Abstract
Background
Integration of afferent somatosensory input with motor-cortex output is essential for accurate movements. Prior studies have shown that tactile input modulates motor-cortex excitability, which is reflected in the reactivity of the ∽20-Hz motor-cortex rhythm. ∽20-Hz rebound is connected to inhibition or deactivation of motor cortex whereas suppression has been associated with increased motor cortex activity. Although tactile sense carries important information for controlling voluntary actions, proprioception likely provides the most essential feedback for motor control.
Methods
To clarify how passive movement modulates motor-cortex excitability, we studied with magnetoencephalography (MEG) the amplitudes and peak latencies of suppression and rebound of the ∽20-Hz rhythm elicited by tactile stimulation and passive movement of right and left index fingers in 22 healthy volunteers.
Results
Passive movement elicited a stronger and more robust ∽20-Hz rebound than tactile stimulation. In contrast, the suppression amplitudes did not differ between the two stimulus types.
Conclusion
Our findings suggest that suppression and rebound represent activity of two functionally distinct neuronal populations. The ∽20-Hz rebound to passive movement could be a suitable tool to study the functional state of the motor cortex both in healthy subjects and in patients with motor disorders.
Keywords: Beta rebound, beta rhythm, magnetoencephalography, motor-cortex excitability, proprioception, sensorimotor integration
Introduction
Tactile and proprioceptive input coordinates and recalibrates motor cortex activity by regulating its excitability (Cassim et al. 2000, 2001; Gaetz and Cheyne 2006). Several studies have shown that ∽20-Hz oscillatory brain activity is initially decreased (suppression; event-related desynchronization, ERD) and subsequently increased (rebound; event-related synchronization, ERS) in response to tactile stimulation (Pfurtscheller 1981; Salmelin and Hari 1994; Hari et al. 1997; Salenius et al. 1997; Neuper and Pfurtscheller 2001). The suppression is suggested to reflect an activated state of the motor cortex, whereas the rebound has been associated to a deactivated or inhibited state of the motor cortex (Pfurtscheller et al. 1996, 1997; Cassim et al. 2000; Neuper and Pfurtscheller 2001; Takemi et al. 2013).
In studies with cats and monkeys, the primary motor cortex (MI) has been shown to receive proprioceptive input via direct thalamocortical connections (Asanuma et al. 1979; Friedman and Jones 1981), via primary somatosensory cortex (SI; areas 3a and 2) and via secondary somatosensory cortex (SII; Jones and Wise 1977; Jones et al. 1978; Jones 1983). Single-cell recordings in humans have demonstrated that proprioceptive input activates MI more than tactile input (Goldring and Ratcheson 1972). As proprioception specifically signals the internal state of the locomotor system, it is likely that it modulates motor-cortex excitability more than tactile input. Prior EEG studies have shown modulation of the ∽20-Hz motor cortex rhythm to passive movement (Cassim et al. 2001; Alegre et al. 2002) but there are no studies comparing the effects of tactile versus proprioceptive stimuli on motor-cortex excitability.
New rehabilitation approaches, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and pharmacological manipulation with antidepressive medication, aim at enhancing plasticity after stroke by altering the excitatory–inhibitory balance of the two hemispheres (Nibuya et al. 1995; Duman et al. 2000; Maya Vetencourt et al. 2008; Chollet et al. 2011; Sung et al. 2013). Despite some promising results, it is still difficult to estimate which patients will benefit from these interventions, in which time window they should be used and what effect these interventions have on the excitability of the two hemispheres. The modulation of the ∽20-Hz motor-cortex rhythm by somatosensory stimulation could be a tool to monitor alterations in motor-cortex excitability for example after stroke and guide the selection of rehabilitation methods.
We examined the ∽20-Hz oscillatory brain activity during tactile stimulation and passive movement of the index fingers in 22 healthy subjects with whole-scalp magnetoencephalography (MEG). The aim was to clarify how these two different somatosensory stimulus types affect motor cortex excitability.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
We studied 22 healthy volunteers; 11 males and 11 females, age 42–70 years, mean 59 ± 12 years, all right-handed and with no history of neurological disorders. The local Ethics Committee approved the study protocol and all subjects gave written informed consent prior to the measurements.
Stimulation
Tactile stimuli (duration 140 ms, peak at 50 ms) were delivered using pneumatic diaphragms driven by compressed air to the tips of both index fingers alternately with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1.5 sec, resulting in an ISI of 3 sec for one side.
For passive movements, an experienced nurse extended briskly the subject's index finger and lowered it back to the initial position, with an ISI of about 3 sec. Right and left index fingers were stimulated separately. To produce as pure proprioceptive stimulation as possible we minimized cutaneous tactile stimulation during the finger lift by covering the middle phalanx with a surgical tape, to which a rigid aluminum stick was attached with a Velcro strap (Fig.1A). This assured that the possible tactile stimulation was as constant as possible. Furthermore, the tip of the finger was not allowed to touch the device. A 3-axis accelerometer (ADXL335 iMEMS accelerometer, Analog Devices Inc., Norwood, MA) was attached on the nail of the index finger and its signals, acquired with the MEG system, were used to determine finger kinematics and the onset of movement (Fig.1).
Two vertically placed optical gates with horizontally placed optical sensors were used to identify successful passive movements. The lower gate was located just above the finger at resting position and the upper gate was located 30 mm higher. The finger was moved through the optical gates, and the passive movement was accepted only if the finger passed through both gates with the lower gate preceding the upper one within 500 ms (Fig.1A). As the movement had already started before the index finger reached the lower gate, the actual onset of the movement was calculated off-line from the accelerometer signals in 17 subjects. In five subjects, the accelerometer signal was not available for technical reasons. These subjects were excluded from the analysis where the information of the exact onset of passive movement was needed (grand average of time-frequency representations; see Data analysis) but retained for other analyzes and statistical tests between the stimulus conditions. For these analyses, the onset of the movement of the five subjects was estimated from the average onset in the other subjects (N = 17).
The beginning and end of passive movement were determined from the Euclidean norm of the three orthogonal accelerometer channels (i.e., acceleration magnitude), averaged across all accepted passive movements for each subject and each hand separately. The beginning of the movement was set to the transient onset of linear increase in the mean acceleration magnitude signal for each subject separately (Fig.1). The end of the movement was similarly determined as the transient stop of linear decline in the mean acceleration magnitude signal.
MEG recordings
We employed a 306-channel whole-scalp MEG system (Vectorview™, Elekta Oy, Helsinki, Finland). The helmet-shaped sensor array comprises 102 triple-sensor elements, each housing a magnetometer and two orthogonal planar gradiometers. The data of 18 subjects were recorded in Aalto University (Aalto NeuroImaging, MEG-core) and four with identical measurement and stimulus devices in the BioMag Laboratory (Helsinki University Central Hospital, Helsinki, Finland). During the measurements, the subjects were comfortably seated in a magnetically shielded room in an upright position with the scalp covered by the sensor array. The subjects were instructed to keep their head and posture still, try to avoid excessive blinking, to be relaxed and not to pay attention to the stimuli. Earplugs were used to avoid responses to possible stimulus-related acoustic noise. The nurse was present inside the magnetically shielded room to perform the passive movement of index finger and to observe and guide the subject. The subject's head position with respect to the MEG sensors was determined with the help of four indicator coils (attached to the forehead and mastoids). Prior to the MEG data collection, the locations of the coils and three anatomical landmarks (right and left preauricular points and nasion) as well as 50–100 additional points on the head surface were determined with a 3D digitizer. The head position was measured in each session. Electro-oculogram was utilized to record vertical eye movements. The MEG signals were band-pass filtered to 0.03–330 Hz and digitized at 1000 Hz. Online averaging was performed to monitor the number of accepted trials and continuous data were simultaneously collected for subsequent analysis.
In all recording sessions, tactile stimulation of the right and left index fingers were performed first, followed by a 3-min recording of isometric contraction of both right and left extensor carpi radialis for estimating cortex-muscle coherence (the results are reported elsewhere). Thereafter, continuous resting state data were recorded with eyes closed/eyes open for 3 min each. Finally, cortical responses to passive movement of both index fingers were recorded. About 60 accepted trials for each hand were collected for both tactile and passive-movement sessions.
We performed control measurements in four subjects with two different ISIs (1.5 sec and 3 sec) and two durations (140 msec and 1130 msec) of tactile stimuli to test the effects of latency and stimulus duration on the ∽20-Hz rebound.
Data analysis
To suppress environmental interference, the raw data were first processed with the temporal signal-space separation method (tSSS; Taulu and Simola 2006). Head movement compensation method (Taulu and Kajola 2005; Nenonen et al. 2012) implemented in MaxFilter software (version 2.2.11; Elekta Oy, Helsinki, Finland) was also used.
Amplitude spectra were estimated from the resting state data (eyes open). Half-overlapping 2048-sample Hanning-windowed segments of the continuous data were Fourier-transformed and the magnitudes averaged. For each subject, the MEG channel showing the strongest spectral peak at around 20 Hz over the rolandic region was chosen in both left and right hemispheres. Time–frequency representations (TFR) of both tactile and passive-movement responses in the frequency range of 3–40 Hz were calculated for 17 subjects using 7-cycle Morlet wavelets to determine the frequency band of strongest modulation in the ∽20-Hz range.
Before analyzing tactile stimulus-induced modulations of rhythmic activity, the averaged somatosensory evoked responses were subtracted from each trial of the continuous data. Temporal spectral evolution method (TSE; Salmelin and Hari 1994) was used to quantify the modulation of rhythmic activity; the continuous data were filtered to the frequency band showing the strongest modulation (15–25 Hz in all subjects; determined from the TFR), rectified and averaged time-locked to the stimulus onset. The analysis period was –200–1500 ms for tactile stimulation and –200–2500 ms for passive movement. The 200-ms prestimulus time was used to determine the baseline level for rhythmic activity.
The peak amplitudes of suppression and rebound over the rolandic area were quantified from one channel showing the strongest suppression and another channel showing the strongest rebound of the 15–25-Hz activity both in the ipsi- and contralateral hemisphere to the stimulated hand. The peak latency was determined as the time point where the suppression/rebound was strongest. Thereafter, the relative peak amplitudes were calculated as changes of amplitudes with respect to the individual prestimulus baselines (−200–0 ms) and defined as % values. The relative peak amplitudes and peak latencies of suppressions and rebounds were compared with paired two-tailed t-test.
Duration (mean) and acceleration magnitude (mean and peak) were computed to describe kinematics of the passive-movement stimuli. The duration was defined as the time between the beginning and end of passive movement determined from the mean magnitude of the index finger acceleration (see Stimulation). The kinematics between right and left index fingers were compared with paired two-tailed t-test.
Source modeling
Temporal spectral evolution was computed also in the source space in one subject (S12) whose head magnetic resonance image (MRI) was available to localize the sources of the strongest rebound. FreeSurfer software (Fischl et al. 1999) was utilized to segment the cranial volume (for a single-compartment boundary element model) and the cortical mantle from a structural 3D head MRI of the subject. Thereafter, cortically constrained L2 minimum norm estimate was computed (“MNE Software”; Gramfort et al. 2014; nlx_151346). Noise covariance was estimated from a 2-min recording without a subject, filtered to 15–25 Hz. MatLab (Mathworks, Natick, MA) functions were written for the TSE calculations in source space.
Results
Kinematics of passive movements
Figure1B shows the MEG signal, the acceleration magnitude signal, and the optical gate signal in one subject during a single passive movement of the right index finger. The modulation of the rolandic ∽20-Hz MEG rhythm is discernible.
Kinematics of the passive movement were comparable between the right and left index fingers; no differences were found in the mean (right: 0.19 ± 0.04 g and left: 0.20 ± 0.03 g, N = 17; p = 0.74) and peak (right: 0.40 ± 0.10 g and left: 0.40 ± 0.06 g, N = 17; p = 0.96) acceleration magnitudes or in the mean movement duration (right: 1097 ± 11 ms and left: 1108 ± 10 ms, N = 17; p = 0.30).
Spontaneous brain activity
At rest with eyes open, amplitude spectra around 20 Hz showed typically 2–4 peaks over the rolandic region at 15–28 Hz in the left and 15–25 Hz in the right hemisphere.
Modulation of the ∽20-Hz rhythm
In the TFR analysis, the maximum rebound of the ∽20-Hz rhythm to tactile stimulation and passive movement of the index finger were observed over the same channels as the strongest ∽20-Hz peaks in the amplitude spectra. Figure2 shows the grand average TFR (N = 17). The maximum modulation (suppression and rebound) of ∽20-Hz activity is observed in the 15–25-Hz range in all subjects for both types of stimuli. Therefore, this frequency range was chosen to quantify the modulation of ∽20-Hz activity. Due to the short ISI of 1.5 sec between tactile stimuli of left and right index fingers the subsequent ipsilateral responses are visible (see also Fig.3A).
All subjects showed modulation of ∽20-Hz activity in response to both tactile stimulation and passive movement of the index fingers. Figure3A illustrates ∽20-Hz modulation in one subject (S12). Modulation of the rhythm was observed bilaterally to unilateral stimulation but it was stronger in the hemisphere contralateral to the stimulated hand. Figure3B illustrates source locations of the maximum rebound in one subject. The strongest source is located in the anterior part of the contralateral central sulcus.
Maximum rebound and suppression amplitudes were not observed over the same channel in most subjects. The maximum rebound was found in a more anterior channel than the maximum suppression in 20/22 subjects for tactile stimulation and in 19/22 for passive movement, suggesting different generator areas for these two components.
Figure4 shows the grand average of the ∽20-Hz rhythm TSE analysis of 22 subjects. Passive movement elicited a stronger rebound than tactile stimulus. The rebound peak amplitude was on average 35% stronger to passive movement than to tactile stimulation in contralateral hemispheres to right and left index finger stimulation: 95 ± 12% versus 60 ± 8%, (*p < 0.02) to right index finger passive movement and tactile stimulation, respectively, and 89 ± 14% versus 55 ± 6% (**p < 0.01) to left-sided stimulation.
In contrast, the strength of the suppression did not differ between these two stimulus types: 34 ± 2% versus 28 ± 2%; (p = 0.06) to right index finger passive movement and tactile stimulation, respectively, and 35 ± 2% versus 30 ± 3% (p = 0.13) to left-sided stimulation. The mean peak amplitudes and latencies of the responses to both types of stimuli are listed in Table 1.
Table 1.
Right-hand stimulation | Left-hand stimulation | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Passive | Tactile | Passive | Tactile | Passive | Tactile | Passive | Tactile | |
LH | LH | RH | RH | RH | RH | LH | LH | |
Rebound | ||||||||
Relative amplitude (%) | 95 ± 12 | 60 ± 8 | 48 ± 6 | 27 ± 4 | 89 ± 14 | 55 ± 6 | 53 ± 8 | 28 ± 3 |
Peak latency (ms) | 1420 ± 70 | 800 ± 50 | 1420 ± 50 | 830 ± 50 | 1440 ± 50 | 780 ± 40 | 1430 ± 70 | 730 ± 50 |
Suppression | ||||||||
Relative amplitude (%) | 34 ± 2 | 28 ± 2 | 31 ± 2 | 27 ± 2 | 35 ± 2 | 30 ± 3 | 32 ± 2 | 26 ± 2 |
Peak latency (ms) | 520 ± 20 | 300 ± 20 | 590 ± 40 | 360 ± 20 | 450 ± 30 | 270 ± 10 | 580 ± 40 | 290 ± 20 |
Figure5 illustrates the different behavior of rebound and suppression amplitudes in all subjects. The rebound is significantly stronger to passive movement than to tactile stimulation both in the contra- and ipsilateral hemispheres, whereas there is no significant difference between the suppression amplitudes in either hemisphere.
The peak latencies of suppression and rebound did not differ between the hemispheres within either stimulus type. Both suppression and rebound peak latencies were longer to passive movements than to tactile stimuli in both hemispheres (***p < 0.001) due to different durations of the two stimuli. Durations of suppression and rebound were not compared because of different stimulus lengths.
Discussion
The present study shows that passive movement is a strong modulator of the ∽20-Hz motor-cortex rhythm. The rebound of the rhythm was significantly stronger to passive movement than to tactile stimulation, whereas the magnitude of the suppression did not differ between these two stimulus types.
A ∽20-Hz rebound is observed after termination of movement or somatosensory stimulation and is therefore associated with deactivation or inhibition of the motor cortex (Salmelin et al. 1995b; Pfurtscheller et al. 1996, 1997; Cassim et al. 2000, 2001). This is further supported by TMS studies showing decreased motor-cortex excitability (decreased motor-evoked potentials) after cutaneous and median-nerve stimulation at latencies comparable with that of the ∽20-Hz rebound (Chen et al. 1999; Abbruzzese et al. 2001). Furthermore, a combined MEG and magnetic resonance spectroscopy study showed a positive correlation between the ∽20-Hz rebound strength and the inhibitory neurotransmitter gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration, suggesting that the rebound represents a period of GABAergic inhibition in MI (Gaetz et al. 2011).
Afferent somatosensory input has been proposed to affect motor functions by modulating motor-cortex excitability (Asanuma et al. 1979; Asanuma and Arissian 1984; Favorov et al. 1988; Ridding and Rothwell 1999a; Cassim et al. 2001; Houdayer et al. 2006; Reyns et al. 2008). In accordance, tactile or median-nerve stimulation alone, with no active movement, is sufficient to elicit a ∽20-Hz rebound, reflecting the modulatory effect of afferent input on motor-cortex activity (Salmelin and Hari 1994; Schnitzler et al. 1995, 1997; Pfurtscheller and Neuper 1997; Salenius et al. 1997; Hari et al. 1998, 2014; Neuper and Pfurtscheller 2001; Stancák et al. 2003; Pfurtscheller et al. 2005; Laaksonen et al. 2012). The rebound is abolished by blocking afferent input under ischemia (Cassim et al. 2001) or by sensory deafferentation (Reyns et al. 2008). Moreover, Cassim et al. (2000) showed prolonged synchronization of the ∽20-Hz rhythm during sustained isometric contraction, suggesting that the rebound is related to continuous flow of afferent proprioceptive input. Hence, the increase in the ∽20-Hz rhythm might represent ongoing coordination of sensory input and motor output (Gaetz and Cheyne 2006). These studies suggest that afferent input—but not movement as such—is necessary and sufficient to elicit the ∽20-Hz rebound.
Animal studies have shown that the primary motor cortex (MI) receives proprioceptive input via direct thalamocortical connections (Asanuma et al. 1979), via primary somatosensory cortex (SI; areas 3a and 2) and via secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) (Jones and Wise 1977; Jones et al. 1978; Jones 1983). Studies in monkeys have shown that the connections from area 3a (the primary projection area for proprioception) to MI are faster and more direct than from area 3b (primary projection area for tactile input), which has no or only sparse connections to MI (Jones et al. 1978; Jones 1983; Mima et al. 1997). In line with that, input from proprioceptors arrives to MI at similar short latencies (5–10 ms) as to SI (Devanandan and Heath 1975; Lucier et al. 1975).
In accordance, the present study showed that the rebound was stronger to passive movement than to tactile stimulation. This is congruent with earlier studies showing that magnitude of rebound might depend on activated musvle mass (Pfurtscheller et al. 1998) and voluntary movement and mixed-nerve stimulation elicit a stronger rebound than pure tactile stimulation (Houdayer et al. 2006). Furthermore, selective laser stimulation of nociceptive C-fibers did not produce any rebound, whereas a weak rebound was observed after stimulation of Aδ fibers (Raij et al. 2004). These studies suggest that the magnitude of the ∽20-Hz rebound depends on the type and quantity of afferent input. Tactile stimulation activates mainly exteroceptive afferents, whereas passive movement activates primarily proprioceptors and to a lesser extent exteroceptors. The stronger rebound to passive movement indicates stronger interaction of proprioceptive versus tactile input with motor output.
There are substantial differences in the proprioceptive and tactile stimulation in the present study that may have had an effect on rebound. Stimulus duration in passive movement (1130 msec) is significantly longer than in tactile stimulation (140 msec). However, as the tactile stimulus activates rapidly adaptive cutaneous receptors, it is not likely that stimulus duration would significantly affect the rebound peak amplitude although it might have an effect on rebound duration. Accordingly, our control measurements with tactile stimuli presented with durations of 140 msec and 1130 msec showed no difference in rebound peak amplitude although it prolonged the rebound duration.
Tactile stimuli were delivered to index fingers alternately with an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1.5 sec, resulting in an ISI of 3 sec for one side, whereas passive movements were performed with a 3 sec ISI for one side at the time. As unilateral stimulation has a bilateral effect on motor-cortex oscillations, although much weaker in ipsilateral side, the tactile stimuli occurring at 1.5 sec to the ipsilateral side could have an effect on the rebound. However, the rebound peaked to tactile stimuli at 793 ± 34 msec, that is much earlier than the subsequent ipsilateral stimulus arriving at 1500 msec, and therefore the ipsilateral stimulus is not likely to affect the peak amplitude. Furthermore, an earlier study using the same tactile stimulus in healthy subjects with an ISI of 3005 msec (Laaksonen et al. 2012) showed similar rebound amplitudes (57 ± 5% vs. 61 ± 11%) than the present study with an alternating 1.5 sec ISI, indicating that the shorter ISI does not affect the peak amplitude nor peak latency of the rebound.
The suppression of the ∽20-Hz rhythm starts within 500 msec after movement or somatosensory stimulation onset and is believed to reflect increased activity of the motor cortex (Salmelin et al. 1995a; Hari et al. 1997, 1998; Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva 1999; Müller et al. 2003; Raij et al. 2004). Similarly, suppression of this rhythm has been observed to painful laser stimuli at latencies comparable with the estimated conduction velocities of the two nociceptive fiber systems, which suggests that also noxious input excites (or disinhibits) MI. In addition, reaction times for lifting the index finger to tones were observed to be shorter during suppression induced by noxious input than before the onset of the 20-Hz suppression, indicating facilitation of the motor cortex (Raij et al. 2004). Even motor imagery causes suppression in the ∽20-Hz rhythm (Hari et al. 1998), and the suppression has been associated with significantly higher motor-evoked potentials, reflecting increased MI activation (Takemi et al. 2013).
The suppression and rebound of the ∽20-Hz rhythm have been reported to be generated within the same cortical region and have been interpreted to represent different levels of activation of the same neuronal population (Pfurtscheller 1992; Szurhaj et al. 2003). In the present study, we found that the stimulus type affected the suppression and rebound differently. This finding is in line with studies showing no difference in the suppression magnitude while the rebound amplitude varied between rapid and slow finger movements (Stancák and Pfurtscheller 1995, 1996), index and four-finger flexion (Salmelin et al. 1995a), brief ballistic wrist movement and onset of sustained isometric wrist extension (Alegre et al. 2002) and different types of ballistic movements (Wheaton et al. 2009). Furthermore, the peak frequencies of suppression (∽21 Hz) and rebound (∽19 Hz) have been shown to differ slightly (Pihko et al. 2014), and suppression and rebound have even occurred simultaneously in different beta bands (Pfurtscheller et al. 1997). The location of the maximum ∽20-Hz suppression typically appears posterior to the central sulcus, in the vicinity of the SI hand area, while the rebound is often localized anterior to the central sulcus in the MI hand region (Salmelin et al. 1995a,b; Pfurtscheller et al. 1996; Jurkiewicz et al. 2006). Accordingly, in our study the strongest rebound was observed over more anterior MEG channels than the maximal suppression.
Taken together, there is accumulating evidence that the neuronal populations generating ∽20-Hz suppression and rebound are both anatomically and functionally distinct. The suppression seems to be more independent of the type and quantity of somatosensory input and might thus reflect “all or none” -type of activation of the motor cortex, whereas the rebound is modulated strongly according to the type and strength of somatosensory input.
The peak latency of the ∽20-Hz rebound to both tactile and proprioceptive stimulation is long (in the present study 680–1500 ms). In addition to direct thalamocortical connections, MI receives afferent input from SI, SII, posterior parietal cortex and supplementary motor area that are activated for several hundreds of milliseconds after a somatosensory stimulus (Jones and Wise 1977; Jones et al. 1978; Shibasaki et al. 1980; Jones 1983; Mori et al. 1989; Donoghue and Sanes 1994; Hari et al. 1990; Weiller et al. 1996; Alary et al. 1998, 2002; Disbrow et al. 2000; Druschky et al. 2003). It is possible that the rebound reflects inflow of afferent input from these multiple higher order somatosensory areas. Furthermore, a weak positive correlation has been found between the rebound strength and the amplitude of SII evoked responses in stroke patients, whereas the SI response amplitudes did not correlate with the rebound (Laaksonen et al. 2012). Interaction of SII and motor cortex is demonstrated also by some pathological conditions such as Unverricht–Lundborg disease, Parkinson's disease and focal dystonia where deficient SII activation and altered motor-cortex excitability have been observed in the same patients (Boecker et al. 1999; Silén et al. 2000; Abbruzzese et al. 2001; Forss et al. 2001).
A previous study (Laaksonen et al. 2012) indicated that the ∽20-Hz rebound strength is correlated with the recovery of hand function after stroke. The present results show that proprioceptive stimulation elicits an even stronger rebound of the ∽20-Hz rhythm than tactile stimulation. Hence, modulation of the ∽20-Hz rhythm to passive movement, which does not require an active contribution of the subject and is not affected by changes in tactile sensitivity, could be a useful tool to evaluate alterations in cortical excitability after stroke. As the arising possibilities to boost recovery from stroke aim at modifying the excitatory–inhibitory balance of the brain, their effectiveness could be objectively evaluated by monitoring alterations in the ∽20-Hz rebound.
Conclusions
In the present study, passive movement elicited a stronger ∽20-Hz rebound than tactile stimulation, indicating that proprioceptive input modulates motor-cortex excitability more than tactile input. However, the magnitude of suppression did not differ between these two different stimuli, suggesting that suppression and rebound may represent activity of two functionally distinct neuronal populations.
Acknowledgments
We thank Mia Illman for help with the recordings and analysis, Jyrki Mäkelä for allowing us to perform some of the MEG measurements in Biomag Laboratory (HUCH, Meilahti University Hospital, Helsinki) and Suvi Heikkilä for her assistance in those recordings. The study was financially supported by the Helsinki University Central Hospital Research Fund, the SalWe Research Program for Mind and Body (Tekes; the Finnish Funding Agency for Technology and Innovation, Grant 1104/10), and Academy of Finland Postdoctoral Research Grant #13266133.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
References
- Abbruzzese G, Marchese R, Buccolieri A, Gasparetto B. Trompetto C. Abnormalities of sensorimotor integration in focal dystonia: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Brain. 2001;124:537–545. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.3.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alary F, Doyon B, Loubinoux I, Carel C, Boulanouar K, Ranjeva JP, et al. Event-related potentials elicited by passive movements in humans: characterization, source analysis, and comparison to fMRI. NeuroImage. 1998;8:377–390. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alary F, Simoes C, Jousmäki V, Forss N. Hari R. Cortical activation associated with passive movements of the human index finger: an MEG study. NeuroImage. 2002;15:691–696. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alegre M, Labarga A, Gurtubay IG, Iriarte J, Malanda A. Artieda J. Beta electroencephalograph changes during passive movements: sensory afferences contribute to beta event-related desynchronization in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 2002;331:29–32. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00825-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asanuma H. Arissian K. Experiments on functional role of peripheral input to motor cortex during voluntary movements in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 1984;52:212–227. doi: 10.1152/jn.1984.52.2.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asanuma H, Larsen KD. Zarzecki P. Peripheral input pathways projecting to the motor cortex in the cat. Brain Res. 1979;172:197–208. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90532-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boecker H, Ceballos-Baumann A, Bartenstein P, Weindl A, Siebner HR, Fassbender T, et al. Sensory processing in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease: investigations with 3D H(2)(15)O-PET. Brain. 1999;122(Pt 9):1651–1665. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassim F, Szurhaj W, Sediri H, Devos D, Bourriez J, Poirot I, et al. Brief and sustained movements: differences in event-related (de)synchronization (ERD/ERS) patterns. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2000;111:2032–2039. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00455-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassim F, Monaca C, Szurhaj W, Bourriez JL, Defebvre L, Derambure P, et al. Does post-movement beta synchronization reflect an idling motor cortex? NeuroReport. 2001;12:3859–3863. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200112040-00051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R, Corwell B. Hallett M. Modulation of motor cortex excitability by median nerve and digit stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 1999;129:77–86. doi: 10.1007/s002210050938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chollet F, Tardy J, Albucher J-F, Thalamas C, Berard E, Lamy C, et al. Fluoxetine for motor recovery after acute ischaemic stroke (FLAME): a randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:123–130. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70314-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devanandan MS. Heath PD. Proceedings: a short latency pathway from forearm nerves to area 4 of the baboon's cerebral cortex. J. Physiol. (Lond) 1975;248:43P–44P. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Disbrow E, Roberts T. Krubitzer L. Somatotopic organization of cortical fields in the lateral sulcus of Homo sapiens: evidence for SII and PV. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;418:1–21. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000228)418:1<1::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donoghue JP. Sanes JN. Motor areas of the cerebral cortex. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1994;11:382–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druschky K, Kaltenhäuser M, Hummel C, Druschky A, Huk WJ, Neundörfer B, et al. Somatosensory evoked magnetic fields following passive movement compared with tactile stimulation of the index finger. Exp. Brain Res. 2003;148:186–195. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1293-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duman RS, Malberg J, Nakagawa S. D'Sa C. Neuronal plasticity and survival in mood disorders. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;48:732–739. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00935-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Favorov O, Sakamoto T. Asanuma H. Functional role of corticoperipheral loop circuits during voluntary movements in the monkey: a preferential bias theory. J. Neurosci. 1988;8:3266–3277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-09-03266.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI. Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. NeuroImage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forss N, Silén T. Karjalainen T. Lack of activation of human secondary somatosensory cortex in Unverricht-Lundborg type of progressive myoclonus epilepsy. Ann. Neurol. 2001;49:90–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman DP. Jones EG. Thalamic input to areas 3a and 2 in monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 1981;45:59–85. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.45.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetz W. Cheyne D. Localization of sensorimotor cortical rhythms induced by tactile stimulation using spatially filtered MEG. NeuroImage. 2006;30:899–908. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetz W, Edgar JC, Wang DJ. Roberts TPL. Relating MEG measured motor cortical oscillations to resting γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration. NeuroImage. 2011;55:616–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldring S. Ratcheson R. Human motor cortex: sensory input data from single neuron recordings. Science. 1972;175:1493–1495. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4029.1493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gramfort A, Luessi M, Larson E, Engemann DA, Strohmeier D, Brodbeck C, et al. MNE software for processing MEG and EEG data. NeuroImage. 2014;86:446–460. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Hämäläinen H, Hämäläinen M, Kekoni J, Sams M. Tiihonen J. Separate finger representations at the human second somatosensory cortex. Neuroscience. 1990;37:245–249. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(90)90210-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Salmelin R, Mäkelä JP, Salenius S. Helle M. Magnetoencephalographic cortical rhythms. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1997;26:51–62. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00755-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Forss N, Avikainen S, Kirveskari E, Salenius S. Rizzolatti G. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1998;95:15061–15065. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hari R, Bourguignon M, Piitulainen H, Smeds E, De Tiège X. Jousmäki V. Human primary motor cortex is both activated and stabilized during observation of other person's phasic motor actions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014;369:20130171. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houdayer E, Labyt E, Cassim F, Bourriez JL. Derambure P. Relationship between event-related beta synchronization and afferent inputs: Analysis of finger movement and peripheral nerve stimulations. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006;117:628–636. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones EG. The nature of the afferent pathways conveying short-latency inputs to primate motor cortex. Adv. Neurol. 1983;39:263–285. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones EG. Wise SP. Size, laminar and columnar distribution of efferent cells in the sensory-motor cortex of monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 1977;175:391–438. doi: 10.1002/cne.901750403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones EG, Coulter JD. Hendry SH. Intracortical connectivity of architectonic fields in the somatic sensory, motor and parietal cortex of monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 1978;181:291–347. doi: 10.1002/cne.901810206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurkiewicz MT, Gaetz WC, Bostan AC. Cheyne D. Post-movement beta rebound is generated in motor cortex: evidence from neuromagnetic recordings. NeuroImage. 2006;32:1281–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laaksonen K, Kirveskari E, Mäkelä JP, Kaste M, Mustanoja S, Nummenmaa L, et al. Effect of afferent input on motor cortex excitability during stroke recovery. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012;123:2429–2436. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucier GE, Rüegg DC. Wiesendanger M. Responses of neurones in motor cortex and in area 3A to controlled stretches of forelimb muscles in cebus monkeys. J Physiol. (Lond) 1975;251:833–853. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maya Vetencourt JF, Sale A, Viegi A, Baroncelli L, De Pasquale R, O'Leary OF, et al. The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science. 2008;320:385–388. doi: 10.1126/science.1150516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mima T, Ikeda A, Terada K, Yazawa S, Mikuni N, Kunieda T, et al. Modality-spesific organization for cutaneous and proprioceptive sense in human primary sensory cortex studied by chronic epicortical recording. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1997;104:103–107. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(96)96142-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori A, Waters RS. Asanuma H. Physiological properties and patterns of projection in the cortico-cortical connections from the second somatosensory cortex to the motor cortex, area 4 gamma, in the cat. Brain Res. 1989;504:206–210. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91358-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller GR, Neuper C, Rupp R, Keinrath C, Gerner HJ. Pfurtscheller G. Event-related beta EEG changes during wrist movements induced by functional electrical stimulation of forearm muscles in man. Neurosci. Lett. 2003;340:143–147. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00019-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nenonen J, Nurminen J, Kičić D, Bikmullina R, Lioumis P, Jousmäki V, et al. Validation of head movement correction and spatiotemporal signal space separation in magnetoencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2012;123:2180–2191. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.03.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuper C. Pfurtscheller G. Event-related dynamics of cortical rhythms: frequency-specific features and functional correlates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2001;43:41–58. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00178-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nibuya M, Morinobu S. Duman RS. Regulation of BDNF and trkB mRNA in rat brain by chronic electroconvulsive seizure and antidepressant drug treatments. J. Neurosci. 1995;15:7539–7547. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07539.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G. Central beta rhythm during sensorimotor activities in man. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1981;51:253–264. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90139-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G. Event-related synchronization (ERS): an electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1992;83:62–69. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90133-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G. Lopes da Silva FH. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999;110:1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G. Neuper C. Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 1997;239:65–68. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(97)00889-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Neuper A., Jr Stancák C. Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band–an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: a review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1996;24:39–46. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00066-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Edlinger A., Jr Stancák G. On the existence of different types of central beta rhythms below 30 Hz. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1997;102:316–325. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(96)96612-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Zalaudek K. Neuper C. Event-related beta synchronization after wrist, finger and thumb movement. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1998;109:154–160. doi: 10.1016/s0924-980x(97)00070-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfurtscheller G, Neuper C, Brunner C. da Silva FL. Beta rebound after different types of motor imagery in man. Neurosci. Lett. 2005;378:156–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pihko E, Nevalainen P, Vaalto S, Laaksonen K, Mäenpää H, Valanne L, et al. Reactivity of sensorimotor oscillations is altered in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a magnetoencephalographic study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35:4105–4117. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raij TT, Forss N, Stancák A. Hari R. Modulation of motor-cortex oscillatory activity by painful Adelta- and C-fiber stimuli. NeuroImage. 2004;23:569–573. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reyns N, Houdayer E, Bourriez JL, Blond S. Derambure P. Post-movement beta synchronization in subjects presenting with sensory deafferentation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008;119:1335–1345. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridding MC. Rothwell JC. Afferent input and cortical organisation: a study with magnetic stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 1999;126:536–544. doi: 10.1007/s002210050762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salenius S, Schnitzler A, Salmelin R, Jousmäki V. Hari R. Modulation of human cortical rolandic rhythms during natural sensorimotor tasks. NeuroImage. 1997;5:221–228. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmelin R. Hari R. Spatiotemporal characteristics of sensorimotor neuromagnetic rhythms related to thumb movement. Neuroscience. 1994;60:537–550. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90263-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmelin R, Forss N, Knuutila J. Hari R. Bilateral activation of the human somatomotor cortex by distal hand movements. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1995a;95:444–452. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00193-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmelin R, Hämäläinen M, Kajola M. Hari R. Functional segregation of movement-related rhythmic activity in the human brain. NeuroImage. 1995b;2:237–243. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnitzler A, Salmelin R, Salenius S, Jousmäki V. Hari R. Tactile information from the human hand reaches the ipsilateral primary somatosensory cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 1995;200:25–28. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12065-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnitzler A, Salenius S, Salmelin R, Jousmäki V. Hari R. Involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery: a neuromagnetic study. NeuroImage. 1997;6:201–208. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shibasaki H, Barrett G, Halliday E. Halliday AM. Cortical potentials following voluntary and passive finger movements. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1980;50:201–213. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90147-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silén T, Forss N, Jensen O. Hari R. Abnormal reactivity of the approximately 20-Hz motor cortex rhythm in Unverricht Lundborg type progressive myoclonus epilepsy. NeuroImage. 2000;12:707–712. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stancák A., Jr Pfurtscheller G. Desynchronization and recovery of beta rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements in man. Neurosci. Lett. 1995;196:21–24. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)11827-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stancák A., Jr Pfurtscheller G. Event-related desynchronisation of central beta-rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements of dominant and nondominant hand. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 1996;4:171–183. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(96)00031-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stancák A, Svoboda J, Rachmanová R, Vrána J, Králík J. Tintera J. Desynchronization of cortical rhythms following cutaneous stimulation: effects of stimulus repetition and intensity, and of the size of corpus callosum. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003;114:1936–1947. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00201-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sung W-H, Wang C-P, Chou C-L, Chen Y-C, Chang Y-C. Tsai P-Y. Efficacy of coupling inhibitory and facilitatory repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to enhance motor recovery in hemiplegic stroke patients. Stroke. 2013;44:1375–1382. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.000522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szurhaj W, Derambure P, Labyt E, Cassim F, Bourriez J-L, Isnard J, et al. Basic mechanisms of central rhythms reactivity to preparation and execution of a voluntary movement: a stereoelectroencephalographic study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2003;114:107–119. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00333-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takemi M, Masakado Y, Liu M. Ushiba J. Event-related desynchronization reflects downregulation of intracortical inhibition in human primary motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2013;110:1158–1166. doi: 10.1152/jn.01092.2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taulu S. Kajola M. Presentation of electromagnetic multichannel data: the signal space separation method. J. Appl. Phys. 2005;97:124905. [Google Scholar]
- Taulu S. Simola J. Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Phys. Med. Biol. 2006;51:1759–1768. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiller C, Jüptner M, Fellows S, Rijntjes M, Leonhardt G, Kiebel S, et al. Brain representation of active and passive movements. NeuroImage. 1996;4:105–110. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheaton L, Fridman E, Bohlhalter S, Vorbach S. Hallett M. Left parietal activation related to planning, executing and suppressing praxis hand movements. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2009;120:980–986. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.02.161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]