Abstract
Dominant and subordinate dispositions are not only determined genetically but also nurtured by environmental stimuli during neuroendocrine development. However, the relationship between early life environment and dominance behavior remains elusive. Using the IntelliCage-based competition task for group-housed mice, we have previously described two cases in which environmental insults during the developmental period altered the outcome of dominance behavior later in life. First, mice that were repeatedly isolated from their mother and their littermates (early deprivation; ED), and second, mice perinatally exposed to an environmental pollutant, dioxin, both exhibited subordinate phenotypes, defined by decreased occupancy of limited resource sites under highly competitive circumstances. Similar alterations found in the cortex and limbic area of these two models are suggestive of the presence of neural systems shared across generalized dominance behavior.
Keywords: dominance behavior, social behavior, early life environment, IntelliCage-based competition task, mouse
Introduction
Social dominance is a universal behavioral feature exhibited by social animals across species and is considered one of the few robust and reliable social behavior indices in experimental animals. Dominance behavior is exhibited primarily in competitive situations, where individuals with tendencies to dictate to others are referred to as dominant, whereas those being dictated to are referred to as subordinates (Rowell, 1974). Generally, dominant individuals gain priority of access to resources and copulation (Dewsbury, 1982; Akbaripasand et al., 2014), reflecting the ecological significance of social dominance.
Numerous intrinsic factors are thought to be involved in generating dominance behavior, such as levels of aggressiveness and anxiety (Chase et al., 2002), and dominance behavior has been used as an indicator to study affective disorders in experimental animals (Malatynska and Knapp, 2005). Although there is a substantial genetic influence determining these intrinsic characteristics (Braw et al., 2006; Malkesman et al., 2006; Babri et al., 2014), the development of social behavioral disposition is presumably nurtured by the environment as well. In particular, social environment in early life has a profound influence on the development of the social brain (Champagne and Curley, 2005) and the subsequent expression of social behaviors in adulthood (Fleming et al., 1999; Veenema, 2012; Branchi et al., 2013). Manipulations of the neonatal social environment are widely used experimental procedures in rodents and primates to investigate the developmental consequences of stress, childhood adversity, or trauma during early life. The use of such animal models has proven successful in advancing our understanding of how mother–infant and peer interactions, for instance, alter developmental trajectories. Alterations in aggressive and anxiety traits have been recognized in rats that were repeatedly isolated from their mother (maternal separation; MS) and their littermates (early deprivation; ED) during the neonatal period (Biagini et al., 1998; Marmendal et al., 2006; Rees et al., 2006) or post-weaning period (Toth et al., 2012). These dispositions arise presumably from abnormalities in the stress response system comprising the corticolimbic circuit and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Pryce et al., 2011; Birnie et al., 2013; Rincon-Cortes and Sullivan, 2014), in which its developmental programming is susceptible to stressful stimuli during critical periods in life. This observation is supported epidemiologically, with parental loss, physical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect having been shown to be important for determining developmental outcomes, including neuroendocrine stress response (Laurent et al., 2014). Other environmental factors known to modify affective or social behavior in maturity include perinatal exposure to toxic chemicals (Disney et al., 2008; Haijima et al., 2010; Xu et al., 2012; Hamilton et al., 2014; Kiryanova and Dyck, 2014), and some of these chemical-induced behavioral abnormalities are associated with alterations in the stress response system (Glavas et al., 2007; Poimenova et al., 2010).
It is hypothesized that early life environment, particularly one that affects the neuroendocrine stress response system, shapes the neural basis of social behavior, which in turn may contribute to the hierarchical status within a group later in life. However, the contribution of the early-life environment to social dominance is largely unknown. Here we describe two mouse models that exhibit subordinate behavior in adulthood as a result of insults during development: neonatal ED manipulation (Benner et al., 2014) and perinatal exposure to an environmental pollutant, dioxin (Endo et al., 2012). We will also discuss the possible neurological foundations underlying social dominance.
Methods for assessing dominance
Social dominance in wild animals is often determined by field observations (Gesquiere et al., 2011). Although replicating a true natural setting is a challenge in a laboratory-based experimental setup, machine-based behavioral phenotyping technologies specialized for monitoring colonies of mice have been developed (Freund et al., 2013; Ohayon et al., 2013; Weissbrod et al., 2013). They may be developed further in the near future to provide suitable tools for evaluating complex social structures such as hierarchy. Currently, however, a hierarchy is commonly assessed based on a dominant or a subordinate phenotype exhibited by one-to-one competitions, e.g., the tube test (Lindzey et al., 1961), the social interaction test (Coura et al., 2013), the urine-marking assay (Desjardins et al., 1973; Drickamer, 2001), the dominant–submissive relationship (DSR) paradigm (Feder et al., 2010), and the resident intruder test (Kaliste-Korhonen and Eskola, 2000). In other words, dominance hierarchies have been studied under the premise that dominant–subordinate relationships between pairs of individuals account for the overall hierarchical structure of a colony. Because no more than two mice can be tested at a time in the above paradigms, the efficiency of generating rankings within the tested colony is greatly compromised.
We have recently established a behavioral test protocol for quantifying dominance behavior in group-housed mice (Endo et al., 2012) using a commercially available machine-based behavioral phenotyping system called an IntelliCage apparatus (Galsworthy et al., 2005) (Figure 1A). The IntelliCage-based competition task is contextually similar to the paradigm presented in the visible burrow system established for rats (Blanchard et al., 1988, 1995). In both systems, the individual animal's behaviors are assessed in a social environment, and a group of mice is subjected to a social stress resulting from competition for resources. In the visible burrow system, animals are classified as dominant or subordinate by agonistic interactions (attacks and guarding behavior) manually scored by video monitoring. In the IntelliCage-based competition task, the mice that occupy the limited resource sites at the beginning of the session are classified as dominants, while those that fail to achieve access to the resource sites are classified as subordinates.
In the competition task protocol, mice are deprived of water throughout the day, except during the 3 h session period between 2200 and 0100, to establish motivation for accessing the corner chambers for water as a reward (Figure 1B). Once a session begins, over a dozen mice compete against each other, as in a game of musical chairs, for the water in the limited access sites situated in the four corner chambers (Figure 1C). Because of the daylong water deprivation, the competition is greatest at the beginning of the session (approximately 22:00–22:05), and the occupancy of the corner chambers is monopolized by the dominant mice. During the following period (approximately 22:05–22:10), the subordinate mice can gain access to the corner chambers. After a while, the intense competition subsides. In this system, the mode of competition can be manipulated by adjusting the number of mice in a cage and the number of available corner chambers (Figure 1D).
Competitive subordinance in group-housed mice
We have previously shown that ED mice, generated by isolating neonates from their mother and littermates for 3 h per day for the first 2 weeks after birth (Pryce and Feldon, 2003; Millstein et al., 2006), exhibit subordinate behavior in the IntelliCage-based competition task (Benner et al., 2014). We have also shown that mice perinatally exposed to a low dose of dioxin, a ubiquitous environmental pollutant, exhibit subordinate behavior in adulthood (Endo et al., 2012). In both cases, the subordinate behavior was attributable to developmental abnormality that occurred during early life, long before the time at which the behavioral tests were conducted.
The subordinate behaviors were persistently present throughout the competition task sessions for both the ED mice and the mice perinatally exposed to dioxin. A reasonable hypothesis is that the subordinate mice's motivation toward the reward is lower than that of the dominant mice, and accordingly accounts for decreased occupancy of the corner chambers. In the IntelliCage-based competition task, the level of motivation can be assessed by several means as follows: (i) evaluating the water consumption under a basal, non-competitive condition; (ii) evaluating the total dwell time and frequency of visits made within the session. If all of the mice have an equal level of motivation, an equal duration and number of total visits would be expected, although the timing of the visits may differ depending on the dominance behavior; and (iii) evaluating the subordinate mice's motivation for drinking in the absence of dominant mice (Figure 1D).
It is notable that in both mice models, the subordinate mice did not differ from the dominant mice in terms of water consumption per day and motivation for drinking water at the beginning of the water-availability period. Furthermore, the removal of the dominant mice from the cage ameliorates the subordinate mice's visiting behavior. Taken together, these observations emphasize that social environment plays an imperative role in determining the behavior of these mice, and that the early life environment can alter the vulnerability to social–emotional challenges in adulthood. The subordinate behavior may reflect a social–phobic temperament, resembling that of social anxiety disorder or autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in humans. In contrast, an abnormality in competitive dominance may be manifested in the hyperdominance of individuals, a behavior considered suggestive of conduct disorder observed in humans.
Possible neural basis of dominance behavior
The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is one of the major brain regions associated with the dominant–subordinate phenotype assessed by the IntelliCage-based competition task. This observation is consistent with previous reports on animals (Gesquiere et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2011) and humans (Zink et al., 2008; Freeman et al., 2009). In ED mice, the expression of the Map2 gene, which is considered to be involved in dendritic remodeling associated with synaptic plasticity, is significantly reduced in mPFC, and a significant correlation is observed between the dominance level and Map2 expression level (Benner et al., 2014). This observation is consistent with a previous report describing a significant association between dominance rank and synaptic efficiency in mPFC in mice (Wang et al., 2011). The mPFC of mice born to dams perinatally exposed to a low dose of dioxin showed reduced expression of the immediate early genes (IEGs), c-Fos and Arc, indicating reduced neuronal activity (Endo et al., 2012). The mPFC is considered to undergo experience-dependent changes. For example, social experience-related reductions in dendritic spine density and IEG expression in mPFC were found in rats exposed to ethanol during gestation (Hamilton et al., 2010). The prefrontal acetylcholine system has recently been shown to be involved in dominance behavior characterized by the social interaction test (Coura et al., 2013). In addition to the relationship of mPFC with the dominance trait, fMRI studies of humans showed that mPFC is associated with social phobia (Blair et al., 2010) and social anxiety disorders (Shang et al., 2014).
The amygdala is another brain region in which ED mice and dioxin-exposed mice share similar neurological characteristics (Endo et al., 2012; Benner et al., 2014). In both cases, c-Fos expression was elevated in the basolateral amygdala (BLA), and its expression level was inversely correlated with dominance rank in the ED study. BLA plays important regulating roles in anticipatory anxiety (Savonenko et al., 1999), social cue processing (Adolphs, 2001; Truitt et al., 2007), and stimulus–reward processing (Murray, 2007). Its function is strongly affected by early life stress both in humans (Marusak et al., 2014; Suzuki et al., 2014) and rodents (Caldji et al., 1998; Berman et al., 2014; Tzanoulinou et al., 2014). Amygdala activity habituates to repeated presentations of social stimuli in healthy subjects (Wedig et al., 2005), suggesting its role in social adaptation. However, abnormal BLA excitation has been suggested to occur in social anxiety disorder and ASD (Truitt et al., 2007; Kleinhans et al., 2009). BLA is particularly sensitive to early life stress and has a critical window (Koppensteiner et al., 2014). Children who experienced early life stress were observed to have enhanced amygdala activity (Maheu et al., 2010; Tottenham, 2012; Gee et al., 2013). Importantly, functional connectivity between the mPFC and amygdala has been recognized (Likhtik et al., 2005, 2014). The integrity of the mPFC–amygdala circuit is hypothesized to be a critical determinant of the self-regulation of socio-emotional behavior in response to one's social environment, characteristically disrupted in patients with ASD (Bachevalier and Loveland, 2006).
Social recognition and social memory are thought to contribute to the maintenance of the dominance hierarchy. Social memory, distinct from other types of memory, involves a special neural circuit relaying signals from olfactory social cues (e.g., pheromones) to the medial amygdala (MeA), which innervates the lateral septum (LS) and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST). The neural circuit that involves the regions listed above is highly stress-responsive and regulates aggressive behavior (Ferguson et al., 2002; Nelson and Trainor, 2007). The neuropeptides vasopressin and oxytocin regulate social behavior and stress responses, and the role of oxytocin receptors in the long-term establishment of dominance hierarchies has been reported (Timmer et al., 2011).
Stress and dominance
An association between dominance behavior and neuroendocrine stress response has been an intriguing subject in the field of social neuroscience. Experiencing dominance hierarchies can be stressful to both subordinate and dominant individuals (Blanchard et al., 1995; Gesquiere et al., 2011), and neuroendocrine characteristics associated with the stress of being subordinate have been reviewed (Blanchard et al., 1993). In general, social subordinance is associated with hypercortisolism or feedback resistance (Sapolsky et al., 1997), whereas glucocorticoid signaling is involved in agonistic behaviors, including dominance, under conditions when hierarchy has not been established. Corticosterone administration affects aggressive behavior in resident intruder conflicts (Mikics et al., 2004), but does not affect intracolony aggression in colonies that have already been established to have stable social relationships (Mikics et al., 2007). However, glucocorticoids are thought to play a critical role in the establishment of a dominance hierarchy and in the long-term maintenance of dominant–subordinate relationships. Rats exposed to stresses just before the first social encounter tend to become subordinate toward unfamiliar rats that were not exposed to the same stresses and have similar attributes, such as body weight and trait anxiety; and the dominant–subordinate relationship established between a given pair of rats persists over time (Cordero and Sandi, 2007). It is thus implied that sensitivity and reactivity toward the stress response (HPA axis function and regulation) have a major effect not only on determining the hierarchical phenotype at the time of a first social encounter but also on the long-term maintenance of an individual's dominance behavior.
Importantly, the integrity of the neuroendocrine stress response system can be modulated by external insults such as disrupted neonatal social environment and perinatal exposure to a neurotoxic chemical. Accumulating reports show that the HPA axis is programmed, at least in part, by early-life events (Matthews, 2002). In particular, early-life stress can modify the development of HPA functioning and thereby influence behavior as well as susceptibility to certain diseases in adulthood. In non-human primates, prenatal stress, experimentally induced by gestational glucocorticoid exposure, influences social play behavior and HPA axis function (Mustoe et al., 2014). Similarly, hyperaggressive traits have been observed with repeated corticosterone administration to peripubertal rats (Veenit et al., 2013).
Previous studies have shown the effects of ED on behavior in adulthood and HPA axis function (Ruedi-Bettschen et al., 2004, 2006; Marmendal et al., 2006; Rees et al., 2006, 2008). However, the developmental toxicity to the neuroendocrine stress response system of perinatal dioxin exposure has not been thoroughly assessed in mice. The HPA axis manifests acute toxicity upon dioxin exposure in primates (Shridhar et al., 2001) and rats (Balk and Piper, 1984; Bestervelt et al., 1993). For example, TCDD administration increases adrenal sensitivity to adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in adult rats (Dibartolomeis et al., 1987). In addition, pituitary gland toxicities have been shown in vivo (Moore et al., 1989) and in vitro, resulting in, for example, increases in the gene expression of the ACTH precursor proopiomelanocortin (POMC) (Bestervelt et al., 1998; Huang et al., 2000, 2002) and ACTH and corticosterone secretion (Pitt et al., 2000). Recent studies have shown that prenatal dioxin exposure reduces the expression of pituitary hormones (Takeda et al., 2014) and decreases the circulating level of corticosterone in pregnant dams and their fetuses. This response causes in utero growth retardation that can be rescued by supplying corticosterone to dioxin-exposed dams (Hattori et al., 2014). These findings suggest that the HPA axis is disrupted in the perinatal dioxin exposure model.
Conclusions
We have described two cases in which early-life environmental manipulations have induced alterations in dominance behavior. These studies extend previous observations that social behavior can be shaped by environment, and show that competitive dominance is a robust, reliable, and also highly sensitive trait allowing the evaluation of the effects of developmental insults on neuroendocrinological systems in mice. Dominance is presumably more complex than one-to-one competition and is highly dependent on the social environment. The IntelliCage-based competition task permits the determination of the individual mouse's level of dominance in a group, given that the task is presented simultaneously to over a dozen mice in a single apparatus. Thus, it is considered that the dominance in this test represents not merely competitive but social dominance. In addition, an evaluation of the correlation between the level of dominance and the gene expression patterns in the ED model cannot be achieved by other standardized behavioral assays used to investigate the social status in rodents.
Author contributions
SB, TE, MK, and CT wrote the paper.
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported in part by JSPS Research Fellowships for Young Scientists (SB and TE), JSPS Kakenhi (26241016 to MK and 24221003 to CT), the Health and Labor Sciences Research Grant from MHLW (MK), and by MEXT SRPBS (10036021 to CT).
References
- Adolphs R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 231–239. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Akbaripasand A., Krkosek M., Lokman P. M., Closs G. P. (2014). Does social status within a dominance hierarchy mediate individual growth, residency and relocation? Oecologia 176, 771–779. 10.1007/s00442-014-3038-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babri S., Doosti M. H., Salari A. A. (2014). Strain-dependent effects of prenatal maternal immune activation on anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in offspring. Brain Behav. Immun. 37, 164–176. 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachevalier J., Loveland K. A. (2006). The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 97–117. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balk J. L., Piper W. N. (1984). Altered blood levels of corticosteroids in the rat after exposure to 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin. Biochem. Pharmacol. 33, 2531–2534. 10.1016/0006-2952(84)90732-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benner S., Endo T., Endo N., Kakeyama M., Tohyama C. (2014). Early deprivation induces competitive subordinance in C57BL/6 male mice. Physiol. Behav. 137, 42–52. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman A. K., Lott R. B., Donaldson S. T. (2014). Periodic maternal deprivation may modulate offspring anxiety-like behavior through mechanisms involving neuroplasticity in the amygdala. Brain Res. Bull. 101, 7–11. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2013.12.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bestervelt L. L., Pitt J. A., Nolan C. J., Cai Y., Piper D. W., Dybowski J. A., et al. (1998). In vitro 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin interference with the anterior pituitary hormone adrenocorticortropin. Toxicol. Sci. 44, 107–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bestervelt L. L., Pitt J. A., Nolan C. J., Piper W. N. (1993). TCDD alters pituitary-adrenal function. II: evidence for decreased bioactivity of ACTH. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 15, 371–376. 10.1016/0892-0362(93)90053-Q [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biagini G., Pich E. M., Carani C., Marrama P., Agnati L. F. (1998). Postnatal maternal separation during the stress hyporesponsive period enhances the adrenocortical response to novelty in adult rats by affecting feedback regulation in the CA1 hippocampal field. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 16, 187–197. 10.1016/S0736-5748(98)00019-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birnie A. K., Taylor J. H., Cavanaugh J., French J. A. (2013). Quality of maternal and paternal care predicts later stress reactivity in the cooperatively-breeding marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi). Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 3003–3014. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.08.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair K. S., Geraci M., Hollon N., Otero M., Devido J., Majestic C., et al. (2010). Social norm processing in adult social phobia: atypically increased ventromedial frontal cortex responsiveness to unintentional (embarrassing) transgressions. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 1526–1532. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09121797 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard D. C., Sakai R. R., McEwen B., Weiss S. M., Blanchard R. J. (1993). Subordination stress: behavioral, brain, and neuroendocrine correlates. Behav. Brain Res. 58, 113–121. 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90096-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard D. C., Spencer R. L., Weiss S. M., Blanchard R. J., McEwen B., Sakai R. R. (1995). Visible burrow system as a model of chronic social stress: behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20, 117–134. 10.1016/0306-4530(94)E0045-B [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard R. J., Flannelly K. J., Blanchard D. C. (1988). Life-span studies of dominance and aggression in established colonies of laboratory rats. Physiol. Behav. 43, 1–7. 10.1016/0031-9384(88)90089-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branchi I., Curley J. P., D'Andrea I., Cirulli F., Champagne F. A., Alleva E. (2013). Early interactions with mother and peers independently build adult social skills and shape BDNF and oxytocin receptor brain levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 522–532. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braw Y., Malkesman O., Dagan M., Bercovich A., Lavi-Avnon Y., Schroeder M., et al. (2006). Anxiety-like behaviors in pre-pubertal rats of the Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) and Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) animal models of depression. Behav. Brain Res. 167, 261–269. 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldji C., Tannenbaum B., Sharma S., Francis D., Plotsky P. M., Meaney M. J. (1998). Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 5335–5340. 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Champagne F. A., Curley J. P. (2005). How social experiences influence the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 704–709. 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase I. D., Tovey C., Spangler-Martin D., Manfredonia M. (2002). Individual differences versus social dynamics in the formation of animal dominance hierarchies. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 5744–5749. 10.1073/pnas.082104199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordero M. I., Sandi C. (2007). Stress amplifies memory for social hierarchy. Front. Neurosci. 1, 175–184. 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.013.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coura R. S., Cressant A., Xia J., de Chaumont F., Olivo-Marin J. C., Pelloux Y., et al. (2013). Nonaggressive and adapted social cognition is controlled by the interplay between noradrenergic and nicotinic receptor mechanisms in the prefrontal cortex. FASEB J. 27, 4343–4354. 10.1096/fj.13-231084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desjardins C., Maruniak J. A., Bronson F. H. (1973). Social rank in house mice: differentiation revealed by ultraviolet visualization of urinary marking patterns. Science 182, 939–941. 10.1126/science.182.4115.939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dewsbury D. A. (1982). Dominance rank, copulatory behavior, and differential reproduction. Q. Rev. Biol. 57, 135–159. 10.1086/412672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dibartolomeis M. J., Moore R. W., Peterson R. E., Christian B. J., Jefcoate C. R. (1987). Altered regulation of adrenal steroidogenesis in 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin-treated rats. Biochem. Pharmacol. 36, 59–67. 10.1016/0006-2952(87)90382-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disney E. R., Iacono W., McGue M., Tully E., Legrand L. (2008). Strengthening the case: prenatal alcohol exposure is associated with increased risk for conduct disorder. Pediatrics 122, e1225–e1230. 10.1542/peds.2008-1380 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drickamer L. C. (2001). Urine marking and social dominance in male house mice (Mus musculus domesticus). Behav. Processes 53, 113–120. 10.1016/S0376-6357(00)00152-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endo T., Kakeyama M., Uemura Y., Haijima A., Okuno H., Bito H., et al. (2012). Executive function deficits and social-behavioral abnormality in mice exposed to a low dose of dioxin in utero and via lactation. PLoS ONE 7:e50741. 10.1371/journal.pone.0050741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feder Y., Nesher E., Ogran A., Kreinin A., Malatynska E., Yadid G., et al. (2010). Selective breeding for dominant and submissive behavior in Sabra mice. J. Affect. Disord. 126, 214–222. 10.1016/j.jad.2010.03.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson J. N., Young L. J., Insel T. R. (2002). The neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 23, 200–224. 10.1006/frne.2002.0229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming A. S., O'Day D. H., Kraemer G. W. (1999). Neurobiology of mother–infant interactions: experience and central nervous system plasticity across development and generations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 673–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman J. B., Rule N. O., Adams R. B., Jr., Ambady N. (2009). Culture shapes a mesolimbic response to signals of dominance and subordination that associates with behavior. Neuroimage 47, 353–359. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freund J., Brandmaier A. M., Lewejohann L., Kirste I., Kritzler M., Kruger A., et al. (2013). Emergence of individuality in genetically identical mice. Science 340, 756–759. 10.1126/science.1235294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galsworthy M. J., Amrein I., Kuptsov P. A., Poletaeva I. I., Zinn P., Rau A., et al. (2005). A comparison of wild-caught wood mice and bank voles in the Intellicage: assessing exploration, daily activity patterns and place learning paradigms. Behav. Brain Res. 157, 211–217. 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.06.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gee D. G., Gabard-Durnam L. J., Flannery J., Goff B., Humphreys K. L., Telzer E. H., et al. (2013). Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 15638–15643. 10.1073/pnas.1307893110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gesquiere L. R., Learn N. H., Simao M. C., Onyango P. O., Alberts S. C., Altmann J. (2011). Life at the top: rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science 333, 357–360. 10.1126/science.1207120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glavas M. M., Ellis L., Yu W. K., Weinberg J. (2007). Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure on basal limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal regulation: role of corticosterone. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 1598–1610. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00460.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haijima A., Endo T., Zhang Y., Miyazaki W., Kakeyama M., Tohyama C. (2010). In utero and lactational exposure to low doses of chlorinated and brominated dioxins induces deficits in the fear memory of male mice. Neurotoxicology 31, 385–390. 10.1016/j.neuro.2010.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton D. A., Barto D., Rodriguez C. I., Magcalas C. M., Fink B. C., Rice J. P., et al. (2014). Effects of moderate prenatal ethanol exposure and age on social behavior, spatial response perseveration errors and motor behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 269, 44–54. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton D. A., Candelaria-Cook F. T., Akers K. G., Rice J. P., Maes L. I., Rosenberg M., et al. (2010). Patterns of social-experience-related c-fos and Arc expression in the frontal cortices of rats exposed to saccharin or moderate levels of ethanol during prenatal brain development. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 66–74. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hattori Y., Takeda T., Fujii M., Taura J., Ishii Y., Yamada H. (2014). Dioxin-induced fetal growth retardation: the role of a preceding attenuation in the circulating level of glucocorticoid. Endocrine 47, 572–580. 10.1007/s12020-014-0257-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang P., Ceccatelli S., Håkansson H., Grandison L., Rannug A. (2002). Constitutive and TCDD-induced expression of ah receptor-responsive genes in the pituitary. Neurotoxicology 23, 783–793. 10.1016/S0161-813X(02)00040-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang P., Rannug A., Ahlbom E., Hakansson H., Ceccatelli S. (2000). Effect of 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin on the expression of cytochrome P450 1A1, the aryl hydrocarbon receptor, and the aryl hydrocarbon receptor nuclear translocator in rat brain and pituitary. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 169, 159–167. 10.1006/taap.2000.9064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaliste-Korhonen E., Eskola S. (2000). Fighting in NIH/S male mice: consequences for behaviour in resident-intruder tests and physiological parameters. Lab. Anim. 34, 189–198. 10.1258/002367700780457626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiryanova V., Dyck R. H. (2014). Increased aggression, improved spatial memory, and reduced anxiety-like behaviour in adult male mice exposed to fluoxetine early in life. Dev. Neurosci. 36, 396–408. 10.1159/000363102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleinhans N. M., Johnson L. C., Richards T., Mahurin R., Greenson J., Dawson G., et al. (2009). Reduced neural habituation in the amygdala and social impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 467–475. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koppensteiner P., Aizawa S., Yamada D., Kabuta T., Boehm S., Wada K., et al. (2014). Age-dependent sensitivity to glucocorticoids in the developing mouse basolateral nucleus of the amygdala. Psychoneuroendocrinology 46, 64–77. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laurent H. K., Neiderhiser J. M., Natsuaki M. N., Shaw D. S., Fisher P. A., Reiss D., et al. (2014). Stress system development from age 4.5 to 6: family environment predictors and adjustment implications of HPA activity stability versus change. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 340–354. 10.1002/dev.21103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Likhtik E., Pelletier J. G., Paz R., Pare D. (2005). Prefrontal control of the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 25, 7429–7437. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2314-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Likhtik E., Stujenske J. M. A., Topiwala M., Harris A. Z., Gordon J. A. (2014). Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear and innate anxiety. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 106–113. 10.1038/nn.3582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lindzey G., Winston H., Manosevitz M. (1961). Social dominance in inbred mouse strains. Nature 191, 474–476. 10.1038/191474a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maheu F. S., Dozier M., Guyer A. E., Mandell D., Peloso E., Poeth K., et al. (2010). A preliminary study of medial temporal lobe function in youths with a history of caregiver deprivation and emotional neglect. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 34–49. 10.3758/CABN.10.1.34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malatynska E., Knapp R. J. (2005). Dominant–submissive behavior as models of mania and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 715–737. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malkesman O., Maayan R., Weizman A., Weller A. (2006). Aggressive behavior and HPA axis hormones after social isolation in adult rats of two different genetic animal models for depression. Behav. Brain Res. 175, 408–414. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marmendal M., Eriksson C. J., Fahlke C. (2006). Early deprivation increases exploration and locomotion in adult male Wistar offspring. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 85, 535–544. 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marusak H. A., Martin K. R., Etkin A., Thomason M. E. (2014). Childhood trauma exposure disrupts the automatic regulation of emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1038/npp.2014.311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews S. G. (2002). Early programming of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 13, 373–380. 10.1016/S1043-2760(02)00690-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikics E., Barsy B., Haller J. (2007). The effect glucocorticoids on aggressiveness in established colonies of rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 160–170. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikics E., Kruk M. R., Haller J. (2004). Genomic and non-genomic effects of glucocorticoids on aggressive behavior in male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 618–635. 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00090-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millstein R. A., Ralph R. J., Yang R. J., Holmes A. (2006). Effects of repeated maternal separation on prepulse inhibition of startle across inbred mouse strains. Genes Brain Behav. 5, 346–354. 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00172.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore R. W., Parsons J. A., Bookstaff R. C., Peterson R. E. (1989). Plasma concentrations of pituitary hormones in 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin-treated male rats. J. Biochem. Toxicol. 4, 165–172. 10.1002/jbt.2570040305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray E. A. (2007). The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 489–497. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustoe A. C., Taylor J. H., Birnie A. K., Huffman M. C., French J. A. (2014). Gestational cortisol and social play shape development of marmosets' HPA functioning and behavioral responses to stressors. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 1229–1243. 10.1002/dev.21203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson R. J., Trainor B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 536–546. 10.1038/nrn2174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohayon S., Avni O., Taylor A. L., Perona P., Roian Egnor S. E. (2013). Automated multi-day tracking of marked mice for the analysis of social behaviour. J. Neurosci. Methods 219, 10–19. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitt J. A., Buckalew A. R., House D. E., Abbott B. D. (2000). Adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) and corticosterone secretion by perifused pituitary and adrenal glands from rodents exposed to 2,3,7, 8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin (TCDD). Toxicology 151, 25–35. 10.1016/S0300-483X(00)00257-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poimenova A., Markaki E., Rahiotis C., Kitraki E. (2010). Corticosterone-regulated actions in the rat brain are affected by perinatal exposure to low dose of bisphenol A. Neuroscience 167, 741–749. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pryce C. R., Aubert Y., Maier C., Pearce P. C., Fuchs E. (2011). The developmental impact of prenatal stress, prenatal dexamethasone and postnatal social stress on physiology, behaviour and neuroanatomy of primate offspring: studies in rhesus macaque and common marmoset. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 214, 33–53 10.1007/s00213-010-1989-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pryce C. R., Feldon J. (2003). Long-term neurobehavioural impact of the postnatal environment in rats: manipulations, effects and mediating mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 57–71. 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00009-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rees S. L., Akbari E., Steiner M., Fleming A. S. (2008). Effects of early deprivation and maternal separation on pup-directed behavior and HPA axis measures in the juvenile female rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 50, 315–321. 10.1002/dev.20292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rees S. L., Steiner M., Fleming A. S. (2006). Early deprivation, but not maternal separation, attenuates rise in corticosterone levels after exposure to a novel environment in both juvenile and adult female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 175, 383–391 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rincon-Cortes M., Sullivan R. M. (2014). Early life trauma and attachment: immediate and enduring effects on neurobehavioral and stress axis development. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne) 5:33. 10.3389/fendo.2014.00033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowell T. E. (1974). The concept of social dominance. Behav. Biol. 11, 131–154. 10.1016/S0091-6773(74)90289-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruedi-Bettschen D., Feldon J., Pryce C. R. (2004). Circadian- and temperature-specific effects of early deprivation on rat maternal care and pup development: short-term markers for long-term effects? Dev. Psychobiol. 45, 59–71. 10.1002/dev.20014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruedi-Bettschen D., Zhang W., Russig H., Ferger B., Weston A., Pedersen E. M., et al. (2006). Early deprivation leads to altered behavioural, autonomic and endocrine responses to environmental challenge in adult Fischer rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 2879–2893. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05158.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapolsky R. M., Alberts S. C., Altmann J. (1997). Hypercortisolism associated with social subordinance or social isolation among wild baboons. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 54, 1137–1143. 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830240097014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savonenko A., Filipkowski R. K., Werka T., Zielinski K., Kaczmarek L. (1999). Defensive conditioning-related functional heterogeneity among nuclei of the rat amygdala revealed by c-Fos mapping. Neuroscience 94, 723–733. 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00331-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shang J., Fu Y., Ren Z., Zhang T., Du M., Gong Q., et al. (2014). The common traits of the ACC and PFC in anxiety disorders in the DSM-5: meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. PLoS ONE 9:e93432. 10.1371/journal.pone.0093432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shridhar S., Farley A., Reid R. L., Foster W. G., Van Vugt D. A. (2001). The effect of 2,3,7,8-tetrachlorodibenzo-p-dioxin on corticotrophin-releasing hormone, arginine vasopressin, and pro-opiomelanocortin mRNA levels in the hypothalamus of the cynomolgus monkey. Toxicol. Sci. 63, 181–188. 10.1093/toxsci/63.2.181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki H., Luby J. L., Botteron K. N., Dietrich R., McAvoy M. P., Barch D. M. (2014). Early life stress and trauma and enhanced limbic activation to emotionally valenced faces in depressed and healthy children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 53, 800.e10–813.e10. 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.04.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeda T., Taura J., Hattori Y., Ishii Y., Yamada H. (2014). Dioxin-induced retardation of development through a reduction in the expression of pituitary hormones and possible involvement of an aryl hydrocarbon receptor in this defect: a comparative study using two strains of mice with different sensitivities to dioxin. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 278, 220–229. 10.1016/j.taap.2014.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timmer M., Cordero M. I., Sevelinges Y., Sandi C. (2011). Evidence for a role of oxytocin receptors in the long-term establishment of dominance hierarchies. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 2349–2356. 10.1038/npp.2011.125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toth M., Tulogdi A., Biro L., Soros P., Mikics E., Haller J. (2012). The neural background of hyper-emotional aggression induced by post-weaning social isolation. Behav. Brain Res. 233, 120–129. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N. (2012). Human amygdala development in the absence of species-expected caregiving. Dev. Psychobiol. 54, 598–611. 10.1002/dev.20531 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truitt W., Sajdyk T., Dietrich A., Oberlin B., McDougle C., Shekhar A. (2007). From anxiety to autism: spectrum of abnormal social behaviors modeled by progressive disruption of inhibitory neuronal function in the basolateral amygdala in Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology(Berl) 191, 107–118. 10.1007/s00213-006-0674-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzanoulinou S., Riccio O., de Boer M. W., Sandi C. (2014). Peripubertal stress-induced behavioral changes are associated with altered expression of genes involved in excitation and inhibition in the amygdala. Transl. Psychiatry 4, e410. 10.1038/tp.2014.54 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenema A. H. (2012). Toward understanding how early-life social experiences alter oxytocin- and vasopressin-regulated social behaviors. Horm. Behav. 61, 304–312. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veenit V., Cordero M. I., Tzanoulinou S., Sandi C. (2013). Increased corticosterone in peripubertal rats leads to long-lasting alterations in social exploration and aggression. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:26. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang F., Zhu J., Zhu H., Zhang Q., Lin Z., Hu H. (2011). Bidirectional control of social hierarchy by synaptic efficacy in medial prefrontal cortex. Science 334, 693–697. 10.1126/science.1209951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wedig M. M., Rauch S. L., Albert M. S., Wright C. I. (2005). Differential amygdala habituation to neutral faces in young and elderly adults. Neurosci. Lett. 385, 114–119. 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.05.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissbrod A., Shapiro A., Vasserman G., Edry L., Dayan M., Yitzhaky A., et al. (2013). Automated long-term tracking and social behavioural phenotyping of animal colonies within a semi-natural environment. Nat. Commun. 4, 2018. 10.1038/ncomms3018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu X., Hong X., Xie L., Li T., Yang Y., Zhang Q., et al. (2012). Gestational and lactational exposure to bisphenol-A affects anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in mice. Horm. Behav. 62, 480–490. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zink C. F., Tong Y., Chen Q., Bassett D. S., Stein J. L., Meyer-Lindenberg A. (2008). Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron 58, 273–283. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]