Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Apr 16.
Published in final edited form as: Ethn Dis. 2008 Summer;18(3):289–293.

Psychosocial Factors Contribute to Resting Blood Pressure in African Americans

Mildred A Pointer 1, Jonathan N Livingston 1, Sadiqa Yancey 1, Marilyn K McClelland 1, Richard D Bukoski 1
PMCID: PMC4398995  NIHMSID: NIHMS678551  PMID: 18785441

Abstract

Objectives

African Americans as a group have higher blood pressure than individuals of northern European ancestry (non-Hispanic Whites). We investigate whether psychosocial factors explain the resting blood pressure of healthy, community-dwelling African Americans in our study.

Participants

A convenience sample of self-reported normotensive African Americans aged 18–65 years who live in the North Carolina Triangle region.

Design

The study protocol consisted of three resting blood pressure sessions with assessment of the following psychosocial factors: anger expression, interpersonal support, anxiety, depression, hostility, active coping, and perceived racism. Additional clinical assessments were height, weight, waist girth, fasting glucose, insulin, triglycerides, and cholesterol.

Results

Resting systolic blood pressure was positively associated with male sex (P<.001) and positively correlated with age (P<.0001), waist girth (P<.0001), body mass index (P=.023), and a Cook Medley Hostility subscale identified as aggressive responding (P=.031). Mean arterial pressure was positively correlated with age (P<.0001), waist girth (P=.0041), Spielberger Anger Expression subscale anger control (P=.023), and aggressive responding (P=.020).

Conclusions

Anger and hostility are significantly associated with resting blood pressure and may modulate behavioral and traditional (biologic) risk factors that determine cardiovascular physiology.

Keywords: Hypertension, Psychosocial Factors, Resting Blood Pressure, African Americans, Health Disparity

Introduction

Cardiovascular disease, including stroke, myocardial infarction, and atherosclerotic vascular disease, is the leading cause of morbidity and mortality in Western society and disproportionately affects the African American community.13 Essential hypertension, which is a significant risk factor for stroke, myocardial infarction, and renal failure, is overrepresented in this minority population.46 Although mechanistic hypotheses have been proposed to explain the disproportionate prevalence of hypertension among African Americans,7 the etiology of essential hypertension in this population is incompletely understood.

Psychosocial factors have been suggested to play a key role in explaining the disparate prevalence of hypertension and cardiovascular disease.8 Some of the earliest work showing that socioeconomic status and active coping were associated with the increased prevalence of hypertension among African American was reported by James et al from a population of African Americans living in the northeastern part of North Carolina.8 Since these historical reports, other studies have supported this initial finding and included several other psychosocial factors such as attitudes toward (feeling supported and valued) and responses to (hostility, anger, depression, and anxiety) environmental stress.9 Individuals who score high for one or more psychosocial traits such as hostility, anxiety, depression, and stress are at increased risk for the development of cardiovascular disease.10

A recent hypothesis proposes that psychosocial factors explain the disparate prevalence of hypertension and stroke in African Americans. The mechanism could be a result of psychosocial factors that affect behavior (unhealthy diet, smoking, and lack of exercise) or physiologic processes such as the neuroendocrine system’s regulation of blood pressure.11 Psychosocial factors also include environmental stress such as racism; discrimination; smoking; traditional high-salt, high-fat southern diet; and the response to stressors, including active coping, anger, and hostility.

Our study examines resting blood pressure, psychosocial measures, and clinical measures in a group of African Americans living in the Triangle region of North Carolina. The North Carolina Triangle region is in the stroke belt (ie, a geographic region with a high occurrence of stroke).15

Methods

A convenience sample of 176 persons (63 men, 113 women) of self-reported African descent were recruited by fliers on campus. Entry criteria were age 18–65 years; residing in surrounding North Carolina Triangle regions of Durham, Orange, and Wake Counties; having no known (self-reported) cardiovascular disease; and not taking any antihypertensive medication. Informed consent was obtained for each participant in accordance with North Carolina Central University institutional review board approval and federal guidelines.

Study participants were scheduled at either 9 am or 1 pm for the three-hour study protocol. Trained staff measured blood pressure with a GE Dinamap Pro 100 automatic sphygmomanometer and a cuff size appropriate for the body size. Each participant was allowed five minutes to sit quietly before taking the first resting blood pressure. The instrument was set to make measurements every minute for five minutes. In the second resting session, measurements were taken every minute for 10 minutes, while the third session was similar to the first session, ie, measurements were made every minute for five minutes. The instrument measured systolic and diastolic blood pressure (SBP, DBP), heart rate, and mean arterial pressure (MAP).

Psychosocial phenotypes were assessed by using the following validated self-report instruments: chronic exposure to racism was evaluated with the Perceived Racism Scale;16 social support with the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List;14 hostility with Barefoot’s version of the Cook Medley Hostility Scale;17 depression with the Beck Depression Index;18 active coping with John Henry Active Coping;19 and anger with Spielberger Anger Expression.20 We also assessed the level of anxiety with Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.21

On the following day, each participant reported to the student health center for collection of a fasting blood sample for serum isolation and DNA extraction. Lab Core analyzed serum samples for insulin, glucose, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, total cholesterol, and triglycerides. The homeostasis model assessment (HOMA) was calculated as an index of insulin sensitivity.22

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed with SAS for Windows version 9.1.3 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC). Psychosocial scales and subscales used scoring protocols documented in prior research as indicated above and were confirmed with factor analysis. Cronbach α values ≥.6 were verified for each psychosocial scale with SAS PROC CORR. Data reduction for SBP, DBP, MAP, heart rate, and other clinical measures with multiple measurements used SAS PROC MEANS to calculate mean, standard deviation, standard error, median, and quartile scores as appropriate.

Examination of the PROC Univariate output indicated many of the clinical measures were not normally distributed. Since the data were not normally distributed, we used the Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney nonparametric test to assess sex differences in the clinical measures.

SAS PROC CORR calculated Pearson correlations between psychosocial and clinical measures or psychosocial and blood pressure measures. PROC REG with the stepwise option implemented the stepwise algorithm for variable selection of a multivariate model for predicting blood pressure. PROC GLM provided multivariate parameter estimates and confidence intervals. To assess the assumptions for the multivariate model, PROC UNIVARIATE with the normal option plotted residuals and provided the Shapiro-Wilk statistic for normality.

Results

Sample Characteristics

The average age of our sample was 32 years (Table 1). Men had higher SBP, and women had a higher heart rate. Additionally, women had higher insulin levels and HOMA values, which suggests that the women in this group were more likely to be insulin resistant than were men. We observed no sex differences for the traditional metabolic syndrome risk factors.

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of 176 self-reported normotensive African Americans recruited from the North Carolina Triangle region

Characteristic Total n Overall Mean (SD) Women, n Women, Mean (SD) Men, n Men, Mean (SD)
Age, years 176 32 (12) 113 33 (13) 63 32 (12)
SBP, mm Hg 175 121 (14) 112 118 (15)* 63 126 (12)*
DBP, mm Hg 175 74 (11) 112 74 (10) 63 74 (11)
MAP, mm Hg 175 91 (10) 112 90 (10) 63 93 (10)
Heart rate, bpm 175 69 (11) 112 71 (9)* 63 66 (13)*
Insulin resistance (HOMA) 101 1.6 (1.6) 63 2 (1.9)* 39 .9 (.9)*
Glucose, mg/dL 118 72 (29) 76 75 (30) 42 65.7 (26)
Insulin, µU/mL 105 8.4 (7.5) 66 9.9 (7.9)* 39 5.9 (6)*
Waist, cm 148 87 (16) 95 86 (15) 53 90 (16)
BMI, kg/m2 165 28.9 (6.6) 107 29.3 (6.8) 58 28.0 (6)
Triglycerides, mg/dL 116 88 (41) 74 87 (41) 42 89 (43)
Total cholesterol, mg/dL 116 182 (43) 74 185 (46) 42 178 (37)
HDL cholesterol, mg/dL 116 57 (16) 74 58 (16) 42 54 (15)
LDL cholesterol, mg/dL 116 107 (39) 74 109 (41) 42 104 (36)

SD = standard deviation, SBP = systolic blood pressure, DBP, diastolic blood pressure, MAP = mean arterial pressure, HOMA = homeostasis model assessment, BMI = body mass index, HDL = high-density lipoprotein, LDL = low-density lipoprotein.

*

Significant (P<.05) difference between men and women at baseline according to the Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney nonparametric test.

Resting Blood Pressure and Metabolic Syndrome Risk Factors

We found a positive correlation of SBP, DBP, MAP, and heart rate with age. We did not find a correlation of resting blood pressure with any of the lipid variables (triglycerides, total cholesterol, LDL cholesterol, and HDL cholesterol). However, we did find that heart rate was positively correlated with insulin (P<.0005), glucose (P<.0098), and insulin resistance (HOMA) (P<.0001). Waist circumference was significantly correlated with SBP and MAP (P<.0001, .0041, respectively). Body mass index was significantly correlated with SBP (P<.023).

Resting Blood Pressure and Psychosocial Factors

Resting blood pressure parameters were analyzed for association with psychosocial factors such as hostility, anger, social support, depression, and active coping. Resting SBP, DBP, and MAP positively correlated with the Cook Medley Hostility aggressive responding subscale (P<.031, .023, and .020, respectively). Resting SBP, DBP, and MAP positively correlated with the anger control subscale of Spielberger Anger Expression (P<.045, .048, and .023, respectively). Resting blood pressure parameters were not significantly associated with active coping, depression, perceived racism, anxiety, or interpersonal support. (Table 3)

Table 3.

Correlation of psychosocial factors to resting cardiovascular parameters

Perceived
Racism N=173
Anger Expression
(SAE) N=50
Anger-control
(SAE) N=50
Aggressive Responding
(CMH) N=50
ISEL
N=169
John Henryism
(JHAC) N=174
BDI
N=172
Mean resting −.07 .23 .29 .31 .05 .05 .08
SBP .34 .094 .045 .031 .49 .52 .29
Mean resting −.07 .25 .28 .32 .12 .07 .07
DBP .38 .079 .048 .023* .12 .36 .36
Mean resting −.08 .28 .32 .33 .10 .07 .09
MAP .28 .048 .023* .020* .21 .36 .25
Mean resting −.11 .21 .19 .06 .04 −.07 .09
HR .14 .15 .18 .65 .61 .33 .26
*

= significant at p=.025 by two-tail test.

= significant at .05 using one-tail test; in each cell the top number represents Pearson Correlation Coefficient and bottom number Prob > |r| under H0: Rho=0.

Significant parameters in the prediction of resting SBP were age, sex, and body mass index (Table 4). Psychosocial factors of aggressive responding and anger control were not significant, indicating a covariance with other parameters already in the model. Life-time perceived racism was a significant parameter. Verifications of the assumptions of multivariate modeling raised concerns because the residuals were not normally distributed. We attempted various transformations of the model parameters but did not find a regression model that adhered to the assumptions. Thus, we emphasize correlations rather than the results of the multivariate modeling.

Table 4.

Multivariate regression model for resting systolic blood pressure with 156 observations and r2 of .32

Variable Coefficient (β) Standard Error 95% Confidence Interval t value P value
Intercept 101.11* 5.87 89.51 to 112.71 17.22 <.0001
Age .50 .08 .34 to .67 6.10 <.0001
Female sex −1.69* 2.12 −14.87 to −6.51 −5.05 <.0001
Male sex .00* . . . .
Body mass index .42 .15 .12 to .72 2.77 .0063
Lifetime perceived racism −.10 .03 −.17 to −.03 −2.91 .0042
Hostility subscale, aggressive responding 3.83 4.57 −5.20 to 12.87 .84 .4025
Anger subscale, anger control .27 .45 −.62 to 1.16 .59 .5538
*

Parameters with values that are not uniquely estimable.

Discussion

We found that psychosocial factors such as anger and hostility predict resting blood pressure in healthy, normotensive African Americans from the Triangle region of North Carolina in addition to traditional risk factors such as age, sex, and waist circumference. These data demonstrate that responses to environmental stresses of everyday living play a substantial role in normal resting blood pressure regulation in young, healthy African Americans.

Our results are similar to those of previous studies of the association of psychosocial factors and resting blood pressure in educated African Americans. These studies also show a positive relationship between anger and blood pressure.9,23 Thus, our cohort appears to be representative of other comparable African American cohorts studied for psychosocial and blood pressure interaction. The interpretation of positive association of anger control and blood pressure has been explained as a result of a combination of fear of reprimand and guilt from feeling anger or hostile activation of the sympathetic nervous system.24,25 In contrast, anger expression also was positively correlated with blood pressure, as reported by others.23,26 However, some studies have found opposite associations.27,28 These disparate findings may be explained by the influence of social construct on expression of emotions;25 that is, how the surrounding community or family views anger expression. The choice to control anger comes with consequences to health. More studies are needed to define the physiological pathways involved in the blood pressure response to anger

Hostility, as assessed by the aggressive responding subscale of the Cook-Medley scale, was positively associated with DBP and heart rate, while John Henryism showed no significant association. Others have also reported such an association of hostility and resting SBP, DBP, and total peripheral resistance but no association of John Henryism and resting blood pressure.[9,29,30] The failure to see an association of John Henryism and blood pressure may have a socioeconomic basis. In a previous study, higher blood pressure was associated with low education levels or low socioeconomic status combined with high John Henry Active Coping scores.8 Our cohort had a median education level of 14 years. In addition, many of our subjects were college students at the time of the study. Therefore, John Henryism may not play a role in blood pressure at this socioeconomic level. Unlike John Henryism, however, hostility may be independent of socioeconomic status.

I We did not find a correlation of perceived racism and resting blood pressure. Others have reported a lack of an association of racism and blood pressure.[31] Multivariate regression models (Table 2) identifed chronic exposure to perceived racism as a contributing factor to resting blood pressure in this sample with a negative coefficient. Perhaps this finding is explained by the fact that most of the participants had lived in a homogenous cultural environment (mostly Black neighborhoods, schools, and college) most of their lifetimes.

Table 2.

Correlation of metabolic syndrome indices to resting cardiovascular parameters among 176 self-reported normotensive African Americans recruited from the North Carolina Triangle region

Age
n=175
Glucose
n=118
Insulin
n=105
HOMA
n=101
TG
n=116
TC
n=116
HDL
n=116
LDL
n=116
BMI
n=164
Waist
n=149
Mean resting .41 −.03 −.07 −.15 .03 .11 .13 .04 .18 .34
SBP <.0001* .73 .49 .13 .73 .26 .16 .66 .023* <.0001*
Mean resting .44 −.01 −.10 −.19 .024 .06 .14 .01 −.005 .12
DBP <.0001* .91 .33 .055 .80 .52 .13 .93 .95 .14
Mean resting .45 −.02 −.08 −.17 .06 .08 .16 .01 .08 .23
MAP <.0001* .81 .47 .082 .71 .40 .10 .87 .29 .0041*
Mean Resting .18 .24 .33 .37 −.08 .05 .12 .02 .06 .12
HR .015* .0098* .0005* .0001* .41 .56 .20 .85 .45 .14
*

Significant at P=.025 by two-tailed test; in each cell the top number represents Pearson Correlation Coefficient and bottom number Prob > |r| under H0: Rho=0.

This study demonstrates that psychosocial factors such as anger and hostility predict resting blood pressure in healthy normotensive African Americans. Thus, psychosocial factors should be evaluated in addition to the more traditional risk factors (insulin resistance, cholesterol, waist circumference) when assessing cardiovascular risk in African Americans. Future research will increase the sample size and include a longitudinal study to evaluate if psychosocial factors continue to affect blood pressure and if these factors facilitate or mitigate the progression towards cardiovascular disease.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr Redford Williams of Duke University School of Medicine for his mentoring. This work was supported by grants from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute HL59868, the National Center on Minority Health and Health Disparity P20 MD00175, and the National Institute of Health/National Library of Medicine F38 LM008882.

Footnotes

Author Contributions

Design concept of study: Pointer, Livingston, Bukoski (deceased)

Acquisition of data: Pointer, Livingston, Yancey, McClelland, Bukoski (deceased)

Data analysis and interpretation: Pointer, Livingston, Yancey, McClelland

Manuscript draft: Pointer, Livingston, McClelland

Statistical expertise: McClelland

Acquisition of funding: Pointer, Livingston, McClelland, Bukoski (deceased)

Administrative, technical, or material assistance: Yancey, Bukoski (deceased)

Supervision: Pointer, Livingston

References

  • 1.Frerichs RR, Chapman JM, Maes EF. Mortality due to all causes and to cardiovascular diseases among seven race-ethnic populations in Los Angeles County, 1980. Int J Epidemiol. 1984;13(3):291–298. doi: 10.1093/ije/13.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Houghton JL, Prisant LM, Carr AA, Flowers NC, Frank MJ, et al. Racial differences in myocardial ischemia and coronary flow reserve in hypertension. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1994;23(5):1123–1129. doi: 10.1016/0735-1097(94)90600-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gupta V, Nanda NC, Yesilbursa D, Huang WY, Li Q, Gomez CR, et al. Racial differences in thoracic aorta atherosclerosis among ischemic stroke patients. Stroke. 2003;34(2):408–412. doi: 10.1161/01.str.0000050643.32175.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jones DW, Chambless LE, Folsom AR, et al. Risk factors for coronary heart disease in African Americans: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study, 1987–1997. Arch Intern Med. 2002;162(22):2565–2571. doi: 10.1001/archinte.162.22.2565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ruilope LM, Campo C, Lahera V. The kidney and arterial hypertension. Drugs. 1993;46(Suppl 2):108–112. doi: 10.2165/00003495-199300462-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Joint National Committee. Hypertension prevalence and the status of awareness, treatment, and control in the United States. Final report of the Subcommittee on Definition and Prevalence of the 1984 Joint National Committee. Hypertension. 1985;7(3 Pt 1):457–468. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.7.3_pt_1.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Grim CE, Henry JP, Myers HF. High blood pressure in blacks: salt, slavery, survival, stress, and racism. In: Brenner J, editor. Hypertension, Pathophysiology, Diagnosis, and Management. 2nd ed. New York: Raven Press, Ltd; 1995. pp. 171–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.James SA, Strogatz DS, Wing SB, Ramsey DL, et al. Socioeconomic status, John Henryism, and hypertension in blacks and whites. Am J Epidemiol. 1987;126(4):664–673. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Adams JH, Aubert RE, Clark VR. The relationship among John Henryism, hostility, perceived stress, social support, and blood pressure in African American college students. Ethn Dis. 1999;9(3):359–368. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Barefoot JC, Dahlstrom WG, Williams WB., Jr Hostility, CHD incidence, and total mortality: a 25-year follow-up study of 255 physicians. Psychosom Med. 1983;45(1):59–63. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198303000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Williams RB. Invited commentary: socioeconomic status, hostility, and health behaviors–does it matter which comes first? AmJ Epidemiol. 2003;158(8):743–746. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Howard G, Prineas R, Moy C, et al. Racial and geographic differences in awareness, treatment, and control of hypertension: the REasons for Geographic And Racial Differences in Stroke study. Stroke. 2006;37(5):1171–1178. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000217222.09978.ce. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McNeilly MD, Anderson NB, Armstead CA, et al. The perceived racism scale: a multidimensional assessment of the experience of white racism among African Americans. Ethn Dis. 1996;6(1–2):154–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Barefoot JC, Dodge KA, Peterson BL, Dahlstrom WG, Williams RB., Jr The Cook-Medley hostility scale: item content and ability to predict survival. Psychosom Med. 1989;51(1):46–57. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198901000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961:561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.James SA, Keenan NL, Strogatz DS, Browning SR, Garrett JM. Socioeconomic status, John Henryism, and blood pressure in black adults. The Pitt County Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;135(1):59–67. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Spielberger CD, Johnson EH, Russell SF, Crane RJ, Jacobs GA, Worden TJ. The experience and expression of anger: construction and validation of an anger expression scale. In: Chesney MA, Rosenman RH, editors. Anger and Hostility in Cardiovascular and Behavioral Medicine. Washington, DC: Hemisphere; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Spielberger CD. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Matthews DR, Hosker JP, Rudenski AS, Naylor BA, Treacher DF, Turner RC. Homeostasis model assessment: insulin resistance and beta-cell function from fasting plasma glucose and insulin concentrations in man. Diabetologia. 1985;28(7):412–419. doi: 10.1007/BF00280883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Goldstein HS, Edelberg R, Meier CF, Davis L. Relationship of resting blood pressure and heart rate to experienced anger and expressed anger. Psychosom Med. 1988;50(4):321–329. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198807000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Alexander F. Emotional factors in essential hypertension. Psychosom Med. 1939;1:175–179. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jorgensen RS, Kolodziej ME. Suppressed anger, evaluative threat, and cardiovascular reactivity: A tripartite profile approach. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007;66:102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Everson SA, Goldberg DE, Kaplan GA, Julkunen J, Salonen JT. Anger expression and incident hypertension. Psychosom Med. 1998;60(6):730–735. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199811000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dimsdale JE, Pierce C, Schoenfeld D, Brown A, Zusman R, Graham R. Suppressed anger and blood pressure: the effects of race, sex, social class, obesity, and age. Psychosom Med. 1986;48(6):430–436. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198607000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Davidson K, MacGregor MW, Stuhr J, Dixon K, MacLean D. Constructive anger verbal behavior predicts blood pressure in a population-based sample. Health Psychol. 2000;19(1):55–64. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cooper DC, Waldstein SR. Hostility differentially predicts cardiovascular risk factors in African American and White young adults. J Psychosom Res. 2004;57(5):491–499. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jackson LA, Adams-Campbell LL. John Henryism and blood pressure in black college students. J Behav Med. 1994;17(1):69–79. doi: 10.1007/BF01856883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Clark R. Interactive but not direct effects of perceived racism and trait anger predict resting systolic and diastolic blood pressure in black adolescents. Health Psychol. 2006;25(5):580–585. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES