Skip to main content
Chest logoLink to Chest
. 2015 Apr;147(4):1179–1192. doi: 10.1378/chest.14-1617

The Pathophysiology of Insomnia

Jessica C Levenson 1, Daniel B Kay 1, Daniel J Buysse 1,
PMCID: PMC4388122  PMID: 25846534

Abstract

Insomnia disorder is characterized by chronic dissatisfaction with sleep quantity or quality that is associated with difficulty falling asleep, frequent nighttime awakenings with difficulty returning to sleep, and/or awakening earlier in the morning than desired. Although progress has been made in our understanding of the nature, etiology, and pathophysiology of insomnia, there is still no universally accepted model. Greater understanding of the pathophysiology of insomnia may provide important information regarding how, and under what conditions, the disorder develops and is maintained as well as potential targets for prevention and treatment. The aims of this report are (1) to summarize current knowledge on the pathophysiology of insomnia and (2) to present a model of the pathophysiology of insomnia that considers evidence from various domains of research. Working within several models of insomnia, evidence for the pathophysiology of the disorder is presented across levels of analysis, from genetic to molecular and cellular mechanisms, neural circuitry, physiologic mechanisms, sleep behavior, and self-report. We discuss the role of hyperarousal as an overarching theme that guides our conceptualization of insomnia. Finally, we propose a model of the pathophysiology of insomnia that integrates the various types of evidence presented.


Insomnia disorder is characterized by dissatisfaction with sleep quantity or quality, associated with difficulty falling asleep, frequent nighttime awakenings with difficulty returning to sleep, and/or awakening earlier in the morning than desired.1,2 The disorder is also characterized by significant distress or impairment in functioning, and daytime symptoms including fatigue, daytime sleepiness, impairment in cognitive performance, and mood disturbances. Insomnia is differentiated from sleep deprivation by difficulty sleeping despite having adequate opportunity to sleep.1 Prevalence estimates of insomnia vary, with 30% to 43% of individuals reporting at least one nighttime insomnia symptom.3-6 Most reports suggest prevalence rates of insomnia disorder at 5% to 15%.4,5,7,8 Insomnia is a chronic problem in 31% to 75% of patients,1,6,7 with more than two-thirds of patients reporting symptoms for at least 1 year.9

Although progress has been made in recent years regarding our understanding of the nature, etiology, and pathophysiology of insomnia,6,10-12 there is still no universally accepted model. This may be related to the heterogeneity of insomnia, its highly comorbid nature, or differences in what level of analysis the models use, from phenomenology to physiology. To be comprehensive, an etiologic or pathophysiologic model of insomnia should explain features such as the heterogeneity of symptoms and the risk insomnia confers for other comorbid conditions, such as depression and cardiometabolic syndrome. It should also explain the discrepancy between subjective (self-report) and objective (polysomnography [PSG]) measures of insomnia symptoms reported by some individuals with insomnia (see Reference 13 for a review). Greater understanding of the pathophysiology of insomnia may provide important information regarding how, and under what conditions, the disorder develops as well as potential targets for prevention and treatment. The aims of this review are (1) to summarize current breadth of knowledge on the pathophysiology of insomnia and (2) to present a model of the pathophysiology of insomnia that draws on evidence from various domains. Our article is intended to provide a brief overview of these topics for clinicians and researchers whose main focus is not insomnia. More extensive reviews of this topic can be found elsewhere.12,14,15 Our article is primarily informed by perspectives drawn from psychology, psychiatry, and clinical neuroscience.

Levels of Analysis: An Approach to Understanding Insomnia

Although evidence-based assessments and treatments for mental disorders have been developed, diagnostic criteria for these conditions—including insomnia—are grounded in clinical consensus.16 Further progress depends on better understanding the etiology and pathophysiology of mental health problems. One framework for doing this has been offered by the National Institute of Mental Health’s “Research Domain Criteria” initiative. While recognizing the value of current diagnostic categories, the National Institute of Mental Health has begun to emphasize observable “domains” of brain function pertinent to mental health. These research domains, such as positive emotion, negative emotion, and arousal, often show similar patterns of dysregulation across traditional diagnostic categories and can be examined across levels of analysis from genes to symptoms. These points pertain to insomnia as well. The first two editions of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders introduced > 25 diagnoses with “insomnia” as a cardinal symptom,2,17 but evidence for the reliability, validity, and distinct pathophysiology of these insomnia phenotypes has proved elusive. Partially as a consequence of this, both the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Third Edition18 and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, Fifth Edition,1 now propose a single major category for Insomnia Disorder or Chronic Insomnia Disorder. Nevertheless, there remains an impetus for the field to develop an evidence-based model of insomnia that accounts for heterogeneity in cause, symptoms, course, comorbidities, and consequences. This review considers evidence across seven levels of analysis based on the Research Domain Criteria framework: genetic, molecular, cellular, neuroanatomic, physiologic, behavioral, and self-report.

Hyperarousal: An Overarching Theme

Insomnia is often considered to be a disorder of hyperarousal,19 or increased somatic, cognitive, and cortical activation.20,21 Individuals with insomnia may experience physiologic hyperarousal in both central (cortical) and peripheral (autonomic) nervous systems (see References 20, 22, 23 for full review). Hyperarousal in insomnia can also refer to cognitive and emotional processes, with several theories suggesting that cognitive and affective hyperarousal at bedtime may contribute to both acute and chronic insomnia.24,25 Despite the frequent attention to hyperarousal in the literature, it is not frequently defined. In this report we conceptualize hyperarousal as heightened physiologic, affective, or cognitive activity, which interferes with the natural “disengagement from […] the environment”26 and decreases the likelihood of sleep. Hyperarousal may be detected using such measures as increased cortisol, heart rate variability, EEG, or even self-report (eg, “I can’t turn off my mind,” “I feel so keyed up”). One of the challenges in identifying hyperarousal is that an individual always has some level of arousal, and the exact threshold for categorizing hyperarousal is not well defined. Thus, most studies have identified hyperarousal by noting differences between insomnia and control groups, rather than denoting a specific threshold.27 We propose hyperarousal as an overarching theme that, along with other contributory factors, enriches our understanding of the pathophysiology of insomnia at each level of analysis and across levels in an integrated model.

Genetics of Sleep and Insomnia

Sleep-wake traits, such as sleep duration and timing, are heritable28 and regulated by numerous genes.29 Animal and human studies also implicate genetic mechanisms in the etiology of insomnia. Seugnet et al30 isolated insomnia-like Drosophila flies (ins-1 flies) with rest-activity traits that resemble human insomnia, including decreased rest time, increased latency to a resting state after lights out, greater fragmentation of rest periods, and heightened activity levels. Whole-genome transcript profiling of ins-1 flies identified 755 genes with human homologs that were differentially expressed compared with wild-type flies. Genes found to be conserved in ins-1 flies are associated with sensory perception, metabolism, cell surface signaling, and neuronal activity and may have implications for understanding the genetics of human insomnia.

Most genetic studies in humans have used a limited set of self-report items to categorize insomnia symptom phenotypes and yielded a wide range in heritability estimates for insomnia (h2 = 0-81%) across family history and twin studies.31-33 Studies using more stringent criteria to define insomnia have produced more realistic and reliably modest h2 estimates ranging from 31% to 58%.32,34,35 Candidate gene studies have identified gene variants that may be involved in the pathophysiology of insomnia, including Apoε4,36 PER34/4,37 HLA DQB1*0602,38 homozygous Clock gene 3111C/C Clock,39 and short (s-) allele of the 5-HTTLPR.40 A genomewide association study found numerous single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) significantly associated with insomnia symptoms.41 The most significant SNPs occurred within genes involved in neuroplasticity (eg, ROR1, PLCB1, EPHA4, and CACNA1A), stress reactivity (eg, STK39, USP25, and MARP10), neuronal excitability (eg, GABRB1 and DLG2), and mental health (eg, NPAS3).41

Overall, current evidence suggests significant heritability and multigene involvement in the pathophysiology of insomnia. Genes linked to brain functioning, arousal regulation, and sleep-wake processes have been most consistently found to be associated with insomnia. The complex interplay of these genes may account, at least in part, for the heterogeneity observed in insomnia symptoms and consequences. Future genetic studies with detailed assessment of sleep and health history of patients with chronic insomnia disorder may further refine our understanding of genetic factors involved in the development and characteristics of insomnia.

Molecular Mechanisms of Sleep and Insomnia

Numerous sleep regulatory substances are linked to circadian rhythmicity and sleep regulation. Although recognizing the oversimplification,42 we argue that endogenous molecules can be categorized as primarily wake-promoting/sleep-suppressing (eg, catecholamines, orexin, and histamine) and sleep-promoting/wake-suppressing substances (eg, γ-aminobutyric acid [GABA], adenosine, serotonin, melatonin, prostaglandin D2).43 Very few molecular studies have been conducted in insomnia and have focused on only a limited set of molecules (eg, cortisol and GABA). Table 1 lists studies linking various molecules to insomnia.41,44-55 Findings are mixed across studies, and no consistent pattern for a specific type of molecule (sleep vs wake promoting) has emerged. Despite contradictory evidence,52 results have largely been interpreted within the context of the hyperarousal hypothesis. For example, increased45 and decreased46 GABA in the occipital cortex of patients with insomnia have been reported to be consistent with the hyperarousal model of insomnia. However, sleep regulatory molecules interact with each other in complex ways (described in more detail later), and many of their effects are dependent on the milieu of the brain state; that is, they are state-dependent. These factors make it highly unlikely that all cases of insomnia can be explained by alterations in any single type of molecule (eg, hyperarousal-related). A more sophisticated conceptualization holds that chronic insomnia results from disintegration of the alternating rhythms of wake-promoting and sleep-regulatory molecules in the brain.56 Constant routine, in-home PSG, and sleep-deprivation studies, particularly those that examine wake- and sleep-promoting molecules (or their mRNA and associated micro-RNA) during different states across the 24-h day, may prove fruitful in elucidating the molecular underpinnings of chronic insomnia. In addition, studies that more fully link this level of analysis with the genetic underpinnings are also needed.

TABLE 1 ] .

Insomnia Related Molecules (Neurotransmitters and Hormones)

Molecule Method Insomnia vs Control Subjects Reference
Calcium Blood serum levels 41
γ-Aminobutyric acid Average brain spectroscopy 44
Occipital cortex spectroscopy 45
Anterior cingulate and occipital cortex spectroscopy 46
Melatonin Evening wake/early sleep blood serum levels 47
Urinary excretion Shifted 48
Noradrenaline Urinary excretion 49
Corticotropin-releasing hormone Blood serum levels 50
Adrenocorticotropic hormone Blood serum levels 50
24-h blood plasma levels 51
Cortisol Evening and morning salivary levels ↑ (Evening), ns (morning) 49
Blood serum levels ns 47
Salivary levels ns 52
Evening and morning salivary levels ns (Evening), ↓ (morning) 53
Evening and morning salivary levels ns 54
Evening wake/early sleep blood plasma levels 55
Evening/early sleep blood plasma levels 51
Blood serum levels 50

↓ = insomnia less than control subjects; ↑ = insomnia greater than control subjects; ns = no significant difference.

Cellular Mechanisms of Sleep and Insomnia

Many of the molecules involved in sleep-wake regulation are produced by specific brain structures with widespread projections throughout the brain. There is, however, mounting evidence that many sleep regulatory molecules affect neurons locally, in the regions in which they are produced. In local sleep theory proposed by Krueger et al,57 sleep is defined as a fundamental emergent property of highly interconnected neurons, or cortical columns. Local sleep propensity and slow wave amplitude are posited to be dependent on accumulation of sleep-regulatory substances (eg, tumor necrosis factor-α and IL-1β)58,59 resulting from prior neuronal use. Synchronous firing within cortical columns is postulated to propagate slow wave activity in adjacent regions through humoral and electric interactions, leading eventually to a “global” sleep state in the entire organism.

From this perspective, insomnia may not be a “whole-brain” event (ie, a simple matter of imbalance between global amounts of sleep and wake). An animal model of insomnia has demonstrated simultaneous localized Fos activation in both sleep-promoting and wake-promoting regions during global sleep.60 In humans, spectral EEG methods have identified heightened regional electrical brain activity in patients with insomnia during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep.61,62 Merica et al61 proposed that the lack of objective sleep disruption in many patients with insomnia may be due to isolated neuronal groups remaining active during PSG-defined sleep. This dynamic in the brain may be experienced as wakefulness by many patients with insomnia and miscategorized as “normal” sleep based on standard PSG criteria.63 Advances in neuroimaging technology would be needed to determine whether insomnia is associated with a distributed pattern of wakefulness at the neuronal level or is better characterized by a region-specific persistence of wake-like brain activity during globally defined EEG sleep more consistent with the next level of analysis.64

Sleep-Wake Regulation and Neural Circuitry of Sleep

On the global level, sleep is regulated by coordinated wake and sleep brain networks. Insomnia may plausibly involve dysregulation within these networks.

Wake Systems and Hyperarousal in Insomnia

The major wake-promoting systems of the brain include the “bottom-up” reticular activating system, limbic networks, and the “top-down” cognitive systems. Neural systems originating in the brainstem, thalamus, and hypothalamus65-67 constitute the ascending reticular activating system. This system projects to the cortex via the thalamus and basal forebrain and includes cholinergic pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei, noradrenergic locus coeruleus nuclei, serotoninergic dorsal and median raphe nuclei, the parabrachial nucleus, the histaminergic tuberomammillary nucleus of the posterior hypothalamus (TMN), and basal forebrain cholinergic nuclei. Orexin/hypocretin neurons of the lateral hypothalamus project to all of the arousal-promoting centers in the brainstem and hypothalamus and reinforce their activity. Emotional and cognitive systems can enhance monoaminergic expression and lead to suppression of sleep-promoting regions such as the ventrolateral preoptic area (VLPO). Inputs to the arousal system may suppress the firing of VLPO neurons, disinhibiting the orexin/hypocretin and TMN neurons and thereby opposing sleep pressure.

During initiation of normal sleep, arousal systems are down-regulated by inhibition from the VLPO and median preoptic area (MnPO). The activation of arousal centers at the end of the sleep period is sufficient to terminate sleep. Activity of arousal systems (eg, cortisol) responsible for alertness is modulated by the circadian timing system.68,69 Insomnia is often considered a disorder of excessive activation of the arousal systems of the brain (ie, hyperarousal).19 Hyperarousal in the physiologic, emotional, or cognitive networks is believed to prevent sleep regulatory processes from naturally occurring in patients with insomnia (see References 20, 22-25). However, other evidence suggests that hyperarousal is neither necessary nor sufficient for the development of chronic insomnia. For example, many patients with insomnia show no signs of cardiovascular, body temperature, or cortisol marker of hyperarousal,52 and not all individuals with these common markers of hyperarousal develop insomnia.

Sleep Systems: Two-Process Model of Sleep Regulation

The activity of arousal and sleep centers described above is modulated by two critical physiologic processes: wake-dependent (homeostatic) sleep drive and circadian rhythmicity. These two processes have been described in the two-process model and related conceptualizations, such as the opponent-process model of sleep-wake regulation.70 According to the two-process model of sleep regulation,71 sleep propensity is regulated by the interaction of a wake-dependent process (process S) and a relatively wake-independent circadian process (process C). Process S dictates that greater brain use during wakefulness increases sleep need and is measured by greater θ activity in the waking EEG72,73 and higher amplitude EEG power in the δ range (0.5-4.5 Hz) during NREM sleep.58 On the network level, sleep onset is driven by activation of GABAergic and galanin neurons in the VLPO and MnPO.65 Accumulation of extracellular adenosine during prior wakefulness has been posited as a primary input to these systems. Axons from VLPO/MnPO send outputs to arousal centers in the hypothalamus and brainstem, inhibiting arousal-promoting neurons of TMN, dorsal and median raphe nuclei, and locus coeruleus while simultaneously promoting sleep. Circadian sleep propensity is regulated by intrinsic circadian oscillations governed by the suprachiasmatic nuclei of the hypothalamus. Exogenous light, melatonin, and social factors can influence the suprachiasmatic nuclei-regulated circadian processes in the body, such as REM sleep, body temperature, and endogenous melatonin. Optimal sleep is believed to occur when the S- and C-processes driving sleep are appropriately coordinated.

One hypothesis based on the two-process model is that insomnia results from insufficient sleep propensity during the desired sleep period because of dysfunction in the S- or C-process. Evidence is mixed on whether patients with insomnia compared with control subjects have less robust slow wave activity following sleep deprivation suggestive of a deficiency in process S.74-76 Evidence linking insomnia to markers of circadian dysfunction, including delayed or advanced core body temperature rhythms, or increased mean nocturnal core body temperature in different insomnia phenotypes, suggest dysregulation of process C.77 However, some studies failed to find an association between core body temperature and insomnia.52

Sleep Switch

The mutually inhibitory circuitry of the VLPO and arousal centers of the brain is often described as a central “flip-flop switch” regulating the activity of wake and sleep promoting systems to produce bistable sleep-wake states.65-67 From this perspective, sleep and wake states are achieved via reciprocal inhibition between the VLPO/MnPO regions and monoaminergic brainstem and hypothalamic arousal centers (see Reference 67 for review). Overriding the flip-flop switch has been proposed as a mechanism of insomnia.78 Although maintaining heightened awareness in the presence of sleep debt may be necessary and beneficial in rare times of crisis, insomnia may result from chronic coactivation of sleep and wake circuits during the desired sleep period. This conceptualization of insomnia is consistent with an animal study showing that the reciprocal inhibitory innervation between the VLPO and the arousal system can decouple under stressful conditions, resulting in a unique state with simultaneous sleep and wake features.60 According to this model, the core feature of insomnia is not reduced sleep or excessive wakefulness but rather the simultaneous activation of brain structures responsible for each state.

Other authors have proposed that insomnia is an unstable state in which individuals rapidly transition in and out of sleep-wake states (ie, a flickering switch). Individuals with greater subjective-objective sleep discrepancy may have more brief arousals from sleep (eg, Reference 79) and more frequent sleep-wake transitions during the sleep onset interval,80 consistent with rapid switching between sleep and wake states.

Structural and Functional Neuroimaging

Although slow wave activity during NREM sleep is commonly believed to be global and homogeneous in the cerebrum, numerous studies have demonstrated that sleep is a dynamic process in space and time. Compared with a resting wake state, NREM sleep is associated with lower whole-brain metabolism, particularly in the cortical association areas.81 More specifically, increased slow wave activity during NREM sleep corresponds with reduced cerebral blood flow globally but most strongly correlates with decreases in brain regions involved in adapting behavior to environmental pressures (ventrolateral prefrontal cortex), conscious processes (anterior cingulate, precuneus/upper cuneus, mediotemporal cortex), action selection (basal ganglia), and generation of slow oscillations during slow wave sleep (brainstem, midbrain, and thalamic structures).82 Principal components analysis of PET scan data identified two brain networks associated with sleep: (1) reduced blood flow in frontal and parietal association cortices and hippocampus, and increased flow in the cerebellum; and (2) reduced blood flow in the thalamus and a region that, on visual inspection, overlaps with the precuneus and cuneus.83

Lesion and structural neuroimaging studies also suggest that specific brain regions may be associated with insomnia. Animal studies demonstrated that lesions of the thalamus,84 raphe nucleus,85 or mediobasal preoptic area86 result in insomnia. von Economo87 observed the sleep of patients affected by the encephalitis pandemic of 1918, and these observations revealed an association between insomnia and lesions in the anterior hypothalamus. In addition, patients with traumatic brain injuries who endorse insomnia symptoms had overlapping lesions in the left dorsomedial frontal cortex.88 Structural imaging studies have identified reduced gray matter volume in left orbitofrontal, prefrontal, precuneus, and temporal cortices in patients with insomnia.89-93 These structures may represent dysfunctional nodes in networks of sleep/wake regulation.

Functional imaging studies suggest that patients with insomnia have smaller reductions in brain activity during NREM sleep relative to resting wake. Specifically, the frontoparietal cortex, medial temporal lobes, thalamus, anterior cingulate, precuneus, and brain stem arousal networks have been implicated.94,95 Corsi-Cabrera et al96 also examined the topographic distribution of brain wave activity associated with wakefulness as an index of cortical activation during the sleep onset period among individuals with primary insomnia. They found higher β activity in left frontal and frontal midline regions during W and N1 and higher levels of temporal coupling linking the frontal, parietal, and posterior midline regions during the sleep onset period in primary insomnia as compared with control subjects. One overarching hypothesis is that the regional patterns of greater activation during sleep in patients with insomnia reflects impaired deactivation and disengagement of brain regions involved in executive control, attention, and self-awareness and may contribute to the experience of insomnia.96

Electrophysiologic and Physiologic Dysregulation in Insomnia

Hyperarousal has been examined using various electrophysiologic (EEG) and physiologic measures during sleep and wakefulness. EEG indicators of hyperarousal include increased high-frequency EEG activity (β and γ), decreased δ activity, and increased REM EEG arousals. As discussed later, physiologic measures include increased body temperature, skin resistance, metabolic rate, and heart rate, among others.

NREM Sleep Instability

The Neurocognitive Model of insomnia posits that acute insomnia may be perpetuated by maladaptive behavioral coping strategies and may develop into chronic insomnia as a result of conditioned arousal.21 Conditioned arousal is the repeated association of sleep-related cues with wakefulness and/or arousal, which, over time, results in an arousal response when a sleep-related stimulus is presented.97 The neurocognitive model focuses specifically on cortical arousal as the mechanism underlying chronic insomnia, as indexed by high-frequency EEG activity (β and γ, 16-50 Hz). This EEG activity is hypothesized to increase around sleep onset as a result of classic conditioning (ie, a learned response to cues associated with sleep). Data to support this hypothesis show diminished δ and increased high-frequency NREM EEG power among patients with insomnia, as well as an association between high-frequency EEG activity and subjective sleep complaints.98,99 High-frequency EEG enhances sensory and information processing, which may contribute to the subjective-objective discrepancy that often characterizes insomnia.21 Evidence is mixed regarding insomnia-control differences in high-frequency EEG activity during wakefulness.72,100 However, high-frequency waking EEG activity significantly correlates with high-frequency EEG activity during NREM100 and with self-reported hyperarousal symptoms.72 These findings support the hypothesis that high-frequency EEG power in insomnia is a marker of CNS hyperarousal.101

REM Sleep Instability

The REM sleep instability model102 hypothesizes that the subjective experience of insomnia is related to decreased REM sleep percent and increased REM EEG arousals.103 In one study, arousals and awakenings during REM more precisely distinguished patients with insomnia from good sleepers than NREM parameters.102 Fragmented REM sleep may promote the perception of increased wakefulness and nonrestorative sleep in insomnia, which may contribute to subjective-objective sleep discrepancies insomnia.102

Physiologic Hyperarousal

Additional support for the involvement of hyperarousal in the pathophysiology of insomnia comes from findings examining various measures of physiologic arousal among individuals with insomnia. As early as 1967, Monroe104 showed that poor sleepers had increased body temperature, vasoconstrictions, body movements, and skin resistance as compared with good sleepers. Insomnia has also been associated, in some studies, with increased 24-h metabolic rate (as measured by oxygen consumption),27,105 24-h adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol levels,51 and heart rate.106 Some investigators have demonstrated greater inhibition of facial muscle activity and increased cardiac vagal tone in response to sleep-related emotional stimuli among individuals with insomnia as compared with good sleepers.107 Others have specifically examined the sleep-onset period, finding increased frontalis electromyogram, increased heart rate, and decreased finger temperature among subjects with insomnia as compared with control subjects up to the point of sleep onset.108 Findings have also shown sympathetic activation among patients with insomnia during sleep onset, as evidenced by consistently lower cardiac pre-ejection period (PEP) values as compared with good sleepers.106,109 Cardiac PEP is the time interval from the beginning of ventricular depolarization (marked by the onset of the QRS complex in the ECG) to the opening of the aortic valve. PEP duration is inversely related to β-adrenergic tone.106 Thus, lower PEP values indicate enhanced activation of the sympathetic nervous system.

Many studies investigating physiologic arousal and hyperarousal in insomnia have included small samples and have not been consistently replicated.52 For this reason, it is not possible to specify diagnostic thresholds for any single physiologic measure in insomnia. Noting these findings, Varkevisser et al52 cautioned against overemphasizing hyperarousal in the conceptualization of chronic insomnia. Although hyperarousal is an important heuristic concept in many models of insomnia, several conflicting points remain to be resolved. First, the extent to which hyperarousal is a cause or a consequence of insomnia has not been elucidated. Second, targeting hyperarousal in the treatment of insomnia (eg, through relaxation training) is often less effective than approaches that enhance sleep processes (eg, sleep restriction and hypnotic medications). Third, markers of hyperarousal can also be interpreted as insufficient inhibition of arousal by sleep-promoting processes.

Behavioral and Cognitive Contributions to Insomnia

Various biologic mechanisms regulate sleep and contribute to insomnia. Behavioral and cognitive mechanisms (ie, beliefs that contribute to specific behaviors) can also regulate sleep and contribute to, and exacerbate, insomnia.

Perpetuating Factors

The “3P model,”110 a diathesis-stress model, describes a set of predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating factors that may contribute to the development and maintenance of insomnia. Predisposing factors, such as age or sex, make an individual more susceptible to insomnia, and precipitating factors are events that coincide with the onset of insomnia, such as major stressors.111 Perpetuating factors, the largest focus of the 3P model, are behaviors and beliefs that maintain insomnia,111 such as increasing time in bed to “catch up” on sleep.110 However, extended time in bed perpetuates insomnia because it leads to increased wakefulness, fragmented sleep, variability in sleep timing,110 and associations between the sleep environment and wakefulness. Thus, initial attempts to reduce symptoms of insomnia may evolve into perpetuating factors themselves.

Stimulus Control

In 1972, Bootzin112 proposed that stimuli associated with sleep (eg, a quiet, dark bedroom) become discriminative stimuli that reinforce sleep. Insomnia may result from inadequate sleep-promoting stimuli or from the presence of stimuli that are antithetical to sleeping,112 such as phone calls, reading, or worry. Stimulus control therapy for insomnia aims to separate the stimuli associated with sleep from the stimuli associated with other activities.112

Cognitive Model

The cognitive model of insomnia25 proposes that individuals with insomnia are susceptible to excessive worry and unpleasant intrusive thoughts, particularly those related to getting enough sleep and the consequences of sleep disturbance. This worry may develop into sleep-related anxiety, lead to increased vigilance for sleep-related threats (eg, watching the clock at night), and ultimately result in an exaggeration of the magnitude of the actual sleep disruption. Cognitive therapy for insomnia challenges these maladaptive cognitive processes and limits the behaviors that maintain unhelpful beliefs and insomnia.113

In the psychobiologic inhibition model,24,114 a variant of the cognitive model, sleep is thought of as automatic,24 whereas insomnia is thought of as a failure of automatic sleep. The model specifically focuses on the attention-intention-effort pathway as one sleep inhibitory process, in which three processes occur: (1) increased selective attention to sleep and symptoms of insomnia; (2) an increase in the subjective value of sleep, which may contribute to explicit “intention to sleep”; and (3) gradual development of increased effort to sleep, described as “sleep effort syndrome.”114 Thus, treatment of insomnia should focus on cognitive strategies that aim to reverse sleep-related attentional bias113 and behavioral strategies115 aimed at reducing sleep effort.

Self-Report and Insomnia

Unlike many other sleep disorders, insomnia disorder relies on self-report for diagnosis; physiologic markers of sleep dysregulation (PSG) or hyperarousal are not routinely indicated for the evaluation of insomnia.116-118 Several groups have developed self-report measures aimed at detecting insomnia and assessing insomnia-related experiences and impairments. Focus groups that captured patients’ subjective experience of insomnia highlighted the pervasive impact of the disorder, the perception that others do not fully understand the impact of insomnia, and the importance of daytime symptoms of insomnia.119 Self-report measures that were developed to assess presleep thought content120 and sleep-related quality of life impairment121 in insomnia demonstrated that (1) presleep cognitive activity among patients with insomnia focuses on rehearsal/planning, sleep and its consequences, and autonomic experiences, among others, and (2) the most common impairments pertain to energy/motivation, performance at work, cognitive functioning, and emotion regulation. This work has also shown that self-report symptom scales reliably discriminate individuals with insomnia and good sleepers.120,122

Although PSG usually shows abnormalities in sleep architecture and continuity among individuals with insomnia, the magnitude of patient-control differences is often small, and the severity of objective findings is often less than that obtained by self-report.1 Nevertheless, differences among patients with insomnia in subjective and objective measures may have important clinical implications. For instance, a meaningful discrepancy between subjective and objective measurement of sleep is prevalent among patients with insomnia with objective normal sleep duration but not among patients with insomnia with objective short sleep.123 Moreover, compared with patients with insomnia who have normal overall sleep duration, patients with insomnia with short objectively measured sleep duration are at increased risk for adverse health outcomes, including hypertension, diabetes, physiologic hyperarousal, cognitive difficulties, and even mortality.124,125 The magnitude and night-to-night variability of discrepancy between self-reported and objectively measured sleep may itself constitute a high-risk presentation that may be informative for examining the etiology and pathophysiology of insomnia.126,127 Thus, objective assessments of sleep may be useful adjunctive measures in predicting the biologic severity and medical impact of insomnia, and cost-effective objective measures of sleep should be considered in the standard diagnostic procedure for insomnia to differentiate phenotypes.124,125

Integration and Treatment Implications

Our intent in this article has been to outline the mechanisms by which insomnia develops and is maintained, highlighting findings in the literature at various levels of analysis. Table 2 summarizes the evidence at each unit of analysis, which indicates that evidence for the pathophysiology has been generated using numerous methodologies based on a range of theoretical scientific perspectives. Integration of the evidence presented here allows us to propose one possible model for the pathophysiology of insomnia. Figure 1 depicts this model, in which insomnia is most likely to develop in those who have increased genetic risk and who experience abnormalities in neurobiological processes. These trait-like vulnerabilities may lead to neurophysiologic hyperarousal and to psychologic and behavioral processes, which, individually or together, increase an individual’s risk for developing insomnia and associated downstream health consequences. Precipitating stressors and other person-specific factors (eg, age, sex) moderate these relationships. The extent to which an individual with insomnia shows evidence of abnormalities in each of the processes depicted may vary among different individuals.

TABLE 2 ] .

Evidence for the Pathophysiology of Insomnia at Each Unit of Analysis

Unit of Analysis Evidence for This Perspective in Pathophysiology of Insomnia
Genes Elevated family risk for insomnia
Elevated genetic risk in twin studies
Insomnia phenotype in drosophila related to mutations in 755 genes with human homologs
Candidate gene studies support association between aspects of insomnia and Apoε4, PER34/4, HLA DQB1*0602, 3111C/C Clock, short (s-) allele of the 5-HTTLPR
Numerous SNPs identified in human genomewide association study studies
Molecules Mixed findings regarding the role of wake- and sleep-promoting molecules in insomnia; no consistent pattern for a specific type of molecule has emerged
Unlikely that all cases of insomnia can be explained by alterations in any single molecule type
See Table 1 for links between various molecules and insomnia
Cells Simultaneous localized Fos activation in both sleep-promoting and wake-promoting regions during global sleep in rats. Individual cortical columns show sleep-like activity while other parts of the brain show wake-like activity
Neuronal use results in modulation of gene expression in sleep regulatory substance, which acts locally in the brain to promote sleep
Circuits In animals, lesions of the anterior ventral and the dorsomedial thalamus, raphe nucleus, or paramedial preoptic area results in insomnia
Less robust slow wave activity following sleep deprivation among subjects with insomnia than control subjects (process S); delayed and advanced core body temperature rhythms and heightened nocturnal core body temperature linked to insomnia (process C)
Those reporting greater subjective-objective sleep discrepancy demonstrate sleep-related behaviors consistent with rapid switching between sleep and wake states
Reduced gray matter volume in left ventromedial prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and temporal cortices in patients with insomnia
Patients with insomnia have smaller reductions in brain activity during NREM sleep relative to resting wake
Higher β activity in left frontal and frontal midline regions during W and N1, higher levels of temporal coupling linking the frontal, parietal, and posterior midline regions during the sleep onset period in insomnia as compared with control subjects
Physiology Diminished δ and increased high-frequency NREM EEG power among patients with insomnia
Association between high-frequency EEG activity and subjective sleep complaints
High-frequency waking EEG activity correlates with high-frequency EEG activity during NREM, and with self-reported hyperarousal symptoms
Arousals and awakenings during REM sleep more precisely distinguished subjects with insomnia from good sleepers than the NREM parameters
Insomnia associated with increased body temperature, vasoconstrictions, body movements, skin resistance, 24-h metabolic rate, 24-h ACTH, cortisol levels
Continuous sympathetic hyperactivation among subjects with insomnia during sleep onset
Behavior Some efficacious treatments for insomnia focus on resolving the behavioral and cognitive factors contributing to and exacerbating insomnia. These include:
 Increasing the association between the bed and being asleep
 Reestablishing a consistent sleep-wake schedule
 Restricting time in bed to increase sleep drive and, subsequently, sleep efficiency
 Reducing somatic tension or intrusive thoughts that are antithetical to sleep
 Psychotherapy targeting maladaptive beliefs about sleep
Self-reports Self-report measures discriminate subjects with insomnia from good sleepers
Presleep mentation among subjects with insomnia focuses on rehearsal/planning, sleep and its consequences, and autonomic experiences
The areas of energy/motivation, performance at work, cognitive functioning, and emotion regulation are the most commonly reported sleep-related impairments in insomnia
Underestimation of sleep duration is prevalent among those with a subjective complaint of insomnia but objectively normal sleep duration
The magnitude of discrepancy between self-reported and objectively measured sleep may itself constitute a high risk factor for insomnia.

ACTH = adrenocorticotropic hormone; NREM = non-rapid eye movement; SNP = single-nucleotide polymorphism.

Figure 1 –

Figure 1 –

Model of the pathophysiology of insomnia. GABA = γ-aminobutyric acid; SNP = single-nucleotide polymorphism.

Accordingly, interventions aimed at preventing or resolving symptoms of insomnia may target various aspects of the identified pathophysiologic processes. The current gold standard psychologic treatment of insomnia is Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia,128-130 which is typically composed of multiple treatment elements: stimulus control therapy, sleep restriction therapy, relaxation training, cognitive therapy, and sleep hygiene education (see Reference 131 for a review). These treatment elements focus on: (1) increasing the association between the bed and being asleep; (2) reestablishing a consistent sleep-wake schedule; (3) restricting time in bed to increase sleep drive and, subsequently, sleep efficiency; (4) reducing somatic tension or intrusive thoughts that are antithetical to sleep; (5) targeting maladaptive beliefs about sleep; and (6) maintaining good sleep practices.131 As reviewed by Buysse132 and Morin and Benca,7 the efficacy of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia for the treatment of insomnia has been demonstrated. Although these approaches make efforts toward reducing cognitive and emotional arousal, there has also been a call for therapies that target physiologic hyperarousal across the night and day.22 Some studies have documented changes in physiologic measures with pharmacologic therapies, but much more work is needed to draw conclusions about the efficacy of these approaches.22

In addition, several efficacious pharmacologic treatments for insomnia target various aspects of the identified pathophysiologic processes (see References 133, 134 for review). For example, benzodiazepine receptor agonists (eg, temazepam, zolpidem), which are generally effective in the treatment of insomnia, promote sleep by enhancing widespread inhibitory activity of GABA. The tricyclic drug doxepin has shown efficacy for sleep initiation and maintenance insomnia; although not US Food and Drug Administration-approved, trazodone is also widely used for insomnia. The sedative effect of these drugs is mainly achieved by targeting the histaminergic arousal system. More recently, there has been progress in the development of orexin receptor antagonists for the treatment of insomnia (eg, almorexant, suvorexant); suvorexant was recently approved by the Food and Drug Administration for this indication. These drugs target the orexin system that promotes arousal of brainstem/hypothalamic arousal centers. Other medications have been suggested for the treatment of insomnia, but additional work is needed to demonstrate safety and efficacy. Future work on the pathophysiology of insomnia may help to identify specific mechanisms in specific patient groups, which may lead to more targeted pharmacotherapy.

Conclusions

Evaluating evidence from a range of domains and across various levels demonstrates not only the advances made in understanding the pathophysiology of insomnia but also the areas in which additional support is needed and the type of analysis that might fill a gap in the literature. For example, human genetic studies that include more refined sleep measures (ie, clinical assessment, sleep diaries, actigraphy, PSG) may provide greater power needed to identify the role of genes in the pathophysiology of insomnia. At the neural and physiologic levels, future work should examine the potential role of impaired “switching processes” and local and circuit-level sleep dysregulation. Functional imaging, high-density EEG, magnetoencephalography, and magnetic resonance spectroscopy are useful tools for such studies. Last, additional studies at all levels of analysis should more deeply examine subjective-objective discrepancies as one clue to insomnia pathophysiology. Greater understanding of the pathophysiology of insomnia may provide important information regarding new targets for prevention and treatment.

Acknowledgments

Financial/nonfinancial disclosures: The authors have reported to CHEST the following conflicts of interest: Dr Levenson receives royalties from American Psychological Association books and receives grant support from the American Psychological Foundation. Dr Buysse has served as a consultant for Merck & Co, Inc; Medscape; Purdue Pharma LP; Emmi Solutions, LLC; Eisai Co, Ltd; CME Outfitters, LLC; and Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co, Ltd. Dr Kay has reported that no potential conflicts of interest exist with any companies/organizations whose products or services may be discussed in this article.

Role of sponsors: The sponsor had no role in the design of the study, the collection and analysis of the data, or the preparation of the manuscript.

ABBREVIATIONS:

GABA

γ-aminobutyric acid

MnPO

median preoptic area

NREM

non-rapid eye movement

PEP

pre-ejection period

PSG

polysomnography

REM

rapid eye movement

SNP

single-nucleotide polymorphism

TMN

tuberomammillary nucleus of the posterior hypothalamus

VLPO

ventrolateral preoptic area

Footnotes

FUNDING/SUPPORT: Dr Buysse is supported by the National Institutes of Health [Grants MH024652, MH102412, AG020677, and HL125103]. Drs Levenson and Kay are supported by the National Institutes of Health [Grant HL082610, T32, PI Buysse].

Reproduction of this article is prohibited without written permission from the American College of Chest Physicians. See online for more details.

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hauri PJ, Sateia MJ, eds. International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. 2nd ed. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Walsh JK, Coulouvrat C, Hajak G, et al. Nighttime insomnia symptoms and perceived health in the America Insomnia Survey (AIS). Sleep. 2011;34(8):997-1011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ohayon MM, Reynolds CF., III Epidemiological and clinical relevance of insomnia diagnosis algorithms according to the DSM-IV and the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD). Sleep Med. 2009;10(9):952-960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Morin CM, LeBlanc M, Bélanger L, Ivers H, Mérette C, Savard J. Prevalence of insomnia and its treatment in Canada. Can J Psychiatry. 2011;56(9):540-548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Morin CM, Jarrin DC. Epidemiology of insomnia: prevalence, course, risk factors, and public health burden. Sleep Med Clin. 2013;8(3):281-297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Morin CM, Benca R. Chronic insomnia. Lancet. 2012;379(9821):1129-1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Morin CM, LeBlanc M, Daley M, Gregoire JP, Mérette C. Epidemiology of insomnia: prevalence, self-help treatments, consultations, and determinants of help-seeking behaviors. Sleep Med. 2006;7(2):123-130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Morin CM, Bélanger L, LeBlanc M, et al. The natural history of insomnia: a population-based 3-year longitudinal study. Arch Intern Med. 2009;169(5):447-453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Perlis M, Shaw PJ, Cano G, et al. Models of insomnia. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practices of Sleep Medicine. St. Louis, MO: Elsevier; 2011:850-865. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Buysse DJ. Etiology and pathogenesis of insomnia. In: Kushida C, ed. The Encyclopedia of Sleep. Waltham, MA: Academic Press; 2013:177-182. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pigeon WR, Cribbet MR. The pathophysiology of insomnia: from models to molecules (and back). Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2012;18(6):546-553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Harvey AG, Tang NK. (Mis)perception of sleep in insomnia: a puzzle and a resolution. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(1):77-101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Roth T, Roehrs T, Pies R. Insomnia: pathophysiology and implications for treatment. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(1):71-79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Riemann D, Kloepfer C, Berger M. Functional and structural brain alterations in insomnia: implications for pathophysiology. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29(9):1754-1760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Houts AC. The diagnostic and statistical manual’s new white coat and circularity of plausible dysfunctions: response to Wakefield, part 1. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39(3):315-345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders Association; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 3rd ed. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Hyperarousal and insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 1997;1(2):97-108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K, Feige B, et al. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(1):19-31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, Bootzin RR, Wyatt JK. Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioural model and a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res. 1997;6(3):179-188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Hyperarousal and insomnia: state of the science. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(1):9-15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Feige B, Baglioni C, Spiegelhalder K, Hirscher V, Nissen C, Riemann D. The microstructure of sleep in primary insomnia: an overview and extension. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;89(2):171-180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Espie CA. Insomnia: conceptual issues in the development, persistence, and treatment of sleep disorder in adults. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:215-243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Harvey AG. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther. 2002;40(8):869-893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Normal human sleep: an overview. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and Practices of Sleep Medicine. St. Louis, MO: Elsevier; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Sleep. 1995;18(7):581-588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Partinen M, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M, Putkonen P, Langinvainio H. Genetic and environmental determination of human sleep. Sleep. 1983;6(3):179-185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kelly JM, Bianchi MT. Mammalian sleep genetics. Neurogenetics. 2012;13(4):287-326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Seugnet L, Suzuki Y, Thimgan M, et al. Identifying sleep regulatory genes using a Drosophila model of insomnia. J Neurosci. 2009;29(22):7148-7157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Gregory AM, Rijsdijk FV, Eley TC. A twin-study of sleep difficulties in school-aged children. Child Dev. 2006;77(6):1668-1679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Drake CL, Friedman NP, Wright KP, Jr, Roth T. Sleep reactivity and insomnia: genetic and environmental influences. Sleep. 2011;34(9):1179-1188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Watson NF, Goldberg J, Arguelles L, Buchwald D. Genetic and environmental influences on insomnia, daytime sleepiness, and obesity in twins. Sleep. 2006;29(5):645-649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gehrman PR, Meltzer LJ, Moore M, et al. Heritability of insomnia symptoms in youth and their relationship to depression and anxiety. Sleep. 2011;34(12):1641-1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wing YK, Zhang J, Lam SP, et al. Familial aggregation and heritability of insomnia in a community-based study. Sleep Med. 2012;13(8):985-990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wang CC, Lung FW. The role of PGC-1 and Apoε4 in insomnia. Psychiatr Genet. 2012;22(2):82-87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brower KJ, Wojnar M, Sliwerska E, Armitage R, Burmeister M. PER3 polymorphism and insomnia severity in alcohol dependence. Sleep. 2012;35(4):571-577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zeitzer JM, Fisicaro RA, Grove ME, Mignot E, Yesavage JA, Friedman L. Faster REM sleep EEG and worse restedness in older insomniacs with HLA DQB1*0602. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187(3):397-400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Serretti A, Benedetti F, Mandelli L, et al. Genetic dissection of psychopathological symptoms: insomnia in mood disorders and CLOCK gene polymorphism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;121B(1):35-38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Deuschle M, Schredl M, Schilling C, et al. Association between a serotonin transporter length polymorphism and primary insomnia. Sleep. 2010;33(3):343-347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ban HJ, Kim SC, Seo J, Kang HB, Choi JK. Genetic and metabolic characterization of insomnia. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e18455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Clinton JM, Davis CJ, Zielinski MR, Jewett KA, Krueger JM. Biochemical regulation of sleep and sleep biomarkers. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7(suppl 5):S38-S42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Griffith LC. Neuromodulatory control of sleep in Drosophila melanogaster: integration of competing and complementary behaviors. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(5):819-823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Winkelman JW, Buxton OM, Jensen JE, et al. Reduced brain GABA in primary insomnia: preliminary data from 4T proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS). Sleep. 2008;31(11):1499-1506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Morgan PT, Pace-Schott EF, Mason GF, et al. Cortical GABA levels in primary insomnia. Sleep. 2012;35(6):807-814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Plante DT, Jensen JE, Schoerning L, Winkelman JW. Reduced γ-aminobutyric acid in occipital and anterior cingulate cortices in primary insomnia: a link to major depressive disorder? Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(6):1548-1557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Riemann D, Klein T, Rodenbeck A, et al. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary insomnia. Psychiatry Res. 2002;113(1-2):17-27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lack LC, Mercer JD, Wright H. Circadian rhythms of early morning awakening insomniacs. J Sleep Res. 1996;5(4):211-219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Seelig E, Keller U, Klarhöfer M, et al. Neuroendocrine regulation and metabolism of glucose and lipids in primary chronic insomnia: a prospective case-control study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4):e61780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Xia L, Chen GH, Li ZH, Jiang S, Shen J. Alterations in hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal/thyroid axes and gonadotropin-releasing hormone in the patients with primary insomnia: a clinical research. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):e71065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin HM, et al. Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: clinical implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2001;86(8):3787-3794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Varkevisser M, Van Dongen HP, Kerkhof GA. Physiologic indexes in chronic insomnia during a constant routine: evidence for general hyperarousal? Sleep. 2005;28(12):1588-1596. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Hohagen F. Sleep disturbances are correlated with decreased morning awakening salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(9):1184-1191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Zhang J, Lam SP, Li SX, et al. A community-based study on the association between insomnia and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: sex and pubertal influences. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014;99(6):2277-2287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Rodenbeck A, Huether G, Rüther E, Hajak G. Interactions between evening and nocturnal cortisol secretion and sleep parameters in patients with severe chronic primary insomnia. Neurosci Lett. 2002;324(2):159-163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dzierzewski JM, O’Brien EM, Kay D, McCrae CS. Tackling sleeplessness: psychological treatment options for insomnia in older adults. Nat Sci Sleep. 2010;2:47-61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Krueger JM, Rector DM, Roy S, Van Dongen HP, Belenky G, Panksepp J. Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):910-919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Yoshida H, Peterfi Z, García-García F, Kirkpatrick R, Yasuda T, Krueger JM. State-specific asymmetries in EEG slow wave activity induced by local application of TNFalpha. Brain Res. 2004;1009(1-2):129-136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Yasuda T, Yoshida H, Garcia-Garcia F, Kay D, Krueger JM. Interleukin-1beta has a role in cerebral cortical state-dependent electroencephalographic slow-wave activity. Sleep. 2005;28(2):177-184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Cano G, Mochizuki T, Saper CB. Neural circuitry of stress-induced insomnia in rats. J Neurosci. 2008;28(40):10167-10184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Merica H, Blois R, Gaillard JM. Spectral characteristics of sleep EEG in chronic insomnia. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10(5):1826-1834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Perlis ML, Merica H, Smith MT, Giles DE. Beta EEG activity and insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(5):363-374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Borbély AA. Secrets of Sleep. New York, NY: Basic Books, Inc; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Buysse DJ, Germain A, Hall M, Monk TH, Nofzinger EA. A neurobiological model of insomnia. Drug Discov Today Dis Models. 2011;8(4):129-137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Saper CB, Chou TC, Scammell TE. The sleep switch: hypothalamic control of sleep and wakefulness. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24(12):726-731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature. 2005;437(7063):1257-1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Saper CB, Fuller PM, Pedersen NP, Lu J, Scammell TE. Sleep state switching. Neuron. 2010;68(6):1023-1042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Monk TH, Leng VC, Folkard S, Weitzman ED. Circadian rhythms in subjective alertness and core body temperature. Chronobiologia. 1983;10(1):49-55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Weitzman ED, Zimmerman JC, Czeisler CA, Ronda J. Cortisol secretion is inhibited during sleep in normal man. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1983;56(2):352-358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Edgar DM, Dement WC, Fuller CA. Effect of SCN lesions on sleep in squirrel monkeys: evidence for opponent processes in sleep-wake regulation. J Neurosci. 1993;13(3):1065-1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1(3):195-204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wolynczyk-Gmaj D, Szelenberger W. Waking EEG in primary insomnia. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz). 2011;71(3):387-392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Vyazovskiy VV, Tobler I. Theta activity in the waking EEG is a marker of sleep propensity in the rat. Brain Res. 2005;1050(1-2):64-71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Besset A, Villemin E, Tafti M, Billiard M. Homeostatic process and sleep spindles in patients with sleep-maintenance insomnia: effect of partial (21 h) sleep deprivation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;107(2):122-132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Stepanski E, Zorick F, Roehrs T, Roth T. Effects of sleep deprivation on daytime sleepiness in primary insomnia. Sleep. 2000;23(2):215-219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Pigeon WR, Perlis ML. Sleep homeostasis in primary insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(4):247-254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Lack LC, Gradisar M, Van Someren EJ, Wright HR, Lushington K. The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12(4):307-317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Saper CB, Cano G, Scammell TE. Homeostatic, circadian, and emotional regulation of sleep. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493(1):92-98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Smith S, Trinder J. The effect of arousals during sleep onset on estimates of sleep onset latency. J Sleep Res. 2000;9(2):129-135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Moul DE, Germain A, Cashmere JD, Quigley M, Miewald JM, Buysse DJ. Examining initial sleep onset in primary insomnia: a case-control study using 4-second epochs. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3(5):479-488. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Miewald JM, et al. Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. Brain. 2002;125(pt 5):1105-1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Maquet P, Degueldre C, Delfiore G, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of human slow wave sleep. J Neurosci. 1997;17(8):2807-2812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Andersson JL, Onoe H, Hetta J, et al. Brain networks affected by synchronized sleep visualized by positron emission tomography. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1998;18(7):701-715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Villablanca J, Salinas-Zeballos ME. Sleep-wakefulness, EEG and behavioral studies of chronic cats without the thalamus: the ‘athalamic’ cat. Arch Ital Biol. 1972;110(3):383-411. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Jouvet M. Sleep and serotonin: an unfinished story. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21(suppl 2):24S-27S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Sallanon M, Denoyer M, Kitahama K, Aubert C, Gay N, Jouvet M. Long-lasting insomnia induced by preoptic neuron lesions and its transient reversal by muscimol injection into the posterior hypothalamus in the cat. Neuroscience. 1989;32(3):669-683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.von Economo C. Sleep as a problem of localization. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1930;71(3):249-259. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Koenigs M, Holliday J, Solomon J, Grafman J. Left dorsomedial frontal brain damage is associated with insomnia. J Neurosci. 2010;30(47):16041-16043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Stoffers D, Moens S, Benjamins J, et al. Orbitofrontal gray matter relates to early morning awakening: a neural correlate of insomnia complaints? Front Neurol. 2012;3:105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Stoffers D, Altena E, van der Werf YD, et al. The caudate: a key node in the neuronal network imbalance of insomnia? Brain. 2014;137(pt 2):610-620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Altena E, Vrenken H, Van Der Werf YD, van den Heuvel OA, Van Someren EJ. Reduced orbitofrontal and parietal gray matter in chronic insomnia: a voxel-based morphometric study. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):182-185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Joo EY, Noh HJ, Kim JS, et al. Brain gray matter deficits in patients with chronic primary insomnia. Sleep. 2013;36(7):999-1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Riemann D, Voderholzer U, Spiegelhalder K, et al. Chronic insomnia and MRI-measured hippocampal volumes: a pilot study. Sleep. 2007;30(8):955-958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, Price JC, Miewald JM, Kupfer DJ. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in insomnia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(11):2126-2128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Nofzinger EA, Nissen C, Germain A, et al. Regional cerebral metabolic correlates of WASO during NREM sleep in insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2006;2(3):316-322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Corsi-Cabrera M, Figueredo-Rodríguez P, del Río-Portilla Y, Sánchez-Romero J, Galán L, Bosch-Bayard J. Enhanced frontoparietal synchronized activation during the wake-sleep transition in patients with primary insomnia. Sleep. 2012;35(4):501-511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Perlis ML, Smith MT, Jungquist C, et al. Cognitive Behavioral Treatment of Insomnia: A Session-by-Session Guide. 1st ed. New York, NY: Springer Verlag; 2005:182. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Krystal AD, Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK, Marsh GR. NREM sleep EEG frequency spectral correlates of sleep complaints in primary insomnia subtypes. Sleep. 2002;25(6):630-640. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Perlis ML, Smith MT, Andrews PJ, Orff H, Giles DE. Beta/Gamma EEG activity in patients with primary and secondary insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep. 2001;24(1):110-117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Wu YM, Pietrone R, Cashmere JD, et al. EEG power during waking and NREM sleep in primary insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9(10):1031-1037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Cortoos A, Verstraeten E, Cluydts R. Neurophysiological aspects of primary insomnia: implications for its treatment. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(4):255-266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K, Nissen C, Hirscher V, Baglioni C, Feige B. REM sleep instability—a new pathway for insomnia? Pharmacopsychiatry. 2012;45(5):167-176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Feige B, Al-Shajlawi A, Nissen C, et al. Does REM sleep contribute to subjective wake time in primary insomnia? A comparison of polysomnographic and subjective sleep in 100 patients. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(2):180-190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Monroe LJ. Psychological and physiological differences between good and poor sleepers. J Abnorm Psychol. 1967;72(3):255-264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Bonnet MH, Arand DL. Physiological activation in patients with Sleep State Misperception. Psychosom Med. 1997;59(5):533-540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Covassin N, de Zambotti M, Sarlo M, De Min Tona G, Sarasso S, Stegagno L. Cognitive performance and cardiovascular markers of hyperarousal in primary insomnia. Int J Psychophysiol. 2011;80(1):79-86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Baglioni C, Lombardo C, Bux E, et al. Psychophysiological reactivity to sleep-related emotional stimuli in primary insomnia. Behav Res Ther. 2010;48(6):467-475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Freedman RR, Sattler HL. Physiological and psychological factors in sleep-onset insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1982;91(5):380-389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.De Zambotti M, Covassin N, De Min Tona G, Sarlo M, Stegagno L. Sleep onset and cardiovascular activity in primary insomnia. J Sleep Res. 2011;20(2):318-325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Spielman AJ, Caruso LS, Glovinsky PB. A behavioral perspective on insomnia treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1987;10(4):541-553. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Dikeos DG, Soldatos CR. The condition of insomnia: etiopathogenetic considerations and their impact on treatment practices. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2005;17(4):255-262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Bootzin RR. Stimulus control treatment for insomnia. Proceedings of the American Psychological Association. 1972;7:395-396. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Harvey AG, Sharpley AL, Ree MJ, Stinson K, Clark DM. An open trial of cognitive therapy for chronic insomnia. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45(10):2491-2501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Espie CA, Broomfield NM, MacMahon KM, Macphee LM, Taylor LM. The attention-intention-effort pathway in the development of psychophysiologic insomnia: a theoretical review. Sleep Med Rev. 2006;10(4):215-245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Means MK, Edinger JD. Behavioral treatment of insomnia. Expert Rev Neurother. 2002;2(1):127-137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.National Institutes of Health. NIH State-of-the-Science Conference Statement on Manifestations and Management of Chronic Insomnia in Adults. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Lichstein KL, Morin CM. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of insomnia. Sleep. 2006;29(9):1155-1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Chesson A, Jr, Hartse K, Anderson WM, et al. ; Standards of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Practice parameters for the evaluation of chronic insomnia. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine report. Sleep. 2000;23(2):237-241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Carey TJ, Moul DE, Pilkonis P, Germain A, Buysse DJ. Focusing on the experience of insomnia. Behav Sleep Med. 2005;3(2):73-86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Harvey KJ, Espie CA. Development and preliminary validation of the Glasgow Content of Thoughts Inventory (GCTI): a new measure for the assessment of pre-sleep cognitive activity. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43(pt 4):409-420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Kyle SD, Crawford MR, Morgan K, Spiegelhalder K, Clark AA, Espie CA. The Glasgow Sleep Impact Index (GSII): a novel patient-centred measure for assessing sleep-related quality of life impairment in Insomnia Disorder. Sleep Med. 2013;14(6):493-501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Smith S, Trinder J. Detecting insomnia: comparison of four self-report measures of sleep in a young adult population. J Sleep Res. 2001;10(3):229-235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Fernandez-Mendoza J, Calhoun SL, Bixler EO, et al. Sleep misperception and chronic insomnia in the general population: role of objective sleep duration and psychological profiles. Psychosom Med. 2011;73(1):88-97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Vgontzas AN, Fernandez-Mendoza J. Insomnia with short sleep duration: nosological, diagnostic, and treatment implications. Sleep Med Clin. 2013;8(3):309-322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Vgontzas AN, Fernandez-Mendoza J, Liao D, Bixler EO. Insomnia with objective short sleep duration: the most biologically severe phenotype of the disorder. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17(4):241-254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Williams JM, Kay DB, Rowe M, McCrae CS. Sleep discrepancy, sleep complaint, and poor sleep among older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68(5):712-720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Kay DB, Dzierzewski JM, Rowe M, McCrae CS. Greater night-to-night variability in sleep discrepancy among older adults with a sleep complaint compared to noncomplaining older adults. Behav Sleep Med. 2013;11(2):76-90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Morin CM. Insomnia: Psychological Assessment and Management. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Williams J, Roth A, Vatthauer K, McCrae CS. Cognitive behavioral treatment of insomnia. Chest. 2013;143(2):554-565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Morin CM, Espie CA. Insomnia: A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Morin CM. Cognitive-behavioral approaches to the treatment of insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(suppl 16):33-40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Buysse DJ. Insomnia. JAMA. 2013;309(7):706-716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Ioachimescu OC, El-Solh AA. Pharmacotherapy of insomnia. Expert Opin Pharmacother. 2012;13(9):1243-1260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Szabadi E. Selective targets for arousal-modifying drugs: implications for the treatment of sleep disorders. Drug Discov Today. 2014;19(5):701-708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Chest are provided here courtesy of American College of Chest Physicians

RESOURCES