Abstract
Decline in cognitive performance in old age is linked to both suboptimal neural processing in grey matter (GM) and reduced integrity of white matter (WM), but the whole-brain structure-function-cognition associations remain poorly understood. Here we apply a novel measure of GM processing–moment-to-moment variability in the blood oxygenation level-dependent signal (SDBOLD)—to study the associations between GM function during resting state, performance on four main cognitive domains (i.e., fluid intelligence, perceptual speed, episodic memory, vocabulary), and WM microstructural integrity in 91 healthy older adults (aged 60-80 years). We modeled the relations between whole-GM SDBOLD with cognitive performance using multivariate partial least squares analysis. We found that greater SDBOLD was associated with better fluid abilities and memory. Most of regions showing behaviorally relevant SDBOLD (e.g., precuneus and insula) were localized to inter- or intra-network “hubs” that connect and integrate segregated functional domains in the brain. Our results suggest that optimal dynamic range of neural processing in hub regions may support cognitive operations that specifically rely on the most flexible neural processing and complex cross-talk between different brain networks. Finally, we demonstrated that older adults with greater WM integrity in all major WM tracts had also greater SDBOLD and better performance on tests of memory and fluid abilities. We conclude that SDBOLD is a promising functional neural correlate of individual differences in cognition in healthy older adults and is supported by overall WM integrity.
Introduction
Cognitive performance, such memory, reasoning, perceptual speed, and maintenance of semantic knowledge, relies on the neural processing in grey matter (GM) and the integrity of white matter (WM). Many neuroimaging studies attempt to link age-related differences in cognitive performance with either blood-oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal magnitude and localization [1,2] or WM integrity [3]. However, designs and results of the functional studies are region-, and cognitive task-specific, and therefore yield mixed results. As a consequence, combining whole-brain GM function with WM structure in aging remains a challenge and has been rarely attempted. Clearly, a more general functional measure would be useful in linking GM processing with WM integrity and individual differences in cognition in aging.
Recently, measuring the variability in the BOLD signal (SDBOLD) has emerged as a novel frontier in mapping human brain function in aging [4,5]. The brain–a dynamic system that undergoes spontaneous or external stimuli-driven moment-to-moment reconfigurations [6–11] –is inherently variable [12]. Older adults were shown to have reduced SDBOLD in many GM regions compared to younger adults [13], and SDBOLD was associated with faster and more consistent performance [14]. These positive associations between SDBOLD and cognition in aging, however, are based only on performance on a perceptual matching task (instantaneous match-to-sample, attentional cueing, and delayed match-to-sample [14]). Here we address the yet unexplored questions: Can the link between higher SDBOLD and cognitive performance be extended to other cognitive abilities known to decline with age, such as reasoning, speed, and episodic memory [15,16]? Is there a general pattern of higher SDBOLD that supports cognitive functioning across cognitive domains or are patterns of SDBOLD optimal for a specific cognitive function? Does WM integrity support behaviorally relevant SDBOLD in the aging brain?
We collected resting-state fMRI and diffusion images as well as well-normed laboratory measures of fluid intelligence, perceptual speed, episodic memory, and vocabulary [17–20] from 104 healthy participants (60–80 years). Previous research showed that resting-state and task-related signal amplitude fluctuations are linearly related across subjects and voxels and may be governed by same neuronal and physiological mechanism [21]. Therefore, our approach of using resting-state signal minimizes the effect of task on SDBOLD, allowing SDBOLD to be related to a broad range of cognitive abilities. We predicted greater SDBOLD, especially in hub regions highly connected within brain networks [22,23], to be related to better fluid abilities and memory, as they require more moment-to-moment adaptability in brain network utilization (e.g. association formation, mental rotation). We expected SDBOLD to have a weaker relationship with vocabulary knowledge and perceptual speed, relying on rather stereotypical responses and semantic retrieval. We modeled the relations between whole-GM SDBOLD with cognitive performance using multivariate partial least squares analysis (PLS; [24]). Finally, as greater WM integrity, measured as fractional anisotropy (FA), predicts higher cognitive performance and GM processing efficiency in older adults [3,25–27], we proposed WM integrity as a candidate structural correlate of the behaviorally relevant SDBOLD in the aging brain.
We found that greater SDBOLD was associated with better fluid abilities and memory, and this behaviorally relevant SDBOLD was associated with WM integrity.
Methods
Participants
A University of Illinois Institutional Review Board approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants and the study was performed in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. Participants received financial reimbursement. We collected MRI and behavioral data from 111 community-dwelling healthy older adults (37 males). The sample contained more females because fewer older males met the above inclusion criteria or showed willingness to participate in the study. Eligible participants met the following criteria: (1) were between the ages of 60 and 79 years old, (2) were free from psychiatric and neurological illness and had no history of stroke or transient ischemic attack, (3) scored ≥ 27 on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) and >21 on a Telephone Interview of Cognitive Status (TICS-M) questionnaire, (4) scored < 10 on the geriatric depression scale (GDS-15), (5) scored ≥ 75% right-handedness on the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire, (6) demonstrated normal or corrected-to-normal vision of at least 20/40 and no color blindness, (7) were cleared for suitability in the MRI environment, that is, no metallic implants that could interfere with the magnetic field or cause injury, no claustrophobia, and no history of head trauma. The participants were a pre-intervention cross-sectional subsample from an on-going randomized controlled exercise trial (“Influence of Fitness on Brain and Cognition II” at ClinicalTrials.gov, clinical study identifier NCT01472744), from whom good quality anatomical and resting state functional MRI (see section 2.4 and 2.6) was available.
Cognitive assessment and analysis
We administered a cognitive battery as described in the Virginia Cognitive Aging Project [17–20] to measure latent constructs of fluid intelligence, perceptual speed, episodic memory, and vocabulary (for more details on each task see Table 1). The computer-based tasks were programmed in E-prime version 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA) and administered on computers with 17” cathode ray tube monitors.
Table 1. Cognitive battery and the result of dimensionality reduction with PCA.
Task | Construct | Description | Administration | Source | Fluid abilities | Perceptual Speed | Memory | Vocabulary |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Matrix reasoning | Fluid intelligence | Select pattern that best completes the missing cell in a matrix | Computer-based | [77] | .628 | – | – | .418 |
Shipley abstraction | Fluid intelligence | Determine the letters, words, or numbers that best complete a progressive sequence | Paper-pencil | [78] | .525 | – | – | .564 |
Letter sets | Fluid intelligence | Identify which of five groups of letters is different from the others | Computer-based | [79] | .346 | .410 | – | .575 |
Spatial relations | Spatial reasoning | Determine which three dimensional object could be constructed by folding the two dimensional object | Computer-based | [80] | .788 | – | – | – |
Paper folding | Spatial reasoning | Determine the pattern of holes that would result from a sequence of folds and a punch through folded paper | Computer-based | [79] | .856 | – | – | – |
Form boards | Spatial reasoning | Determine shapes needed to fill in a space | Computer-based | [79] | .725 | – | – | – |
Digit symbol | Perceptual speed | Use a code table to write the correct symbol below each digit | Paper-pencil | [81] | – | .756 | – | – |
Letter/pattern comparison | Perceptual speed | Same or different comparison of pairs of letter strings/patterns | Paper-pencil | [82] | –/.346 | .845/.797 | – | – |
Logical memory | Episodic memory | Recall as many idea units as possible from three stories | Computer-based/paper-pencil | [83] | – | – | .752 | .319 |
Free recall | Episodic memory | Recall as many words as possible across four word trial lists | Computer-based/ paper-pencil | [83] | – | – | .789 | – |
Paired associates | Episodic memory | Recall the second words from word pairs | Computer-based/ paper-pencil | [84] | – | – | .787 | – |
WAIS vocab. | Vocabulary | Define words out loud | Experimenter/ paper-pencil | [81] | – | – | – | .778 |
Picture vocab. | Vocabulary | Name the objects presented | Experimenter/ paper-pencil | [85] | .383 | – | – | .720 |
Synonym/ antonym | Vocabulary | Choose the word most similar/opposite in meaning to the target | Computer-based | [86] | – | – | – | .876/.857 |
Note. Columns 6–9: Standardized component loadings from a 4-factor PCA extraction. For clarity, only loadings above 0.30 are displayed. Rotation method: varimax with Kaiser normalization. Rotation converged in 6 iterations. Pairwise exclusion was performed.
To obtain components representing the four cognitive constructs and to confirm the validity of task structure as presented in [20], we performed principal component analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation. Individual scores on each of the 16 tasks were first screened for outliers and winsorized (maximum 3 cases out of 91 (<3%) were adjusted per variable). The resulting constructs are presented in Table 1 and the component scores were saved as variables.
Some participants did not complete all tasks in the cognitive battery, which resulted in a final sample of 91 participants (29 males, age range 60–78, Mage = 65 ± 4 years, years of education 12–26, Medu = 17 ± 4 years).
MRI acquisition
We acquired all images during a single session on a 3T Siemens Trio Tim system with 45 mT/m gradients and 200 T/m/sec slew rates (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). T2*-weighted resting state images were acquired with fast echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) contrast (6min, TR = 2s, TE = 25ms, flip angle = 80 degrees, 3.4 x3.4 mm2 in-plane resolution, 35 4mm-thick slices acquired in ascending order, Grappa acceleration factor = 2, 64 × 64 matrix). The participants were instructed to lay still with eyes closed. Additionally, gradient field maps were acquired to account for geometric distortions caused by magnetic field inhomogeneity [28]. The gradient field map was collected as 35, 4mm-thick slices, 3.4 x 3.4 mm2 in-plane resolution, TR = 700ms, TE = 10ms, and flip angle = 35 degrees.
High-resolution structural MR scans were acquired using a 3D MPRAGE T1-weighted sequence (TR = 1900 ms; TE = 2.32 ms; TI: 900 ms; flip angle = 9°; matrix = 256 × 256; FOV = 230mm; 192 slices; resolution = 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm; GRAPPA acceleration factor 2) and used as an intermediate step in registration of functional images to standard MNI space.
DTI images were acquired with a twice-refocused spin echo single-shot Echo Planar Imaging sequence [29] to minimize eddy current-induced image distortions. The protocol consisted of a set of 30 non-collinear diffusion-weighted acquisitions with b-value = 1000s/mm2 and two T2-weighted b-value = 0 s/mm2 acquisitions, repeated two times (TR/TE = 5500/98 ms, 128 x 128 matrix, 1.7x1.7 mm2 in-plane resolution, FA = 90, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, and bandwidth of 1698 Hz/Px, comprising 40 3-mm-thick slices). Resting state and DTI images were obtained parallel to the anterior-posterior commissure plane with no interslice gap.
BOLD variability (SDBOLD) calculation
Data preprocessing was carried out using FSL v5.0.1 (FMRIB's Software Library, http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; [30]). The preprocessing included high pass filtering (> 0.008Hz), slice timing correction, rigid body motion correction using MCFLRT [31], and removal of non-brain tissue with the Brain Extraction Tool [32]. Data from all subjects was screened for motion and all participants moved within a voxel dimension (< 4mm). Functional images of each participant were aligned to the standard stereotaxic space of the MNI 152 T1 2mm3 template supplied in FSL in a three-step procedure. To improve the registration between the participant’s functional and anatomical images we utilized the gradient field map data. First, the gradient field map was unwrapped via PRELUDE [33], then geometric distortions in the EPI-related images due to local magnetic inhomogeneity differences were compensated for with the use of gradient field map data via FUGUE within FSL [33]. Eleven out of 91 participants had missing field map images. Second, each participant’s low-resolution functional images were aligned with their high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images using the Boundary-Based Registration in FSL [34]. Third, the anatomical images were aligned to MNI 152T1 2mm3 template using 12 degrees of freedom affine linear registration [31].
Next, as recommended by [13], we used Multivariate Exploratory Linear Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (MELODIC v3.10) tool in FSL [35] to decompose the 4D fMRI time series into spatial and temporal components. AZB together with Chanheng He and CNW identified artifact components for each subject using the criteria outlined in [36] based on the spatial pattern, time course, and power spectrum properties that were characteristic of physiological noise, motion, and scanner-related artifacts. The artifactual components were regressed out from the time series yielding the post-ICA ‘cleaned’ data. This post-ICA functional data as well as the six motion parameters outputted earlier by motion correction were bandpass filtered to restrict the frequencies in our data to. 008 < f <. 1 Hz [37]. Next, we extracted mean time series from two nuisance regions of interest (deep temporal white matter, bilateral lateral ventricles) in the post-ICA filtered data. The goal of including these two nuisance regressors is to remove residual cardiorespiratory physiological noise that would be captured by signal changes in the white matter and ventricles [38–41] and was not removed by the ICA cleanup. The two nuisance regressors (timeseries from white matter and ventricles) were regressed out using the general linear model with FEAT 6.00 (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/analysis/research/feat/). Finally, we calculated the standard deviation (SDBOLD) across the whole timeseries for each voxel and smoothed the images with a 6mm Gaussian kernel. The resulting SDBOLD maps were upsampled to MNI space using the registration steps described above. To restrict all multivariate analyses to the GM, we masked the SDBOLD maps with the grey matter tissue prior provided in FSL, thresholded at probability > 0.37. The intermediate outcomes of all the above procedures were carefully inspected by AZB and CNW.
PLS multivariate analysis of relations among SDBOLD, cognitive performance and fitness
First, we made sure that all behavioral variables were normally distributed and any outliers (> 2.5 SD) were accounted for by winsorizing, where not more than 2 cases were corrected per variable (2%).
The behavioral PLS analysis [42,43] begins with a correlation matrix (CORR) between our variables of interest (four cognitive constructs) and each voxel’s signal (SDBOLD); correlations are calculated across subjects. Then, this CORR matrix is decomposed via singular value decomposition (SVD): SVDCORR = USV’. This decomposition produces a left singular vector of behavioral weights (U), a right singular vector of SDBOLD weights (V), and a diagonal matrix of singular values (S). In other words, this analysis produces orthogonal latent variables (LVs) that optimally represent relations between behavior and SDBOLD in grey matter voxels. Each LV contains a spatial pattern depicting the brain regions where the SDBOLD shows the strongest relation to behavior. Each brain weight (in V) is proportional to the correlation between behavior and SDBOLD in all of the tracts. To obtain a summary measure of each participant’s expression of a particular LV pattern, we calculated within-person “brain scores” by multiplying each voxel (i)’s weight (V) from each LV (j) produced from the SVD in equation (1) by the SDBOLD value in that voxel for person (k), and summing over all (n) brain voxels: . Thus, in a single measure, a brain score indicates the degree to which a subject expresses the multivariate spatial pattern captured by a given behavior-driven latent variable. Significance of detected relations between multivariate spatial patterns and cognitive performance was assessed using 1000 permutation tests of the singular value corresponding to each LV. A subsequent bootstrapping procedure revealed the robustness of voxel saliences across 1000 bootstrapped resamples of our data [44]. By dividing each voxel’s mean salience by its bootstrapped standard error, we obtained “bootstrap ratios” as normalized estimates of robustness. We thresholded bootstrap ratios at a value of ≥ 3.00, which approximates a 99% confidence interval and corresponds to p-value of <.001.
DTI analysis
DTI allows inferences about WM microstructure in vivo by quantifying the magnitude and directionality of diffusion of water within a tissue [45]. Visual checks were performed on every volume of the raw data of every participant by AZB. Sixty-six participants had good quality DTI data. In one dataset, one volume with the corresponding b-vectors and b-values was deleted from the dataset before processing due to artifact. Next, DTI data were processed using the FSL Diffusion Toolbox v.3.0 (FDT: http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) in a standard multistep procedure, including: (a) motion and eddy current correction of the images and corresponding b-vectors, (b) removal of the skull and non-brain tissue using the Brain Extraction Tool [32], and (c) voxel-by-voxel calculation of the diffusion tensors. Using the diffusion tensor information, FA maps were computed using DTIFit within the FDT. All motion- and eddy-current outputs, as well as FA images were visually inspected.
We used TBSS [46,47], a toolbox within FSL v5.0.1, to create a representation of main WM tracts common to all subjects (WM “skeleton”). This included: (a) nonlinear alignment of each participant’s FA volume to the 1 x 1 x 1 mm3 standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI152) space via the FMRIB58_FA template using the FMRIB’s Nonlinear Registration Tool (FNIRT, [48]; http://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~dr/software), (b) calculation of the mean of all aligned FA images, (c) creation of the WM “skeleton” by perpendicular non-maximum-suppression of the mean FA image and setting the FA threshold to 0.25, and (d) perpendicular projection of the highest FA value (local center of the tract) onto the skeleton, separately for each subject. The outputs of all the above processing steps were carefully inspected by AZB. Given that SDBOLD is a relatively new way to asses brain function and its structural brain correlates are not yet understood, we did not make any regional predictions and used a global FA measure, obtained by averaging FA over the whole skeleton for each participant.
Post hoc statistical analyses
All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (v.16, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). We used multiple step-wise linear regressions (with chronological age and gender) to investigate the relationships between brains scores from SDBOLD-cognition and global FA. Two participants brain scores had outlier values > 2.5 SD and their values were winsorized, which did not change the results and was used for display purposes.
The demographic data, FA values, behavioral scores, and brain scores are available in S1 Dataset.
Results
Correlations between cognitive performance and SDBOLD
To investigate the relationships between SDBOLD and performance on four main cognitive domains, we first performed principal component analysis (PCA) on 16 tasks from Table 1 to reduce their dimensionality. We replicated the findings of the Salthouse studies [17–20] by obtaining the four expected components of fluid intelligence, perceptual speed, episodic memory, and vocabulary (Table 1). Only speed (r = -.33 p = .002) and memory (r = -31, p = .003) components were negatively related to age, whereas fluid abilities (r = -.18 p = .098) and vocabulary (r = .11 p = .320) were not.
Next, to identify multivariate across-subject patterns of relations between SDBOLD at rest in the entire GM and the scores from the four cognitive components we performed behavioral PLS analysis. Importantly, previous studies related SDBOLD within spliced fixation periods in blocked fMRI series to performance on task [13,14], while the current study is the first application of resting state data in investigating behaviorally relevant SDBOLD. The behavioral PLS analysis begins with the correlation matrix between the individual scores on the four cognitive components and each voxel’s SDBOLD; correlations are calculated across subjects. Then, this matrix is decomposed via singular value decomposition. This decomposition produces orthogonal latent variables (LVs) that optimally represent relations between SDBOLD in GM voxels and cognitive performance. Each LV contains a spatial pattern depicting the brain regions where the activity shows the strongest relation to performance. In this analysis, because we examined the association with four cognitive components, four outcome latent variables (LV) were possible. We predicted that if there are domain-specific patterns of optimal SDBOLD, then multiple LVs may be significant, each representing an association between a different cognitive construct and BOLD variability. Alternatively, if SDBOLD is a more general feature common to different cognitive functions, there should be one LV representing the brain-performance relationship.
Our results supported the latter hypothesis: the PLS multivariate analysis yielded one significant latent variable (permuted p = 0.023, 59.46% cross-block covariance explained by this LV), suggesting that, overall, higher SDBOLD was related to better performance on fluid and memory constructs and lower performance on vocabulary. This relationship was reversed in only two small clusters (Fig. 1A). The same analysis with additional controlling for the global signal (i.e. centering the mean across the volumes) yielded the same spatial pattern, where higher SDBOLD was related to better performance on fluid and memory constructs (permuted p = 0.007, 60.41% cross-block covariance explained by this LV).
If the PLS model was run with vocabulary only (1 LV possible), only the clusters in temporal fusiform and cerebellum were above p <. 001 threshold, but the overall model was not significant. This suggests that the red-yellow cluster shown in Fig. 1A is attributable to the relationship with vocabulary. Similarly, a model with 4 cognitive constructs and additionally chronological age (5 LVs possible) explained only ca. 3% more of cross-block covariance than the four construct model from Fig. 1, and showed the same spatial pattern. This suggests that age is not driving the function-performance result from Fig. 1A. In this model age was positively related to vocabulary performance, but inversely to memory and fluid abilities, and SDBOLD.
Perceptual speed did not significantly contribute to the observed performance–SDBOLD correlation pattern, although there was a trend towards greater perceptual speed being related to lesser SDBOLD. Peak voxels’ location and bootstrap ratios are reported in Table 2.
Table 2. Significant clusters representing SDBOLD and cognitive performance relationship.
Region | MNI coordinates (x, y, z) | BSR | p-value | Cluster size (voxels) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Fusiform/Visual | 26, -76, -16 | -6.12 | 0.0000 | 4313 |
Posterior parietal | 36, -60, 42 | -5.80 | 0.0000 | 1255 |
Inferior parietal lobule/SMG | -48, -42, 38 | -5.63 | 0.0000 | 479 |
Precuneus | 6, -60, 44 | -5.09 | 0.0000 | 842 |
Lingual/V2 | -10, -50, -2 | -4.57 | 0.0000 | 492 |
MFG | -30, 16, 62 | -4.53 | 0.0000 | 122 |
STG | -52, -26, -2 | -4.46 | 0.0000 | 516 |
Occipital cortex | 10, -86, 42 | -3.89 | 0.0001 | 51 |
Cingulate (ant/post) | 2, -16, 34 | -3.86 | 0.0001 | 291 |
Lateral occipital (V4) | -46, -80, -18 | -3.86 | 0.0001 | 20 |
Superior Thalamus/fornix | 4, -16, 18 | -3.85 | 0.0001 | 46 |
SFG/SMA | 4, 20, 52 | -3.83 | 0.0001 | 76 |
Temporal fusiform | -20, -56, -12 | -3.82 | 0.0001 | 59 |
M1/premotor | 14, -26, 70 | -3.75 | 0.0002 | 46 |
Lingual/cerebellum | -10, -74, -20 | -3.73 | 0.0002 | 79 |
SFG/MFG | -20, 4, 72 | -3.66 | 0.0002 | 10 |
Inferior parietal | -34, -74, 32 | -3.62 | 0.0003 | 14 |
Temporal lobe | 42, -14, -28 | -3.60 | 0.0003 | 149 |
Inferior parietal/SMG | -50, -22, 26 | -3.59 | 0.0003 | 128 |
Superior parietal lobule | 32, -38, 40 | -3.54 | 0.0004 | 12 |
STG | -60, -12, -8 | -3.54 | 0.0004 | 56 |
MTG | -54, -52, 10 | -3.54 | 0.0004 | 44 |
MFG | 40, 12, 36 | -3.50 | 0.0005 | 51 |
Inferior parietal/SMG | 62, -30, 32 | -3.49 | 0.0005 | 56 |
Superior parietal/precuneus | 14, -42, 62 | -3.48 | 0.0005 | 30 |
Insula | 40, 12, -12 | -3.48 | 0.0005 | 34 |
Superior parietal | -12, -64, 60 | -3.47 | 0.0005 | 21 |
Superior parietal/precuneus | 0.0, -40, 56 | -3.45 | 0.0006 | 23 |
Superior Thalamus/fornix | 18, -30, 14 | -3.44 | 0.0006 | 41 |
Temporal pole | 38, 16, -22 | -3.43 | 0.0006 | 19 |
MTG | 46, -60, 0 | -3.43 | 0.0006 | 32 |
Occipital | -4, -84, 42 | -3.39 | 0.0007 | 16 |
Dentate gyrus | -24, -28, -4 | -3.34 | 0.0008 | 28 |
Cerebellum | 8, -36, -24 | -3.34 | 0.0008 | 15 |
Precuneus/Parietal | 22, -72, 28 | -3.32 | 0.0009 | 27 |
Precuneus | 12, -68, 26 | -3.17 | 0.0015 | 10 |
Hippocampus cornu ammonis | -22, -14, -12 | -3.17 | 0.0015 | 30 |
Cuneus/superior parietal | -16, -82, 32 | -3.16 | 0.0016 | 10 |
Cerebellum | -2, -64, -52 | 3.42 | 0.0006 | 14 |
Temporal fusiform | -28, -16, -44 | 3.41 | 0.0006 | 11 |
All peaks and clusters were determined using a voxel extent ≥10, minimum distance 10mm, and bootstrap ratio (BSR) ≥ 3.00. MNI, Montreal Neurological Institute (mm).
WM integrity predicts function-cognition relations independent of age
Next, we investigated whether the observed associations between memory and fluid performance and SDBOLD are related to the integrity of structural connections in the brain. To examine this hypothesis, we first obtained a summary measure of each participant’s expression of the significant LV pattern by calculating “brain scores”. This involved multiplying each voxel’s weights from the significant LV by the SDBOLD in that voxel for each person, and summing it over all brain voxels. Thus, in a single measure, a brain score indicates the degree to which a subject expresses the multivariate spatial pattern of performance–SDBOLD associations reported in the LV depicted in Fig. 1 (see Methods for more details on brains score calculation). Specifically, a person with a higher brain score showed better performance on memory and fluid abilities and greater SDBOLD in the voxels depicted in Fig. 1A.
Finally, we performed a multiple regression analysis with the brain scores as a dependent variable, age as the first independent variable and global FA (mean FA across the main WM tracts) as the second independent variable. Note that DTI data was available from only 66 out of 91. We included age in the model as both global FA (r = -.38 p = .002 n = 66) and brain scores (r = .21 p = .048, n = 91) were negatively related to age. In addition, memory was negatively related to age (see previous section). Therefore, it was important to test whether the SDBOLD–performance association is related to WM microstructure beyond the effects of chronological age. Indeed, we found that higher FA accounted for a significant amount of variance in brain scores, in addition to variance related to age (R2 Δ age = 0.041, F c Δ age = 2.77, df = 64/1, p-value = .101; R2 Δ globalFA = 0.12. F Δ globalFA = 8.7, df = 63/1, p-value = .004). We also note that global FA was not related to perceptual speed, memory and vocabulary components (p >. 50) and was related to fluid abilities only at a trend level (r = .23 p = .068, n = 66). Together, our results suggest that global WM integrity is associated with behaviorally relevant variability in the BOLD signal, beyond the effects of age.
We run an additional PLS model including age, four behavioral scores, and global FA (n = 66). It yielded one LV (p = .005, cross block covariance explained of 63%), where greater FA and younger age was related to greater SDBOLD. Global FA contributed most to the relationship (r>0.4), and age to a lesser degree (r>0.2). Greater fluid intelligence and memory were also related to greater SD BOLD, but their contribution to the model was not significant (while vocabulary and processing speed showed a negative non-significant association). This result confirms that WM integrity is related to SDBOLD, that brain structure-function relationship may be stronger than brain-performance associations, and this issue should be further investigated (see Discussion). We highlight, however, that the purpose of this article was to investigate the structural WM correlates of behaviorally relevant SDBOLD only.
Discussion
We investigated the associations between resting SDBOLD and performance on four distinct cognitive constructs in healthy older adults with a whole-brain, multivariate approach. We demonstrated that 1) better fluid abilities and memory was linked to greater SDBOLD in multiple regions including precuneus, insula, temporal, parietal, and prefrontal regions, and cingulate, and 2) behaviorally relevant SDBOLD pattern was shared by fluid abilities and memory. Moreover, inter-individual differences in these SDBOLD-cognition relationships were related to the global WM integrity, above and beyond the effects of chronological age.
Association of SDBOLD with performance differs by cognitive domain
A previous study reported that greater SDBOLD in healthy adults was associated with younger age, faster, and more consistent response times (RT) across three levels of a perceptual match-to-sample task (immediate comparison, cued short-delay comparison, and delayed comparison; [14]). Our results provide further evidence for greater SDBOLD being related to better performance in aging. Specifically, we showed that the cognitive constructs requiring adaptive and flexible processing–fluid abilities and memory–were driving this positive SDBOLD–performance association. For example, tasks defining the fluid abilities require abstract reasoning and problem solving that enable optimal adaptation to a changing and complex environment [49]. Similarly, episodic memory involves association formation and binding, as well as flexible and context-dependent retrieval. As a result, both fluid abilities and memory should benefit from greater dynamic range and the ability to explore different network states at the neuronal level [4,12,50].
On the contrary, the vocabulary construct representing semantic knowledge requires robust retrieval of information from long-term memory that was acquired, stored, and reinforced over years. Thus, vocabulary knowledge operates on “hard-wired”, automatic and repetitive responses and therefore may benefit from less SDBOLD at the neural level. As an additional behavioral PLS analysis with only vocabulary construct did not yield a significant LV, this result relating lower SDBOLD to better vocabulary performance should be treated as preliminary and further investigated with more cognitive tasks defining this domain.
The dissociation of SDBOLD–performance relationship between the cognitive domains parallels their differential sensitivity to age. Namely, advanced age is related to decline in fluid abilities, memory and speed, with relative sparing of vocabulary knowledge [51,52]. The regions where we observed an association of SDBOLD with fluid abilities and memory (visual cortex, temporal pole, insula, cingulate, parietal cortex, lateral frontal regions) overlap with regions showing decreased SDBOLD in older compared to younger adults [13]. Therefore, we speculate that SDBOLD might be one of the neural correlates underlying the discrepancy of age-related effects on the four main cognitive domains. Further exploration of this claim should be done by extending analyses to samples with broader age range.
Behaviorally relevant SD BOLD may support integration of brain networks
Many regions where we observed a positive association of SDBOLD with fluid abilities and memory have been defined as degree-based hubs, “rich club” regions, or connector hubs in structural and functional network analyses: posterior cingulate cortex, superior frontal, parietal and insular cortex, as well as inferior temporal and fusiform cortex [22,53–55]. Brain “hubs” are regions with high connectivity degree in a given neural community [23,55,56], while “rich-club” regions are the high-degree hubs that tend to connect to each other [57]. Of particular relevance to our findings are the connector hubs: regions highly connected primarily to distinct brain networks [58–60]. Such connector hubs are localized to the insula, parietal, premotor, lateral occipital, and dorsal superior frontal cortex [60], where we also observed higher SDBOLD in better performing older adults. Connector hubs integrate functionally segregated domains with possibly very distinct processing or oscillatory properties. We therefore suggest that the hub’s high connectivity with multiple brain functional networks requires or results in the higher moment-to-moment variability in neural function, which should be reflected by greater SDBOLD. Importantly, we predict that such SDBOLD related to a region’s cross-talk between different neural networks should be driven by high variability in signal frequency and not only by variability as a result of high amplitude signal with a constant frequency. Clearly, our results need to be followed by a direct comparison of SDBOLD patterns with functional connectivity network properties, time-frequency analyses to tease apart time-constant SDBOLD from time-varying SDBOLD, as well as changes in SDBOLD and power-law exponents in fMRI signal between rest and task states [61,62], and their significance for cognitive performance in aging.
Despite careful removal of physiological noise with ICA, we acknowledge that some of SDBOLD regions, such as posterior cingulate, occipital cortex and regions near large vessels such as temporal pole and regions along the brain midline, may partly overlap with respiratory or cardiac-related fluctuations [63,64]. High static cerebral blood flow (CBF) and high amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in CBF at rest in regions such as posterior cingulate cortex and insula, however, suggest that spontaneous fluctuations of fMRI signal in these regions are neuronally-driven rather than of vasomotor origin [65].
Finally, we note that our analysis yielded one model for memory and fluid abilities instead of two LVs specific for each cognitive construct. This further supports the possibility that the hub-related pattern of greater SDBOLD represents a common rather than a domain-specific neural feature. In other words, our findings suggest that preserving high SDBOLD in regions associated with intra- and inter-network communication is linked with better performance on a set of cognitive tasks requiring flexible neural processing. We speculate that hub regions that show greater SDBOLD during spontaneous brain activity at rest would also have the capacity for increased neural processing complexity during cognitive tasks (e.g. memory and reasoning) [5,62]. Longitudinal designs and broader age ranges should help to tease apart age-related from individual differences in SDBOLD.
White matter as a scaffold for behaviorally relevant SDBOLD
Our study provided the first evidence for an association between whole-brain behaviorally relevant variability in the BOLD signal and WM integrity. Therefore, our study further extends previous reports on a positive relationship between WM integrity and task-related changes in BOLD signal [27,66], and structure-function brain network properties [54,67,68]. We propose that poor WM integrity, most likely due to age-related changes in myelination, precludes fast and reliable signal transduction. Consequently, optimal interaction between brain hubs within or between brain networks becomes impaired [69,70]. For instance, some signals may be “lost” in between the GM regions, others may not arrive in a timely fashion to be optimally integrated in the neural processes [27,71], or the resting kinetic energy of the system may not be sufficient to adjust to externally driven cognitive challenges [5,62]. This may result in reduced processing complexity that could be detected as reduced SDBOLD at rest and during task, for instance, during the creation of mnemonic representations or updating information during mental rotation.
Our result that older adults with greater FA in all major WM tracts had greater SDBOLD and better performance on memory and fluid abilities converges with previous reports on relationships between diffusivity properties and fluid intelligence defined by reasoning abilities, cognitive flexibility, episodic memory, and processing speed in older adults [25,72–75]; for a review see also [76]. Although we observed only a trend relationship between WM integrity and fluid abilities, this lack of strong diffusion-cognition association may be because our participants represented a relatively narrow age range and being relatively high functioning, healthy older adults (all qualified for the MRI, aerobic capacity test and an exercise intervention), which may limit the variability in the FA and behavioral measures.
Together, our data suggests that magnitude and spatial pattern of SDBOLD that is linked to high cognitive performance–and therefore represents optimal complexity of neural processing–relies on the integrity of structural brain connectivity via WM in the healthy aging brain. Our findings lay foundation for future investigations addressing more specific questions about structural correlates of SDBOLD. One direction will be to define the regional (both GM and WM) specificity of WM- SDBOLD associations in aging and across lifespan. Another important issue that needs to be addressed is the role of cortical atrophy and the related partial volume effect in estimating SDBOLD in aging population, and the possible mediating role of GM volume on the SDBOLD–cognition associations.
Conclusions
We found that greater SDBOLD in multiple brain regions, most of which have been identified as inter- or intra-network connecting hubs, was linked to better fluid abilities and memory. This suggests that optimal dynamic range of neural processing in hub regions may support cognitive operations that specifically rely on moment-to-moment processing adaptability and flexibility. Moreover, we showed that this behaviorally relevant SDBOLD is supported by global WM integrity. We conclude that SDBOLD is a promising functional neural correlate of individual differences in cognition in healthy older adults.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Holly Tracy and Nancy Dodge for MRI data collection, and Anya Knecht and Susan H. Herrel for project coordination.
Data Availability
The FA values, behavioral scores, and brain scores are available in Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) grant (R37-AG025667) and a grant from Abbott Nutrition through the Center of Nutrition, Learning and Memory at the University of Illinois. AZB was supported by the “Changing Viewpoints” grant from the Robert Bosch Foundation.
References
- 1. Rajah MN, D’Esposito M (2005) Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. Brain 128: 1964–1983. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16049041. Accessed 2010 Jul 27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Hafkemeijer A, van der Grond J, Rombouts SARB (2012) Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta 1822: 431–441. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21807094. Accessed 2014 Jul 26. 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Madden DJ, Bennett IJ, Burzynska A, Potter GG, Chen N-K, Song AW (2012) Diffusion tensor imaging of cerebral white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Biochim Biophys Acta 1822: 386–400. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21871957. 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Grady CL, Garrett DD (2014) Understanding variability in the BOLD signal and why it matters for aging. Brain Imaging Behav 8: 274–283. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24008589. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. 10.1007/s11682-013-9253-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL (2013) The modulation of BOLD variability between cognitive states varies by age and processing speed. Cereb Cortex 23: 684–693. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3823571&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract . Accessed 2014 Jul 14. 10.1093/cercor/bhs055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Deco G, Jirsa V, McIntosh AR, Sporns O, Kötter R (2009) Key role of coupling, delay, and noise in resting brain fluctuations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 10302–10307. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2690605&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 29. 10.1073/pnas.0901831106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR (2011) Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 43–56. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21170073. Accessed 2014 Jul 11. 10.1038/nrn2961 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Ghosh A, Rho Y, McIntosh AR, Kötter R, Jirsa VK (2008) Noise during rest enables the exploration of the brain’s dynamic repertoire. PLoS Comput Biol 4: e1000196 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2551736&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 16. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL (2001) A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98: 676–682. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 14647&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Raichle ME, Snyder AZ (2007) A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage 37: 1083–90; discussion 1097–9. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17719799. Accessed 2014 Jul 11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Raichle ME (2010) Two views of brain function. Trends Cogn Sci 14: 180–190. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20206576. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Faisal AA, Selen LPJ, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 292–303. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2631351&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. 10.1038/nrn2258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL (2010) Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. J Neurosci 30: 4914–4921. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20371811. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5166-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL (2011) The importance of being variable. J Neurosci 31: 4496–4503. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3104038&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5641-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Craik F, Salthouse T (2000) Handbook of Aging and Cognition. 2nd ed Hillsdale, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Birren J, Schaie K, editors (2006) Handbook of the psychology of aging. 6th ed San Diego, CA. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Salthouse TA (2004) Localizing age-related individual differences in a hierarchical structure. Intelligence 32. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3866028&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 18. Salthouse TA (2005) Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology 19: 532–545. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16060828. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Salthouse TA (2010) Influence of age on practice effects in longitudinal neurocognitive change. Neuropsychology 24: 563–572. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2933088&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. 10.1037/a0019026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Salthouse TA, Ferrer-Caja E (2003) What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychol Aging 18: 91–110. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12641315. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Kannurpatti SS, Rypma B, Biswal BB (2012) Prediction of Task-Related BOLD fMRI with Amplitude Signatures of Resting-State fMRI. Front Syst Neurosci 6: 7 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3294272&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Nov 3. 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O (2011) Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci 31: 15775–15786. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22049421. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, Wedeen VJ, et al. (2008) Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biol 6: e159 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2443193&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2010 Aug 9. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. McIntosh AR, Bookstein FL, Haxby J V, Grady CL (1996) Spatial pattern analysis of functional brain images using partial least squares. Neuroimage 3: 143–157. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9345485. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Haász J, Westlye ET, Fjær S, Espeseth T, Lundervold A, Lundervold AJ (2013) General fluid-type intelligence is related to indices of white matter structure in middle-aged and old adults. Neuroimage 83: 372–383. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23791837. Accessed 2014 Jun 23. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Burzynska AZ, Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Gluth S, Bäckman L, Li S-C, et al. (2011) Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging. Hum Brain Mapp. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21739526. Accessed 2011 Jul 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Burzynska AZ, Garrett DD, Preuschhof C, Nagel IE, Li S-C, Bäckman L, et al. (2013) A scaffold for efficiency in the human brain. J Neurosci 33: 17150–17159. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24155318. Accessed 2014 Jul 15. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1426-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Jezzard P, Balaban RS (1995) Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations. Magn Reson Med 34: 65–73. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7674900. Accessed 2014 Jul 17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Reese TG, Heid O, Weisskoff RM, Wedeen VJ (2003) Reduction of eddy-current-induced distortion in diffusion MRI using a twice-refocused spin echo. Magn Reson Med 49: 177–182. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12509835. Accessed 2014 Jul 17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1: S208–19. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15501092. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002) Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17: 825–841. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12377157. Accessed 2014 Jul 17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Smith SM (2002) Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 17: 143–155. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12391568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Jenkinson M (2003) Fast, automated, N-dimensional phase-unwrapping algorithm. Magn Reson Med 49: 193–197. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12509838. Accessed 2014 Jul 9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Greve DN, Fischl B (2009) Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48: 63–72. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2733527&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2010 Jul 16. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Beckmann CF, Smith SM (2004) Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 23: 137–152. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14964560. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Kelly RE, Alexopoulos GS, Wang Z, Gunning FM, Murphy CF, Morimoto SS, et al. (2010) Visual inspection of independent components: defining a procedure for artifact removal from fMRI data. J Neurosci Methods 189: 233–245. Available: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165027010001627. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.03.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Hallquist MN, Hwang K, Luna B (2013) The nuisance of nuisance regression: spectral misspecification in a common approach to resting-state fMRI preprocessing reintroduces noise and obscures functional connectivity. Neuroimage 82: 208–225. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23747457. Accessed 2014 Jul 17. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Birn RM, Diamond JB, Smith MA, Bandettini PA (2006) Separating respiratory-variation-related fluctuations from neuronal-activity-related fluctuations in fMRI. Neuroimage 31: 1536–1548. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16632379. Accessed 2014 Jul 11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. De Munck JC, Gonçalves SI, Faes TJC, Kuijer JPA, Pouwels PJW, Heethaar RM, et al. (2008) A study of the brain’s resting state based on alpha band power, heart rate and fMRI. Neuroimage 42: 112–121. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18539049. Accessed 2014 Jul 17. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Chang C, Glover GH (2009) Effects of model-based physiological noise correction on default mode network anti-correlations and correlations. Neuroimage 47: 1448–1459. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2995588&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Weissenbacher A, Kasess C, Gerstl F, Lanzenberger R, Moser E, Windischberger C (2009) Correlations and anticorrelations in resting-state functional connectivity MRI: a quantitative comparison of preprocessing strategies. Neuroimage 47: 1408–1416. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19442749. Accessed 2014 Jul 15. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Krishnan A, Williams LJ, McIntosh AR, Abdi H (2011) Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. Neuroimage 56: 455–475. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20656037. Accessed 2012 Oct 4. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. McIntosh AR, Lobaugh NJ (2004) Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances. Neuroimage 23 Suppl 1: S250–63. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15501095. Accessed 2012 Oct 4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Efron B, Tibshirani R (1993) An introduction to the bootstrap. London: Chapman and Hall. [Google Scholar]
- 45. Beaulieu C (2002) The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system—a technical review. NMR Biomed 15: 435–455. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12489094. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Mackay CE, et al. (2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31: 1487–1505. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16624579. Accessed 2014 Jul 12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H, Jenkinson M, Rueckert D, Nichols TE, Miller KL, et al. (2007) Acquisition and voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data with tract-based spatial statistics. Nat Protoc 2: 499–503. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17406613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Rueckert D, Sonoda LI, Hayes C, Hill DL, Leach MO, Hawkes DJ (1999) Nonrigid registration using free-form deformations: application to breast MR images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 18: 712–721. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10534053. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Cattell RB (1963) Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. J Educ Psychol: 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Lippe S, Garrett D, Grady C, Jirsa V (2010) The development of a noisy brain. Arch Ital Biol 148: 323–337. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21175017. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Cavanaugh J, Blanchard-Fields F (2006) Adult development and aging. 5th ed Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing/Thomson Learning. [Google Scholar]
- 52. Li S-C, Lindenberger U, Hommel B, Aschersleben G, Prinz W, Baltes PB (2004) Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychol Sci 15: 155–163. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15016286. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, Hedden T, et al. (2009) Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 29: 1860–1873. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2750039&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 16. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Grayson DS, Ray S, Carpenter S, Iyer S, Dias TGC, Stevens T, et al. (2014) Structural and functional rich club organization of the brain in children and adults. PLoS One 9: e88297 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3915050&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. 10.1371/journal.pone.0088297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Van den Heuvel MP, Hulshoff Pol HE (2010) Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 20: 519–534. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20471808. Accessed 2014 Jul 12. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Sporns O, Honey CJ, Kötter R (2007) Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS One 2: e1049 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2013941&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Colizza V, Flammini A, Serrano MA, Vespignani A (2006) Detecting rich-club ordering in complex networks. Nat Phys 2: 110–115. Available: 10.1038/nphys209. Accessed 2014 Jul 18. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Guimerà R, Mossa S, Turtschi A, Amaral LAN (2005) The worldwide air transportation network: Anomalous centrality, community structure, and cities’ global roles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 7794–7799. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 1142352&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Joyce KE, Laurienti PJ, Burdette JH, Hayasaka S (2010) A new measure of centrality for brain networks. PLoS One 5: e12200 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2922375&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 19. 10.1371/journal.pone.0012200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, Petersen SE (2013) Evidence for hubs in human functional brain networks. Neuron 79: 798–813. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23972601. Accessed 2014 Jul 15. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. He BJ (2011) Scale-free properties of the functional magnetic resonance imaging signal during rest and task. J Neurosci 31: 13786–13795. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3197021&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Sep 7. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2111-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Garrett DD, McIntosh AR, Grady CL (2013) Brain Signal Variability is Parametrically Modifiable. Cereb Cortex. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23749875. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Birn RM (2012) The role of physiological noise in resting-state functional connectivity. Neuroimage 62: 864–870. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22245341. Accessed 2014 Jul 10. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Murphy K, Birn RM, Bandettini PA (2013) Resting-state fMRI confounds and cleanup. Neuroimage 80: 349–359. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23571418. Accessed 2014 Jul 15. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Zou Q, Wu CW, Stein EA, Zang Y, Yang Y (2009) Static and dynamic characteristics of cerebral blood flow during the resting state. Neuroimage 48: 515–524. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2739419&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Oct 31. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Zhu Z, Johnson NF, Kim C, Gold BT (2013) Reduced Frontal Cortex Efficiency is Associated with Lower White Matter Integrity in Aging. Cereb Cortex: bht212–. Available: http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/content/early/2013/08/17/cercor.bht212.short. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. 10.1093/cercor/bht212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran JP, Meuli R, et al. (2009) Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106: 2035–2040. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 2634800&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 13. 10.1073/pnas.0811168106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O (2013) An anatomical substrate for integration among functional networks in human cortex. J Neurosci 33: 14489–14500. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24005300. Accessed 2014 Jul 9. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Burzynska AZ, Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Li S-C, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L, et al. (2011) Microstructure of Frontoparietal Connections Predicts Cortical Responsivity and Working Memory Performance. Cereb Cortex. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21350048. Accessed 2011 Aug 22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Bartzokis G (2004) Age-related myelin breakdown: a developmental model of cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 25: 5–18; author reply 49–62. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14675724. Accessed 2010 Oct 2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Charlton RA, Barrick TR, Markus HS, Morris RG (2013) Verbal working and long-term episodic memory associations with white matter microstructure in normal aging investigated using tract-based spatial statistics. Psychol Aging 28: 768–777. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23957226. Accessed 2014 Jul 27. 10.1037/a0032668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Borghesani PR, Madhyastha TM, Aylward EH, Reiter MA, Swarny BR, Schaie KW, et al. (2013) The association between higher order abilities, processing speed, and age are variably mediated by white matter integrity during typical aging. Neuropsychologia 51: 1435–1444. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23507612. Accessed 2014 Jun 24. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Charlton RA, Barrick TR, Markus HS, Morris RG (2010) The relationship between episodic long-term memory and white matter integrity in normal aging. Neuropsychologia 48: 114–122. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19699215. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Lockhart SN, Mayda AB V, Roach AE, Fletcher E, Carmichael O, Maillard P, et al. (2012) Episodic memory function is associated with multiple measures of white matter integrity in cognitive aging. Front Hum Neurosci 6: 56 Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3305887&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 26. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Metzler-Baddeley C, Jones DK, Belaroussi B, Aggleton JP, O’Sullivan MJ (2011) Frontotemporal connections in episodic memory and aging: a diffusion MRI tractography study. J Neurosci 31: 13236–13245. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21917806. Accessed 2014 Jul 23. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2317-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Salthouse TA (2011) Neuroanatomical substrates of age-related cognitive decline. Psychol Bull 137: 753–784. Available: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid = 3132227&tool = pmcentrez&rendertype = abstract. Accessed 2014 Jul 14. 10.1037/a0023262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Raven J (1962) Advanced progressive matrices, sets I and II. London: H.K. Lewis; [Google Scholar]
- 78. Zachary RA (1986) Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- 79. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Dermen D (1976) Manual for Kit of Factor-referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. [Google Scholar]
- 80. Bennett GK, Seashore HG, Wesman AG (1997) Differential Aptitude Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- 81. Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Third. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- 82. Salthouse TA, Babcock RL (1991) Decomposing Adult Age Differences in Working Memory. Dev Psychol 27: 763–776. Available: http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid = 2-s2.0-58149206091&partnerID = tZOtx3y1. [Google Scholar]
- 83. Wechsler D. (1997) Wechsler Memory Scale Third. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- 84. Salthouse TA, Fristoe N, Rhee SH (1996) How localized are age-related effects on neuropsychological measures? Neuropsychology 10: 272–285. Available: http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid = 2-s2.0-0029968304&partnerID = tZOtx3y1. Accessed 2014 Jul 30. [Google Scholar]
- 85. Woodcock RW, Johnson MB (1990) Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Allen, TX: DLM. [Google Scholar]
- 86. Salthouse TA (1993) Speed and knowledge as determinants of adult age differences in verbal tasks. Journals Gerontol 48 Available: http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid = 2-s2.0-0027389663&partnerID = tZOtx3y1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The FA values, behavioral scores, and brain scores are available in Supporting Information files.