Abstract
Background:
Sociodemographic inequalities in the stage of diagnosis and cancer survival may be partly due to differences in the appraisal interval (time from noticing a bodily change to perceiving a reason to discuss symptoms with a health-care professional). A number of symptom appraisal models have been developed describing the psychological factors that underlie how people make sense of symptoms, although none explicitly focus on sociodemographic characteristics.
Methods:
We therefore conducted a conceptual review synthesising all symptom appraisal models, and focus on potential links with sociodemographics that could be the focus of future research.
Results:
Common psychological elements across nine symptom appraisal models included knowledge, attention, expectation and identity, all of which could be sensitive to sociodemographic factors. For example, lower socioeconomic status (SES), male sex and older age are associated with lower health literacy generally and lower cancer symptom knowledge. Limited attentional resources, lower expectations about health and lack of social support also hamper symptom interpretation, and would be likely to be more prevalent in those from lower SES backgrounds. Symptom heuristics (‘rules of thumb') may lead to symptoms being normalised because they are common within the social network, potentially disadvantaging older populations.
Conclusions:
A better understanding of the processes through which people interpret their symptoms, and the way these processes differ by sociodemographic factors, could help guide the development of interventions with the aim of reducing inequalities in cancer outcomes.
Keywords: cancer, symptom, bodily change, appraisal, patient presentation, appraisal interval, sociodemographics, inequalities
In England there are notable socioeconomic inequalities in cancer survival (Rachet et al, 2010; Ellis et al, 2012), and cancer survival generally decreases with increasing age (Office for National Statistics, 2013). Lower socioeconomic status (SES), male sex and older age were risk factors for later stage at diagnosis across seven common cancers, and it was estimated that eliminating sociodemographic inequalities in England would result in 5600 fewer patients a year being diagnosed at an advanced stage, which would in turn translate into substantial improvements in cancer survival rates (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2013).
The majority of cancers are diagnosed following symptomatic presentation in primary care (Elliss-Brookes et al, 2012). The timing of help-seeking for potential cancer symptoms is therefore a potentially modifiable route to improving early diagnosis (Richards, 2009). The Model of Pathways to Treatment focusses on conceptualising and identifying potential areas of delay (Scott et al, 2012). This is a descriptive framework of events (e.g., detecting a bodily change), processes (e.g., patient appraisal) and intervals. The patient interval is described as including both an appraisal component (time from noticing a bodily change to perceiving a reason to discuss it with a health-care professional) and a help-seeking component (time from perceiving a reason to discuss symptoms with a health-care professional to first consultation). Variation in the patient interval has been highlighted as a potential source of inequalities in the stage of diagnosis for ‘easy-to-suspect' cancers (e.g., breast), because for patients subsequently diagnosed with these cancers the diagnostic interval tends to be short and equitable (e.g., Lyratzopoulos and Abel, 2013).
There is also evidence demonstrating that lower SES, male sex and older age are associated with delayed presentation for cancer symptoms (Ramirez et al, 1999; Macleod et al, 2009; Forbes et al, 2014). Data from the English Routes-to-Diagnosis project reported that lower SES and older age were also associated with a higher incidence of emergency diagnoses across cancer types (McPhail et al, 2013), also indicating that there may be differences in the way symptoms are appraised or acted upon.
Research into sociodemographic inequalities has tended to focus on health-care use post presentation (Smith et al, 2005; Macleod et al, 2009). However, the appraisal interval (before GP visit) has been estimated to account for over two-thirds of the patient interval (Ristvedt and Trinkaus, 2005). It is therefore important to try to understand how sociodemographic factors affect the processes of symptom interpretation, and the determinants of decisions to seek help. This could help in developing targeted interventions designed to reduce sociodemographic inequalities. This is particularly important at a time when reducing the proportion of cancer patients diagnosed as an emergency is a priority for contemporary health-care systems (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2014).
Materials and methods
This literature review synthesises models of symptom appraisal and identifies elements of the models that may have application to sociodemographic differences. It is not a systematic review but rather a synthesis of models that highlight elements relevant to how people perceive, and act on, their symptoms. A number of different models of symptom appraisal may be relevant to unpacking patient response processes. In these models, symptoms (which may also be termed bodily sensations or changes) are considered ‘an outcome of a perceptual process' (Cioffi, 1991), and interpretation of bodily sensations is subject to complex psychosocial influences (Pennebaker, 1982; Cacioppo et al, 1986; Cioffi, 1991).
We used a keyword search of electronic databases (PubMed, Web of Science and PsychINFO) to identify relevant models using the terms symptom, somatic, illness, perception, appraisal, interpret and model. Additional models were found from reference lists. Inclusion criteria for models were broad and included the following: (1) the model has been published in English journals or books (no date limits were set); (2) the model started from before the presence of noticeable symptoms; and (3) the model was predominantly about how people notice and interpret symptoms. We did not include the Model of Pathway to Treatment because it is not a unique model of symptom appraisal in its own right but rather a framework in which existing psychological theories of appraisal and help-seeking are integrated. We do, however, include those symptom appraisal models that Scott et al (2012)integrated into the appraisal interval, and draw on the authors' overall discussion of symptom interpretation.
The aim was to provide a conceptual framework to help understand sociodemographic differences in symptom appraisal. We focussed principally on SES because lower SES is a risk factor for advanced stage at diagnosis across several cancer types (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2013), but we adopted a multi-faceted view incorporating occupational status, income, material resources, education and neighbourhood characteristics as markers of SES. We also explored evidence for age and sex differences, although the magnitude and direction of age and sex inequalities in the stage of cancer diagnosis varies by cancer type (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2013). The primary outcome was to identify potential implications of the concepts/models for understanding inequalities in the earlier diagnosis of cancer.
Results
Symptom appraisal models
The symptom appraisal models included in this review are summarised in Table 1. There are broad commonalities in their description of the process of symptom appraisal, which are categorised as detection of bodily changes, interpretation of bodily changes and responses to interpretation. These are briefly outlined below.
Table 1. Summary of symptom appraisal models.
Symptom appraisal model | Authors | Central arguments/factors | Examples of elements with potential links to inequalities |
---|---|---|---|
Situational adaption model | Alonzo, (1979) | Bodily sensations appraised within a social and cultural reality. Key assumption is that people are able to ‘contain' or side-line signs and symptoms within socially defined situations. | Identity: how bodily signs/sensations are understood as symptoms in the social and cultural arena. |
Self-regulation theory/common sense model of illness | Leventhal et al (1997) | Representations of illness appraised and coped with according to self-regulation model. On detecting a bodily change, selection of coping response is driven by emotion and components of illness representations: identity (label), cause (attribution), timeline (duration), consequences (impact on daily life) and curability/control. Emphasises role of re-appraisals within a self-regulatory system. | Knowledge (i.e. symptom label), expectation-based biases, identity (e.g. roles). |
Illness action model | Dingwall, (2001) | Sociological model of symptom perception. Bodily sensations may cause a ‘disturbance in equilibrium', which people seek to reconcile to continue as normal. Bodily sensations are perceived, interpreted and acted on and the outcome is evaluated. | Attention (competing demands), knowledge, identity (e.g. influence of others) |
Cue competition theory | Pennebaker, (1982) | Appraisal of symptoms depends on cognitive resources available, when external sensory information is limited, more cognitive resources can be deployed to internal bodily sensations. | Attentional processes |
Cognitive perceptual model of symptom perception | Cioffi, (1991) | Symptom appraisal comprised of meaning assignment, perceptual attention, and situational influences. Encompasses medical and psychosocial perspectives and role of attentional processing. Once bodily change detected, the search for a cause begins. Links to Pennebaker's theory. | Knowledge, attentional processes, expectation-based biases, identity |
Psychophysiological comparison theory | Cacioppo et al (1986); Andersen et al (1995) | Assumes people are motivated to maintain a reasonable physiological condition. Proposes several principles of symptom appraisal including: attribution (cause), logical consistency (familiarity), optimistic bias, comorbidity (number of symptoms). | Knowledge, expectation-based biases (e.g. optimism/pessimism), identity |
Symptom interpretation model (SIM) | Teel et al (1997) | Builds on Leventhal's Common Sense Model of Illness. Focuses on symptom experience from an intrapersonal perspective, where the interaction of knowledge and attribution of symptoms is considered critical. | Knowledge, expectation-based biases (i.e. heuristics, rules of thumb) |
Kolk's symptom perception model | (Kolk et al, 2003) | Developed from Cioffi and Pennebaker's work. Highlights the effects of trait negative affect, selective attention and external stressors. | Attentional processes |
Symptom and illness attitude model (SIAM) | Petersen et al (2011) | Synthesises/integrates models of illness and bodily sensations. Identifies moderators in forming mental representations of symptoms that may be promising targets for intervention. | Expectation-based biases (i.e. role of beliefs and emotion), identity (i.e. self-complexity). |
Detection of bodily changes
Detection of bodily changes is the starting point across models of symptom appraisal, variously referred to as ‘recognising a disturbance in the human system' (Teel et al, 1997), ‘internal somatic information' (Kolk et al, 2003), ‘somatic label' (Cioffi, 1991) and ‘situational stimuli' (Leventhal et al, 1997). Bodily changes may be general (e.g., fatigue) or localised (e.g., rash and lump). They could be visible (e.g., mole), palpable (e.g., lump) or audible (e.g., joint clicks). They may represent a difference in frequency or intensity of normal bodily sensations or a novel event (Scott et al, 2012). The common theme across symptom appraisal models is that for a bodily change to be detected it must be of sufficient magnitude or significance. External conditions may affect this; for example, in the Competition of Cues Theory, Pennebaker (1982) suggested that awareness of internal states is a function of the ratio of the quantity and salience of internal to external information. New bodily changes may go unnoticed if the external environment is high on stimulation, whereas fluctuations in normal bodily sensations may be noticed and attended to if the external environment is lacking (Cacioppo et al, 1986).
Interpretation of bodily changes
Following detection of a bodily change, individuals are assumed to go through a process of interpretation, which may involve labelling, categorising and evaluating the bodily change. Detection and interpretation will likely overlap, because bodily changes may become salient through interpretation (Alonzo, 1979; Pennebaker, 1982). For example, someone who considers they may have the flu may infer that they are feeling unusually hot. Interpretation is open to inaccuracy and is influenced by a range of factors such as knowledge, emotions (e.g., fear), co-morbidity, social context and previous experience. For example, a previous ‘false alarm' may influence the appraisal of new or recurrent symptoms, making a benign attribution more likely (Renzi et al, 2015).
Responses to interpretation
The final strand common to symptom appraisal models is the response to interpretation, defined as the actions and behaviours that follow the interpretation process. In terms of the appraisal interval, the end point is the decision that the symptom warrants medical attention (or not). If the interpretation process has resulted in a benign hypothesis (e.g., a change in the appearance of a mole interpreted as an insect bite), this will prolong the appraisal interval for persistent symptoms (Andersen et al, 1995; Walter et al, 2014). During the appraisal interval, other responses can also occur, including self-monitoring, self-management and seeking advice from family and friends (Scott et al, 2012). Behaviour resulting from symptom interpretation can also be avoidant rather than help-seeking, such as avoiding touching or looking at a symptom (Dingwall, 2001), or finding a distractor (Cioffi, 1991). Both the common sense model and the illness action model propose that output processes be subsequently appraised to see whether they have been successful (Leventhal et al, 1997; Dingwall, 2001). This self-regulation approach allows for ongoing cycles of symptom appraisal and re-appraisal.
Elements of symptom appraisal models and their potential links with sociodemographic factors
All of the symptom appraisal models identify factors that broadly resemble ‘knowledge', ‘attention', ‘expectation' and ‘identity' as determinants of progress along the detection–interpretation–response pathway (see Figure 1). Examples of these elements in relation to each model are summarised in Table 1, and are explained in detail below. Each section also explores how these factors could contribute to sociodemographic differences in symptom appraisal and provides some examples of possible future interventions (see Table 2). It is important to note that the different elements do not operate in silos: rather, they influence one another during the appraisal process. For example, health knowledge (e.g., that persistent cough can be an early warning sign of cancer) may be influenced by previous experience/expectations (e.g., I've previously had a cough like this and it turned out to be related to my asthma).
Figure 1.
The influence of sociodemographic factors and psychosocial ‘elements' on symptom appraisal. Demonstration of how sociodemographic factors may influence the psychosocial ‘elements' of symptom appraisal. Responses to interpretation may be re-evaluated, leading to new cycles of detection and interpretation. Knowledge is defined as familiarity, awareness or understanding of bodily sensations acquired through experience or education. Attention is defined as focussing on relevant stimuli while ignoring distractors. Expectation encompasses pre-existing beliefs, contextual biases and general heuristics (i.e., shortcuts). Identity refers to the distinct characteristics of an individual and their role in society.
Table 2. Examples of possible future intervention research.
Element | Recommendation |
---|---|
Knowledge | Target public awareness campaigns at demographic groups where awareness and health literacy are known to be low |
Attention | Emphasise short-term benefits of presenting with symptoms (e.g. reassurance), alongside long-term gains (e.g. early diagnosis) |
Target demographic groups where body vigilance is known to be lower, offering practical action plans | |
Expectation | ‘Myth buster' campaigns targeted at age and sex stereotypes |
Raise awareness of age-related risk | |
Identity | Increase the public's confidence in cancer as a curable/survivable disease |
Harness positive influence of social support | |
Community level interventions |
Knowledge
Overview. Across models the concept of ‘knowledge' is described as ‘exemplars', 'schemes', ‘schemas', ‘mental representations', ‘prototypes' and ‘awareness', but in all cases is conceived of a cognitive factor that guides the interpretation process. When bodily changes are detected, people use their existing knowledge to aid interpretation.
Sociodemographics differences. Population-based surveys in the United Kingdom using the cancer awareness measure (CAM; Stubbings et al, 2009) show that knowledge about cancer ‘warning signs' is lower in lower SES groups, men and older people (Wardle et al, 2001; Robb et al, 2009; Hvidberg et al, 2014). Modules of the CAM for specific cancer sites also show that knowledge of warning signs for lung, colorectal and cervical cancer is lower in lower SES groups (Power et al, 2011; Low et al, 2012; Simon et al, 2012). Men were also less likely to know the warning signs for colorectal cancer (Power et al, 2011). If sociodemographic characteristics are associated with being less likely to hold schemata for relevant ‘warning signs', this would influence the symptom interpretation process and in turn make help-seeking less likely. Consistent with this, lower awareness of cancer ‘alarm' symptoms is associated with a longer ‘anticipated' help-seeking interval (Robb et al, 2009; Quaife et al, 2014).
Older age, lower education, lower income and being male are also associated with lower health literacy (von Wagner et al, 2007), defined as ‘the capacity to obtain, process and understand basic medical information and services needed to make appropriate health decisions' (American Institute of Medicine, 2004). Lower health literacy may make it more difficult to understand and thus benefit from public health messages aimed at increasing knowledge, because concepts such as ‘early detection' are not well understood (Doak et al, 1996). However, whether health literacy is the primary explanation of sociodemographic differences in awareness of warning signs has not been investigated.
These results indicate that one avenue by which to reduce inequalities in earlier diagnosis is the targeting of particular demographic groups in which awareness and health literacy are known to be lower. In the United Kingdom, there are some campaigns targeting specific groups (e.g., breast cancer awareness campaign for women over 70 years; Cancer Research UK, 2014). If successful, these could be followed by campaigns targeting other symptoms or groups.
Attention
Overview. The detection phase of symptom appraisal is inherently linked to attention, because self-focussed attention is required to detect a symptom in the first instance. Attention is also important in interpretation because symptom appraisal is dependent on available cognitive resources to understand the explanation for the symptom and its implications. Kolk et al (2003) proposed that attention is influenced by the availability of internal resources for selective attention (i.e., focussing on relevant stimuli while ignoring distractors), the presence of external information (the events occurring in the person's environment) and negative affectivity. The Competition of Cues Theory also highlighted the effect of competing demands or external stressors in making it less likely that symptoms are noticed and interpreted (Pennebaker, 1982).
Sociodemographics differences. Several aspects of the ‘attention' element could relate in different ways to sociodemographics. Traits relevant to depleted attentional resources, such as ‘reactive responding' (an information-processing style characterised by reduced capacity for dealing with novel stressors) (von Wagner et al, 2011), have been linked with lower SES. Low consideration of future consequences has also been associated with lower SES (Guthrie et al, 2009). This may be because people from more deprived backgrounds need to concentrate on the present (von Wagner et al, 2011), or because there are so many external uncontrollable threats that internal attention appears unimportant (Nettle, 2010).
In line with the Cue Competition Theory, the theme of ‘competing demands' has been associated with lower SES, because demanding social environments often characterise lower SES households (Gallo, 2009). Competing family and work demands have also been raised by women as a reason for not seeking help for breast cancer symptoms (Facione and Facione, 2006), particularly among women aged 35–54 years (Grunfeld et al, 2003).
Women are shown to report more symptoms and visit the doctor more often than men (Vanwijk and Kolk, 1997). Pennebaker (1982) argued that there is not a simple correspondence between physiological change and symptoms; rather, women may be more attentive to their internal states. Evidence from the cancer literature has indicated that women were more likely to engage in actions to detect possible cancer symptoms (e.g., examining skin for changes) compared with men (de Nooijer et al, 2002; van Osch et al, 2007). Future work could explore other sociodemographic differences in body vigilance (Schmidt et al, 1997).
Negative effect may also contribute to SES differences in attention. According to the reserve capacity model, lower SES individuals experience more depression, anxiety and hostility compared with high SES individuals (Gallo, 2009). Negative effect makes symptoms more likely to be noticed, but more likely to be attributed to psychological causes (e.g., stress) (Kolk et al, 2003).
Targeting of attentional components may require a multi-faceted approach. For example, people low in consideration of future consequences could benefit from emphasis on the short-term benefits of presenting early with symptoms (e.g., reassurance) alongside the potential long-term gains (earlier diagnosis). If evidence supports the notion that there are individual differences in body vigilance, campaigns could be targeted at groups who are less likely to notice changes in their body, offering practical action plans to encourage them to make time to attend to their body.
Expectation
Overview. Expectation-based processes feature in a number of symptom perception models and refer to the contribution of beliefs and emotions in guiding the perception and interpretation of bodily changes (Petersen et al, 2011). Expectations can refer to pre-existing beliefs about bodily sensations, contextual biases or general heuristics (rules of thumb) that simplify symptom interpretation (Petersen et al, 2011). Expectations such as how long a symptom normally lasts (duration rule), how common symptoms are (prevalence rule) and how new or different they are (novelty rule) can influence appraisal, as can heuristics based on age (as individuals grow older they expect bodily changes to occur because of ageing rather than illness) and gender (e.g., women experiencing cardiac symptoms do not consider cardiac causes, as heart problems are expected to occur in men, and not in women) (Scott et al, 2012).
Sociodemographics differences. Sociodemographic characteristics have clear implications for both the prevalence rule and age or sex stereotypes. The prevalence rule highlights that, if symptoms are considered prevalent, people are less likely to appraise them as serious or as warranting medical attention (Scott et al, 2012). As well as attributing symptoms to age rather than illness, older people may experience more symptoms simultaneously due to co-morbidities, making symptom interpretation more difficult. The possibility of co-morbidity leading to misinterpretation of symptoms may also be more likely in lower SES groups, because they tend to have higher levels of symptoms (Pennebaker, 1982; Kroenke and Spitzer, 1998; Kolk et al, 2003). With regard to the influence of stereotypes, women are more likely to have advanced stage at diagnosis compared with men for bladder cancer (Barbiere et al, 2011), and one possibility is that they are more likely to misattribute their symptoms to common female conditions such as cystitis.
Expectation-based biases may require correcting with ‘myth buster' campaigns that highlight the pitfalls of making assumptions based on age or sex. Raising awareness of age-related risk of cancer may also militate against ‘normalising' attributions in the older population.
Identity
Overview. In the Symptom and Illness Attitude Model, Petersen et al (2011) argued that taking into account the complex nature of the self can help illuminate the process of symptom appraisal. They proposed that people have multiple identities that influence how they perceive bodily changes. For example, if ‘being a parent' is the most accessible identity, it may make it less likely that illness-related schemata are activated in the presence of bodily sensations. Other symptom appraisal models also emphasise the importance of identity. The common sense model of illness described the influence of ‘groups' and ‘roles' (e.g., negotiating the ‘sick role' alongside commitments associated with the ‘family role'). Alonzo's situational-adaption model emphasised that symptoms are always appraised within socially defined situations, and one key goal may be to ‘contain' symptoms and prevent them from having an impact on daily life by adopting processes such as normalising (Alonzo, 1979).
On the other hand, social situations may facilitate symptom interpretation and earlier diagnosis. In the illness action model, other people are considered to have a role in recognising and interpreting bodily changes (Dingwall, 2001). Wyke et al (2013) suggested that the influence of social structure can be direct (e.g., ability to obtain child care) or indirect (e.g., social networks' access to resources such as knowledge and health literacy). Qualitative research showed that discussing bodily sensations with others stimulated early detection (de Nooijer et al, 2001), and a review of help-seeking for lung symptoms concluded that social networks influenced the sanctioning of symptom seriousness in individuals who may otherwise be reluctant to perceive a reason to contact a doctor (Chatwin and Sanders, 2013). The potential impact of social facilitation on earlier diagnosis was highlighted by data from the United States, which showed that married patients were less likely to present with metastatic disease and less likely to die as a result of their cancer (Aizer et al, 2013).
Sociodemographics differences. Self-identity (i.e., the way people perceive themselves) is influenced by characteristics, including their sociodemographic profile. Self-identity is constructed within a social and cultural reality (Alonzo, 1979) and may also help explain cultural differences in symptom appraisal (Alonzo, 1979; Andersen et al, 2010).
Men and people from lower SES backgrounds rate their expected life expectancy as significantly lower compared with women and people from higher SES backgrounds (Wardle and Steptoe, 2003). Holding negative expectations about future health may affect symptom appraisal by making people more likely to ‘contain' or side-line worrying bodily changes by failing to acknowledge them as symptoms (Andersen et al, 2010). In support of this, UK data showed that people from lower SES backgrounds were less likely to consult compared with people from higher SES backgrounds when they appraised their symptoms as severe, but more likely to consult when they appraised their symptoms as trivial (Elliott et al, 2012).
The experience of others who have worse conditions/illnesses (more likely in lower SES environments and older populations) may also influence interpretation (i.e., the symptom does not seem so important anymore) (Petersen et al, 2011). Lower SES is also associated with lower social support, both emotional (e.g., providing encouragement) and instrumental (e.g., help with child care) (Taylor and Seeman, 1999), which may influence how people notice and interpret symptoms.
One possibility is that negative expectations of future health (based on self-identity) may contribute to inaction when people are faced with potential cancer symptoms. Modifying identity, particularly based on a sociodemographic group, is likely to be difficult. However, one possibility would be to increase the public's confidence in cancer as a curable and survivable disease, resulting in higher future expectations. Another possibility would be to harness the positive influence of social support in aiding symptom interpretation. Both of these suggestions may be well served at the community level, with involvement of potential users of the intervention from the start.
Discussion
A synthesis of nine models of symptom appraisal identified common processes, which included knowledge, attention, expectation and identity. In some cases there is evidence of sociodemographic variation in these elements, which could explain the sociodemographic differences in responses to possible cancer symptoms. Lower symptom awareness and health literacy in men, older people and people from lower SES backgrounds may contribute to misinterpretation of possible cancer symptoms. Attentional aspects may have an impact because higher external demands and negative effects, particularly in those from lower SES backgrounds, make interpreting bodily changes challenging. Expectation-based biases resulting from co-morbidity, stereotypes and the age–illness rule influence symptom interpretation, and sociodemographic differences in identity and life experience may hamper the detection or appropriate interpretation of symptoms. Conversely, social sanctioning may facilitate recognition of symptom seriousness and encourage help-seeking in higher SES groups.
Future research and interventions
The next step is to design studies within specific cancer types to test the mediation of sociodemographic differences by symptom appraisal processes. This cancer-specific approach will also allow for differential effects of sociodemographics to be observed according to cancer type. The resulting evidence base can be used to inform the development of targeted interventions, designed, for example, to make it more likely that people appraise their symptoms accurately (knowledge), are more vigilant for bodily changes (attention), are less pessimistic (expectation) and have a better understanding of their personal risk (identity), with the goal of reducing time to help-seeking.
Limitations
This review provides a synthesis of symptom appraisal models to draw out common elements that may help understand inequalities in potential cancer symptoms. The primary aim was to highlight links between the psychological constructs from the models and demographic characteristics that could be the focus of future research. As the focus was on stimulating a research agenda, this review was not amenable to other types of review such as meta-analysis or meta-synthesis. In addition, because the literature review was not exhaustive, we may have omitted studies that would have provided a valuable contribution. The conceptual framework was developed with cancer in mind, but, as with models of help-seeking behaviour, may also be applicable to other illnesses (Scott et al, 2012).
We focussed more on SES than on age or sex because there is more consistent evidence that lower SES is associated with advanced-stage cancer (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2013). However, we also presented possible linkages with age and sex inequalities, although the direction of the effect is often dependent on cancer type (Lyratzopoulos et al, 2013). We did not discuss the influence of ethnicity due to scant evidence in the United Kingdom regarding the association between ethnicity, help-seeking and stage at diagnosis. It has also been suggested that ethnicity data are not well recorded (Jack et al, 2009, 2010), and, to date, the majority of help-seeking studies have been in ethnically homogeneous samples (Marlow et al, 2014). One strength of the proposed framework is that ethnicity can be readily incorporated at a later stage. The evidence presented in the review is mainly drawn from UK contexts, but as the elements extracted from symptom appraisal models are based on individual characteristics (e.g., knowledge) our framework should be generalisable to other health-care settings.
Despite the suggestion that the appraisal interval contributes significantly towards delay in symptomatic presentation, there is paucity of evidence distinguishing appraisal and help-seeking intervals. However, progress has been made. One qualitative study with ovarian cancer patients described how bodily sensations ‘transformed' into symptoms that were in need of further care (Brandner et al, 2014). It is important to remember that appraising symptoms as requiring medical attention does not necessarily equate to help-seeking; people may choose to avoid seeking help if they fear the outcome of medical consultation (Whitaker et al, 2015). Thus, our review must be considered in combination with the literature on models of help-seeking/consultation behaviour (Scott et al, 2012; Wyke et al, 2013) and wider integration with psychological theory (e.g., social cognitive theory; Bandura, 1997).
In a comparison between the common sense model of illness, the illness action model and the network episode model (the latter was excluded from the current review because the model starts after the presence of noticeable symptoms), Wyke et al (2013) highlighted similar elements to those discussed in this review, including the importance of knowledge and the role of social networks/candidacy in aiding symptom interpretation. Our discussion of the interaction of sociodemographic factors and elements such as ‘identity' in symptom appraisal provides a starting point for future research, but would benefit from further exploration. For example, ‘identity' as we discuss it overlaps with the concept of candidacy both in terms of people being ‘candidates' for certain diseases, including cancer (Macdonald et al, 2013), and in their candidacy for health care, which is determined by a complex interaction between themselves and cultural and organisational systems (Dixon-Woods et al, 2006; Mackenzie et al, 2013). The notion of candidacy may help explain the differential uptake of health care according to sociodemographic groups (Dixon-Woods et al, 2006).
Conclusions
This conceptual review has synthesised models of symptom appraisal and highlighted elements that may help explain inequalities in the appraisal interval. It provides a framework to design studies that will provide a better understanding of the mechanisms through which people interpret their symptoms, and the relationship of these mechanisms to sociodemographic factors. This understanding should help guide the development of future intervention strategies to reduce inequalities in symptomatic presentation.
Acknowledgments
Cancer Research UK Early Diagnosis Advisory Group grant; C33872/A18555.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- Aizer AA, Chen MH, McCarthy EP, Mendu ML, Koo S, Wilhite TJ, Graham PL, Choueiri TK, Hoffman KE, Martin NE, Hu JC, Nguyen PL. Marital status and survival in patients with cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2013;31 (31:3869–3876. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2013.49.6489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alonzo AA. Everyday illness behavior: a situational approach to health status deviations. Soc Sci Med. 1979;13:397–404. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Institute of Medicine 2004Health Literacy: A prescription to end confusion . http://www.iom.edu/~/media/Files/Report%20Files/2004/Health-Literacy-A-Prescription-to-End-Confusion/healthliteracyfinal.pdf (last accessed 10/02/2015).
- Andersen BL, Cacioppo JT, Roberts DC. Delay in seeking a cancer diagnosis - delay stages and psychophysiological comparison processes. Br J Soc Psychol. 1995;34:33–52. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1995.tb01047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen RS, Paarup B, Vedsted P, Bro F, Soendergaard J. ‘Containment' as an analytical framework for understanding patient delay: a qualitative study of cancer patients' symptom interpretation processes. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71 (2:378–385. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.03.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Self-efficacy: the Exercise of Control. New York, NY, USA; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Barbiere JM, Saeb-Parsy K, Greenberg DC, Wright KA, Brown CH, Neal DE, Lyratzopoulos G. Trends in the use of radiotherapy and radical surgery for patients with bladder urothelial cell carcinoma in East Anglia, 1995-2006. BJU Int. 2011;108 (7:1106–1114. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2010.10058.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandner S, Muller-Nordhorn J, Stritter W, Fotopoulou C, Sehouli J, Holmberg C. Symptomization and triggering processes: ovarian cancer patients' narratives on pre-diagnostic sensation experiences and the initiation of healthcare seeking. Soc Sci Med. 2014;119:123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Andersen BL, Turnquist DC, Petty RE.1986Psychophysiological comparison processes: interpreting cancer symptoms Women with Cancer: Psychological PerspectivesAndersen BL (ed.), pp142–171.Springer: New York, NY, USA [Google Scholar]
- Cancer Research UK 2014. Be Clear on Cancer: Breast cancer in women over 70 campaign http://www.cancerresearchuk.org/cancer-info/spotcancerearly/naedi/beclearoncancer/breastover70/ (last accessed 01 October 2014).
- Chatwin J, Sanders C. The influence of social factors on help-seeking for people with lung cancer. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 2013;22 (6:709–713. doi: 10.1111/ecc.12078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cioffi D. Beyond attentional strategies: a cognitive-perceptual model of somatic interpretation. Psychol Bull. 1991;109 (1:25–41. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Nooijer J, Lechner L, de Vries H. A qualitative study on detecting cancer symptoms and seeking medical help; an application of Andersen's model of total patient delay. Patient Educ Couns. 2001;42 (2:145–157. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(00)00104-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Nooijer J, Lechner L, de Vries H. Early detection of cancer: knowledge and behavior among Dutch adults. Cancer Detect Prev. 2002;26 (5:362–369. doi: 10.1016/s0361-090x(02)00121-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dingwall R. Aspects of illness. Ashgate Pub Ltd: Farnham; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Dixon-Woods M, Cavers D, Agarwal S, Annandale E, Arthur A, Harvey J, Hsu R, Katbamna S, Olsen R, Smith L, Riley R, Sutton A. Conducting a critical interpretive synthesis of the literature on access to healthcare by vulnerable groups. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2006;6 (1:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-6-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doak LG, Doak CC, Meade CD. Strategies to improve cancer education materials. Oncol Nurs Forum. 1996;23 (8:1305–1312. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott AM, McAteer A, Hannaford PC. Incongruous consultation behaviour: results from a UK-wide population survey. BMC Fam Pract. 2012;13:21. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-13-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis L, Coleman MP, Rachet B. How many deaths would be avoidable if socioeconomic inequalities in cancer survival in England were eliminated? A national population-based study, 1996-2006. Eur J Cancer. 2012;48 (2:270–278. doi: 10.1016/j.ejca.2011.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliss-Brookes L, McPhail S, Ives A, Greenslade M, Shelton J, Hiom S, Richards M. Routes to diagnosis for cancer—determining the patient journey using multiple routine data sets. Br J Cancer. 2012;107 (8:1220–1226. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2012.408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Facione NC, Facione PA. The cognitive structuring of patient delay in breast cancer. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63 (12:3137–3149. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes L, Warburton F, Richard MA, Ramirez A. Risk factors for delay in symptomatic presentation: a survey of cancer patients. Br J Cancer. 2014;111 (3:581–588. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2014.304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallo LC. The Reserve Capacity Model as a framework for understanding psychosocial factors in health disparities. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2009;1 (1:62–72. [Google Scholar]
- Grunfeld EA, Hunter MS, Ramirez AJ, Richards MA. Perceptions of breast cancer across the lifespan. J Psychosom Res. 2003;54 (2:141–146. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00522-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie LC, Butler SC, Ward MM. Time perspective and socioeconomic status: a link to socioeconomic disparities in health. Soc Sci Med. 2009;68 (12:2145–2151. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hvidberg L, Pedersen A, Wulff C, Vedsted P. Cancer awareness and socio-economic position: results from a population-based study in Denmark. BMC Cancer. 2014;14 (1:581. doi: 10.1186/1471-2407-14-581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack RH, Davies EA, Moller H. Breast cancer incidence, stage, treatment and survival in ethnic groups in South East England. Br J Cancer. 2009;100:545–550. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6604852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack RH, Davies EA, Moller H. Prostate cancer incidence, stage at diagnosis, treatment and survival in ethnic groups in South-East England. BJU Int. 2010;105 (9:1226–1230. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2009.08940.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolk AM, Hanewald GJFP, Schagen S, van Wijk CMTG. A symptom perception approach to common physical symptoms. Soc Sci Med. 2003;57 (12:2343–2354. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00451-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL. Gender differences in the reporting of physical and somatoform symptoms. Psychosom Med. 1998;60 (2:150–155. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199803000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal H, Benyamini Y, Brownlee S, Deifenbach M, Leventhal EA, Patrick-Miller L, Robitaille C.1997Illness representations: theoretical foundationsIn Petrie K, Weinman J (eds)Perceptions of Health and Illness Harwood Academic: Amsterdam, The Netherlands; 19–45. [Google Scholar]
- Low EL, Simon AE, Lyons J, Romney-Alexander D, Waller J. What do British women know about cervical cancer symptoms and risk factors. Eur J Cancer. 2012;48 (16:3001–3008. doi: 10.1016/j.ejca.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyratzopoulos G, Abel G. Earlier diagnosis of breast cancer: focusing on symptomatic women. Nat Rev Clin Oncol. 2013;10:544. doi: 10.1038/nrclinonc.2012.126-c1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyratzopoulos G, Abel G, Brown C, Rous B, Vernon S, Roland M, Greenberg D. Socio-demographic inequalities in stage of cancer diagnosis: evidence from patients with female breast, lung, colon, rectal, prostate, renal, bladder, melanoma, ovarian and endometrial cancer. Ann Oncol. 2013;24 (3:843–850. doi: 10.1093/annonc/mds526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyratzopoulos G, Saunders CL, Abel GA. Are emergency diagnoses of cancer avoidable? A proposed taxonomy to motivate study design and support service improvement. Future Oncol. 2014;10 (8:1329–1333. doi: 10.2217/fon.14.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macdonald S, Watt G, Macleod U. In search of the cancer candidate: can lay epidemiology help. Sociol Health Illn. 2013;35 (4:575–591. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2012.01513.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackenzie M, Conway E, Hastings A, Munro M, O'Donnell C. Is candidacy a useful concept for understanding journeys through public services? A critical interpretive literature synthesis. Soc Policy Admin. 2013;47 (7:806–825. [Google Scholar]
- Macleod U, Mitchell ED, Burgess C, Macdonald S, Ramirez AJ. Risk factors for delayed presentation and referral of symptomatic cancer: evidence for common cancers. Br J Cancer. 2009;101 (Suppl 2:S92–S101. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marlow LAV, McGregor LM, Nazroo JY, Wardle J. Facilitators and barriers to help-seeking for breast and cervical cancer symptoms: a qualitative study with an ethnically diverse sample in London. Psychooncology. 2014;23 (7:749–757. doi: 10.1002/pon.3464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McPhail S, Elliss-Brookes L, Shelton J, Ives A, Greenslade M, Vernon S, Morris EJA, Richards M. Emergency presentation of cancer and short-term mortality. Br J Cancer. 2013;109 (8:2027–2034. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2013.569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nettle D. Why are there social gradients in preventative health behavior? A perspective from behavioral ecology. PLoS One. 2010;5 (10:e13371. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Office for National Statistics . Statistical Bulletin: Cancer Survival in England: Patients Diagnosed 2007-2011 and Followed up to 2012. ONS: Newport, NSW, Australia; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker J. The Psychology of Physical Symptoms. Springer: New York, NY, USA; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Petersen S, van den Berg RA, Janssens T, Van den Bergh O. Illness and symptom perception: a theoretical approach towards an integrative measurement model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31:428–439. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power E, Simon A, Juszczyk D, Hiom S, Wardle J. Assessing awareness of colorectal cancer symptoms: measure development and results from a population survey in the UK. BMC Cancer. 2011;11 (1:366. doi: 10.1186/1471-2407-11-366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quaife SL, Forbes LJL, Ramirez AJ, Brain KE, Donnelly C, Simon AE, Wardle J. Recognition of cancer warning signs and anticipated delay in help-seeking in a population sample of adults in the UK. Br J Cancer. 2014;110 (1:12–18. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2013.684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rachet B, Ellis L, Maringe C, Chu T, Nur U, Quaresma M, Shah A, Walters S, Woods L, Forman D, Coleman MP. Socioeconomic inequalities in cancer survival in England after the NHS cancer plan. Br J Cancer. 2010;103 (4:446–453. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez AJ, Westcombe AM, Burgess CC, Sutton S, Littlejohns P, Richards MA. Factors predicting delayed presentation of symptomatic breast cancer: a systematic review. Lancet. 1999;353 (9159:1127–1131. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(99)02142-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Renzi C, Whitaker KL, Wardle J. Over-reassurance and under-support after a 'false alarm': a review of the impact on subsequent cancer symptom attribution and help-seeking. BMJ Open. 2015;5 (2:e007002. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards MA. The size of the prize for earlier diagnosis of cancer in England. Br J Cancer. 2009;101 (S125:S129. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ristvedt SL, Trinkaus KM. Psychological factors related to delay in consultation for cancer symptoms. Psychooncology. 2005;14 (5:339–350. doi: 10.1002/pon.850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robb K, Stubbings S, Ramirez A, Macleod U, Austoker J, Waller J, Hiom S, Wardle J. Public awareness of cancer in Britain: a population-based survey of adults. Br J Cancer. 2009;101:S18–S23. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Lerew DR, Trakowski JH. Body vigilance in panic disorder: evaluating attention to bodily perturbations. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997;65:214–220. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.2.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott SE, Walter FM, Webster A, Sutton S, Emery J. The model of pathways to treatment: conceptualization and integration with existing theory. Br J Health Psychol. 2012;18:45–65. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8287.2012.02077.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon AE, Juszczyk D, Smyth N, Power E, Hiom S, Peake MD, Wardle J. Knowledge of lung cancer symptoms and risk factors in the UK: development of a measure and results from a population-based survey. Thorax. 2012;67 (5:426–432. doi: 10.1136/thoraxjnl-2011-200898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith LK, Pope C, Botha JL. Patients' help-seeking experiences and delay in cancer presentation: a qualitative synthesis. Lancet. 2005;366 (9488:825–831. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67030-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stubbings S, Robb K, Waller J, Ramirez A, Austoker J, Macleod U, Hiom S, Wardle J. Development of a measurement tool to assess public awareness of cancer. Br J Cancer. 2009;101:S13–S17. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6605385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Psychosocial resources and the SES-health relationship. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896 (1:210–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teel CS, Meek P, McNamara AM, Watson L. Perspectives unifying symptom interpretation. Image J Nurs Sch. 1997;29 (2:175–181. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.1997.tb01553.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Osch L, Lechner L, Reubsaet A, de Nooijer J, de Vries H. Passive cancer detection and medical help seeking for cancer symptoms: (in)adequate behavior and psychosocial determinants. Eur J Cancer Prev. 2007;16:266–274. doi: 10.1097/01.cej.0000236241.10125.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanwijk CMTG, Kolk AM. Sex differences in physical symptoms: the contribution of symptom perception theory. Soc Sci Med. 1997;45 (2:231–246. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00340-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Wagner C, Good A, Whitaker KL, Wardle J. Psychosocial determinants of socioeconomic inequalities in cancer screening participation: a conceptual framework. Epidemiol Rev. 2011;33:135–147. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxq018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Wagner C, Knight K, Steptoe A, Wardle J. Functional health literacy and health-promoting behaviour in a national sample of British adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61 (12:1086–1090. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.053967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter FM, Birt L, Cavers D, Scott S, Emery J, Burrows N, Cavanagh G, MacKie R, Weller D, Campbell C. "This isn't what mine looked like": a qualitative study of symptom appraisal and help seeking in people recently diagnosed with melanoma. BMJ Open. 2014;4 (7:e005566. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-005566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle J, Steptoe A. Socioeconomic differences in attitudes and beliefs about healthy lifestyles. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57 (6:440–443. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.6.440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle J, Waller J, Brunswick N, Jarvis MJ. Awareness of risk factors for cancer among British adults. Public Health. 2001;115:173–174. doi: 10.1038/sj/ph/1900752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitaker KL, Macleod U, Winstanley K, Scott SE, Wardle J. Help-seeking for cancer ‘alarm' symptoms: a qualitative interview study of primary care patients in the UK. Br J Gen Pract. 2015;65 (631:e96–e105. doi: 10.3399/bjgp15X683533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyke S, Adamson J, Dixon D, Hunt K. Consultation and illness behaviour in response to symptoms: a comparison of models from different disciplinary frameworks and suggestions for future research directions. Soc Sci Med. 2013;86:79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]