Abstract
Building upon prior findings of elevated problems among East Coast suburban youth through the 11th grade, this study establishes disproportionately high incidence of maladjustment across three disparate samples: East Coast Suburban youth at the end of their senior year in high school, and 11th and 12th graders in (a) a Northwest suburb and (b) an East Coast city. Both East Coast samples showed pronounced elevations in substance use, whereas the Northwest suburban sample showed marked vulnerability in serious internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Across all samples, parents’ low perceived containment for substance use (lax repercussions on discovering use) was a major vulnerability factor, followed by parents’ knowledge of their teens’ activities. Overall, adolescents’ symptom levels were more strongly related to their relationships with mothers than with fathers. An exception was boys’ apparent vulnerability to fathers’, but not mothers’, perceived depressive symptoms. As with affluent eighth graders, we found that “overscheduling” in extracurriculars is not a critical vulnerability factor among these high school students. Finally, youth reports suggested that most affluent parents do not indiscriminately bail their children out of all problem situations (although a small subset, apparently, do). Results are discussed along with the implications for practice and for future research.
Affluent youth are a “newly identified at-risk group,” according to an editorial statement in Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry (Koplewicz, Gurian, & Williams, 2009, p. 1053). The authors note that affluenza, a metaphorical illness connoting hyperinvestment in material wealth, is rapidly spreading among upper-middle class, white-collar families. The children concomitantly show elevations in various maladjustment domains (substance use, depression, and anxiety), indicating the urgent need for preventive interventions (Koplewicz et al., 2009). In support of their arguments, the authors cite review articles as well as empirical research by our group on suburban youth in the Northeast (Luthar, 2003; Luthar & Becker, 2002; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a).
At this stage, we know little about the generalizability of our early findings. As reported in our 2005 review (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a), our first affluent cohort comprised high schoolers from an affluent Northeastern suburb (Luthar & D’Avanzo, 1999). Cohort II and Cohort III were middle schoolers from another suburb in the Northeast, with the former assessed cross-sectionally and the latter followed over time. Overall, findings with Cohort III, also known as the New England Study of Suburban Youth (NESSY) cohort, did show elevations in problems starting at around the seventh grade (Luthar & Becker, 2002; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005b), and escalating by the 10th and 11th grades (Luthar & Ansary, 2005; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008).
What remains to be seen is the degree to which (a) NESSY students experienced elevated problems even when well past the major stress of college admissions (at the end of high school), and, more importantly, (b) whether our early findings might generalize across disparate geographical areas, which is more important. To illustrate, youth in suburbs might be more prone to boredom, and thus to problem behaviors including substance use, than those in large cities where there are multiple opportunities for “safe” leisure activities such as theater, music, or sports (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a).
Accordingly, in this paper, we present data on three samples of youth. For comparison purposes, we present data from the NESSY cohort as they were completing their senior year of high school. In addition, we present data for 11th and 12th graders from (a) a suburban public school in the Northwest and (b) an independent school in a large East Coast city.
Outcome Domains
In research on resilience and vulnerability, it is logical to focus primarily on those outcome domains that are most threatened by the “risk factor” studied (Luthar, 2006; Luthar & Brown, 2007; Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000). Accordingly, we focus here on spheres where youth in wealthy, ultra achievement-oriented communities are likely to be vulnerable. These include the frequency of substance use (including alcohol, marijuana, and cigarettes), as well as rebellious, rule-breaking behaviors (Ansary & Luthar, 2009; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008). In the internalizing category, our focus is on anxious/depressed as well as somatic problems, which often derive from underlying distress.
We consider these domains with two sets of questions in mind. The first is whether students in any of the three geographically disparate cohorts might actually show “normal” patterns of adjustment problems, when compared against the yardstick of national normative data. The second question concerns vulnerability and protective factors. What are the major dimensions distinguishing youth who overindulge in substance use, for example, compared to those who do not?
Risk Modifiers: Replicating Prior Findings and Exploring New Constructs
In examining potentially salient risk modifiers, our goal is to replicate prior findings and also consider new potentially important constructs. Our first set of our replicative analyses is aimed at corroborating the claim that “overinvolvement” in extracurricular activities is, in essence, a scapegoat in explaining the distress of the most troubled affluent youth. Among eighth graders (the NESSY cohort), Luthar, Shoum, and Brown (2006) showed that the number of hours in extracurriculars explained minimal variance in children’s maladjustment scores; far more important were aspects of family relationships, particularly high levels of parent criticism, and low expectations for adolescents’ achievement.
This pattern may hold true for older teens as well; yet, it is plausible that the pressures from extracurriculars do affect students in the thick of the college application process. In academically elite communities, establishing one’s “stardom” at extracurriculars could, conceivably, be a source of much pressure for high school juniors and seniors.
We also attempt to replicate, across our three disparate samples, the importance of perceived parent “containment,” particularly for substance use (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008). This construct captures youngsters’ beliefs that parents will impose serious consequences for various misbehaviors. Among 11th grade suburban youth, we found that even after considering many other parenting dimensions, parenting containment for substance use was strongly associated with levels of use among both girls and boys (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008).
Finally, we seek to replicate findings on stronger links, with early adolescents’ self-reported distress, of the quality of relationships with their mothers as opposed to fathers (Luthar & Becker, 2002). It is unclear whether trends previously documented among middle school students will also hold true among youth who are in late adolescence.
New Parenting Dimensions Explored
Guided by qualitative research (as we were, in assessing parent containment; see Luthar & Goldstein, 2008), we created a measure to explore another potential risk modifier: parents’ tendencies to bail their teenagers of out problem situations. There are ubiquitous suggestions in the popular press that rather than letting their teens suffer the consequences of rash, illicit behaviors, affluent parents routinely intervene and inappropriately protect their offspring (e.g., Marano, 2008; Mogel, 2001).
A second dimension we explored was adolescent perceptions of parents’ depression. Parental depression is a particularly insidious risk factor, as it pervades not only specific aspects of parent functioning but also the overall ambience in the home (Beardslee, 2002; Hammen, Bistricky, & Ingram, 2010; Luthar & Sexton, 2007). Whereas the effects of maternal depression on youth are well documented, we considered, here, associations for paternal depression as well. In upper middle-class communities, fathers are more often than not the primary wage earners. With the vicissitudes of the American economy over the last decade and several high wage earners losing some or much of their previously guaranteed earnings, distress among affected fathers may well spread to others in the family.
Summary
In his early, pioneering works on resilience, Norman Garmezy presciently showed that family poverty does not necessarily connote youth maladjustment, as several critical protective processes can promote positive outcomes (Garmezy, 1971, 1974). In the last decade, our own research has suggested a parallel postulate at the other end of the socioeconomic status; that is, that family wealth can connote significant risks to adolescents’ adjustment. Based largely on studies of suburban students in the Northeast, we have attempted to illuminate salient factors that differentiate the more poorly functioning “privileged” youth from others.
Extending our past work, in this report on affluent high school students, we seek first to determine if elevations in maladjustment previously seen in the East Coast suburbs might generalize to Northwest suburbs, and to students in a large East Coast city. Second, we seek to replicate earlier findings (a) of the importance of parental containment for substance use, and of relationships with mothers more so than fathers; and (b) that extracurricular activity involvement is not a potent vulnerability factor. Finally, we aim to explore the potential significance of two risk modifiers not previously considered among affluent youth; that is, parents bailing their teens out of “problem situations,” and adolescents’ perceptions of depression in both mothers and fathers.
Methods
Sample
As noted earlier, we present data from the NESSY cohort, involving a group of suburban students first studied when they were in the sixth grade (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005b) and followed annually ever since. Our analyses here are based on (a) the NESSY cohort on completing high school, and on 11th and 12th graders from (b) a suburban public school in the Northwest and (c) an independent school in a large East Coast city.
Characteristics of samples are shown in Table 1. As seen there, most students at all three schools came from Caucasian families, with highly educated parents. Median family incomes were almost three times the national level of about $50,000 in 2000 (United States Bureau of the Census, 2000). Overall, therefore, all three samples were clearly from upper middle class backgrounds.
Table 1.
E-Suburba | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
---|---|---|---|
Grade | 12 | 11 & 12 | 11 & 12 |
Number of | |||
Females | 120 | 262 | 70 |
Males | 132 | 275 | 68 |
Participation | 79.5% | 82.9% | 86% |
White | 88.0% | 67.2% | 75.4% |
Black | 1.6% | 1.1% | 3.6% |
Hispanic | 4.4% | 2.6% | 4.3% |
Asian | 2.8% | 18.0% | 7.2% |
Parents’ education | |||
College degree (mother/father) | 48%/32% | 41%/34% | 30%/27% |
Graduate degree (mother/father) | 37%/52% | 43%/52% | 63%/64% |
Median family incomeb | $153,131 (1999) | $110,830 (1999) | $149,367 (2000) |
Note: E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
Participants in the longitudinal New England Study of Suburban Youth (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008).
From Census data.
As in our prior studies, students’ participation was based on passive consent procedures, as data collections were done as part of school-based initiatives on positive youth development. To ensure that parents were well informed, administrators sent letters to all homes via US mail before each wave of data collection. Students were informed that their participation was voluntary and on completion of data collection, questionnaires were stored with subject numbers as identifiers (for further details on methods and procedures, please see Luthar & Goldstein, 2008; Yates, Tracy, & Luthar, 2008).
Procedure
All three samples were assessed during May of their senior year (2005, 2006, and 2006 for the East-Suburb, West-Suburb, and East-Urban samples, respectively). All measures were administered in groups. NESSY students, who were completing their sixth annual assessment for this study, received a $30 gift certificate. Administrators at the other two schools did not wish to provide students with incentives; despite this, participation rates were over 80% (see Table 1).
Measures
Unless otherwise indicated, all measures have been used in our past research (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008; Luthar et al., 2006) with good reliability and validity. In this study, alpha coefficients of the various subscales were generally similar across schools; in the interest of brevity, we provide average values for girls and boys across the three samples.
Substance use
We used the Monitoring the Future Study Survey (Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 1984), which has well-documented reliability and validity. As in our previous studies (Luthar & Becker, 2002; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008), we created a composite substance use variable by adding scores for cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana. Alpha coefficients in this sample were 0.79 and 0.77 for girls and boys, respectively.
Rule breaking
The Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) contains 112 items encompassing internalizing and externalizing domains (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The externalizing subscale central to our analyses, rule breaking, had a values of 0.76 and 0.77 for females and males, respectively, in this study. (Coefficients for aggressive behavior were 0.81 and 0.84, respectively.)
Anxious–depressed and somatic complaints
These two YSR internalizing subscales (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001), central to our analyses, had respective α values of 0.85 and 0.77 among girls and 0.84 and 0.82 among boys. (Coefficients for withdrawn–depressed were 0.75 and 0.77 among girls and boys, respectively.)
Perceived parent containment
Based on a 14-item, 5-point scale, Luthar and Goldstein (2008) assessed the seriousness of students’ anticipated parental repercussions on discovering different errant teen behaviors across four areas of nonconformity: substance use, delinquency, rudeness, and academic disengagement. In this study, reliability coefficients among females and males respectively were as follows for the respective four subscales: 0.88 and 0.91, 0.84 and 0.84, 0.74 and 0.80, and 0.73 and 0.71.
Closeness to parents
The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987), contains 50 items (25 pertaining to each parent) rated on a 5-point scale. Scores are obtained for an overall attachment score as well as three subscale scores, for which we obtained the following α coefficients for mother/father in this sample: trust: girls, 0.94/0.94, boys, 0.91/0.92; communication: girls, 0.93/0.91, boys, 0.86/0.87; alienation: girls, 0.85/0.83, boys, 0.76/0.79.
Parent criticism
We used the four-item subscale of the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, 1990), including items such as, “I am punished for doing things less than perfectly.” Alpha coefficients were 0.82 and 0.78 among girls and boys, respectively, in this sample.
Parent expectations
The five-item parental expectations subscale of the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (Frost et al., 1990), with items such as, “My parents set very high standards for me,” yielded α coefficients of 0.81 and 0.80 for girls and boys, respectively.
Parent knowledge
Participants were asked about how much their parents know about their activities (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008), via a five-item, 5-point scale. Illustrative items include “My parents know where I am after school,” and “My parents know who my friends are.” Alpha coefficients were 0.84 and 0.79 for girls and boys, respectively.
Extracurricular involvement
Following Luthar et al. (2006), we asked students about the number of hours (range = 0–8 or more) spent in four categories of activities outside of school hours and in the presence of an adult. These categories included sports, art or theater, academic activities, and civic activities.
New parenting dimensions assessed: Parent bailing out
As in our previous research on complex aspects of parent–child relations (Luthar, Doyle, Suchman, & Mayes, 2001), we created a measure, consisting of six vignettes, of situations to assess the degree to which teens expected their parents would bail them out of problem situations. An example: “For a major academic course, you copied part of an essay from an internet source. The teacher discovered this and plans to fail you for cheating. Would your parents protest?” Responses were rated on a 5-point scale (definitely not to definitely, yes; see Appendix A). Internal consistency coefficients for the Parent Bailing Out (PBO) score were 0.73 and 0.72 for girls and boys, respectively.
To explore the validity of this measure, we correlated it with the four-parent containment scores, with which it should, conceptually, show strong links. Results showed only modest associations. Furthermore, simple correlations between PBO and outcome variables were generally small and nonsignificant, clearly indicating low predictive value.
Based on these initial findings, we did not consider PBO scores in multivariate analyses of our central hypotheses. We do, however, provide descriptive responses to the six vignettes (see the Results Section), both to inform current stereotypes about affluent parents and, potentially, to help guide future quantitative research on these issues.
Parent depression
The depression section of the Family History Screen (Weissman et al., 2000) was used to measure teen’s perceptions of depressive symptoms in each parent (dichotomous responses: yes/no). Illustrative items are, “Did anyone in your family ever seem to experience greatly reduced interest or pleasure in everyday activities?” and “Did anyone ever seem more irritable than usual?” Alpha coefficients for mothers/fathers, respectively, were 0.67/0.64 among girls and 0.58/0.61 among boys.
Despite the modest reliability, both mother and father perceived depression showed multiple correlations with several parenting dimensions as well as outcomes. Thus, we elected to include this dimension in multivariate tests of central hypotheses.
Results
Descriptive data
Table 2 presents means and standard deviations on all variables, separately by school and gender. A two-way (School × Gender) multivariate analysis of variance1 showed significant main effects for school (Wilks λ = 0.20, p < .001), and gender (Wilks λ = 0.81, p < .001). The interaction effect was not significant.
Table 2.
All Schools
|
E-Suburb
|
W-Suburb
|
E-Urban
|
|||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Girls | Boys | Girls | Boys | Girls | Boys | Girls | Boys | School | Gender | |
|
||||||||||
Variable | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | F (η2) | F (η2) |
Outcome Variables | ||||||||||
| ||||||||||
Substance use | 5.16 (5.11) | 5.48 (5.37) | 7.18 (5.57) | 6.57 (5.48) | 3.60 (4.31) | 4.73 (5.41) | 7.57 (4.95) | 6.32 (4.41) | 32.20*** (0.07) | 0.38 (0.01) |
Rule breaking | 5.79 (3.98) | 6.15 (4.20) | 5.70 (3.57) | 5.67 (3.90) | 5.98 (4.28) | 6.63 (4.56) | 5.21 (3.44) | 5.17 (2.70) | 4.74** (0.01) | 0.01 (0.00) |
T scorea | 59 | 60 | 59 | 58 | 59 | 60 | 59 | 58 | ||
Aggressive | 6.06 (4.45) | 5.48 (4.71) | 5.21 (4.08) | 4.58 (4.39) | 6.35 (4.59) | 5.92 (4.95) | 6.48 (4.42) | 5.45 (4.08) | 6.34** (0.01) | 3.85 (0.00) |
T score | 53 | 52 | 52 | 51 | 53 | 52 | 53 | 52 | ||
Anxious–depressed | 6.40 (4.75) | 3.53 (3.81) | 4.32 (3.80) | 2.19 (3.30) | 7.07 (4.87) | 4.04 (3.95) | 7.47 (4.76) | 4.17 (3.59) | 76.24*** (0.06) | 28.99*** (0.08) |
T score | 56 | 54 | 52 | 52 | 59 | 57 | 59 | 57 | ||
Withdrawn–depressed | 3.45 (2.72) | 2.64 (2.64) | 2.83 (2.46) | 2.22 (4.42) | 3.67 (2.82) | 2.86 (2.76) | 3.73 (2.64) | 2.64 (2.59) | 6.62** (0.02) | 16.28*** (0.02) |
T score | 55 | 54 | 52 | 54 | 55 | 54 | 55 | 54 | ||
Somatic | 3.91 (3.37) | 2.17 (2.88) | 3.29 (2.89) | 2.00 (2.92) | 4.03 (3.50) | 2.29 (3.00) | 4.55 (3.56) | 2.06 (2.27) | 2.78 (0.01) | 55.86*** (0.06) |
T score | 54 | 52 | 54 | 51 | 57 | 52 | 57 | 51 | ||
Internalizing | 13.72 (9.24) | 8.28 (7.94) | 8.74 (7.64) | 6.29 (7.30) | 14.73 (7.64) | 9.15 (7.30) | 15.49 (9.48) | 8.74 (7.19) | 15.68*** (0.04) | 68.36*** (0.07) |
Externalizing | 11.82 (7.56) | 11.62 (8.21) | 10.91 (6.73) | 10.26 (7.66) | 12.29 (8.08) | 12.55 (8.80) | 11.66 (6.80) | 10.51 (5.96) | 5.09** (0.01) | 0.70 (0.01) |
| ||||||||||
Predictor Variables: Perceptions of Parents | ||||||||||
| ||||||||||
Criticism | 9.28 (3.95) | 8.75 (3.51) | 8.33 (3.84) | 8.23 (3.34) | 10.03 (3.93) | 9.31 (3.52) | 8.19 (3.60) | 7.63 (3.45) | 19.14*** (0.04) | 2.66 (0.00) |
Expectations | 15.51 (4.90) | 15.18 (4.64) | 13.98 (4.89) | 13.60 (4.63) | 16.39 (4.72) | 16.08 (4.37) | 14.99 (4.91) | 14.81 (4.90) | 23.51*** (0.05) | 0.65 (0.00) |
Knowledge | 22.66 (4.80) | 22.51 (4.02) | 21.49 (5.42) | 22.44 (3.39) | 23.87 (4.50) | 22.83 (4.41) | 22.63 (3.95) | 22.25 (3.57) | 9.15*** (0.02) | 0.22 (0.00) |
Containmentb | ||||||||||
Drugs | 13.17 (5.11) | 12.87 (4.69) | 11.44 (4.72) | 11.20 (4.22) | 14.36 (5.05) | 13.85 (4.74) | 11.70 (4.80) | 12.27 (4.37) | 33.08*** (0.07) | 0.03 (0.00) |
Delinquency | 13.07 (2.33) | 12.68 (2.53) | 13.34 (1.93) | 12.52 (2.57) | 12.91 (2.54) | 12.51 (2.60) | 12.93 (2.49) | 13.20 (2.10) | 1.45 (0.00) | 2.73† (0.00) |
Rudeness | 13.37 (3.55) | 12.72 (3.31) | 13.98 (3.56) | 12.66 (3.11) | 13.04 (3.69) | 12.56 (3.46) | 13.31 (3.15) | 13.20 (3.05) | 2.36 (0.01) | 5.71* (0.01) |
Academics | 10.62 (2.72) | 10.81 (2.75) | 10.40 (2.46) | 10.61 (2.67) | 10.60 (2.92) | 10.82 (2.92) | 11.10 (2.33) | 11.20 (2.13) | 2.40 (0.01) | 0.69 (0.00) |
Trust | ||||||||||
Mother | 40.07 (8.98) | 39.96 (7.74) | 40.18 (8.50) | 39.35 (6.36) | 39.52 (9.35) | 39.53 (8.37) | 41.95 (8.20) | 42.83 (6.98) | 6.54** (0.01) | 0.00 (0.00) |
Father | 39.61 (8.20) | 39.37 (7.89) | 39.37 (7.92) | 38.86 (6.92) | 38.82 (8.31) | 39.25 (8.20) | 42.90 (7.54) | 40.81 (8.27) | 7.08*** (0.02) | 1.39 (0.00) |
Alienation | ||||||||||
Mother | 14.77 (5.26) | 13.65 (4.32) | 13.87 (4.69) | 13.54 (3.86) | 15.18 (5.74) | 14.01 (4.59) | 14.79 (5.93) | 12.49 (3.90) | 3.74* (0.01) | 11.10*** (0.01) |
Father | 15.53 (5.19) | 13.87 (4.55) | 14.95 (4.13) | 13.89 (4.38) | 15.81 (5.26) | 13.91 (4.69) | 15.44 (6.35) | 13.67 (4.38) | 0.73 (0.00) | 17.31*** (0.02) |
Communication | ||||||||||
Mother | 33.03 (8.75) | 30.88 (7.08) | 33.60 (7.60) | 31.04 (5.66) | 32.42 (9.12) | 30.21 (7.56) | 34.32 (9.14) | 33.16 (7.25) | 5.39** (0.01) | 10.57** (0.01) |
Father | 30.01 (8.59) | 29.40 (7.54) | 29.92 (7.54) | 29.62 (6.48) | 29.23 (8.72) | 28.93 (7.79) | 33.03 (9.14) | 30.85 (8.29) | 6.88** (0.02) | 2.33 (0.00) |
Depression | ||||||||||
Mother | 4.55 (3.14) | 3.65 (2.82) | 4.12 (3.00) | 3.47 (2.64) | 4.16 (3.41) | 3.56 (2.93) | 5.46 (3.85) | 4.32 (2.67) | 6.50** (0.02) | 10.67** (0.01) |
Father | 4.43 (3.17) | 4.28 (3.06) | 4.44 (3.04) | 4.18 (3.03) | 4.40 (3.24) | 4.13 (3.04) | 4.54 (3.16) | 5.06 (3.14) | 1.59 (0.00) | 0.00 (0.00) |
Note: E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
Symptom means shown for Youth Self-Report subscales were first computed from raw data and then converted to T scores using normative sample charts.
Perceived parent containment.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
School differences were seen across most adjustment measures. As shown in Table 2, the East Coast suburban (E-Suburb) and East Coast urban (E-Urban) samples reported relatively high substance use, whereas the Northwest suburban (W-Suburb) and E-Suburb samples had relatively high internalizing and externalizing symptoms.
Among the predictor variables, the W-Suburb sample reported the highest parent containment for drugs (Cont-Drugs). At the same time, this group also had the most difficulties in parent–child relationships with the highest levels of criticism, expectations, and alienation from mothers, and the lowest trust and communication with mothers.
Regarding gender differences, girls reported higher levels of all internalizing symptoms than did boys. They also had higher scores on containment for rudeness and delinquency, and alienation from both fathers and mothers, while simultaneously reporting higher levels of communication with mothers.
Comparisons with national norms: Maladjustment
As in our past reports, we compared maladjustment levels among our samples with national normative data. Figure 1 displays results on substance use. For both genders, substance use levels were elevated, with the exception of W-Suburb girls’ cigarette and marijuana use. Use of alcohol, and frequency of being drunk, showed the most pronounced elevations, particularly in the two East Coast samples.
Figure 2 displays the proportion of youth falling above clinically significant symptom levels on the YSR. As we had expected for these affluent cohorts, we found no elevations on either withdrawn–depressed or aggressive behaviors. At the same time, we did see several elevations on the dimensions hypothesized to reflect vulnerability. The most striking result was that the E-Suburb youth (who were high on substance use; Figure 1) also had high rates of serious rule-breaking behaviors. The E-Urban youth, who were also high on substance use, reflected elevated proportions of anxious–depressed symptoms, as well as somatic problems among girls. Third, the W-Suburb sample, which was relatively low on substance use, showed the most pronounced vulnerability to serious rule-breaking behaviors and to anxious–depressed symptoms.
Overall, therefore, the findings show that none of these three affluent samples were at “low risk” (at, or below, national norms). Rather, each showed elevations in problems across multiple hypothesized areas of maladjustment.
Comparisons with national norms: Dimensions of parent–child relationships
We conducted exploratory analyses to determine if as a group, affluent youth are relatively dissatisfied with parent–child relationships. Although national normative data are not available on the IPPA to our knowledge, we did obtain mean values on a socioeconomically diverse sample of 326 high school students (14–19 years old; Fosco & Grych, 2010). Figure 3 shows that, overall, mean parenting scores of affluent girls and boys were better than those in the other, more diverse sample.
Parents’ “bailing out”: Descriptive data
Figure 4 displays responses for the six vignettes of the PBO, across all three schools (responses were similar). As shown there, patterns varied according to seriousness of infractions; students believed their parents would be more likely to intervene for “minor” violations such as being unprepared for a test, than for major ones such as plagiarism. At the same time, about 1 in 10 students did anticipate parent intervention—probably or definitely—even for serious transgressions involving active plagiarism, as did over 20% for a repeat incident involving alcohol on the school premises.
Multiple regression analyses: Replicating findings on containment
In examining the importance of parent containment dimensions, we controlled for other critical indices of parent child relations (Luthar & Goldstein, 2008): perceived criticism, expectations, and parent attachment. Rather than considering mother and father attachment separately, we included a single dimension (to conserve degrees of freedom, given the small sample size especially in the E-Urban school). Specifically, we considered the higher of the two attachment scores for mothers and fathers “attachment (maximum),” based on evidence of high protective effects of strong relationships with at least one caregiver (Luthar, 2006).2
The results (see Table 3) provided unequivocal support for the importance of containment for adolescents’ substance use, across all schools and genders. Replicating Luthar and Goldstein’s (2008) findings, furthermore, Cont-Drugs and parent knowledge both showed multiple associations with substance use as well as rule breaking. As expected, containment dimensions showed few significant associations with the two internalizing outcomes considered: anxious–depressed and somatic symptoms.
Table 3.
Substance Use: Girls
|
Rule Breaking: Grils
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Parent Predictors | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban |
Containmenta | ||||||
Drugs | −0.55*** | −0.35*** | −0.52*** | −0.25* | −0.12 | −0.19 |
Delinquency | 0.07 | 0.09 | 0.11 | −0.03 | −0.02 | 0.05 |
Rudeness | 0.08 | −0.09 | −0.24† | −0.14 | −0.12 | −0.20 |
Academic | 0.21† | −0.09 | 0.16 | 0.25* | −0.02 | 0.16 |
Knowledge | −0.16† | −0.24*** | −0.38** | −0.05 | −0.29*** | −0.50*** |
Criticism | 0.34* | 0.15† | −0.24 | 0.23† | 0.19* | 0.06 |
Expectations | −0.22† | −0.11 | 0.20 | −0.11 | −0.13 | 0.12 |
Attachment (max)b | 0.10 | −0.01 | −0.21† | −0.26* | −0.11 | −0.09 |
Total R2 | 0.26*** | 0.33*** | 0.51*** | 0.23*** | 0.29*** | 0.41*** |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Girls
|
Somatic: Girls
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Containment | ||||||
Drugs | 0.02 | −0.02 | −0.07 | −0.21† | −0.06; | −0.32* |
Delinquency | −0.10 | 0.04 | 0.03 | −0.08 | 0.08 | 0.13 |
Rudeness | 0.07 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.03 | 0.11 | −0.02 |
Academic | −0.07 | −0.20* | −0.11 | 0.10 | −0.23* | −0.09 |
Knowledge | 0.32** | −0.03 | 0.07 | 0.27* | −0.13† | −0.01 |
Criticism | 0.15 | 0.22* | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.29** | 0.33 |
Expectations | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.32† | −0.16 | −0.9 | 0.18 |
Attachment (max) | −0.19 | −0.13† | −0.06 | −0.23 | −0.11 | −0.05 |
Total R2 | 0.19** | 0.13*** | 0.22† | 0.12† | 0.16*** | 0.24† |
| ||||||
Substance Use: Boys
|
Rule Breaking: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Containment | ||||||
Drugs | −0.44*** | −0.27*** | −0.28* | −0.24** | −0.17* | −0.10 |
Delinquency | −0.05 | 0.08 | 0.09 | −0.04 | 0.05 | 0.19 |
Rudeness | 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.35† | −0.20† | −0.09 | −0.23 |
Academic | 0.08 | 0.05 | 0.17 | 0.22 | 0.08 | 0.07 |
Knowledge | −0.24** | −0.31*** | −0.34* | −0.22* | −0.36*** | −0.13 |
Criticism | 0.09 | −0.06 | 0.13 | 0.48*** | 0.05 | −0.30 |
Expectations | −0.22† | −0.01 | −0.25 | −0.40** | 0.15† | 0.25 |
Attachment (max) | 0.05 | −0.05 | 0.01 | 0.03 | −0.06 | −0.39* |
Total R2 | 0.31*** | 0.19*** | 0.33** | 0.30*** | 0.22*** | 0.24† |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Boys
|
Somatic: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Containment | ||||||
Drugs | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.07 | 0.02 | −0.01 | 0.06 |
Delinquency | −0.19† | −0.02 | −0.12 | −0.23† | 0.02 | 0.19 |
Rudeness | −0.12 | −0.02 | 0.28† | 0.04 | −0.04 | 0.02 |
Academic | 0.23* | 0.02 | −0.10 | 0.22† | −0.08 | −0.17 |
Knowledge | 0.02 | −0.06 | 0.09 | −0.13 | −0.04 | 0.09 |
Criticism | 0.49** | 0.13 | 0.11 | 0.31† | 0.05 | −0.47* |
Expectations | −0.32* | 0.16* | 0.43* | −0.17 | 0.13 | 0.56** |
Attachment (max) | 0.05 | −0.14† | −0.17 | 0.06 | −0.14† | −0.46** |
Total R2 | 0.22*** | 0.13*** | 0.39*** | 0.14* | 0.07* | 0.31* |
Note: Values in italics appear to be due to suppressor effects, because the statistically significant beta weights are opposite in valence to those in parallel zero-order correlations. Hence, they are not interpreted. E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
Perceived parent containment.
Maximum of mother/father attachment.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Exploratory analyses: Cont-Drugs
The mechanisms underlying our Cont-Drugs findings are not clear: it could be that very serious parent consequences are associated with abstinence from substances, or that extreme laxness with very high use, or both. We explored this issue by plotting, on the Y axis, distribution of use scores for alcohol and marijuana (the two most highly used substances), against seriousness of Cont-Drugs on the X axis. Results, shown in Figure 5, showed that at low Cont-Drugs, students tended to report high use of both alcohol and marijuana. At the highest levels of Cont-Drugs, alcohol use tended to be low, as might be expected. Drunkenness and marijuana use, however, showed considerable variations, suggesting that parents’ extreme consequences for substance use failed to serve as a guaranteed deterrent for at least some affluent teens.
Multiple regression analyses: Replicating findings on extracurricular involvement
In our second set of regression analyses, we sought to replicate prior findings that “overscheduling” in extracurricular activities is not a potent “vulnerability factor,” once parenting dimensions are considered (Luthar et al., 2006). The results (see Table 4) were remarkably consistent with earlier findings on eighth graders. Overall, there were negligible associations between hours spent in activities and maladjustment outcomes. There was just one exception: academic hours was linked with internalizing symptoms among girls, across all three schools.
Table 4.
Substance Use: Girls
|
Rule Breaking: Girls
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban |
Parent knowledge | −0.13 | −0.43*** | −0.47*** | −0.21* | −0.46*** | −0.56*** |
Parent criticism | 0.16 | 0.04 | −0.33 | 0.34* | 0.17* | 0.08 |
Parent expectations | −0.15 | −0.13 | 0.16 | −0.16 | −0.16† | 0.05 |
Sports hours | −0.14 | 0.05 | −0.10 | −0.07 | −0.04 | −0.22* |
Arts hours | −0.01 | −0.10 | −0.15 | 0.13 | 0.03 | −0.06 |
Academic hours | 0.00 | 0.05 | 0.03 | 0.11 | 0.07 | −0.08 |
Civic hours | 0.18† | −0.09 | −0.12 | 0.03 | 0.00 | −0.01 |
Total R2 | 0.08 | 0.23*** | 0.28* | 0.16** | 0.27*** | 0.46*** |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Girls
|
Somatic: Girls
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Parent knowledge | 0.14 | −0.10 | 0.06 | 0.06 | −0.16* | −0.06 |
Parent criticism | 0.27* | 0.20* | 0.11 | 0.20 | 0.29** | 0.25 |
Parent expectations | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.30 | −0.22 | −0.21* | 0.13 |
Sports hours | 0.00 | −0.10 | −0.30* | 0.02 | −0.11 | −0.03 |
Arts hours | 0.07 | 0.01 | 0.06 | 0.18† | −0.05 | 0.02 |
Academic hours | 0.18† | 0.10 | 0.25* | 0.28** | 0.19** | 0.27† |
Civic hours | 0.07 | 0.02 | −0.14 | −0.09 | 0.02 | −0.19 |
Total R2 | 0.20*** | 0.10** | 0.38*** | 0.15* | 0.16*** | 0.22† |
| ||||||
Substance Use: Boys
|
Rule Breaking: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Parent knowledge | −0.34*** | −0.39*** | −0.41** | −0.28** | −0.44*** | −0.32* |
Parent criticism | −0.04 | −0.06 | −0.05 | 0.46*** | 0.06 | −0.15 |
Parent expectations | −0.17 | −0.07 | −0.09 | −0.36** | 0.05 | 0.27 |
Sports hours | 0.01 | −0.01 | 0.03 | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.11 |
Arts hours | −0.01 | −0.01 | 0.09 | 0.00 | 0.06 | −0.19 |
Academic hours | 0.06 | 0.06 | −0.13 | −0.05 | −0.02 | 0.17 |
Civic hours | 0.04 | 0.07 | −0.26* | 0.02 | 0.03 | −0.17 |
Total R2 | 0.16** | 0.17*** | 0.32** | 0.21*** | 0.21*** | 0.22† |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Boys
|
Somatic: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Parent knowledge | 0.01 | −0.11 | 0.08 | −0.08 | −0.10 | −0.02 |
Parent criticism | 0.55*** | 0.14 | 0.46* | 0.39** | 0.08 | 0.13 |
Parent expectations | −0.28* | 0.19* | 0.14 | −0.19 | 0.08 | 0.17 |
Sports hours | −0.21* | −0.06 | −0.08 | −0.17† | −0.14† | −0.02 |
Arts hours | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.08 | −0.02 | −0.01 | 0.12 |
Academic hours | −0.10 | 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.02 | 0.02 | −0.17 |
Civic hours | 0.02 | 0.12† | 0.04 | −0.02 | 0.17* | 0.14 |
Total R2 | 0.24*** | 0.17*** | 0.39*** | 0.12* | 0.09* | 0.14 |
Note: Values in italics appear to be due to suppressor effects, because the statistically significant beta weights are opposite in valence to those in parallel zero-order correlations. Hence, they are not interpreted. E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Multiple regression analyses: Dimensions of attachment to mothers versus fathers
Table 5 presents the relative strength of mother versus father in predicting to teens’ adjustment outcomes. (We include substance use among the major outcome variables in this table merely for the sake of consistency; as we know from our prior work, it is proactive parental discipline, rather than closeness to either parent, that is most strongly related to affluent teens’ substance use.) The results provided in Table 5 are again consistent with prior findings on the critical importance of teens’ relationships with their mothers. For all three symptom domains (rule breaking, anxious–depressed, and somatic symptoms), block R squared values were higher for mother versus father variables—among both girls and boys, and in all three schools. The 18 ratios in question (see Table 5) ranged from 1.2 to a high of 17.0, with a median value of about 4.
Table 5.
Substance Use: Girls
|
Rule Breaking: Girls
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban |
Mother trust | 0.59* | 0.00 | −0.18 | 0.16 | −0.12 | −0.31 |
Mother communication | −0.20 | −0.10 | 0.07 | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.00 |
Mother alienation | 0.51* | −0.14 | −0.19 | 0.61** | 0.07 | 0.06 |
Mother variables: R2 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.21*** | 0.12*** | 0.23** |
Father trust | −0.46† | −0.10 | −0.05 | −0.29 | −0.14 | 0.16 |
Father communication | 0.16 | −0.02 | −0.26 | 0.03 | 0.08 | −0.35 |
Father alienation | −0.18 | 0.17 | 0.08 | −0.21 | 0.22 | 0.13 |
Father variables: R2 | 0.04 | 0.04* | 0.07 | 0.02 | 0.05** | 0.04 |
R2 block mother: father | 1.25 | 0.50 | 0.43 | 10.50 | 2.40 | 5.75 |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Girls
|
Somatic: Girls
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother trust | −0.29 | −0.13 | −0.06 | −0.11 | −0.09 | −0.07 |
Mother communication | 0.33 | 0.41** | 0.10 | −0.16 | 0.28 | 0.14 |
Mother alienation | 0.25 | 0.34* | 0.04 | −0.03 | 0.44** | 0.05 |
Mother variables: R2 | 0.19*** | 0.18*** | 0.08 | 0.05 | 0.17*** | 0.06 |
Father trust | 0.06 | −0.12 | −0.19 | 0.03 | 0.01 | −0.15 |
Father communication | 0.16 | 0.07 | 0.01 | 0.34† | 0.09 | −0.05 |
Father alienation | 0.32 | 0.27* | 0.29† | 0.33 | 0.19 | 0.24 |
Father variables: R2 | 0.03 | 0.06*** | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 0.05 |
R2 block mother: father | 6.33 | 3.00 | 1.33 | 1.25 | 17.00 | 1.20 |
| ||||||
Substance Use: Boys
|
Rule Breaking: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother trust | 0.35 | −0.01 | 0.11 | 0.30 | 0.21 | 0.12 |
Mother communication | −0.21 | −0.14 | −0.05 | 0.06 | −0.01 | −0.15 |
Mother alienation | 0.16 | 0.02 | 0.08 | 0.50* | 0.47** | 0.21 |
Mother variables: R2 | 0.01 | 0.03† | 0.03 | 0.07* | 0.18*** | 0.20** |
Father trust | −0.41 | −0.13 | 0.08 | −0.49† | −0.21 | 0.25 |
Father communication | 0.19 | 0.06 | −0.12 | −0.06 | 0.16 | −0.40 |
Father alienation | −0.20 | −0.11 | 0.10 | −0.40 | 0.06 | 0.06 |
Father variables: R2 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.03 |
R2 block mother: father | 0.50 | 7.50 | 3.00 | 1.75 | 9.00 | 6.67 |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Boys
|
Somatic: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother trust | −0.26 | −0.29* | −0.50* | 0.03 | −0.12 | −0.44† |
Mother communication | 0.60*** | 0.26* | 0.32 | 0.26 | 0.10 | 0.29 |
Mother alienation | 0.24 | 0.12 | 0.04 | 0.45* | 0.08 | 0.30 |
Mother variables: R2 | 0.17*** | 0.19*** | 0.21** | 0.06† | 0.10*** | 0.21** |
Father trust | 0.09 | 0.24 | 0.25 | −0.21 | −0.04 | 0.20 |
Father communication | −0.37† | −0.03 | −0.11 | −0.10 | 0.17 | −0.34 |
Father alienation | 0.11 | 0.49*** | 0.41 | −0.33 | 0.34* | −0.21 |
Father variables: R2 | 0.04† | 0.05*** | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.03* | 0.03 |
R2 block mother: father | 4.25 | 3.80 | 5.25 | 3.00 | 3.33 | 7.00 |
Note: Values in italics appear to be due to suppressor effects, because the statistically significant beta weights are opposite in valence to those in parallel zero-order correlations. Hence, they are not interpreted. E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Results of these regressions also showed that of the three attachment dimensions, alienation (from mothers and fathers) was the only one to show multiple unique effects. Closeness and trust with each parent showed only sporadic links.
Multiple regression analyses: Perceived parental depression
In exploring teen’s perception of parent’s depression, once again, we considered the maximum score of attachment to mother versus father in order to conserve degrees of freedom, and also considered the average containment score across the four subscales.3 Even after considering the commonly tested aspects of parent–teen relationships, perceived father depression consistently emerged as a unique, significant predictor across multiple maladjustment outcomes among affluent boys (see Table 6).
Table 6.
Substance Use: Girls
|
Rule Breaking: Girls
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Predictors | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban |
Mother depression | −0.23 | 0.11 | −0.02 | −0.02 | 0.12 | 0.01 |
Father depression | 0.36* | −0.11 | 0.07 | 0.32† | 0.00 | −0.17 |
Attachment (max)a | 0.04 | −0.04 | −0.14 | −0.22† | −0.12† | −0.12 |
Containment averageb | −0.32** | −0.35*** | −0.38** | −0.21* | −0.23*** | −0.19 |
Parental criticism | 0.29† | 0.15† | −0.22 | 0.20 | 0.19* | 0.06 |
Parent expectations | −0.08 | −0.12 | 0.14 | −0.02 | −0.13 | 0.13 |
Parent knowledge | −0.17† | −0.28*** | −0.36** | −0.10 | −0.27*** | −0.42** |
Total R2 | 0.19** | 0.30*** | 0.40*** | 0.27*** | 0.31*** | 0.40*** |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Girls
|
Somatic: Girls
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother depression | −0.06 | 0.22* | 0.16 | 0.03 | 0.15 | 0.23 |
Father depression | 0.19 | 0.04 | 0.13 | −0.18 | 0.13 | 0.20 |
Attachment (max) | −0.21† | −0.11 | −0.02 | −0.24† | −0.05 | 0.09 |
Containment average | −0.05 | −0.03 | −0.01 | −0.13 | −0.06 | −0.16 |
Parental criticism | 0.08 | 0.14 | 0.11 | 0.07 | 0.22* | 0.28 |
Parent expectations | 0.17 | 0.07 | 0.28 | −0.07 | −0.10 | 0.09 |
Parent knowledge | 0.31** | −0.03 | 0.06 | 0.26* | −0.11 | −0.03 |
Total R2 | 0.20** | 0.17*** | 0.25* | 0.13† | 0.20*** | 0.31** |
| ||||||
Substance Use: Boys Rule
|
Breaking: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother depression | 0.21 | −0.19 | −0.07 | 0.14 | 0.01 | 0.16 |
Father depression | 0.12 | 0.25* | 0.27* | 0.30* | 0.34** | 0.30* |
Attachment (max) | 0.08 | 0.00 | 0.05 | 0.07 | −0.02 | −0.43** |
Containment average | −0.27*** | −0.12† | −.034* | −0.14† | −0.05 | −0.19 |
Parental criticism | −0.06 | −0.07 | −0.08 | 0.38** | 0.01 | −0.40* |
Parent expectations | −0.10 | −0.03 | −0.11 | −0.31** | 0.10 | 0.27 |
Parent knowledge | −0.29** | −0.34*** | −0.29* | −0.27** | −0.36*** | −0.01 |
Total R2 | 0.32*** | 0.18*** | 0.37*** | 0.40*** | 0.32*** | 0.36*** |
| ||||||
Anxious–Depressed: Boys
|
Somatic: Boys
|
|||||
E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | E-Suburb | W-Suburb | E-Urban | |
| ||||||
Mother depression | −0.13 | 0.12 | 0.37** | −0.05 | 0.44*** | 0.10 |
Father depression | 0.60*** | 0.26* | −0.10 | 0.60*** | 0.06 | 0.12 |
Attachment (max) | 0.11 | −0.12† | −0.08 | 0.09 | −0.12† | −0.45** |
Containment average | 0.04 | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.16† | −0.01 | 0.00 |
Parental criticism | 0.48*** | 0.10 | 0.34† | 0.24† | −0.04 | −0.41† |
Parent expectations | −0.34** | 0.13† | 0.29† | −0.21 | 0.11 | 0.44* |
Parent knowledge | −0.05 | −0.04 | 0.14 | −0.21* | −0.02 | 0.19 |
Total R2 | 0.39*** | 0.26*** | 0.48*** | 0.37*** | 0.30*** | 0.28* |
Note: Values in italics appear to be due to suppressor effects, because the statistically significant beta weights are opposite in valence to those in parallel zero-order correlations. Hence, they are not interpreted. E-Suburb, East Coast suburban sample; W-Suburb, Northwest Coast suburban sample; E-Urban, East Coast urban sample.
Perceived parent containment.
Maximum of mother/father attachment.
p < .10.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Discussion
The results of this study corroborate suggestions that some “privileged” youth are actually at high risk for serious adjustment problems. Across three geographically diverse samples, adolescents reported elevations, compared to national norms, in one or more domains of substance use and/or rates of clinically significant internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Our findings strongly supported previous suggestions of covariance (a) of teens’ substance use, with anticipated parental repercussions on discovering their use, and (b) of symptom levels, with attachment to mothers more so than to fathers. Concomitantly, we corroborated that stress from too many extracurriculars is not a major “vulnerability factor.” Perceived parental tendency to bail teenagers out of problem situations was also apparently unrelated to maladjustment. Finally, we found that even after considering multiple dimensions of parent–child relationships, adolescent boys’ perceptions of fathers’ depression was related to multiple indices of distress. Each of these findings is discussed in turn.
Maladjustment among affluent youth
Among substance use indicators, the most striking finding was elevations in rates of drinking relative to norms, and in particular, of being drunk in the past month. Rates were pronounced among both the East Coast samples—suburban and urban—with over half of the girls and almost two-thirds of the boys reporting being drunk at least once in the past year (compared to about one-third of youth in norms).
Across the years, our focus groups with affluent teens have revealed several troubling trends regarding drinking. First, binge drinking is distressingly commonplace. Students have remarkably easy access to alcohol, with efficient systems in place to secure large amounts at a moment’s notice. Second, youngsters frequently drink with the deliberate intention of getting drunk; and for every reported incident of serious intoxication, involving stomach-pumping, indiscreet sexuality, and/or violence, there are many others that go undetected by adults. Third, plans to “party hard” are often made as an antidote to the unrelenting stresses of “working hard” in order to achieve excellence across multiple domains of achievement (Luthar & Sexton, 2004).
The trends we are seen in high school are also particularly worrying given the high rates of serious alcohol use on college campuses (Del Boca, Darkes, Greenbaum, & Goldman, 2004; Weitzman, Nelson, & Wechsler, 2003), with as many as 44.4% of college students reporting binge drinking (Wechsler et al., 2002). Furthermore, extreme drinking in college is particularly pronounced among students at highly competitive colleges (Wechsler et al., 2002) and those from affluent and better-educated families (Dantzer, Wardle, Fuller, Pampalone, & Steptoe, 2006). Parallel findings have been reported with marijuana and illicit drug use (Gledhill-Hoyt, Lee, Strote, Wechsler, 2000).
Admittedly, some adolescents eventually “mature out” of heavy drinking; at the same time, substance use during high school is among the strongest predictors of use during college (Reifman & Watson, 2003). Thus, at least a subgroup of our affluent samples is likely to show serious substance use in college. Similarly, there could be a decrease in use once these affluent samples complete college (given new social and career roles and responsibilities; see Schulenberg & Zarrett, 2006). Again, this possibility must be weighed against evidence that 40 and 70% of people remain stable in their drinking patterns (Jackson, Sher, Gotham, & Wood, 2001), with alcohol abuse becoming serious for some (Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002).
Aside from substance use, our three samples all showed elevations in one or more hypothesized serious internalizing and externalizing symptoms. It is interesting that the Northwest suburban youth (who were the lowest of the three samples on substance use) showed the highest vulnerability on anxious–depressed symptoms, as well as rule breaking. In previous research, this same group had also shown significant elevations on nonsuicidal self-injurious behavior (Yates et al., 2008). It should be noted too that the Northwest suburban group had the poorest levels of attachment to parents, in five of the six dimensions we considered: trust, communication, and alienation in relation to mothers and fathers each; they were also the highest on parent criticism.
Across all three samples, the specific domains of students’ vulnerability, mirror, to some degree, the community concerns that had sparked research with the groups, respectively. All our studies of affluent samples had begun with requests from community/school leaders to help tackle specific areas of concern: substance use in the East Coast suburb; depression, suicidality, and feelings of isolation in the Northwest suburb; and generalized high stress levels in the East Coast independent school. Overall, therefore, the heterotypy in the specific types of maladjustment (Price & Ingram, 2010) seen among affluent youth may, to some degree, be related to local community awareness and prevention efforts specific to each.4
Vulnerability and protective factors: Predictors of adjustment problems
Among the “risk modifiers” we examined, findings were the most compelling on parents’ containment for substance use. Across all samples, youth reported high levels of use when they felt their parents were lax in consequences for substance use. At the same time, high Cont-Drugs in itself was not necessarily a fail-safe protective factor; youth with the strictest of parents were not necessarily abstinent. These findings probably reflect, in part, the power of peer influences, incredibly potent in sustaining high substance use in the subculture of affluent adolescence (Luthar & Becker, 2002). In addition, individual vulnerability factors could be implicated, including the tendencies to use substances in efforts to self-medicate (Brown, 2008; Luthar & Becker, 2002; Luthar & D’Avanzo, 1999).
In addition to, and independent of, Cont-Drugs, parents’ knowledge of their children’s whereabouts after school was linked to substance use and with rule-breaking behaviors across subgroups. As we noted earlier (Luthar & Sexton, 2004), affluent parents may be less than vigilant of their children’s activities, lulled into a sense of security given the physical safety of their neighborhoods. In addition, some teenagers routinely lie to their parents. Thus, the parents’ “lack of knowledge” may be as much a result of teenagers’ deliberately withholding information, as it is about parents’ lack of vigilance.
Of the affective indicators of parent–child relationships, parent criticism was the only dimension that showed relatively strong associations across subgroup analyses (although beta weights were sometimes nonsignificant, limited by sample sizes). The potency of this negative indicator, relative to others of a positive valence such as attachment, is supported by prior research on resilience: acrimony from parents to children is highly destructive, especially if it is a consistent pattern (Luthar, 2006).
As with prior results on eight graders (Luthar et al., 2006), we found with parenting dimensions considered, hours in extracurricular activities was unrelated to maladjustment, even among these teens in the thick of “resume building” for college applications. There was just one dimension showing negative links: hours in academics was related to some form of internalizing symptoms among girls across geographic locations. Luthar et al. (2006) had also found this dimension to be related to multiple symptoms among girls. These findings probably reflect high pressures for girls to succeed across multiple areas: to be attractive, popular, well-behaved, and empathic, as well as every bit as academically proficient as boys (Hinshaw & Kranz, 2009). Thus, it is plausible that girls who are struggling academically (and thus receiving extra help as in tutoring after school) are feeling the strains of the inordinately high expectations across multiple domains.
Supporting this argument, girls in all three schools reported higher anticipated consequences from parents for misbehavior across domains. Gender differences were significant for parental containment for rudeness and delinquency, as was shown by Luthar and Goldstein (2008) with eighth graders. It is clear, therefore, that affluent young women face competing and sometimes impossible demands: (a) to be on par with boys in the male dominated worlds of academics and career and (b) and to surpass boys in the traditional, other-centered, feminine roles (Zahn-Waxler, Klimes-Dougan, & Slattery, 2000) of being well-behaved, polite, considerate, and physically attractive to boot (see Becker & Luthar, 2007).
Discrete aspects of relationships with mothers and with fathers
Our findings replicate earlier findings on the critical importance of relationships with mothers. As with younger affluent youth (Luthar & Becker, 2002), even the late adolescents in this sample showed much greater variations, in their self-reported symptoms, as a function of quality of relationships with mothers as opposed to fathers. Across the six analyses, the ratios of variance explained by the blocks of mother versus father attachment variables ranged from 1.2 to a high of 17.0, with a median value of almost 4. Similarly, alienation from mothers was the only parent attachment dimension to be linked with at least one maladjustment dimension across the multiple subsamples (with the exception of E-Urban youth).
That the magnitudes of adolescents’ mother-attachment associations were at least three times as as high (conservatively averaged) than those of father attachment, probably reflects, in part, differences in times spent with each parent. Mothers are generally primary caregivers of their children, across diverse cultural contexts. In affluent communities, children are even more likely to spend more time with their mothers than with fathers, as the latter are usually the primary wage earners with careers demanding long hours (Luthar & Sexton, 2004).
Parent attachment relative to norms
Negative stereotypes about affluent parents are rampant, and they are sometimes fueled by trends in the popular media (perhaps inadvertently). Discussing her book called The Price of Privilege, author Madeline Levine (2008) was quoted in the New York Times as saying that, “wealthy parents today … are more likely to be emotionally distant from their children, and at the same time to insist on high levels of achievement, a potentially toxic blend of influences that can create ‘intense feelings of shame and hopelessness’ in affluent children” (Tough, 2011).
This inference, which was cited as deriving from our research, is not entirely correct. Our programmatic studies do indicate, as contended, that, children of affluent parents exhibit “unexpectedly high rates of emotional problems beginning in junior high school” (Tough, 2011). On the other hand, this by no means implies that all, or even most, wealthy youth are troubled, or that most of their parents are deficient in any way. To the contrary, we have explicitly asserted that our data counter presumptions of generally “poor” parenting in wealthy communities, which is a statement strongly supported by comparisons, in this study, of mean levels of parent attachment with values from economically diverse samples. We have repeatedly cautioned that, “…the range of perceived parenting adequacy is no more constrained among the very wealthy than the very poor” (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005b, p. 223). In all settings there are inevitably some parents who are disengaged, lax, or critical; and in all settings, the quality of parent– child relationships is inevitably related to children’s adjustment outcomes.
Bailing out
Our exploratory analyses counter yet another widespread stereotype: that affluent parents inappropriately bail their children out of all offenses, minor and major. Based on his research in independent schools, Weissbourd (2009, p. 118) describes views, among teachers, of parents as being “out of control,” tenaciously demanding them to overlook teen misbehaviors ranging from rudeness to plagiarism.
Data in this study show that from the children’s perspective, parents’ anticipated intervention varies with the seriousness of offense, more likely with relatively minor infractions (being unprepared for a test) but not necessarily with egregious ones. At the same time, even in the latter category—incidents of outright plagiarism, and a repeat offense involving vodka at school—a distinct subgroup of youth believed their parents would, probably or definitely, protest on their behalf: about 10% for the former offense, and 23% for the latter. These data support Weissbourd’s (2009) assertion that in privileged settings, there is actually a subgroup of parents (albeit perhaps small) who aggressively, even litigiously, protest disciplinary actions imposed by schools.
Our failure to find links between parent bailing out scores and adjustment may partly be because this dimension, like “overinvolvement” in extracurriculars, does not really contribute to maladjustment. A second possibility is that there are curvilinear effects, with negative repercussions for parents either too willing, or inexorably unwilling to intervene, regardless of the infraction. A third possibility is that linear findings actually do exist and might be captured with refinements of our measure. Although the PBO had good reliability levels, it combines variations in both (a) type of offense (e.g., academic, substance use) and (b) seriousness of disciplinary actions. In future research, it might be useful to separately query problem behaviors in discrete areas, all engendering serious disciplinary actions from schools.
Parental depression
We were somewhat surprised to find multiple links between adolescent boys’ perceptions of fathers’ depression and both externalizing and internalizing symptoms. Several factors render these associations noteworthy. First, they emerged in stringent analyses, even after considering multiple aspects of parent– teen relationships. Second, as noted earlier, symptom levels of affluent boys and girls have consistently varied more relative to relationships with mothers than with fathers. Yet, there were no associations here for perceived depression among mothers.
It is possible that patterns we found among these older adolescent boys are a by-product of the young men’s identification with their fathers. On the brink of leaving home to enter the “real world,” these young men may be particularly sensitive to fathers’ visible emotional vulnerability. Alternatively, it is plausible that depressive symptoms may be most manifest in the relationships between men in highly successful, demanding careers and their expectations for, and thus their interactions with, their almost-adult sons.
It is interesting that overall levels of perceived parental depression were generally commensurate among mothers and fathers, if not slightly higher among the latter; this is in stark contrast to the fact that adult women are twice as likely to experience depression than men (Hammen et al., 2010). Our findings may well reflect gender differences in affluent parents’ willingness to seek help when depressed (men are typically more reluctant; see Hinshaw, 2007), and also, in how parents manifest their experienced distress. As Shelley Taylor (2002, 2006) has extensively documented, males typically respond to threats and distress with tendencies to “fight or flight,” that is, behaviors that connote aggressiveness (irritability) or distancing. Women’s instinctive defense mechanisms, by contrast, are to “tend and befriend,” and in particular, to focus on intensively nurturing their offspring. Thus, even though mothers in our research may have actually experienced as many or more symptoms of depression as fathers, their distress simply may not have been as evident in interactions with their children.
Limitations, caveats, and future directions for research and practice
Prominent among the limitations of this work is its cross-sectional nature; this precludes any firm conclusions about the direction of links documented here. It is just as likely, for example, that misbehavior among children leads to parental criticism and emotional withdrawal, as are links in the opposite direction. In the future, the use of longitudinal analyses could enhance understanding of the bidirectional links between different parent dimensions and adolescent outcomes.
The sole use of self-report indices to measure parent–adolescent relationships might be criticized, but this has reflected a deliberate choice in our ongoing programmatic research (Luthar & Becker, 2002; Luthar & Goldstein, 2008; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005b). Our interest is not so much in others’ opinions of parents’ effectiveness, but rather, in adolescents’ own perceptions of their relationships with parents, and how these perceptions might play out in different aspects of their adjustment.
There is a possibility of Type II errors in our findings, so that we did not identify some links that actually did exist. This is particularly true for the E-Urban sample, which was small in size, rendering significant none but the most substantial regression coefficients. Similarly, the reliability of measurement was low for perceived parental depression. In this regard, it should be noted that low reliability limits the likelihood of finding significant links rather than inflating them, so there is little question about the authenticity of significant associations that were found for this construct.
A final limitation is that family “affluence” is confounded with ethnicity in these samples as in others, with the wealthy families generally being of Caucasian backgrounds. Disentangling ethnicity and income in such research is difficult, however, given contemporary demographic patterns; multiple school districts will have to be sampled simultaneously to recruit sufficiently large cohorts of ethnic minority youth from very wealthy families (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a).
In terms of future research directions, it will be useful to examine vulnerability and resilience among affluent youngsters via individual-based analyses rather than only variable-based ones (Luthar et al., 2000; Masten, 2007). Much could be learned from focusing on the subgroup of youth and families who stand out from others in being particularly troubled, and then identifying factors distinguishing them from groups who function well (e.g., Latendresse, 2005; McMahon & Luthar, 2006).
Concerted attention to both aspects of communities and of the students’ personal attributes is also critical. In empirically considering major risk modifiers, our attempts thus far have generally been to disentangle the relative significance of various perceived parenting dimensions, which are clearly critical for youth across contexts (Luthar, 2006). In future efforts to understand the major sources of pressure faced by affluent youth, it is important to examine also aspects of school climate (particularly rigorous expectations and standards around academics; see Anderson, 2011), peer relationships and romantic attachments, as well as individual attributes such as personal values and life goals.
Even as we continue to explore these various dimensions, one directive is already clear for practice and policy, based on our own findings and recently, those of others (see Chassin, Beltran, Lee, Haller, & Villalta, 2010; Hanson & Chen, 2007; Trim & Chassin, 2008). This is that parents must be particularly attentive to substance use among affluent youth. We admit that this is easier said than done, especially given that some experimentation with substances is normative—and even perhaps “healthy” (Shedler & Block, 1990)—during adolescence. Thus, if parents were to categorically disallow 18-year-olds a single drink, even with appropriate adult supervision and discussions about the associated hazards, this could result in a backlash among youth as we have clearly seen in our focus groups. Draconian punitive measures often backfire, with teenagers simply disclosing less and less to their parents, resorting to frequent marijuana use (not as easily detected), and ultimately, engaging in binge drinking once away from home, at college. This said, parents would be wise to guard against extreme laxness around their adolescents’ use of substances. Particularly essential are clear, consistent messages about the unequivocal unacceptability of parties entailing unlimited consumption of alcohol. As common as such social events might be among today’s affluent teens, the dangers are simply far too grave for adults to allow their occurrence, tacitly or otherwise.
With regard to parents’ relentless, and even unethical or illegal pursuit of their children’s high achievement, our findings show that there is a subgroup of wealthy parents who do fit this profile, but this is by no means true for all or even most of them. Affluent adults are typically judged as complicit in their children’s breaking the rules to “get ahead,” as exemplified in recent discoveries that several students paid another highly skilled youth to impersonate them while taking the SAT examinations (Anderson & Applebome, 2011). In one investigative story, a parent was quoted as saying that “he could not ‘imagine that my child would be able to do that and come up with $1000 or $2000 and me not know about it’” (Anderson & Applebome, 2011). Caution regarding such inferences is critical; our own work has shown that raising such sums of money can be trivial for many youth in affluent communities. Aside from misappropriating money from family or friends (see Luthar & Ansary, 2005), income can easily be derived by writing academic papers for struggling students, for example, and from sales of alcohol and even marijuana to others.
Considering these issues in tandem, our central message regarding “privileged” parents is, first, that communities and schools adopt a strict zero-tolerance policy regarding students’ law breaking. Countering the few powerful, litigious parents who hotly protest punitive actions, the much larger group of civic-minded parents must equally vocally support administrators who, like the principal of the school involved in the SAT cheating described earlier, retain a “moral and legal obligation” to report criminal activities to the police (Anderson & Appelbome, 2011). In the words of the district attorney investigating these incidents, “We have to put accountability into the system, and there is none right now…If we can’t teach 16-, 17- and 18-year-olds that cheating is wrong,” she added, “shame on us” (Anderson & Appelbome, 2011).
Second, another message for parents is that they remain vigilant about their almost grown children’s activities outside school. Even the brightest, most academically successful students (and sometimes, especially these) will be drawn to break the rules to get ahead, and as our data show, there can be strong protective effects if parents stay attuned to their children’s friends and leisure activities, beginning even with relatively simple measures like being aware if cases of alcohol are stored in cars or in homes (for pending sales to other minors).
Third, we need more proactive prevention efforts for families in distress. Talks and workshops in affluent communities (Luthar & Sexton, 2004), can be used to help parents see the potential harm, to their children (a) of their belittling, critical communication patterns, and (b) of high depressive problems in the parents themselves, for which they are reluctant to seek help. Particularly useful would be regular support groups held for mothers, who disproportionately shoulder the task of shepherding teens through the diverse challenges of life in privileged settings.
Of the most importance for practice and policy, we in science must proactively disseminate central research findings on this still little-studied “at-risk group,” including results of stringent multivariate analyses, as well as descriptive data that can direct future quantitative hypothesis testing (see Luthar, 1999). Above all, it is critical, always, to underscore that the “pressure-cooker” lives of affluent teens stem from a complex web of multiple influences. Problems in families certainly contribute, as they do in all contexts. The peer group actively reinforces substance use (Balsa, Homer, French, & Norton, 2010; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005a). Intimacy in close friendships, which is critical during adolescence, is inevitably thwarted by competition with one’s closest friends (see Anderson, 2011). Schools relentlessly pursue attendance in college level courses, and the highest possible SAT scores. And ultimately, all these pressures stem from, and are reinforced by, these ubiquitous, core tenets in the macrocosm of American society: more is better; material wealth makes for ultimate happiness; and this, in turn, is best acquired through attendance at elite colleges.
Scientists must regularly describe these pressures to the lay public, via books, commentaries in newspapers, and workshops, with appropriate directions for change. Increasingly, we have seen effective models of such efforts, such as Schwartz’s (2007) writings on ways to reduce academic pressures (e.g., with college admission decisions decided by lotteries of qualified applicants), Weissbourd’s (2011) descriptions about ways in which parents and teachers unwittingly contribute to students’ stress, and by Zimbardo’s (as cited in Tugend, 2011) efforts to foster ethical decision-making patterns among high school students.
Conclusions
In his early writings on resilience among at-risk youth, Norm Garmezy (1971, 1974, 1983) presciently established two principles that have remained cardinal within the field (Luthar, 2006). First, regardless of the risks in a particular socioeconomic setting, some children will thrive even as others falter. Second, that as we in science study resilience, we must retain a concerted focus on “modifiable modifiers.” We have attempted to live up to Garmezy’s legacy in our decade-long research on ostensibly privileged youth, identifying aspects of family functioning that are strongly linked with youth maladjustment, and at the same time, are amenable to change via preventive interventions. Prominent among these salient risk modifiers are parents’ attitudes toward teen substance use; high levels of parent criticism and low expectations of their children; the quality of relationships with mothers, in particular; and teens’ perceptions of depressive problems in their parents. In our future work, we hope to further illuminate the most potent, tractable sets of factors at the community, individual, and family levels that can help youth thrive despite the relentless pressures of upward mobility in the culture of affluence—or what Kasser (2002) has called “The American Nightmare.”
Acknowledgments
We gratefully acknowledge funding from the National Institutes of Health (R01DA014385-06, R13 MH082592, and R01 DA010726-12). Sincere thanks as well to students in our research laboratory for their invaluable contributions to this programmatic work over the years.
Appendix A: Measure of Parents Bailing Out
Directions
When teenagers get into trouble, parents differ in how quickly they come to defend their children. Please indicate how your parents would probably react in the following situations (according to a 5-point scale: 1 = definitely not, 2 = unlikely, 3 = neutral, 4 = likely, and 5 = definitely, yes).
You and a group of friends were seen laughing at a classmate in the hallway, making fun of the student and loudly using names such as “loser.” As punishment, the principal said you could not attend a fun overnight school trip for your grade. Would your parents protest?
You put off completing a very important school assignment, and it was due the next day. You could not finish it without help. Your parents knew that you had left it for the last minute. Would your parents help you with the assignment?
For the third time, you and your friends were caught with vodka on the school premises; the principal planned to report it to the police. Would your parents try and stop the report?
For a major academic course, you copied part of an essay from an internet source. The teacher discovered this and plans to fail you for cheating. Would your parents protest?
You are a star athlete but during a game, you lost your temper and yelled at the head coach. You were thrown off the team. Would your parents try and get you back on the team?
You have a test the next day and you feel stressed out and feel unprepared to take it. Would your parents call you in sick the next day to give you more time to study?
Footnotes
Only subscale scores were included in the multiple analysis of variance, not overall scores (e.g., internalizing, externalizing, or attachment).
Considering the average of mother and father attachment yielded results similar to those reported in Table 2.
Including the apparently most potent containment score—Cont-Drugs—instead yielded results similar to those reported in Table 5.
The Northwestern suburban community sampled in this study was particularly invested in preventing underage drinking. At the time of this writing, we learned that the City Council had just unanimously passed an Underage Drinking Ordinance, holding homeowners liable (and fined) if youth were discovered drinking on their property, even if the adults claimed to be unaware of it.
References
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson J. At Dalton, a student survey reveals dual identities. New York Times. 2011 Nov 22; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/11/23/education/at-dalton-a-student-survey-reveals-dual-identities.html?scp=1&sq=at%20dalton,%20&st=cse.
- Anderson J, Applebome P. Exam cheating on Long Island hardly a secret. New York Times. 2011 Dec 2; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/12/02/education/on-long-island-sat-cheating-was-hardly-a-secret.html?_r=1&hpw.
- Ansary NA, Luthar SS. Distress and academic achievement among adolescents of affluence: A study of externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors and school performance. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:319–341. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armsden GC, Greenberg MT. The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth & Adolescence. 1987;16:427–454. doi: 10.1007/BF02202939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balsa AI, Homer JF, French MT, Norton EC. Alcohol use and popularity: Social payoffs from conforming to peers’ behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2010;21:559–568. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00704.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker BE, Luthar SS. Peer-perceived admiration and social preference: Contextual correlates of positive peer regard among suburban and urban adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2007;17:117–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00514.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beardslee WR. Out of the darkened room: When a parent is depressed; protecting the children and strengthening the family. New York: Hachette Book Group; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Brown SA. Prevalence of alcohol and drug involvement during childhood and adolescence. In: Beauchaine TP, Hinshaw SP, editors. Child and adolescent psychopathology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2008. pp. 405–446. [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Beltran I, Lee M, Haller M, Villalta I. Vulnerability to substance use disorders in childhood and adolescence. In: Ingram RE, Price JM, editors. Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. pp. 113–140. [Google Scholar]
- Dantzer C, Wardle J, Fuller R, Pampalone S, Steptoe A. International study of heavy drinking: Attitudes and sociodemographic factors in university students. Journal of American College Health. 2006;55:83–89. doi: 10.3200/JACH.55.2.83-90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal: Temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:155–164. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fosco GM, Grych JH. Adolescent triangulation into parental conflicts: Longitudinal implications for appraisals and adolescent–parent relations. Journal of Marriage and Family. 2010;72:254–266. [Google Scholar]
- Frost RO, Marten P, Lahart CM, Rosenblate R. The dimensions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1990;13:449–468. [Google Scholar]
- Garmezy N. Vulnerability research and the issue of primary prevention. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1971;41:101–116. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1971.tb01111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garmezy N. Children at risk: The search for the antecedents of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1974;8:14–90. doi: 10.1093/schbul/1.8.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garmezy N. Stressors of childhood. In: Garmezy N, Rutter M, editors. Stress, coping, and development. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1983. pp. 43–84. [Google Scholar]
- Gledhill-Hoyt J, Lee H, Strote J, Wechsler H. Increased use of marijuana and other illicit drugs at US colleges in the 1990s: Results of three national surveys. Addiction. 2000;95:1655–1667. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.951116556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen CL, Bistricky SL, Ingram RE. Vulnerability to depression in adulthood. In: Ingram RE, Price J, editors. Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. pp. 248–281. [Google Scholar]
- Hanson MD, Chen E. Socioeconomic status and substance use behaviors in adolescents: The role of family resources versus family social status. Journal of Health Psychology. 2007;12:32–35. doi: 10.1177/1359105306069073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinshaw S. Mark of shame. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Hinshaw S, Kranz R. The triple bind: Saving our teenage girls from today’s pressures. New York: Ballantine Books; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson KM, Sher KJ, Gotham HJ, Wood PK. Transitioning into and out of large-effect drinking in young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:378–391. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.3.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG. DHHS (ADM) Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office; 1984. Drugs and American high school students: 1975–1983; pp. 85–1374. [Google Scholar]
- Kasser T. The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Koplewicz HS, Gurian A, Williams K. The era of affluence and its discontents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009;48:1053–1055. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b8be5c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latendresse S. Perceptions of parenting among affluent youth: Exploring antecedents of middle school adjustment trajectories. Dissertation Abstracts International, Session B: The Sciences and Engineering. 2005;65:3746. [Google Scholar]
- Levine M. The price of privilege: How parental pressure and material advantage are creating a generation of disconnected and unhappy kids. New York: Harper; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS. Poverty and children’s adjustment. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS. The culture of affluence: Psychological costs of material wealth. Child Development. 2003;74:1581–1593. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00625.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS. Resilience in development: A synthesis of research across five decades. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: Risk, disorder, and adaptation. 2. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 739–795. [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Ansary NS. Dimensions of adolescent rebellion: Risks for academic failure among high- and low-income youth. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:231–250. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Becker BE. Privileged but pressured? A study of affluent youth. Child Development. 2002;73:1593–1610. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Brown PJ. Maximizing resilience through diverse levels of inquiry: Prevailing paradigms, possibilities, and priorities for the future. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:931–955. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. Research on resilience: Reply to commentaries. Child Development. 2000;71:573–575. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, D’Avanzo K. Contextual factors in substance use: A study of suburban and inner-city adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:845–867. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Doyle K, Suchman NE, Mayes L. Developmental themes in women’s emotional experiences of motherhood. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:165–182. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401001110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Goldstein AS. Substance use and related behaviors among suburban late adolescents: The importance of perceived parent containment. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:591–614. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Latendresse SJ. Children of the affluent: Challenges to well-being. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005a;14:49–53. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00333.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Latendresse SJ. Comparable “risks” at the SES extremes: Pre-adolescents’ perceptions of parenting. Development and Psychopathology. 2005b;17:207–230. doi: 10.1017/s095457940505011x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Sexton CC. The high price of affluence. In: Kail RV, editor. Advances in child development. Vol. 32. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2004. pp. 126–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Sexton CC. Maternal drug abuse versus maternal depression: Vulnerability and resilience among school-age and adolescent offspring. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:205–225. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luthar SS, Shoum KA, Brown PJ. Extracurricular involvement among affluent youth: A scapegoat for “ubiquitous achievement pressures? Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:583–597. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marano HE. A nation of wimps: The high cost of invasive parenting. New York: Broadway Books; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS. Resilience in developing systems: Progress and promise as the fourth wave rises. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:921–930. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McMahon TJ, Luthar SS. Substance use, emotional distress, delinquent behavior, and social competence: A longitudinal study of affluent, suburban high school students. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:72–89. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogel W. The blessings of a skinned knee. New York: Penguin; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Price JM, Ingram RE. Future directions in the study of vulnerability to psychopathology. In: Ingram RE, Price JM, editors. Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan. 2. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. pp. 497–510. [Google Scholar]
- Reifman A, Watson WK. Binge drinking during the first semester of college: Continuation and desistance from high school patterns. Journal of American College Health. 2003;52:73–81. doi: 10.1080/07448480309595727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider WJ, Cavell TA, Hughes JN. A sense of containment: Potential moderator of the relation between parenting practices and children’s externalizing behaviors. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:95–117. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;14:54–70. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulenberg JE, Zarrett NR. Mental health during emerging adulthood: Continuity and discontinuity in courses, causes, and functions. In: Arnett JJ, Tanner JL, editors. Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 135–172. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz B. Why the best schools can’t pick the best kids—and vice versa. Los Angeles Times. 2007 Mar 18; Retrieved from http://articles.latimes.com/2007/mar/18/opinion/op-schwartz18.
- Shedler J, Block J. Adolescent drug use and psychological health: A longitudinal inquiry. American Psychologist. 1990;45:612–630. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.5.612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE. The tending instinct: How nurturing is essential to who we are and how we live. New York: Holt; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE. Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:273–277. [Google Scholar]
- Tough P. What if the secret to success is failure? New York Times. 2011 Sep 17; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/09/18/magazine/what-if-the-secret-to-success-is-failure.html?scp=2&sq=the%20character%20test&st=cse.
- Trim RS, Chassin L. Neighborhood socioeconomic status effects on adolescent alcohol outcomes using growth models: Exploring the role of parental alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2008;69:639–648. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tugend A. Doing the ethical thing may be right, but it isn’t automatic. New York Times. 2011 Nov 18; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2011/11/19/your-money/why-doing-the-ethical-thing-isnt-automatic.html?_r=1&scp=2&sq=tugend&st=cse.
- United States Bureau of the Census. American factfinder. 2000 Retrieved October 20, 2010, from http://factfinder2.census.gov.
- Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Seibring M, Nelson TF, Lee H. Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts: findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health college alcohol study surveys: 1993–2001. Journal of American College Health. 2002;50:203–217. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissbourd R. The parents we mean to be: How well-intentioned adults undermine children’s moral and emotional development. New York: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Weissbourd R. The overpressured student. Educational Leadership. 2011;68:22–27. [Google Scholar]
- Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Adams P, Wolk S, Verdeli H, Olfson M. Brief screening for family psychiatric history. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57:675–682. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weitzman ER, Nelson TF, Wechsler H. Taking up binge drinking in college: The influences of person, social group, and environment. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;32:26–35. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00457-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yates TM, Tracy AJ, Luthar SS. Nonsuicidal self-injury among “privileged” youth: Longitudinal and cross-sectional approaches to development processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76:52–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahn-Waxler C, Klimes-Dougan B, Slattery M. Internaliing problems of childhood and adolescence: Prospects, pitfalls, and progress in understanding the development of anxiety and depression. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:443–446. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]