Abstract
Introduction:
Beneficial effects of nicotine on cognitive functioning may contribute to the markedly high rates of smoking among people with schizophrenia. A reduction in the nicotine content of cigarettes to non-addictive levels is being considered as a regulatory strategy for reducing tobacco dependence in the United States. We examined whether switching to very low nicotine content (VLNC) cigarettes impairs cognitive functioning in smokers with and without schizophrenia, andwhether nicotine replacement reverses these effects.
Methods:
Smokers with schizophrenia (SS, n = 29) and control smokers matched on smoking rate but without psychiatric illness (CS, n = 28) smoked usual-brand cigarettes, VLNC cigarettes while wearing 2 placebo patches (PLA), or VLNC cigarettes while wearing 2 nicotine patches totaling 42mg (NIC) for 5hr, and then completed computerized assessments of visual sustained attention, motor speed, visual working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and response variability.
Results:
Across conditions, SS were slower than CS in tasks of motor speed and visual working memory, and had poorer target detectability on a visual sustained attention task. Across groups, functioning in domains of visual sustained attention, inhibitory control, processing speed, and response variability was impaired in the VLNC + PLA condition relative to the usual-brand and VLNC + NIC conditions.
Conclusions:
Dramatically reducing the nicotine content of cigarettes may impair cognitive functioning in heavy smokers with and without schizophrenia, but the use of nicotine replacement while smoking VLNC cigarettes may preserve cognitive functioning in these smokers.
Introduction
Schizophrenia is associated with a threefold higher prevalence of cigarette smoking compared to the general population, and a 20% reduction in lifespan.1,2 One factor that may contribute to smoking persistence in this population is the disruptive effects of abstinence on neurocognitive functioning.3 Cognitive deficits are considered a core feature of schizophrenia4 and are associated with poor functional outcomes in these patients.5 Consistent with this theory, experimental studies have found that smoking abstinence impairs attention and spatial working memory performance in smokers with schizophrenia (SS),and smoking reinstatement reverses these impairments.6,7 Moreover, nicotine manipulations (as opposed to smoking manipulations) specifically affect performance on neurocognitive tasks tapping domains of attention, spatial organization, verbal memory, and processing speed in SS.8
The U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) acquired regulatory authority over tobacco products in 2009, with the enactment of the Family Smoking Prevention and Tobacco Control Act.9 One of the top priorities of the FDA’s Center for Tobacco Products is to fund research studies that examine the effects of reducing the nicotine content of cigarettes,10 which has been proposed as a means of reducing tobacco dependence in the United States).11,12 This regulatory strategy could be particularly beneficial to smokers, such as SS, who have difficulty accessing effective smoking cessation treatments. However, the effects of very low nicotine content (VLNC) cigarette use in SS are largely unknown, and, given the effects of nicotine abstinence on cognition in SS, it is possible that switching to VLNC cigarettes could negatively affect cognitive functioning.
To our knowledge, only one study has examined the effects of VLNC cigarettes on cognitive functioning in SS.13 That study found mixed support for the idea that switching to VLNC cigarettes may negatively affect cognitive performance. Therefore, more work is needed to examine the potential impact of VLNC cigarette use on cognitive performance in SS. Thus, the aim of the current study was to compare the effects of VLNC cigarette smoking, with and without 42mg transdermal nicotine replacement, with usual-brand cigarette smoking ontasks assessing key domains of cognitive functioning in SS and control smokers without psychiatric illness (CS). We hypothesized thatall participants would perform more poorly on these tasks after smoking VLNC cigarettes with placebo patches relative to usual-brand cigarettes, that these decrements would be greater in SS than CS, and that nicotine replacement would reverse these deficits in both groups.
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited from the local community for a study of the subjective and behavioral effects of VLNC cigarettes and nicotine replacement.14 Participants were 18+ years of age, had smoked 20–50 cigarettes per day for at least 1 year, and were highly nicotine-dependent (≥6 on the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence, FTND).15 The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis I (SCID-I)16 was used to confirm diagnoses in SS and ruled out current Axis I psychiatric illness in CS. Exclusionary criteria included medical conditions that precluded transdermal nicotine use, positive pregnancy, or drug toxicity tests at baseline, or positive breath alcohol level at any session. This study was approved by the Brown University Institutional Review Board.
Procedures
Participants underwent an initial session in which they provided a breath carbon monoxide (CO) sample and completed demographic and smoking history questionnaires. In SS, current psychiatric symptom levels were assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).17
In Session 2, participants completed a 5-hr assessment of smoking topography while smoking their usual-brand cigarettes using a Clinical Research Support System (CReSS; Borgwaldt KC, Richmond, VA) device. In the remaining sessions, participants underwent the following conditions during 5-hr periods, with order counterbalanced across participants: VLNC cigarettes + 42mg transdermal nicotine replacement (NIC), VLNC cigarettes + placebo patches (PLA), no cigarettes + NIC, no cigarettes + PLA, usual brand cigarettes. After these 5-hr periods, participants completed measures of cigarette craving, nicotine withdrawal symptoms and cognitive performance, followed by an assessment of usual-brand smoking behavior. The current report focuses on comparing the effects of the usual-brand, VLNC + PLA and VLNC + NIC conditions on cognitive performance in SS and CS; effects of all conditions on craving, withdrawal symptoms and usual-brand smoking behavior have been reported.14 The VLNC cigarettes in this study (Quest 3; Vector Tobacco, Timberlake, NC) contained less than 0.05mg nicotine and 10mg tar. To hold smoking behavior constant across sessions, participants were cued to smoke the VLNC or usual-brand cigarettes according to the rate of their usual-brand smoking from Session 2. PLA and NIC patches (GlaxoSmithKline, Parsippany, NJ) were applied under double-blind conditions to participants’ upper arms (one per arm), for a total of 0 or 42mg NIC. A NIC dose of 42mg was chosen given that 21mg NIC had no effect on craving, withdrawal symptoms or usual-brand smoking in SS in a previous study.18
Cognitive Measures
Participants completed the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II),19 and the motor screening (MOT), rapid visual information processing (RVP), delayed matching to sample (DMS; 12-s delay condition) and simple reaction time (SRT) tests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge UK).20 Cognitive domains of interest were assessed using the following variables from these tests: basic visual and motor functioning were assessed using the MOT mean latency variable, visual sustained attention was assessed using the percent omission errors variable from the CPT-II and the target detection (A′) variable from the RVP, visual working memory was assessed using the accuracy (percent correct) and response latency variables from the DMS-12, inhibitory control was assessed using the percent commission errors variables from the CPT-II and SRT, processing speed was assessed using hit reaction time (RT) variables from the RVP and CPT-II, and response variability was assessed using the hit rate RT standard error variable from the CPT-II. The CPT-II, CANTAB, and other tasks assessing these domains have been found to be sensitive to smoking or nicotine manipulations in SS or CS in previous studies.6,7,13,21–25 The CPT-II was administered on a Dell desktop computer and the CANTAB tests were administered on a Dell laptop computer with a 15″ monitor and a touchscreen overlay (MagicTouch, Keytec, Inc.). Participants began with the MOT test, followed by the RVP, SRT, DMS, and CPT-II tests; alternate test versions,when available, were used across sessions to reduce practice effects.
Data Analysis
Independent-samples t-tests and chi-square tests were performed to examine group differences on demographic and smoking history measures. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) or multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) tests were performed to examine the impact of group (SS, CS) and condition (VLNC + PLA, VLNC + NIC, usual-brand cigarettes) on variableswithin each domain; significant MANOVAs were followed by univariate ANOVAs and Bonferroni-corrected pairwise comparisons. Sphericity was evaluated using Mauchly’sW and, where violated, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were implemented. Because the CANTAB battery was initiated after the CPT II, sample sizes are smaller for domains that include measures from CANTAB tasks; 29 SS and 28 CS completed the CPT II and 21 CS and 18 SS completed all cognitive tests. One outlier value (4 SD about the mean) was removed from the data. Analyses were conducted with SPSS version 22 (IBM). Significance level was set at p < .05. Effect sizes (partial eta squared, ) are provided, with ≤ .05 indicating small, = .06–.13 indicating medium and ≥ .14 indicating large effect sizes.26 Note that speed of performance in the MOT, RVP, and CPT-II tasks may be described as “latency” or “reaction time,” given that these terms are used distinctly according to each task.
Results
Sample Characteristics
There were no significant differences between SS and CS on demographic or smoking variables including age, education, gender, race, ethnicity, CO level, cigarettes smoked per day, or FTND score (Table 1). SS had mild psychiatric symptom levels, similar to other studies of this type.6,7
Table 1.
SS | CS | SS vs. CS | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 29) | (n = 28) | Statistic | p Value | |
Age [M (SD)] | 46.4 (8.1) | 45.1 (11.0) | t 55 = 0.50 | 0.62 |
Male | 62% | 57% | χ2(1) = 0.14 | 0.71 |
Education (years) | 12.0 (2.2) | 12.4 (1.8) | t 55= 0.68 | 0.50 |
Race | χ2(4) = 3.29 | 0.51 | ||
White | 76% | 61% | ||
Black/African-American | 14% | 29% | ||
American Indian/Alaskan Native | 7% | 4% | ||
Asian-American | 0% | 4% | ||
Multiracial/other | 3% | 4% | ||
Hispanic ethnicity | 0% | 4% | χ2(1) = 1.05 | 0.31 |
FTND score | 6.9 (1.5) | 6.9 (1.8) | t 55 = 0.01 | 0.99 |
Cigarettes per day | 25.8 (10.0) | 24.3 (6.9) | t 55 = 0.64 | 0.52 |
Baseline breath CO level (ppm) | 33.1 (24.6) | 27.6 (14.3) | t 55 = 1.03 | 0.31 |
PANSS Total Score | 52.6 (14.7) | |||
Antipsychotic drug class | 69% atypical | |||
17% typical | ||||
7% both |
Note. FTND = Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence; CO = carbon monoxide; ppm = parts per million; PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.
Motor Speed
The mixed factorial ANOVA indicated a significant group effect on mean latency in the MOT task [F (1, 41) = 10.51, p < .01, = .20]. Across conditions, SS were slower to select a target than CS (Table 2). The condition main effect and group × condition interaction on motor speed were not significant.
Table 2.
Control smokers | Smokers with schizophrenia | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Usual brand | VLNC + PLA | VLNC + NIC | Usual brand | VLNC + PLA | VLNC + NIC | |
Motor speed | ||||||
MOT latency** | 887 (271) | 919 (240) | 896 (264) | 1,162 (401) | 1,190 (394) | 1,096 (307) |
Visual sustained attention1 | ||||||
CPT-II Omissions (%) | 1.40 (2.82) | 3.00 (5.71) | 1.19 (2.48) | 2.83 (5.33) | 3.89 (7.63) | 1.94 (2.52) |
RVP A′** | 0.99 (0.02) | 0.98 (0.03) | 0.99 (0.02) | 0.95 (0.06) | 0.96 (0.04) | 0.97 (0.03) |
Visual working memory | ||||||
DMS 12s accuracy | 81.0 (20.5) | 82.9 (17.1) | 84.8 (17.8) | 82.2 (22.6) | 71.1 (27.6) | 75.6 (18.9) |
DMS 12s latency** | 3,009 (635) | 3,144 (1,153) | 2,969 (1,030) | 3,917 (1,406) | 3,972 (1,293) | 4,130 (1,863) |
Inhibitory control | ||||||
CPT-II commissions (%)++ | 32.6 (25.4)a | 38.1 (28.1)b | 37.8 (29.4)ab | 32.1 (20.4)a | 39.1 (22.1)b | 38.4 (17.1)ab |
SRT commissions (%)+++ | 1.13 (1.32)a | 2.00 (1.83)b | 1.09 (1.65)a | 0.67 (1.64)a | 2.94 (3.06)b | 1.22 (2.98)a |
Processing speed | ||||||
RVP latency++ | 346 (72.8)a | 367 (62.6)b | 346 (71.0)a | 407 (122)a | 426 (112)b | 382 (109)a |
CPT-II Hit RT++ | 377 (70.1)a | 407 (94.3)b | 382 (77.9)a | 428 (79.9)a | 442 (87.3)b | 413 (78.9)a |
Response variability | ||||||
CPT-II Hit RT SE ++ | 6.9 (3.8)a | 9.0 (6.2)b | 7.4 (5.7)a | 8.5 (7.1)a | 11.4 (8.1)b | 8.4 (5.3)a |
Note. VLNC = very low nicotine content cigarette; PLA = placebo patch condition; NIC = 42mg nicotine patch condition; MOT = CANTAB motor screening test; CPT-II = Continuous performance test II; DMS = CANTAB delayed matching to sample test; RVP = CANTAB rapid visual information processing test; SRT = CANTAB simple reaction time test; RT = reaction time.
Asterisks indicate significant effects of group (*p < .01). Plus signs indicate significant effects of condition (++ p < .01; +++ p < .001; conditions with different letters (a, b) are significantly different.
1MANOVA results indicated a significant effect of condition on this domain, but follow-up univariate ANOVAs examining effects on each task were not significant.
Visual Sustained Attention
MANOVA results indicated a significant group effect on visual sustained attention measures [Pillai’s Trace = .28, F (2, 37) = 7.08, p < .01, = .28). Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated that SS exhibited poorer RVP detectability (A′) than CS [F (1, 38) = 8.10, p < .01, = .18].There was also a significant main effect of condition on this domain [Pillai’s Trace = .27; F (4, 35) = 3.26, p < .05, = .27]. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated trend-level effects of condition, with medium effect sizes, on each measure [RVP A′: F (1.5, 58.6) = 3.14, p = .06, = .08; CPT omission errors: F (1.7, 65.3) = 2.95, p = .07, = .07]. Across both groups, errors tended to be higher in the VLNC + PLA condition relative to the usual brand or VLNC + NIC conditions (Table 2). The group × condition interaction was not significant.
Visual Working Memory
MANOVA results indicated a significant group effect on visual working memory measures [Pillai’s Trace = .25, F (2, 36) = 5.87, p < .01, = .25]. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated that SS were slower to correctly identify DMS-12 targets than CS [F (1, 37) = 8.66, p < .01, = .19; Table 2]. The Condition main effect and the group × condition interaction were not significant.
Inhibitory Control
MANOVA results indicated a significant condition effect on measures of inhibitory control [Pillai’s Trace = .46, F (4, 36) = 7.58, p < .001, = .46]. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated significant effects of condition on each measure [CPT commission errors: F (2, 78) = 5.23, p < .01, = .12; SRT commission errors: F (1.6, 62.7) = 14.11, p < .001, = .27]. Post hoc comparisons indicated that participants made more CPT commission errors in the VLNC + PLA condition than in the usual brand condition, with errors in the VLNC + NIC condition intermediate between the other two conditions (Table 2). Similarly, participants made significantly more SRT commission errors in the VLNC + PLA condition than in the usual brand or VLNC + NIC conditions, which did not differ from each other. The group main effect and the group × condition interaction were not significant.
Processing Speed
There was a significant condition main effect on measures of processing speed [Wilk’s λ = .67, F (4, 35) = 4.32, p < .01, = .33]. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs indicated significant effects of condition on each measure [RVP latency: F (2, 76) = 5.68, p < .01, = .13; CPT hit RT: F (1.5, 56.4) = 6.12, p < .01, = .14]. Across both groups and in both tasks, processing speed was significantly longer in the VLNC + PLA condition than in the usual brand or VLNC + NIC conditions, which did not differ from each other (Table 2). The group main effect and the group × condition interaction were not significant.
Response Variability
There was a significant main effect of condition on CPT-II hit reaction time standard error [F (2, 110) = 6.82, p < .01, = .11]. Simple effects tests indicated that error variability was significantly higher in the VLNC + PLA condition than in the usual brand or VLNC + NIC conditions, which did not differ from each other (Table 2). The group main effect and the group × condition interaction were not significant.
Discussion
The findings from this study indicate that acute use of VLNC cigarettes, compared to usual-brand cigarettes, negatively affected attention, inhibitory control, processing speed, and response time variability in both SS and CS, and that 42mg NIC reversed this impairment. These findings contrast with our previous report that smoking VLNC cigarettes without co-administration of nicotine was as effective as smoking usual-brand cigarettes at reducing cigarette craving, nicotine withdrawal symptoms and usual-brand smoking in SS and CS.14 As impairments in these domains are thought to have direct implications for the functional outcomes of tobacco users, including smokers with schizophrenia,27 the current findings suggest the need to consider adjunctive nicotine and alternative agents for preservation of cognition in all smokers, should cigarette nicotine content be reduced in the future by regulatory authorities.
The inclusion of non-psychiatric smokers matched on daily smoking rate is an important feature of this study. While some studies have found that SS are more sensitive than CS to the effects of nicotine or smoking manipulations on cognitive measures, these effects differ by task and study. For example, Dépatie et al.22 found that SS were more sensitive than CS to the effects of NIC on a measure of sustained attention (CPT hit rate), but not on measures of inhibitory control or processing speed (CPT commission errors and reaction time). Sacco et al.7 found that SS were more sensitive than CS to the effects of smoking abstinence on visuospatial working memory, but not on sustained attention (CPT II hit rate). Others, as in the present study, have not found that SS and CS differ in their responses to nicotine manipulations.23,28 Some of this inconsistency may be due to different demands of the tasks involved; for example, the visual working memory task in the current study appears less demanding than that used by Sacco et al.7 Thus, more demanding tasks may reveal differential effects of nicotine manipulations on cognitive performance in SS versus CS.
Given the FDA’s interest in a nicotine regulatory policy, it is notable that few studies have examined the acute or long-term effects of VLNC cigarette smoking in people with psychiatric illness, who smoke almost half of the cigarettes consumed in the United States.29 A nicotine regulatory strategy could be particularly effective for helping these smokers quit smoking, and additional research on the potential effects of nicotine regulation on health and psychiatric measures in these smokers is urgently needed to inform the empirical basis for this regulatory policy.
Funding
This research was supported by grant number R01DA14002 to JWT from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and conducted at the Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University, Providence, RI, USA. Salary support for this secondary analysis and manuscript were provided by grants U54DA031659 and P50DA036114 (JWT) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and FDA Center for Tobacco Products (CTP), and grants T32DA016184 (CGA) and K23DA033302 (LCB) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or the Food and Drug Administration. Portions of these data were presented at annual meetings of the College on Problems of Drug Dependence and the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.
Declaration of Interests
None declared.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Amy Adolfo Signore and Netesha Reid for their assistance with data collection, Laura Dionne for her assistance with data management, and the people who volunteered to participate in this study for their contributions.
References
- 1. Hennekens CH, Hennekens AR, Hollar D, Casey DE. Schizophrenia and increased risks of cardiovascular disease. Am Heart J. 2005;150:1115–1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. McClave AK, Whitney N, Thorne SL, et al. Adult tobacco survey—19 States, 2003–2007. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2010;59:1–75 http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss5903a1.htm. Accessed April 3, 2014. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Wing VC, Wass CE, Soh DW, George TP. A review of neurobiological vulnerability factors and treatment implications for comorbid tobacco dependence in schizophrenia. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1248:89–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. George TP, Vessicchio JC, Termine A, et al. Effects of smoking abstinence on visuospatial working memory function in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26:75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Sacco KA, Termine A, Seyal A, et al. Effects of cigarette smoking on spatial working memory and attentional deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:649–659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Weinberger AH, Creeden CL, Sacco KA, George TP. Neurocognitive effects of nicotine and tobacco in individuals with schizophrenia. J Dual Diagn. 2007;3:61–77. [Google Scholar]
- 9. United States Congress. House Committee on Energy and Commerce. Family Smoking Prevention and Tobacco Control Act (H.R. 1256). Washington: U.S. G.P.O, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Ashley DL, Backinger CL, van Bemmel DM, Neveleff DJ. Tobacco regulatory science: research to inform regulatory action at the Food and Drug Administration’s Center for Tobacco Products. Nicotine Tob Res. 2014;16: 1045–1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Benowitz NL, Henningfield JE. Establishing a nicotine threshold for addiction. The implications for tobacco regulation. N Engl J Med. 1994;331:123–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Hatsukami DK, Benowitz NL, Donny E, Henningfield J, Zeller M. Nicotine reduction: strategic research plan. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15:1003–1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Smith RC, Singh A, Infante M, Khandat A, Kloos A. Effects of cigarette smoking and nicotine nasal spray on psychiatric symptoms and cognition in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:479–497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Tidey JW, Rohsenow DJ, Kaplan GB, Swift RM, AhnAllen CG. Separate and combined effects of very low nicotine cigarettes and nicotine replacement in smokers with schizophrenia and controls. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013;15:121–129. 10.1093/ntr/nts098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO. The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict. 1991;86:1119–1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV axis-I Disorders—non-patient edition. New York: Biometric Research Department, 1994.
- 17. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Tidey JW, Rohsenow DJ, Kaplan GB, Swift RM, Adolfo AB. Effects of smoking abstinence, smoking cues and nicotine replacement in smokers with schizophrenia and controls. Nicotine Tob Res. 2008;10:1047–1056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Conners CK.MHS Staff, editors. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II: computer program for windows technical guide and software manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems, 2000.
- 20. Barnett JH, Robbins TW, Leeson VC, Sahakian BJ, Joyce EM, Blackwell AD. Assessing cognitive function in clinical trials in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:1161–1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Schreiber LRN, Grant JE. Association between tobacco smoking and cognitive functioning in young adults. Am J Addict. 2012;21:S14–S19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Dépatie L, O’Driscoll GA, Holahan AV, et al. Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:1056–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Hong LE, Thaker GK, McMahon RP, et al. Effects of moderate-dose treatment with varenicline on neurobiological and cognitive biomarkers in smokers and nonsmokers with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:1195–1206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Myers CS, Robles O, Kakoyannis AN, et al. Nicotine improves delayed recognition in schizophrenic patients. Psychopharmacology. 2004;174:334–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Smith RC, Warner-Cohen J, Matute M, et al. Effects of nicotine nasal spray on cognitive function in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:637–643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Mohamed S, Rosenheck R, Swartz M, Stroup S, Lieberman JA, Keefe RS. Relationship of cognition and psychopathology to functional impairment in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:978–987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Hahn B, Harvey AN, Concheiro-Guisan M, Huestis MA, Holcomb HH, Gold JM. A test of the cognitive self-medication hypothesis of tobacco smoking in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74:436–443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lasser K, Boyd JW, Woolhandler S, Himmelstein DU, McCormick D, Bor DH. Smoking and mental illness: a population-based prevalence study. J Am Med Assoc. 2000;284:2606–2610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]