.
This high-dimensional population representation by mixed selectivity may seem at odds with reports of small subpopulations effectively encoding primary sensory information. However, sparseness and high dimensionality of mixed nonlinear selectivity can coexist and may even combine optimally under appropriate conditions [37]. In fact, when nonlinearly mixed neural representations are also sparse, they naturally achieve an optimal trade-off between the need to maximize the diversity of responses to different stimuli (increasing the stimulus discrimination properties of population responses), and the need to maintain high response reliability (i.e., achieving consistent responses for noisy variations of the input) [37]. Nonetheless, systematic studies comparing the sparseness of neural responses across brain regions will be necessary to clarify the extent to which neural populations take advantage of the potential benefits offered by sparse representations. Neuroimaging appears suited for this goal because it allows comparing neural representation spaces across species and sensory systems, and tracing these over time during task performance [9,14,38].
Correlations of spike rates between neurons
The computational properties of a population code depend also on the pattern of response correlations between neurons [39]. Traditionally, a distinction is made between signal correlations (quantifying the correlation of the trial-averaged neural responses across the different stimuli, and thus quantifying the similarity of stimulus tuning) and noise correlations (quantifying the correlation of trial-by-trial variations of response at fixed stimulus attributes). Much work on population codes has focused on correlations between the firing rates of pairs of neurons.
When tools for simultaneous multi-neuron recordings became widely available in the 1990s, two divergent views were proposed about the impact of noise correlations between firing rates. The first holds that noise correlations of firing rates – if modulated by the stimulus – may act as a separate coding mechanism complementary to firing rates [40,41]. For example, it has been proposed that the dynamic stimulus-dependence of noise correlations among visual neurons (originating from changes in gamma-band synchronization among populations) carries information about whether individual neurons respond to the same or separate objects [42]. This view has deeply influenced the interpretation of neural mass signals measured by neuroimaging because it suggests that changes in correlation (or coherence) between the activities of distinct populations may reflect how these populations encode information. The second view is that noise correlations of firing rates may impose limits on the growth of information with population size [43]. For example, for neurons with identical tuning curves, the presence even of weak positive noise correlations precludes the possibility to improve the accuracy of sensory information encoding by simply averaging the rate of many neurons (correlated noise can only be removed to a limited extent by averaging). This second view influenced studies on neural population codes, and motivated recent work on how neuronal networks may dynamically decrease noise correlations to reduce their potentially limiting effect on information encoding [44,45]. It has also been argued that the limiting effect of correlation may partially be overcome in populations with heterogeneous tuning and correlation properties [46,47].
The potential impact of correlations on population coding has been studied extensively using model-based approaches that quantify stimulus information under various assumptions about correlation structures or readout mechanisms [15,48,49]. This approach has shown that correlations can influence neural population coding in more complex way than described above: depending on the precise pattern of signal and noise correlations, and the choice of readout mechanisms, correlations can increase, decrease, or leave unaffected the information carried by a population [1,15,40,49–51]. An important insight of these theoretical studies is that the precise pattern of noise correlation matters more than noise correlation strength or population size. For example, a recent study [49] considered the ability (quantified using Fisher information) to estimate small changes in a stimulus from population activity, and found that the type of noise correlations that limit the increase of information with population size are so-called differential correlations. Intuitively, such differential correlations correspond to correlated trial-to-trial variability of neural responses that shift the profile of population activity such that it looks like the population activity that would have been elicited by a slightly different stimulus. Thus, this correlated variability makes a noisy fluctuation of population activity look like the signal representing a different stimulus value, and thus acts as a source of noise that cannot be eliminated even by increasing the number of neurons. Whether these information-limiting correlations actually occur can be empirically investigated by computing information versus the number of neurons and investigating whether information saturates with population size. Because information depends crucially on the structure of correlations, extrapolation of its dependence on population size from real data should be performed using decoding approaches [16,49] rather than making strong but difficult-to-validate assumptions about the structure and shape of correlations [49].
Besides determining the amount of information carried by neural populations, noise correlations in firing rates may also be indicative of important computational functions [52]. Noise-correlations may have a role in probabilistic codes representing sensory uncertainty by reflecting correlations in the uncertainty associated with individual stimulus variables [53,54]. For example, for an ambiguous input consistent with one of two possible stimuli, the neurons representing these two stimuli would be negatively correlated because evidence for one stimulus speaks against the other [55]. It has also been argued that positive correlations within larger populations make the code sparser by increasing periods of population quiescence, and hence concentrate information into rare periods of common activity [56–58]. This latter property may strongly shape how cortical activity is seen through neuroimaging.
Correlations of spike timing between neurons
Correlations between spike times can also contribute to population codes, for example by facilitating the readout of temporal response patterns in a relative latency code. Individual neurons can encode stimuli by their response latency (that is, the delay between stimulus onset and neural firing) [59,60]. Although information in the latency of an individual neuron may not be directly accessible to a downstream decoder (because measuring latency requires information about the precise time of stimulus onset), information in the relative latency between neurons may be robustly extracted by exploiting the fact that trial-by-trial shifts of latency are often correlated across a population [61–63]. For example, latencies of different neurons may tend to be all-late or all-early because of global fluctuations in network excitability. Correlated latency shifts preserve the relative order of firing or latency differences across neurons, whereas uncorrelated shifts do not. In auditory cortex, the readout of such relative timing may be further supported by time-reference events, in other words, population response patterns that encode the stimulus time with an early, stimulus-invariant latency [64]. Stimulus-selective neurons can then encode the stimulus identity by the time of their spikes relative to these reference events [64]. Although these relative-latency codes are often studied at the pairwise level, recent work suggests that they may reflect a more general larger-scale organization of population activity: groups of neurons may be co-active in stereotyped sequences (termed packets [65]), and the relative timing and strength of each neuron within this sequence may encode specific stimulus attributes [65,66].
Finally, correlations between spike times may also ensure the transmission of information across areas, for example by facilitating the impact on post-synaptic targets [67,68] or by facilitating the selective read-out of specific combinations of afferents [69,70]. All in all, this suggests that the relative timing between neurons contributes both to representing sensory information and to transmitting this information between areas.
State-dependence of population codes
Local neural activity not only depends on the current sensory input but also on the current brain state [71,72]. Although feed-forward inputs to local circuits provide sensory afferents, the abundance of recurrent and feedback connections, and of neuromodulatory inputs, shapes the background activity on which this sensory information is processed [73–75]. For example, neuromodulatory inputs that are not directly stimulus-related (such as cholinergic or noradrenergic signals that are an essential component of the control of the animal's behavioral state) can profoundly change the excitability of sensory cortical circuits, the degree of correlation among neurons, and the gain and reliability of individual neurons [76,77]. Variations in brain state account for a significant fraction of the trial-to-trial variability of population activity [78].
This state-dependence must have profound – but largely unexplored – implications for how population codes operate. State-dependence may imply that populations transmit information only using codes that are robust to state fluctuations (e.g., relative latencies or relative firing-rates). Alternatively, downstream areas may extract variables indicating the current state from network activity (similarly to procedures used in data analysis [78]) and then use state-dependent decoders to interpret population activity. Progress in understanding state-dependent coding requires better statistical methods for single-trial analysis that can be applied to different measurements of brain activity to disentangle state-dependent and -independent aspects of a code [12,79–81].
Insights on population coding from mass activity
Although a complete description of population codes may require recording all neurons involved in the considered task, important empirical knowledge about population codes can be gained from measurements of neural mass signals with high temporal resolution (LFP, ECOG, EEG, MEG). These measurements lack cellular-level resolution but can be easily applied to multiple brain areas and complex tasks, and are sensitive to both supra- and sub-threshold activity. Importantly, they have the potential to capture indicators of cortical state that cannot be easily extracted from the spiking activity of a few neurons alone [11].
Advances in single-trial data analysis have expanded the use of mass signals to study sensory transformations, permitting researchers to study the same questions using neuroimaging, multi-neuron recordings, and computational models. For example, recent neuroimaging studies extracted features of population activity influencing the variability of single-trial percepts [82–84], and demonstrated that visual object categories [85] or fine details of auditory signals [86–90] can be recovered using stimulus reconstruction or decoding methods.
Additional insights can be gained from mass signals by decomposing them into specific frequency bands and separating the sensory information carried by power and phase of individual bands. Recent work, based on both invasive (LFP) and non-invasive (EEG and MEG) recordings, has individuated the phase of low-frequency activity as a particularly informative feature of mass activity [85,91–95]. In the visual system, detailed sensory features are reflected more in the phase of low-frequency (below 12 Hz) activity than in the power [1,94,96] (Figure 2A). Related findings were made in the auditory system, where the phase of low-frequency activity encodes speech or complex sounds [91–93,95,97] (Figure 2B,C). Stepping beyond single-site analysis, some studies found that the relative timing of neural responses across different sites carries more sensory information than the activation of individual sites [85,98,99]. These results hence suggest that the relative timing of population activity could at least be as important for sensory coding as the strength of activity of an individual population, in line with insights from studies on spiking activity that highlight the role of relative response timing.
An important population coding principle emerging from the analysis of mass signals (both using MEG and EEG [92,94] or intracranial recordings [60,100,101]) is that of multiplexing of sensory information: different frequency bands of population activity each carry complementary information about stimulus features. For example, nested patterns of slower (e.g., delta or theta) and faster (e.g., gamma) auditory cortical rhythmic activity encode complementary aspects of speech (Figure 2B) [92]. These results illustrate the multiple coding dimensions of population signals, and highlight the importance of understanding how specific aspects of coding in spiking activity map onto phase and power of rhythmic mass signals. Specific challenges for understanding the link between non-invasive neuroimaging and neural population codes are outlined in Box 2 and in the outstanding questions (Box 3).
Box 2. Comparisons of stimulus selectivity in mass signals and spiking activity.
Measurements of neural mass activity with high temporal resolution (LFP, MEG, EEG, ECoG) provide rich signals varying on multiple timescales [110] and exhibiting reasonable spatial specificity [110]. Nevertheless, owing to the multiple neural phenomena they capture, and because of uncertainties in source localization and to cancellation of local signals [11,102,108], they are difficult to interpret in terms of the underlying neural processes. However, some relations between mass signals, spiking activity, and sensory encoding are emerging from recent work.
Specific frequency components extracted from MEG and EEG signals seem to be particularly predictive of neural firing rates. Combined neural and EEG recordings from visual cortex show that the phase of low frequency (<10 Hz) and the amplitude of high-frequency (gamma, 40–100 Hz) rhythms carry the most information about the strength of local firing, and each offer complementary information about the firing rate [111]. An indirect comparison of auditory stimulus selectivity in firing rates and EEGs revealed the strongest association with the phase (but not power) of low-frequency components [91]. How do the phase of low-frequency components, and the amplitude of faster components, relate to sensory encoding?
The phase of low-frequency (<12 Hz) rhythms likely reflects changes in local network excitability that shape both stimulus responses and spontaneous background activity. Evidence for this comes from neural recordings [112] and from neuroimaging studies demonstrating a causal relation between the phase of low-frequency activity and single-trial stimulus detection [113–115]. Hence, the phase of low-frequency network activity may indicate whether or not local networks are in a state facilitating the encoding of sensory information and driving perception. Some theories suggest that the excitability fluctuations marked by the low-frequency phase help to prioritize the encoding of salient features [60,115].
Does this phase also indicate what is encoded? Recent work suggests this is the case: low-frequency phase can reflect the slow temporal structure of dynamic sensory stimuli. Experimental data and network models show that slow brain rhythms entrain to low-frequency variations of natural stimuli such as speech, directly inducing sensory information in the phase of these signals (e.g., [91,92,116,117]).
In visual cortices the amplitude of gamma rhythms is the signal component carrying the most sensory information [118]. Models of gamma generation suggest that its amplitude reflects the instantaneous strength of local interactions between inhibitory and excitatory neurons, which is roughly proportional to the stimulus input – if excitation and inhibition are balanced [119]. Thus, gamma amplitude is expected to encode aspects of current stimulus features rather than the slow stimulus dynamics (which is reflected by slower signal components). The validity of this hypothesis requires further experimental tests, as does the causal role of such rhythms for behavior.
Box 3. Outstanding questions.
-
•
How is it possible that mass signals carry sensory information given that only a small fraction of neurons are informative? How does neural heterogeneity affect what we can learn from mass signals?
-
•
How do microscopic-level neural coding mechanisms (neuronal heterogeneity, sparseness) interact with macroscopic scale aspects of neural activity (patterns of coupling across multiple areas or global state changes)?
-
•
Is mixed selectivity a hallmark of higher (association) brain regions, and does the mixed selectivity (previously observed across neurons) also exist in the time or frequency domains of neural activity (i.e., do different temporal signal components exhibit mixed selectivity)?
-
•
What do neural correlations imply for mass signals, and how can we infer some properties of the microscopic structure of neural population codes from mass signals?
-
•
How do local descriptions of cortical state (derived from multi-neuron recordings) relate to state-dependent signatures visible in neural mass activity?
-
•
How can we best integrate recent advances in cellular imaging and histochemical or morphological analysis to disentangle the contribution of specific cell types to neural population codes?
Insights from the combined observations of single neurons and mass signals
Important insights about population codes further arise from studies that simultaneously measure spiking responses and mass signals, or that perform comparative analysis on such data obtained in separate experiments.
One key opportunity of the joint measurements arises from the complementary nature of single-neuron firing and of mass signals: mass signals capture aspects of sub-threshold activity and of intrinsically driven state-changes that cannot be measured by observing the spiking activity of a few neurons alone [11,102]. Thus, low-frequency mass activity can be seen as a measure of the background state fluctuations constituting the ‘context’ that affects processing of the ‘content’ carried by sensory inputs [103]. Examining the responses of individual neurons relative to the phase of low-frequency network rhythms can shed light on how the local circuit context affects the specific sensory content encoded in spiking activity.
Converging studies suggest that single-neuron spike timing depends on the frequency-specific oscillatory LFP phase (Figure 3A), and that the sensory information carried by spiking activity can be better interpreted when the network context during which spikes were emitted is known [101,104] (Figure 3A). A recent study in the human medial temporal lobe showed that perception-related spiking activity is locked to an early stereotyped increase in theta LFP signals, suggesting that specific interactions between network rhythms and firing of individual neurons may define temporal windows facilitating conscious perception [105]. Complementing measurements of spiking activity with LFP recordings may allow differentiating otherwise ambiguous population states and provides a means to disentangle context-related activity fluctuations from those induced by sensory noise or other causes.
Joint measurements of single-neuron spikes and mass signals also facilitate our understanding of the coordination of population codes across brain structures. Recent concurrent multi-site recordings of spiking activity and LFPs have shown that the spiking activity of a single neuron depends not only on the phase of the LFP oscillation at the same site but also on the dynamic patterns of the relationships (or ‘phase coupling’) between the phases of neural oscillations measured by LFP at multiple distant sites [106] (Figure 3B). The similarity of preferences in LFP phase coupling between neurons also predicts the similarity of their firing-rate variations: neurons that prefer similar patterns of phase coupling exhibit similar changes in firing rates with time or task, whereas neurons with different phase preferences show divergent firing-rate dynamics [106]. This provides direct evidence that large-scale functional connectivity shapes local activation patterns and controls the co-activation and coordination of anatomically dispersed but functionally integrated ensembles of neurons, thus breaking new ground in the understanding of the large-scale organization of neural population codes.
Another approach to clarify the neural basis of the sensory information carried by different components of MEG/EEG signals is based on the use of information theoretic or stimulus-decoding methods [16] to compare quantitatively the similarities of stimulus encoding in mass signals and multi-neuron recordings [9,14,38]. This approach revealed (Figure 2C) that the phase (and to a much lesser extent the power) of low-frequency mass activity captures some aspects of the sensory information carried by population spiking activity [91]. Such comparative approaches will be crucial for understanding what mass signals can tell us about the underlying neural information processing (Box 2) and open the possibility to study emerging principles such as multiplexing, mixed selectivity, or of state-dependence across scales of brain measurements.
Concluding remarks
Understanding the properties and principles of cortical population codes requires identifying the most informative patterns within the spatio-temporal complexity of multi-neuron activity and understanding how coding properties are affected by large-scale state changes. Invasive recordings provide detailed access to the heterogeneity, temporal precision, and correlation structure of multi-neuron activity, properties which recent computational studies highlight as being crucial for shaping the information-coding properties of a population. Mass signals, on the other hand, provide more direct access to large-scale changes in network state and connectivity, other crucial properties that shape information coding and which can account for trial-by-trial variations in cognitive tasks. Combining insights from both mass signals and multi-neuron recordings is a key challenge for the future.
In light of the reviewed properties of population codes several questions emerge about the integration of microscopic and macroscopic structure of population codes (Box 3). For example, can the mechanisms that seem crucial at the microscopic scale (such as neuronal heterogeneity, sparseness, or correlations or mixed selectivity) be observed from mass signals? In turn, how do specific patterns of activity observed at the macroscopic scale (such as patterns of phase coupling across multiple areas or global state changes) relate to the coding properties that are crucial at the microscopic scale? For sure, much is to be learned by methods, such as single-trial stimulus decoding or reconstruction methods, that allow a comparative and detailed assessment of similarity and complementarity of activity at each spatial and temporal scale.
To enhance our understanding of population coding, future developments of methods for high-dimensional data analysis and neural modeling will be necessary as well. Recent work has provided improvements in analytical techniques for reducing high-dimensional datasets and extracting the relevant sensory representations [11–14]. In addition, biophysical models of sensory representations in cortical microcircuits allow a principled and rigorous direct link between neural signals and computing architectures, facilitating our understanding of how coding principles are implemented in the neural circuits. Being able to subject different neural signals to the same scientific question and analysis routine, and being able to compute both spiking and mass signals from the same plausible neural network models [11], are two crucial features to further improve our understanding of neural population coding in the future.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to J. Assad, J. Carmena, S. Fusi, F. Jäkel, A. Onken, and R. Schwarzlose for useful feedback on an early version of the manuscript, to J. Carmena and S. Fusi for sharing their figures, and to S. Maistrelli for help with figure reprint permissions. We acknowledge the financial support of the VISUALISE and SICODE projects of the Future and Emerging Technologies (FET) Programme within the Seventh Framework Programme for Research of the European Commission (FP7-ICT-2011.9.11; FP7-600954 and FP7-284553), and of the European Community's Seventh Framework Programme FP7/2007-2013 (PITN-GA-2011-290011), by the UK Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC, BB/L027534/1), by the Max Planck Society, the Wellcome Trust (098433), the Autonomous Province of Trento (Call ‘Grandi Progetti 2012’, project ‘Characterizing and improving brain mechanisms of attention – ATTEND’) and was part of the research program of the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Tübingen, funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF; FKZ: 01GQ1002).
Glossary
- Brain state
brain activity dynamics on a timescale of seconds. It reflects the interactions between ongoing endogenous activity and sensorimotor processing, and is strongly influenced by neuromodulatory systems.
- Complementary information
two neural codes carry complementary information if the information they carry jointly is higher than the information carried by either code individually; this implies that some information not available in one code is provided by the other.
- Decoding
the estimation (by either the brain or an artificial decoder) of a variable of interest (for example, the value of a presented sensory stimulus, or a motor variable) from the observation of a single-trial neural population response.
- Differential correlation
a noise correlation between the firing rates of a pair of neurons whose strength, for each stimulus value, is proportional to the product of the derivatives of the tuning curves.
- Dimensionality of the neural representation
the minimal number of coordinate axes that are needed to describe the variations of responses across all trials to all different experimental conditions (for example, all combinations of sensory stimuli and behavioral responses).
- ECoG
electrocorticography.
- EEG
electroencephalography.
- Encoding
the generation of the set of specific activity patterns that represent a sensory attribute.
- Firing-rate code
neural code that represents stimulus attributes using the number of spikes emitted in response to the stimulus, regardless of their temporal pattern.
- Fisher Information
a measure of the variance of estimation of a particular stimulus value (e.g., contrast of a grating) from the single-trial observation of neural population activity.
- Information
a measure of how much knowledge about which stimulus is being presented can be gained from a single-trial neural population response. Information is often quantified as Shannon Information or Fisher Information.
- Latency code
a specific form of neural code encoding information in the timing of the response onset. The time of the response onset is usually measured with respect to stimulus presentation time, but can be defined also relative to another neural event (relative latency code).
- Local field potential (LFP)
a neurophysiological signal obtained by low-pass filtering extracellular recordings. It captures slow components of both sub- and supra-threshold neural activity.
- Mass signal
a signal that comprises the aggregate neural electric activity in a local region and captures both supra- and sub-threshold phenomena, including spiking and synaptic activity. Examples are LFPs, ECOG, MEG, and EEG.
- MEG
magnetoencephalography.
- Multiplexing of sensory information
neural coding scheme in which complementary information is represented in different frequency components or temporal scales of neural population activity. For example, when different information is represented by the precise timing of individual spikes on the scale of milliseconds and by the slow modulation of the spike count on the scale of hundreds of milliseconds.
- Neural population code
the set of response features of a population of neurons that carry all information about the considered stimuli. These features consist of spatio-temporal sequences of action potentials distributed across neurons and/or time.
- Noise correlation
a measure of correlation between the firing of a pair of neurons that cannot possibly be attributed to the sensory stimulus. Noise correlation is quantified as the correlation between the firing of the neurons in response to a fixed external stimulus.
- Packet
a pattern of firing consisting of a group of neurons firing transiently in a relatively stereotyped sequence. A packet may encode different stimulus features by modulating the relative timing and relative firing rate of the subsets of neurons firing within the sequence.
- Phase/power
the current period within a cycle of a given oscillation / the amplitude of an oscillatory signal.
- Readout mechanisms
a set of biophysical computational mechanisms used by the brain to extract information out of a neural population response.
- Shannon Information
a measure of how much (on average) observation of a neural population response reduces the uncertainty about which stimulus (among those in a set) is being presented. Also called ‘mutual information’.
- Signal correlation
a measure of correlations between the firing of a pair of neurons that are attributable to the sensory stimulus. Signal correlations are quantified as the correlation of tuning of the firing of the pair of neurons to the stimuli (for example, the correlation across stimuli of the tuning curves).
- Temporal spike pattern
a repeatable temporal sequence of spikes that carries information about stimuli.
- Time-reference events
a neural (single-neuron or population) activity pattern that encodes the time of an important event (for example, the onset of a stimulus) by emitting a transient response with a stereotyped, invariant, and short latency.
- Tuning curve
a mathematical function describing the dependence of the trial-averaged firing rate of a neuron upon the value of the stimulus.
- Tuning width
a measure of the selectivity of neural firing to stimuli. In intuitive terms, a narrow (respectively coarse) tuning width means that only few (respectively many) stimuli elicit a strong neural response.
References
- 1.Averbeck B.B. Neural correlations, population coding and computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:358–366. doi: 10.1038/nrn1888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Stanley G.B. Reading and writing the neural code. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:259–263. doi: 10.1038/nn.3330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hebb D.O. Wiley; 1949. The Organization of Behaviour. A Neuropsychological Theory. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jacobs A.L. Ruling out and ruling in neural codes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106:5936–5941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900573106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stark E. Diode probes for spatiotemporal optical control of multiple neurons in freely moving animals. J. Neurophysiol. 2012;108:349–363. doi: 10.1152/jn.00153.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Berenyi A. Large-scale, high-density (up to 512 channels) recording of local circuits in behaving animals. J. Neurophysiol. 2014;111:1132–1149. doi: 10.1152/jn.00785.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Harvey C.D. Choice-specific sequences in parietal cortex during a virtual-navigation decision task. Nature. 2012;484:62–68. doi: 10.1038/nature10918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Scott B.B. Cellular resolution functional imaging in behaving rats using voluntary head restraint. Neuron. 2013;80:371–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kriegeskorte N., Kievit R.A. Representational geometry: integrating cognition, computation, and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Logothetis N.K. Hippocampal–cortical interaction during periods of subcortical silence. Nature. 2012;491:547–553. doi: 10.1038/nature11618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Einevoll G.T. Modelling and analysis of local field potentials for studying the function of cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;14:770–780. doi: 10.1038/nrn3599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cunningham J., Yu B.M. Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordings. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;15:1752–1758. doi: 10.1038/nn.3776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Churchland M.M. Techniques for extracting single-trial activity patterns from large-scale neural recordings. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:609–618. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Haxby J.V. Decoding neural representational spaces using multivariate pattern analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;37:435–456. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Quian Quiroga R., Panzeri S. Extracting information from neuronal populations: information theory and decoding approaches. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:173–185. doi: 10.1038/nrn2578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shamir M. Emerging principles of population coding: in search for the neural code. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014;25:140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Shadlen M.N., Newsome W.T. The variable discharge of cortical neurons: implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:3870–3896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03870.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rothschild G. Functional organization and population dynamics in the mouse primary auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:353–360. doi: 10.1038/nn.2484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Martin K.A., Schroder S. Functional heterogeneity in neighboring neurons of cat primary visual cortex in response to both artificial and natural stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:7325–7344. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4071-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Quiroga R.Q. Decoding visual inputs from multiple neurons in the human temporal lobe. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;98:1997–2007. doi: 10.1152/jn.00125.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Rolls E.T. The representational capacity of the distributed encoding of information provided by populations of neurons in primate temporal visual cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 1997;114:149–162. doi: 10.1007/pl00005615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ince R.A. Neural codes formed by small and temporally precise populations in auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:18277–18287. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2631-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bathellier B. Discrete neocortical dynamics predict behavioral categorization of sounds. Neuron. 2012;76:435–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shenoy K.V. Cortical control of arm movements: a dynamical systems perspective. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;36:337–359. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Barth A.L., Poulet J.F. Experimental evidence for sparse firing in the neocortex. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Houweling A.R., Brecht M. Behavioural report of single neuron stimulation in somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2008;451:65–68. doi: 10.1038/nature06447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Segev I., London M. Untangling dendrites with quantitative models. Science. 2000;290:744–750. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5492.744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Branco T., Hausser M. Synaptic integration gradients in single cortical pyramidal cell dendrites. Neuron. 2011;69:885–892. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Garcia-Lazaro J.A. Independent population coding of speech with sub-millisecond precision. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:19362–19372. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Centanni T.M. Detection and identification of speech sounds using cortical activity patterns. Neuroscience. 2014;258:292–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Engineer C.T. Cortical activity patterns predict speech discrimination ability. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:603–608. doi: 10.1038/nn.2109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zuo Y. Complementary contributions of spike timing and spike rate to perceptual decisions in rat S1 and S2 Cortex. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:357–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kiani R. Differences in onset latency of macaque inferotemporal neural responses to primate and non-primate faces. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1587–1596. doi: 10.1152/jn.00540.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Eskandar E.N., Assad J.A. Dissociation of visual, motor and predictive signals in parietal cortex during visual guidance. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:88–93. doi: 10.1038/4594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rigotti M. The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature. 2013;497:585–590. doi: 10.1038/nature12160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rigotti M. Internal representation of task rules by recurrent dynamics: the importance of the diversity of neural responses. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2010;4:24. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Barak O. The sparseness of mixed selectivity neurons controls the generalization-discrimination trade-off. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:3844–3856. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2753-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kriegeskorte N. Relating population-code representations between man, monkey, and computational models. Front. Neurosci. 2009;3:363–373. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.035.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cohen M.R., Kohn A. Measuring and interpreting neuronal correlations. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:811–819. doi: 10.1038/nn.2842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Panzeri S. Correlations and the encoding of information in the nervous system. Proc. Biol. Sci. 1999;266:1001–1012. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1999.0736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Brette R. Computing with neural synchrony. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012;8:e1002561. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Singer W. Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron. 1999;24:49–65. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80821-1. 111–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zohary E. Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature. 1994;370:140–143. doi: 10.1038/370140a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ecker A.S. Decorrelated neuronal firing in cortical microcircuits. Science. 2010;327:584–587. doi: 10.1126/science.1179867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Renart A. The asynchronous state in cortical circuits. Science. 2010;327:587–590. doi: 10.1126/science.1179850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ecker A.S. The effect of noise correlations in populations of diversely tuned neurons. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:14272–14283. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2539-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Shamir M., Sompolinsky H. Implications of neuronal diversity on population coding. Neural Comput. 2006;18:1951–1986. doi: 10.1162/neco.2006.18.8.1951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haefner R.M. Inferring decoding strategies from choice probabilities in the presence of correlated variability. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:235–242. doi: 10.1038/nn.3309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Moreno-Bote R. Information-limiting correlations. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1410–1417. doi: 10.1038/nn.3807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Abbott L.F., Dayan P. The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Comput. 1999;11:91–101. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schneidman E. Synergy, redundancy, and independence in population codes. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:11539–11553. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-37-11539.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Salinas E., Sejnowski T.J. Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:539–550. doi: 10.1038/35086012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Berkes P. Spontaneous cortical activity reveals hallmarks of an optimal internal model of the environment. Science. 2011;331:83–87. doi: 10.1126/science.1195870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fiser J. Statistically optimal perception and learning: from behavior to neural representations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Haefner R.M., Fiser J. Good noise or bad noise: the role of correlated variability in a probabilistic inference framework. Computational and Systems Neuroscience (Cosyne) Abstracts 2014, I39; Cosyne; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ohiorhenuan I.E. Sparse coding and high-order correlations in fine-scale cortical networks. Nature. 2010;466:617–621. doi: 10.1038/nature09178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Froudarakis E. Population code in mouse V1 facilitates readout of natural scenes through increased sparseness. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:851–857. doi: 10.1038/nn.3707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Macke J.H. Common input explains higher-order correlations and entropy in a simple model of neural population activity. Phys. Rev. Lett. 2011;106:208102. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.106.208102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shriki O. Fast coding of orientation in primary visual cortex. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012;8:e1002536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Panzeri S. Sensory neural codes using multiplexed temporal scales. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Gollisch T., Meister M. Rapid neural coding in the retina with relative spike latencies. Science. 2008;319:1108–1111. doi: 10.1126/science.1149639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.VanRullen R. Spike times make sense. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Gutig R. Computing complex visual features with retinal spike times. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e53063. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Brasselet R. Neurons with stereotyped and rapid responses provide a reference frame for relative temporal coding in primate auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:2998–3008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5435-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Luczak A. Gating of sensory input by spontaneous cortical activity. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1684–1695. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2928-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Havenith M.N. Synchrony makes neurons fire in sequence, and stimulus properties determine who is ahead. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:8570–8584. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2817-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Schoonover C.E. Comparative strength and dendritic organization of thalamocortical and corticocortical synapses onto excitatory layer 4 neurons. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:6746–6758. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Kelly S.T. The role of thalamic population synchrony in the emergence of cortical feature selectivity. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2014;10:e1003418. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Nikolic D. Gamma oscillations: precise temporal coordination without a metronome. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17:54–55. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Akam T., Kullmann D.M. Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15:111–122. doi: 10.1038/nrn3668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Buonomano D.V., Maass W. State-dependent computations: spatiotemporal processing in cortical networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:113–125. doi: 10.1038/nrn2558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Renart A., Machens C.K. Variability in neural activity and behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014;25:211–220. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Douglas R.J., Martin K.A. Mapping the matrix: the ways of neocortex. Neuron. 2007;56:226–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Harris K.D., Thiele A. Cortical state and attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011;12:509–523. doi: 10.1038/nrn3084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Lee S.H., Dan Y. Neuromodulation of brain states. Neuron. 2012;76:209–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Goard M., Dan Y. Basal forebrain activation enhances cortical coding of natural scenes. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1444–1449. doi: 10.1038/nn.2402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Pinto L. Fast modulation of visual perception by basal forebrain cholinergic neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2013;16:1857–1863. doi: 10.1038/nn.3552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Curto C. A simple model of cortical dynamics explains variability and state dependence of sensory responses in urethane-anesthetized auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:10600–10612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2053-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Macke, J.H. et al. (2011) Empirical models of spiking in neural populations. In In Advances in neural information processing systems, pp. 1350–1358
- 80.Ecker A.S. State dependence of noise correlations in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2014;82:235–248. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Marder E. Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron. 2012;76:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.King J.R., Dehaene S. Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: the temporal generalization method. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:203–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Isik L. The dynamics of invariant object recognition in the human visual system. J. Neurophysiol. 2014;111:91–102. doi: 10.1152/jn.00394.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Gross J. Analytical methods and experimental approaches for electrophysiological studies of brain oscillations. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;228:57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Majima K. Decoding visual object categories from temporal correlations of ECoG signals. Neuroimage. 2013;90C:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Mesgarani N. Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science. 2014;343:1006–1010. doi: 10.1126/science.1245994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Pasley B.N. Reconstructing speech from human auditory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2012;10:e1001251. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Cichy R.M. Resolving human object recognition in space and time. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:455–462. doi: 10.1038/nn.3635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Ramkumar P. Feature-specific information processing precedes concerted activation in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:7691–7699. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3905-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Zion Golumbic E. Visual input enhances selective speech envelope tracking in auditory cortex at a ‘cocktail party’. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1417–1426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3675-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Ng B.S.W. EEG phase patterns reflect the selectivity of neural firing. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:389–398. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Gross J. Speech rhythms and multiplexed oscillatory sensory coding in the human brain. PLoS Biol. 2013;11:e1001752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Howard M.F., Poeppel D. Discrimination of speech stimuli based on neuronal response phase patterns depends on acoustics but not comprehension. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:2500–2511. doi: 10.1152/jn.00251.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Schyns P.G. Cracking the code of oscillatory activity. PLoS Biol. 2011;9:e1001064. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Lopour B.A. Coding of information in the phase of local field potentials within human medial temporal lobe. Neuron. 2013;79:594–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Belitski A. Sensory information in local field potentials and spikes from visual and auditory cortices: time scales and frequency bands. J. Comput. Neurosci. 2010;29:533–545. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0230-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ding N., Simon J.Z. Adaptive temporal encoding leads to a background-insensitive cortical representation of speech. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:5728–5735. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5297-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Weisz N. Prestimulus oscillatory power and connectivity patterns predispose conscious somatosensory perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2014;111:E417–E425. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317267111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Liebe S. Theta coupling between V4 and prefrontal cortex predicts visual short-term memory performance. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:456–462. doi: 10.1038/nn.3038. S451–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Canolty R.T., Knight R.T. The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:506–515. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Kayser C. Spike-phase coding boosts and stabilizes information carried by spatial and temporal spike patterns. Neuron. 2009;61:597–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Buzsaki G. The origin of extracellular fields and currents – EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:407–420. doi: 10.1038/nrn3241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Lakatos P. The leading sense: supramodal control of neurophysiological context by attention. Neuron. 2009;64:419–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Montemurro M.A. Phase-of-firing coding of natural visual stimuli in primary visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:375–380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Rey H.G. Timing of single-neuron and local field potential responses in the human medial temporal lobe. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:299–304. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Canolty R.T. Oscillatory phase coupling coordinates anatomically dispersed functional cell assemblies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2010;107:17356–17361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008306107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Kerr J.N., Denk W. Imaging in vivo: watching the brain in action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:195–205. doi: 10.1038/nrn2338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Lopes da Silva F. EEG and MEG: relevance to neuroscience. Neuron. 2013;80:1112–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schneidman E. Synergy from silence in a combinatorial neural code. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:15732–15741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0301-09.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.He B.J. Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:480–487. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Whittingstall K., Logothetis N.K. Frequency-band coupling in surface EEG reflects spiking activity in monkey visual cortex. Neuron. 2009;64:281–289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Lakatos P. An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1904–1911. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Thut G. The functional importance of rhythmic activity in the brain. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:R658–R663. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Ng B.S. A precluding but not ensuring role of entrained low-frequency oscillations for auditory perception. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:12268–12276. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1877-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Jensen O. Temporal coding organized by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37:357–369. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Ding N. Robust cortical entrainment to the speech envelope relies on the spectro-temporal fine structure. Neuroimage. 2013;88C:41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Mazzoni A. Encoding of naturalistic stimuli by local field potential spectra in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2008;4:e1000239. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Fries P. The gamma cycle. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:309–316. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Brunel N., Wang X.J. What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation-inhibition balance. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;90:415–430. doi: 10.1152/jn.01095.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]