Abstract
This study examined the formation and quality of attachment of 65 post-institutionalized (PI) toddlers with their parents at 1-3 and 7-9 months post adoption compared to 52 non-adopted (NA) children. The formation of attachment relationships of PI children with adoptive parents occurred relatively quickly. Children exposed to greater pre-adoption adversity took longer to form an attachment to their adoptive parents, although by 7-9 months post adoption, nearly all (90%) of the children achieved the highest level on an attachment formation rating scale. PI children did not differ from NA children in attachment security, based either on the Attachment Q-Sort or Strange Situation categorical scoring. However, the PI children were more likely to be disorganized in their attachment pattern. Pre-adoption adversity was related to lower Q-sort security scores especially at the initial assessment 1-3 months post adoption. The results indicated that attachment formation and attachment quality in PI children are differentiable constructs with different precursors.
Human attachment is conceptualized as a robust survival mechanism that maintains proximity between a vulnerable infant and protective caregiver and ensures the development of critical regulatory capacities required for learning and independent social functioning (Bowlby 1969/1982). For some infants, however, adverse caregiving environments disrupt or fail to support the development of relational mechanisms (Cicchetti, 2006; Cicchetti & Carlson, 1989; De Bellis, 2005. According to data gathered between 2006 and 2010 (Child Welfare Gateway, 2012), over 500,000 children in the United States experience emotional or physical neglect with detrimental effects on a broad range of developmental outcomes. Institutional rearing, characterized by regimented care, high child-to-caregiver ratios, and frequent caregiver changes, further deprives children of stable, reciprocal caregiver interactions (an “average expectable environment”) required for normal development. The study of children adopted internationally from institutions provides an opportunity to address important questions regarding the effects of chronic early neglect on development and the possibility of recovery following social deprivation. In the current study, the formation of attachment is examined in post-institutionalized (PI) toddlers and their adoptive parents following early social deprivation.
Attachment Development
Although infants enter the world with a biologically-based propensity for social interaction, initiating behaviors that promote infant-caregiver proximity reflexively and without intention (e.g., crying, orienting, clinging; Ainsworth, Bell & Stayton, 1974), the initial repertoire of infant attachment behavior does not provide for the capacity to adapt or survive alone (Bowlby, 1969/1982). The emergence of the attachment system requires reasonable access to adult caregiving co-adapted to infant behaviors in an organized and organizing caregiving environment (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Waters, Kondo-Ikemura, Posada, & Richters, 1991).
During the first year, typically, infants increasingly organize communications and behaviors to serve the formation of attachment and exploration in the course of caregiver-orchestrated interaction and routine care (Sroufe & Waters, 1977). When caregiving structure and supportive signals are absent or markedly discrepant from a species-typical environment, attachment and related systems may be disrupted, or not properly organized, and may require more time and environmental organization than in non-deprived infants (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Waters et al., 1991). Social attachments in animals who do not receive the normal or expected environmental input show fundamental deviations in social interaction across the lifespan (Harlow & Suomi, 1970; Insel, 1997).
There are three primary dimensions that have been considered and assessed to study the development of attachment in humans. Attachment formation concerns the extent to which the child differentiates familiar adults from others, has preference for and orients towards a particular caregiver (Zeanah, Smyke, Koga, Carlson, & The BEIP Group, 2005). This aspect is a prerequisite to establishing a close bond with a caregiver and has been mostly studied with respect to atypical populations such as children living in institutional settings, for instance by using the Attachment Formation Rating developed by Carlson (Carlson, 2002/2011; Zeanah et al., 2005). The extremes of this dimension are a complete lack of differentiation between familiar and unfamiliar adults at the low end and a discriminate attachment at the ideal end of the spectrum. Attachment organization is the second dimension examined by developmental studies focusing on this process and at the ideal end of the spectrum it denotes a coherent pattern of child behaviors and strategies which maintain proximity to the caregiver, particularly when confronted with distressing or fear-inducing events (Carlson, 1998). At the low end of this dimension, attachment is considered “disorganized” and children exhibit inexplicably disoriented or conflicted behaviors towards their caregivers, including unusual behaviors such as freezing, all of which are considered to be a result of frightening or frightened parental behavior (Main & Hesse, 1990). These behaviors have been assessed using laboratory procedures such as the Strange Situation, which monitors reactions to separations from and reunions with the caregiver (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; Main & Solomon, 1990). Attachment security is the third dimension of attachment investigated when infants are deemed to have already formed an attachment relationship that is both discriminate and organized. Infants experiencing sensitive and responsive care use their caregivers as a secure base to resort to in times of distress (thus their attachment pattern is deemed secure), whereas infants experiencing inconsistent or insensitive caregiving exhibit insecure patterns -insecure avoidant or insecure resistant- which consist of either withdrawal of bids for attention towards unavailable caregivers or excessive crying and clinging to inconsistent caregivers (Ainsworth et al., 1978). These secure/insecure classifications can be deduced using the same Strange Situation procedure described or other methods, such as the Attachment Q-sort (Waters & Deane, 1985). We examined all three of these dimensions in the present study and below we briefly review prior studies on the development of attachment in children experiencing early deprivation by referring back to these dimensions and their operationalizations.
A global survey of the prior literature reveals that even following aberrant and abusive conditions of care, such as neglectful institutional care, the majority of human infants are likely to form attachments to new caregivers. Moreover, children who experience sensitive responsive care and form secure relationships with adoptive parents exhibit more positive social development in middle childhood and adolescence (Jaffari-Bimmel, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Mooijaart, 2006; Stams, Juffer, & van IJzendoorn, 2002). Despite the apparent resilience and flexibility of the attachment system, for some infants, specific socio-emotional difficulties may linger years after placement in an adequate caregiving environment (e.g., MacLean, 2003; O'Connor, Marvin, Rutter, Olrick, & Britner, 2003; Rutter & the ERA Study Team, 1998). Developmental challenges may include understanding social boundaries in interactions with strangers (Chisholm, Carter, Ames, & Morison, 1995; Rutter et al., 2007; Zeanah, Smyke, & Dumitrescu, 2002), inhibitory control (Bruce, Tarullo, & Gunnar, 2009), emotion understanding (Wismer Fries & Pollak, 2004), difficulties in interactions and relationships with peers (Vorria, Rutter, Pickles, Wolkind, & Hobsbaum, 1998), and under- or over-reactions to environmental stimuli (e.g., Ames, 1997; Beckett et al., 2002; Zeanah et al., 2002).
Attachment among Institutionalized Children
Early descriptive studies of attachment and post-institutionalized care reported that children were unable to form attachments to new caregivers following a period of early deprivation (e.g., Goldfarb, 1945). However, this research was often based on small samples and included children with histories of multiple foster care placements prior to assessment. Consistent with attachment theory, recent studies of institutionalized children highlight the need for caregiving stability for the formation of attachment of any quality. For example, whereas children living in typical Romanian institutions characterized by high staff turnover and high child-to-caregiver ratios (10-20:1) were not likely to demonstrate a preferred caregiver, lower ratios (4 children to one caregiver) and consistency favored the emergence of a preferred caregiver for 90% of children, compared to 59% in standard institutional care (Zeanah et al., 2002). Nevertheless, having a preferred caregiver cannot be equated with secure or even organized attachment quality. Across studies addressing the effects of institutional care on attachment, using the Strange Situation Procedure (Ainsworth et al., 1978) or a modification, institutionalized children showed high rates of insecure attachment and especially high rates of disorganized attachment (Dobrova-Krol, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Juffer, 2010; Steele, Steele, Jin, Archer, & Herreros, 2009; The St Petersburg-USA Orphanage Research Team, 2008; Vorria et al., 2003; Zeanah et al., 2005).
These studies highlight the conceptual and methodological challenges of studying attachment development in institutionalized children with assessments (e.g., the Strange Situation Procedure or the Attachment Q-sort) created for and validated with normative samples of children with histories of stable reciprocal caregiving experiences. Developed to address these issues, the Attachment Formation Rating (Carlson, 2002/2011; Zeanah et al., 2005) captures the evolution of secure base behavior in caregiving relationships based on ethological and naturalistic observations (Ainsworth, 1967; Tinbergen, 1951/1974). When the rating was applied in an institutional setting in the Bucharest Early Intervention Project, over 65% of children were assigned ratings corresponding to rudimentary and fragmented displays of attachment behavior rather than fully formed or traditional attachment patterns (Zeanah et al., 2005).
Studies of post-institutionalized adopted children have faced the same challenge of attempting to measure and interpret atypical attachment patterns in the absence of specifically designed measures. Early research incorporating parent reports found 80% of caregivers reporting their children attached to them within a year of adoption from London nurseries (Hodges & Tizard, 1989; Tizard & Hodges, 1978; Tizard & Rees, 1974, 1975). Based on more systematic parent questionnaires (using Attachment Q-sort items; Waters & Deane, 1985), post-institutionalized children scored significantly lower on attachment security compared to non-adopted controls approximately 11 months post adoption (Chisholm et al., 1995), with no significant group differences three years later (Chisholm, 1998).
Using observational measures (Attachment Q-Sort [Waters & Deane, 1985] and a Strange Situation standard procedure or modified procedure with the MacArthur Preschool Coding System [Cassidy & Marvin with the MacArthur Working Group, 1992]), a meta-analysis of 17 studies (N = 772) found a significant risk of insecure attachment for children adopted after their first birthday (van den Dries, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2009). Risk of insecure disorganized attachment was found regardless of age of adoption.
The Bucharest Early Intervention Project examined attachment outcomes of institutionalized children randomly assigned to foster care or usual (initially institutional) care (Smyke, Zeanah, Fox, Nelson, & Guthrie, 2010). Using Strange Situation assessments and preschool coding methods, 49.2% of children placed in foster homes displayed secure attachment patterns 11-36 months post placement (Smyke et al., 2010). Due to the delay in measurement, it is difficult to know how quickly these attachments formed.
In another examination of attachment formation in foster care children born and raised in the United States, attachment behaviors were recorded by foster parents using the Parent Attachment Diary daily for two months after placement (Stovall & Dozier, 2000; Stovall-McClough & Dozier, 2004) and it appeared that the majority of infants had formed a consistent attachment pattern within two months, with infants who were younger than 12 months at placement or who had foster parents with secure attachment representations having a higher likelihood of forming secure attachments earlier and displaying higher levels of security overall (Stovall-McClough & Dozier, 2004). Given this pattern of relatively rapid attachment formation, it would be useful to examine this question in a sample of post-institutionalized children.
Research on attachment formation within the first year of post-institutional experience is rare. However, one study found children adopted at 13 months significantly less likely to be securely attached compared to normative distributions (48% and 42% at 2 and 6 months post adoption, respectively) with significantly more disorganization (31% and 40%, respectively; van den Dries, Juffer, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Alink, 2012). The generalizability of these results is limited due to restricted ethnic diversity and younger age of adoption.
In summary, research on attachment formation following severe deprivation suggests that most children form attachments following early institutional care, and for children adopted early (before 12 months of age), attachment security with adoptive caregivers is comparable to that of non-adopted peers (van den Dries et al., 2009). However, children adopted after their first birthday are more likely to show increased insecurity as well as disorganized or atypical relationship patterns with new caregivers. What is not known is the course of the formation of these new relationships, especially for older children, and the influence of the quality of pre-adoptive experience on attachment development.
The current study addresses these questions by examining the development of attachment in toddlers with adoptive parents following social deprivation. The study examines a) the formation of attachment in the first months post adoption (i.e., at 1-3 months and 7-9 months), b) the relations between attachment formation and indices of attachment security during the period when attachment relationships are forming, and c) the association of a cumulative measure of pre-adoption adversity with attachment formation and attachment quality following adoption. Attachment measures include a) an assessment of attachment formation, b) the Q-Sort attachment measure sensitive to behavior beyond infancy, and c) the standard laboratory attachment assessment, the Strange Situation, sensitive to both organized and disorganized patterns.
Methods
Participants
Demographics
Participants were 65 Post-institutionalized (PI) children selected based on international adoption between 15 and 36 months (M = 23.9 months, SD = 5 months) and 52 Non-adopted (NA) children raised in their birth families in the United States. PI children had experienced institutional care in the form of living in an orphanage or baby home overseas for 4 - 34 months (M = 18.1, SD = 7.6; 14% to 100% of pre-adoptive life, median = 94.3%). They were recruited from the Minnesota International Adoption Clinic, located at the University of Minnesota Hospital, and from the Minnesota International Adoption Project Registry, which draws from a diverse sampling of adoption agencies throughout Minnesota. The International Adoption Clinic provides services such as the initial medical visit after adoption, which is recommended for all families to establish the child's baseline physical health. All internationally adopted children who did not meet exclusion criteria (described in more detail below), who were recently adopted into Minnesota during the course of data collection, were in our target age range, and were interested in participating in this research were recruited. The PI and NA groups were matched as closely as possibly by age and sex (no differences in age, t(115) = 1.35, n.s. or sex distribution, χ2 (1) = .50, n.s.; percent female was 58.5% and 51.9 % for PI and NA, respectively), thus we did not control for age or sex in analyses of group differences. The PI children were adopted from 15 countries (21.5% Russia/Eastern Europe, 38.5% Asia, 30.8% Africa and 9.2% Latin America). The NA group was selected to roughly match the typically high socioeconomic status of internationally adoptive parents. The median household annual income was $100-125,000 for the PI group and $75-100,000 for the NA group. Approximately 84.6% and 92% of primary caregivers had completed a college degree in the PI and NA group, respectively.
Exclusion criteria
Recruitment exclusion criteria included congenital or neurological disorders (including seizure disorders, endocrine disorders, heart defects); information regarding these diagnoses was collected from parents during phone recruitment, in questionnaires at the time of assessment, and through contact with the pediatric clinic in the United States where the child was first seen. Additionally, in order to focus on the effect of deprivation during the first postnatal years, PI participants were excluded from analyses if they exhibited facial signs of prenatal alcohol exposure (n = 6) and extremely low IQ (n = 1). Facial signs of fetal alcohol exposure were coded from digital photographs obtained at the second assessment described here and at each of three subsequent sessions approximately 6-7 months apart. After being trained by a licensed clinician on obtaining precise and scorable photos, researchers collected three standardized facial photographs at each session: a frontal view, 3/4 view, and lateral view. The Astley FAS Facial Photographic Analysis Software was then used by trained coders to provide each child with a Facial 4-Digit Code Rank. Coders attained .84 average reliability (using 14% of randomly selected photos) with a licensed clinician using percent agreement on whether the photo was scored to have absent to low concern (code of “1” or “2”) or moderate to high FAS concern (code or “3” or “4”). Children with high FAS concern (codes of “3” or “4”) were excluded from the present analysis and parents were informed of the results of this screening if they elected to be notified during the consent process. Two NA participants were excluded from analyses due to parent-reported Autism diagnosis (n = 1) and child abuse (n = 1) in order to maintain a non-abused, typically-developing comparison group.
Missing data
Missing attachment data were minimal. For Time 1, one PI videotaped assessment was not available due to technical problems. For Time 2, data were missing for 6 individuals due to problems in scheduling and withdrawal from the study (PI: n = 4, NA: n = 2). Participants who withdrew were more likely to be coded as insecurely attached at Time 1 (66.7% insecure, comprised of 3 PI and 1 NA) compared to rates in the entire sample. Given that missing data were under 5% for the main study variables, multiple imputation methods were not used as estimates were not likely to change with replacement.
Procedure
Each parent-child dyad participated in 1.5-hour laboratory sessions at two time points (1-3 months and 7-9 months post-adoption). The sessions were videotaped and consisted of 10 segments: a 10-minute Disinhibited Social Approach procedure, in which the parent was discouraged from interacting with the child and the experimenter was present, being neutral initially but increasingly friendly; a modified Strange Situation procedure including one separation/reunion at Time 1 (6 minutes) and two standard separation/reunions at Time 2 (15 minutes); two parent-child free- and structured-play interactions, lasting 10 minutes each; a 2-minute book-reading episode; and four Lab-TAB (i.e., Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery) vignettes (two eliciting fear and two for positive affect). It must be noted that all these segments were watched to derive the Q-sort Attachment Security scores, but these episodes were not otherwise utilized in the present manuscript. The primary caregiver was invited to participate with the designated toddler. The same caregiver was asked to participate at Times 1 and 2 (PI: 94.1% mothers, NA: 98.1% mothers).
Measures
Attachment Formation Rating
Attachment formation ratings were derived from a modified Strange Situation Procedure at Time 1 (one separation/reunion) and the standard procedure (two separations/reunions) at Time 2 (Ainsworth et al., 1978). The standardized laboratory procedure consists of eight brief episodes designed to activate infant attachment behavior through an increasingly stressful series of infant-mother separations and reunions. Assessments were coded from videotape by expert raters unaware of child group status. The Attachment Formation 5-point rating (Carlson, 2002/2011; Zeanah et al., 2005) documented the degree to which children had developed a preferred relationship with the primary caregiver. Rating development was based on natural observations of infant attachment development with a primary caregiver (Ainsworth, 1967). Ratings of “5” indicated attachment behavioral organization consistent with traditional attachment classifications (Secure, Anxious Avoidant, Anxious Resistant, Disorganized/Disoriented; Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main & Solomon, 1990). Ratings of “4” indicated evidence of attachment figure preference in the context of distress, but incomplete patterning of attachment behavior. Ratings of “3”, “2”, and “1” were assigned for behavioral displays of fragmented attachment behavioral sequences differentially directed toward or in response to the caregiver, isolated attachment signals and responses, and no evidence of attachment behavior, respectively. Attachment Formation ratings have been significantly related to caregiving behavior as well as indices of attachment disturbance in an institutionalized sample (Zeanah et al., 2005). Interrater reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient) was .96 for an institutionalized sample (n = 45; Zeanah et al., 2005) and .77 for the current sample (n = 10).
Attachment Q-sort
A continuous rating of attachment security was derived from videotaped assessments of the entire laboratory session using the Attachment Q-sort (Waters & Deane, 1985). The Attachment Q-sort is appropriate for use with children between 12-48 months, and based on meta-analyses, the measure has been found to be reliably associated with Strange Situation classification security, when used by trained coders rather than parents, and significantly related to parental sensitivity (van Ijzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakersmans-Kranenburg, & Riksen-Walraven, 2004). The Q-sort method minimizes response bias through a process of item sort into a fixed distribution based on the salience of child behaviors (Waters & Deane, 1985). Additionally, the instrument is compatible with a diversity of child-parent interactions such as those included in the one-hour laboratory session. Of the 90 Q-Sort items, 13 home-based items were determined to be non-applicable to the experimental situation and were treated as missing data for all participants. In addition, 10 items were re-worded without changing the description of the child behavior captured by the item (e.g., when new adults “are present” instead of “visit the home”). Flexibility in the Q-sort methodology allowed a change from the classic sorting procedure (10 equal piles of 9 items) to a symmetric quasi-normal distribution of 77 items (8 items in piles 1, 2, 8, and 9, and 9 items in remaining piles). The Q-sort adaptation was developed with German Posada (Purdue University).
Videotaped assessments were coded by two master coders and eight trained coders blind to child status. Master coders were trained to reliability by German Posada, attaining good reliability on the three standard Attachment Q-sort Training videotapes. Average reliabilities with the certified trainer for the two master coders were Pearson r = .68 and .62 based on the 90-item Q-set, and .55 and .57 based on a sample of two tapes from the current study (not included in current analyses) using the selected 77 items (note: these reliabilities can be considered very high, considering that the minimum acceptable inter-rater reliability for two Q-sort distributions derived from independent observations is of r = .30 according to Gjerde, 1986; Wampler, Halverson, Moore, & Walters, 1989). For videotapes from the present study, internal inter-rater reliabilities among pairs of trained coders averaged r = .60.
The security score for each child was calculated as the value of the Pearson correlation between the child's Q-profile and the Criterion Security Q-sort (a composite of attachment expert ratings). Even though there is no natural cut-off score for the Q-sort, the developer of the instrument has suggested that a cut-off score of .30 can sometimes be used for dichotomizing the security variable (secure >=.30; insecure <.30) if it yields comparable percentages of secure and insecure children observed in other samples (Park & Waters, 1989). At Time 1, a cut-off of .30 resulted in 69.2% secure children in the non-adopted, typically developing group, which does not differ from the average estimated distribution across U.S. samples of 67% secure and 33% insecure (van IJzendoorn & Sagi-Schwartz, 2008). Since Q-sort scores increase with age (Clark & Symons, 2000; Symons, Clark, Isaksen, & Marshall, 1998) due to the fact that the instrument measures social behaviors that typically improve with age (e.g., responding readily to mother's suggestions, waiting patiently for mother to do what the child asks, etc.), we used a cut-off of .43 at Time 2 to preserve the same distribution in the NA group (70% secure, 30% insecure). Using different cut-offs at each age for dichotomizing longitudinal assessments in the same children are customary in the literature to account for these improvements with age (e.g., Symons et al., 1998).
Attachment Classification
Traditional attachment classifications were derived from the modified Strange Situation Procedure at Time 1 (one separation/reunion) and the standard procedure (two separations/reunions) at Time 2 (Ainsworth et al., 1978; see description under Attachment Formation Rating). We used a modified (single separation) procedure at Time 1 instead of the standard recommended set of two separations to prevent undue stress on the families and children who were, in many cases, undergoing a challenging family transition 1-3 months after adoption. Individual differences in attachment relationships were coded with respect to the child's gaining comfort in the mother's presence when stressed and using the mother as a secure base from which to explore. Based on the patterning of the child's behavior across eight episodes, mother-child dyads were assigned to one of four major classifications: secure, anxious avoidant, anxious resistant, disorganized/disoriented (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main & Solomon, 1990). Additionally, continuous scores for the Proximity Seeking, Contact Maintenance, Resistance, Avoidance, and Disorganization subscales of this coding system based on the first reunion (Time 1 and Time 2) or the second reunion (only at Time 2) were also compared using t-tests to gain further insight into the development of attachment. Strange Situation secure versus insecure (avoidant or resistant) classification was 72% concordant with the secure/insecure category based on continuous Q-sort security when excluding children with disorganized scores, since the attachment Q-sort does not differentiate organized from disorganized patterns. Behavioral ratings and classifications were assigned from video recordings by independent expert coders (E. Carlson, A. Sroufe, University of Minnesota) blind to child status and remaining data.
Index of Pre-adoption Adversity
A Pre-adoption Index of Adversity was created from information gathered through a phone interview with caregivers within the first year after adoption. Interviews were conducted by a retired international adoption social worker. Parents were instructed to have materials provided by the adoption agency available during the interview. Using information supplied by agencies and gathered when parents traveled to the birth country to pick up the child, the interview yielded the following measures used in the current study: age at adoption, duration of institutional care, number of care settings experienced, reports of severe neglect or abuse prior to institutional placement. Based on caregiver observations of institutions (n = 49), the interviewer also rated the extent to which the child's needs for social contact with adults were met in the institution on a 5-point scale from “poorly” to “well met”. To provide a measure of reliability, 10 interview scenarios were constructed describing institutional care of various qualities. The interviewer and a former international adoption social worker read the scenarios and independently used the information to rate institutional quality. The kappa's were >.80 on all the scales.
In addition, child health at adoption was assessed from medical records obtained from the child's physician as an objective index of pre-adoption adversity. A health composite variable (i.e., number of health risks at adoption) was created. A score of 1 was assigned for the presence of each health-related risk or problem, including: low birth weight, growth delay greater than 2 standard deviations from the mean, evidence of malnutrition, motor delay, psychological/ occupational therapy or speech referrals, chronic disability, and each medical referral obtained. Non-incidences and missing data on risk factors were scored as 0. These values were summed to create a possible range of 0-30 for the composite (i.e., index of neglect). In this study, scores ranged from 0-8 (M = 3.95, SD = 1.9).
The cumulative adversity score was created using variables expected to impact attachment. In the present study, children were 15 months or older at the time they entered full-time adoptive care. However, based on evidence that adoption at two years or older may make formation of a secure/non-disordered attachment more difficult for post-institutionalized children (Smyke et al., 2010; van den Dries et al., 2012), being two or older was assigned one point in the adversity index. Reports of severe neglect or abuse yielded one point. One point each was assigned for extended institutionalization (over half of toddler's life), quality of social care in the institution (bottom half of the distribution), and number of transitions or major care settings (top half of the distribution, or 3 or more settings) prior to adoption. Finally, one point was assigned for 3 or more health problems (top half of the distribution) at adoption. To be scored, children required 3 of the 6 measures to be available. The resulting index ranged from 0 to 6, with a median of 2. See Table 1 for descriptive statistics of pre-adoption adversity indices.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Pre-adoption Adversity Indices.
Frequency | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Range | 0 | 1 | |
Age at adoption (months) | 23.9 | 5 | 15 – 36 | ||
Percentage of pre-adoption life in institutional care | 77% | 29% | 14 – 100 | ||
Rating of social care in institution (1-5 scale) | 3.53 | 1.2 | 1.00 – 5 | ||
Number of major care settings | 2.42 | .85 | 1 – 5 | ||
Number of health risks at adoption | 3.95 | 1.9 | 0 - 8 | ||
Abuse or neglect | 54 | 3 |
Results
Attachment Formation
At Time 1, all but one of the NA children were classified as having a fully formed attachment relationship (i.e., a score of “5”), and all NA children received a score of “5” at Time 2. As shown in Table 2, at Time 1 over half of PI children did not exhibit a fully formed attachment relationship, a significant difference from the NA group. By Time 2 nearly all had formed attachment relationships, and significant differences from the NA group were no longer apparent.
Table 2. Attachment Formation Scores for Post Institutionalized (PI) Children.
Time 1 | Time 2 | |
---|---|---|
Rating of “3” | 10 (15.6%) | 0 (0%) |
Rating of “4” | 30 (46.9%) | 6 (9.8%) |
Rating of “5” | 24 (37.5%) | 55 (90.2%) |
χ2 (2) = 8.12, p = .017
Note: Time 1 = 1-3 months post adoption; Time 2 = 7-9 months post adoption
There was a significant relation between Time 1 and 2 Attachment Formation scores among the PI children, χ2 (2) = 8.12, p = .017, such that all children scoring “5” at Time 1 also scored a “5” at Time 2 and all of the six children scoring a “4” at Time 2 had scored a “3” or a “4” at Time 1. At neither time point did PI children score below “3”.
Although children were assessed within 3 months of their arrival in the US, some families needed to stay with their children in the birth country for a time before bringing the child home. As a result, the range of post-institutional time with the adoptive family ranged from 2 to 18.5 weeks. Children scoring “5” (attached) on the Attachment Formation Rating at Time 1 tended to have been with their families longer than children scoring 3 or 4, t(62) = 1.96, p = .055, partial η2 = .06 at the first assessment.
Attachment Security: Attachment Q-Sort (Time 1 and Time 2)
As shown in Table 3, Q-sort attachment security did not differ by group at either Time 1 or Time 2 assessment (Time 1: MPI = .31, SD = .23, MNA = .38, SD = .18; Time 2: MPI = .46, SD = .15, MNA = .46, SD = .16). A repeated measures analysis of the continuous attachment scores also revealed no significant group difference, F(1, 108) = .74, p = .39, except a main effect of Time, F(1,108) = 28.76, p < .001, partial η2 = .21. The Time interaction was not significant, F(1,108) = 2.27, p = .135, partial η2 = .02.
Table 3. Percent Post-institutionalized and Non-adopted Securely Attached Toddlers Based on Attachment Q-Sort.
PI | NA | χ2 (1) | |
---|---|---|---|
Time 1 | 36 (56.25%) | 36 (69.2%) | 2.05, p = .15 |
Time 2 | 43 (70.5%) | 35 (70%) | 0.003, p = .96 |
Note: Post-Institutionalized Toddlers (PI); Non-adopted Toddlers (NA). Frequencies are presented first in each cell, with percentages in parenthesis.
Attachment Classification: Strange Situation
The distribution of attachment classifications was examined at Time 2 when the majority of the PI group had formed an attachment, yielding meaningful categorical data (see Table 4). At 9 months post adoption, the PI and NA groups differed significantly in attachment classification distribution (χ2(4) = 10.17, p = .038), primarily due to significant differences in rates of disorganized versus organized attachment (χ2(1) = 5.89, p = .015). One NA child was unclassifiable using this scheme (not included in Table 4). Aside from the differences in the disorganized category, there was no overall difference in secure versus insecure classification. Thus, the difference between groups is restricted to attachment disorganization.
Table 4. Frequency (and percentage) Distribution of Attachment Categories at Time 2 (7-9 months post adoption).
Group | Avoidant (A) | Organized Secure (B) | Resistant (C) | Disorganized (D) |
---|---|---|---|---|
PI | 3 (4.9%) | 42 (68.9%) | 2 (3.3%) | 14 (22.9%) |
NA | 0 (0%) | 44 (89.8%) | 2 (4.1%) | 3 (6.1%) |
Note: Post-Institutionalized Toddlers (PI); Non-adopted Toddlers (NA)
Four-way comparison: χ2(4) = 10.17, p = .038;
Organized/disorganized comparison: χ2(1) = 5.89, p = .015
When examining the continuous Strange Situation rating scales (Proximity Seeking, Contact Maintenance, Avoidance, Resistance, and Disorganization) for the two reunion episodes to compare the two groups at Time 2, PI children exhibited significantly more Proximity Seeking and Contact Maintenance during the first (t(107) = 2.45, p = .016; t(106.8) = 2.30, p = .023) and second reunion episodes (t(96) = 2.58, p = .01; t(94.8) = 3.05, p = .003) compared to NA children and the PIs also displayed significantly more signs of disorganization (t(104) = 3.0, p = .003), consistent with the results already described (see Table 5 for means and standard errors). The elevation in Proximity Seeking and Contact Maintenance may be understood in the context of developmental trends from Time 1 to Time 2, such that PI and NA children tended to exhibit less Contact Maintenance (PI: t(57) = 2.32, p = .02; NA: t(47) = 3.2, p =.003) and NAs also showed significantly less Proximity Seeking (NA: t(47) = 2.37, p =.02) with their parent during the first reunion of the session as they got older (comparisons could not be made for the second reunion episode given that only the Time 2 assessment included two separations). Thus it may be that PI children show a more immature (i.e., elevated) pattern of proximity-seeking and contact-maintenance for their age at both time points (Table 5). Furthermore, t-tests revealed that PI children showed significantly more signs of Avoidance and Resistance than NAs during the first reunion at Time 1 but did not differ from non-adopted children on these dimensions at Time 2 (Table 5).
Table 5. Mean scores on Strange Situation subscales by group, time of assessment (T1 or T2) and reunion episode.
PI Mean | PI SE | NA Mean | NA SE | Difference Significant | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
T1 Reunion I Proximity Seeking | 3.32 | 0.23 | 3.18 | 0.24 | |
T1 Reunion I Contact Maintenance | 2.69 | 0.23 | 2.25 | 0.26 | |
T1 Reunion I Resistance | 1.74 | 0.18 | 1.18 | 0.09 | ** |
T1 Reunion I Avoidance | 2.87 | 0.21 | 2.08 | 0.20 | ** |
T1 Disorganization Rating | 3.29 | 0.28 | 1.57 | 0.15 | ** |
T2 Reunion I Proximity seeking | 3.02 | 0.21 | 2.27 | 0.21 | * |
T2 Reunion I Contact Maintenance | 2.10 | 0.19 | 1.51 | 0.16 | * |
T2 Reunion I Resistance | 1.27 | 0.10 | 1.43 | 0.15 | |
T2 Reunion I Avoidance | 2.42 | 0.17 | 2.08 | 0.18 | |
T2 Reunion II Proximity seeking | 3.29 | 0.20 | 2.49 | 0.21 | * |
T2 Reunion II Contact Maintenance | 2.75 | 0.22 | 1.81 | 0.18 | ** |
T2 Reunion II Resistance | 1.40 | 0.13 | 1.29 | 0.13 | |
T2 Reunion II Avoidance | 2.31 | 0.19 | 1.90 | 0.17 | |
T2 Disorganization Rating | 2.88 | 0.24 | 1.87 | 0.22 | ** |
SE = standard error.
Difference between PI and NA means is considered significant at p < .05.
p < .05;
p < .01.
Attachment Formation and Attachment Quality
Although the Q-sort measures of security did not differ by group, we examined whether they differed by Time 1 attachment formation rating. The goal was to establish the predictive value of the Time 1 Attachment Formation ratings for either security or organization. PI children were grouped according to their attachment rating at Time 1 yielding 4 groups (PI: 3 versus 4 versus 5 and NAs). Using a repeated measures analysis of the Q-sort measure at Time 1 and 2, a significant interaction of group and time was found, F(3, 106) = 4.31, p =.007, partial η2= .11. At both Time 1 and 2, children scoring 4 or 5 on the attachment formation scale did not differ in Q-sort measures of attachment security from the NA children. At Time 1 PI children scoring below 4 on attachment formation scored lower on Q-sort security than NA children (see Table 6 for means). PI children who were classified as organized (ABC) versus disorganized (D) at Time 2 did not differ in their Q-sort security scores at Time 1 (t(58)=1.06, p = .29) or Time 2 (t(59) = 1.08, p = .28). In the PI group, Attachment Formation at T1 was marginally positively correlated with being classified as secure (versus insecure) according to the SSP at T2: rho(58) = .24, p = .068, but T1 formation was not significantly associated with being classified as disorganized (versus organized): rho(58) = -.18, p = .21.
Table 6. Attachment Q-Sort Security Mean Scores as a Function of Attachment Formation (AF) at Time 1 (3 months post adoption).
Assessment | AF score “5” n = 23 | Post-Institution AF score “4” n = 28 |
AF score “3” n = 9 | Non-adopted AF score “5” n = 50 |
---|---|---|---|---|
Time 1 | .32 (.04) | .39 (.04) | .14 (.06) | .38 (.03) |
Time 2 | .49 (.03) | .45 (.03) | .45 (.05) | .46 (.02) |
Note: Post-Institutionalized Toddlers (PI); Non-adopted Toddlers (NA). Standard errors are presented in parenthesis adjacent to each mean.
Main effect of time: F(1,106) = 43.4, p < .001, partial η2= .29.
Group by time interaction: F(3, 106) = 4.31, p < .007, partial η2= .11.
Pre-adoption Factors and Attachment
Prior to examining whether pre-adoption adversity affected attachment formation or quality, we examined the effect of birth region in these analyses. Pre-adoption adversity was found to vary by birth region, F(3,61) = 2.83, p = .046, with post-hoc tests indicating that the children adopted from Africa (n = 20, M = 1.7, SD = .98) scored significantly lower (p = .002) – i.e., having experienced less adversity- than children from Russia/Eastern Europe (n = 14, M = 2.7, SD = 1.14) with the children from Asia (n = 25, M = 2.24, SD = .93) and Latin America (n = 6, M = 2.17, SD = 1.17) scoring in between and not differing significantly from each other or from the other regions. Birth region was rank ordered by adversity score and entered as a control variable in subsequent analyses.
Attachment Formation
To address whether pre-adoption adversity predicted length of time required to achieve a fully-formed attachment to the adoptive parent, Time 1 and Time 2 Attachment Formation scores were re-coded and combined. A score of “1” indicated attachment formation (score = “5”) at Time 1; a score of “2” indicated attachment formation at Time 2, and “3” indicated that even at Time 2 the child was scoring below “5”. The distribution on this measure was: “1” = 41%, “2” = 49% and “3” = 10%. After controlling for birth region, F(1, 60) = 10.48, p = .002, the main effect for Time to Form Attachment was significant, F(2,60) = 5.8, p = .005, η2 = .17. The pre-adoption adversity scores for the three groups were: Formed by Time 1 (n = 25, M = 1.96, SD = 1.02), Formed by Time 2 (n = 30, M = 2.13, SD = .97), and Not Formed by Time 2 (n = 6, M = 3.17, SD = .98), with the third group having significantly higher levels of adversity compared to the first one (p = .001) and the second (p = .002).
Attachment Quality
First the association between adversity and Q-sort security was examined controlling for birth region. The partial correlation was significant for Time 1 security such that higher adversity was associated with lower security, r = -.31, df = 57, p = .015, but while in the same direction, the association was not significant at Time 2, r = -.17, df = 58, p = .20. An ANCOVA was computed to examine pre-adoption adversity in relation to the measure of disorganized attachment at the Time 2 attachment assessment (i.e., Strange Situation). The main effect of disorganized attachment was not significant, F(1,58) = .12, p = .73.
Lastly, we examined age at adoption as a separate predictive factor given extensive prior work linking it to socio-emotional outcomes in this population (Smyke et al., 2010; van den Dries et al., 2012). Age at adoption was significantly inversely correlated with Attachment Formation at Time 1 (rho = -.45, p = .04), but associations with Attachment Formation at Time 2 (rho = -.21, p = .08) and Q-sort Attachment security at either Time 1 or Time 2 were not statistically significant (r(69) = .11, p = .37 and r(66) = -.12, p = .32). For a graphical illustration of the gradient in Time 1 Attachment Formation scores by age at adoption, Figure 1 displays these scores by age quartiles.
Discussion
The present report provides critical information regarding the development of attachment relationships in toddlers adopted from institutional care. The results indicate that attachment formation and attachment security in post-institutionalized children are differentiable constructs with different precursors. Specifically, attachment formation appears to be based largely on exposure. It was encouraging to find that even for children who had lived their early lives in institutions with multiple caregivers and few, if any, opportunities to form attachment relationships, the attachment relationship to an adoptive parent formed relatively quickly, within 9 months. At Time 1, not all children had fully formed an attachment pattern, but stability of care predicted formation. At Time 1 those who had been with the parent for fewer days or weeks were less likely to have already formed an attachment than those who had been with the parent for a longer period. Adverse pre-adoption experience also appeared to slow the attachment formation process.
Attachment quality provided a different picture. Post-institutionalized children did not differ from non-adopted children in attachment security (secure base behavior) although the pattern was more likely to be “disorganized”. Length of time with the adoptive parent did not predict attachment security. However, adversity experienced prior to placement was related to lower Q-sort security score especially at the initial assessment 1-3 months post adoption.
Attachment Formation and Quality
The results provide evidence of relatively rapid attachment formation following institutional care for the majority of the children. Indeed, nearly 40% had formed an attachment within 3 months of placement in a family, with nearly all having done so by 9 months after placement. Regarding attachment quality, based on laboratory observations, the Attachment Q-sort revealed similar rates of security in the adopted and non-adopted samples at 9 months post adoption in contrast to a low incidence of security in previous parent Q-sort reports at 11 months post-adoption (e.g., Chisholm et al., 1995). However, the Q-sort does not distinguish between organized and disorganized attachment patterns. Laboratory Strange Situation assessments revealed elevated rates of disorganized attachment in the PI compared to the NA group, consistent with previous findings based on categorical attachment coding from standard and modified laboratory assessments with infant and preschool coding methods (e.g., Chisholm, 1998; Marcovitch et al., 1997; O'Connor et al., 2003; Smyke et al., 2010; van den Dries et al., 2012). Whereas the Attachment Q-Sort approach measures the range of secure base behavior (i.e., child proximity- and contact-seeking behavior when distressed; exploration beyond the caregiver when not distressed) generally related to caregiver sensitivity, the assessment may not readily capture disruptions in the coordination or regulation of behavior, attention, and affect (e.g., sequential or simultaneous contradictory behaviors, lapses in behavioral sequences) reflected in attachment disorganization and typically associated with caregiving that exceeds insensitivity (e.g., separation, maltreatment). Qualitative scales derived from the Strange Situation procedure also revealed no differences between PI and NA children in levels of resistance or avoidance at Time 2; however, PI children tended to show higher levels of proximity-seeking and contact-maintenance during Strange Situation reunion episodes at Time 2 compared to NAs, with PI levels being comparable to those seen in NAs at Time 1. This suggests the possibility of a slightly less mature pattern of behavior among PIs.
The level of disorganization reflected in this post-institutional sample was slightly lower (25%) than average levels reported in previous studies (e.g., 31%, van den Dries et al., 2009). Higher rates of attachment security in the current sample may be due to several reasons. First, the sample of parents was very low-risk, characterized by high-socioeconomic status and access to numerous resources throughout the adoption and post-adoption process. Second, attrition of some families exhibiting insecure attachment patterns may affect estimates, although there was no direct evidence of differential impact. Finally, differences may reflect a historical trend toward improved outcomes in internationally adopted children, attributed to improvements in orphanage conditions (changes from global deprivation to adequate physical care but lack of relational experience; Juffer et al., 2011). In particular, early research on attachment in PI children focused on children adopted in the early 1990s from Eastern European (especially Romanian) orphanages. At the time, these institutions represented extreme deprivation, as indicated by both direct observations and published studies of children adopted from Romania versus other regions (for a discussion, see Hawk & McCall, 2010). Slightly better outcomes might be expected from more recent and diverse samples. The current study included a high percentage of children adopted from Asia (38.5%) and Africa (30.8%) with lower average adversity scores in contrast to a lower percentage of children adopted from Eastern Europe/Russia (21.5%), the area with the worst adversity scores.
Early Adversity and Attachment
Importantly, in the present study, attachment quality in the first few months was predicted by pre-adoptive conditions. An index of early adversity (i.e. child age of two or older at adoption, experiencing abuse or neglect, duration of institutionalization, quality of social care in the institution, number of transitions in care prior to adoption, health problems at adoption) was related to lower continuous Q-sort ratings of attachment security. Prior adversity was not related to disorganization in secure base behavior at the Time 2 attachment assessment (9 months post placement). Thus, while stability in care following early deprivation is critical to relationship formation with adoptive attachment figures, early adverse biological and environmental conditions were central to the quality of newly formed relationships.
Summary and Limitations
Based on the findings in the current study, the attachment system seems to be robust and capable of formation following deprivation and beyond infancy in the majority of children, especially if they have not experienced many changes in care settings or a prolonged period of institutionalization. Stability and quality of care are critical to attachment formation and individual differences in attachment quality. What remains unknown is how pre-adoptive and adoptive influences interact to affect developing child-parent relationship patterns and the function and trajectories of atypical social behaviors (e.g., attachment disorganization, indiscriminate social behavior, impulsivity).
As noted in previous studies, the application of the disorganized classification to institutionalized and post-institutionalized samples requires further review (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2011). Whereas the conceptual origin of the disorganized pattern relates to “fear without solution” (i.e., caregiver as a source of both comfort and threat) in children with already developed relational capacities, disorganization in the context of deprivation may represent progress (with possible wariness) associated with organizing behavioral, emotional, and attentional strategies in social relationships for the first time. The specific antecedents and consequences of disorganization as a phase of attachment development versus conflicted regulatory strategy are unknown. Longitudinal research (e.g., preschool or school-related behavioral outcomes) is needed to determine if early disturbances resembling disorganization represent initial pathways to pathology (versus adjustment indices). Moreover, an understanding of the trajectories of these and related institutional effects is critical to developing targeted intervention strategies.
Limitations of the current study, including typical gaps in information about pre-adoptive experiences, hinder attempts to isolate specific pathways to current behavior or precursors for disorganized patterns of attachment. For instance, we and others have acknowledged that orphanage records are not standardized in any way and are often incomplete or ambiguous (e.g., missing information does not always equate to the non-occurrence of a risk factor) and there is also great variability in the ways that this information is treated by adoption agencies and passed on to parents, thus we must use caution in interpreting results using these variables. In addition, in the current study, baseline assessments immediately upon arrival were not available as most families (understandably) were not able to commit to research within the first two weeks of adoption, with a small number of families postponing their visit by up to 3 months after adoption. Moreover, adoptive caregiving quality was not considered in the current study. Lastly, the comparison group was a high-socioeconomic status, low-risk sample with high rates of attachment security and low rates of disorganization as described above, which may limit generalizability to other populations. However, these families were matched in demographics to those of internationally adopted children, thus providing a more adequate comparison of the rates of security and disorganization that we would expect in such low-risk social contexts. Despite these limitations, the study demonstrates the robust nature of the attachment system. Children in the study showed a remarkable capacity for recovery during months following adoption from institutional care, and the largely positive outcomes resonate with Bowlby's (1988) conceptualization of the flexible nature of attachment in early childhood.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank the parents and children of the Minnesota International Adoption Project for their participation in this research and Bonny Donzella, Kristin Frenn, and the other staff and students for help in data collection and processing. This research was supported by grants MH080905 and MH078105 to Megan R. Gunnar.
References
- Ainsworth MDS. Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MDS, Bell SM, Stayton DF. Infant-mother attachment and social development: Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In: Richards M, editor. The integration of a child into a social world. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MDS, Blehar MC, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Ames EW. Development of Romanian orphanage children adopted to Canada. Final Report to the Human Resources Development Office; Ottawa, Canada: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Steele H, Zeanah CH, Muhamedrahimov RJ, Vorria P, Dobrova-Krol NA, et al. Gunnar M. Attachment and emotional development in institutional care: Characteristics and catch-up. In: McCall RB, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Groark CJ, Groza VK, editors. Children without Permanent Parents: Research, Practice, and Policy Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial No 201. 4. Vol. 76. 2011. pp. 62–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckett C, Bredenkamp D, Castle J, Groothues C, O'Connor TG, Rutter M the ERA Study Team. Behavior problems associated with institutionalized deprivation: A study of children adopted from Romania. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2002;23:297–303. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200210000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol 1, Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969/82. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Bruce J, Tarullo AR, Gunnar MR. Disinhibited social behavior among internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21(1):157–71. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson EA. A prospective longitudinal study of Attachment Disorganization/ Disorientation. Child Development. 1998;69(4):1107–1128. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06163.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson EA. Unpublished manuscript. University of Minnesota; Minneapolis, MN: 2002/2011. Attachment Formation Rating. [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy J, Marvin RS with the MacArthur Working Group. Unpublished manuscript. 4th. University of Virginia; 1992. Attachment organization in preschool children: Procedures and coding manual. [Google Scholar]
- Child Welfare Information Gateway. Child Maltreatment 2010: Summary of key findings. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Children's Bureau; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Chisholm K. A three year follow-up of attachment and indiscriminate friendliness in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Child Development. 1998;69:1092–1106. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06162.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chisholm K, Carter MC, Ames EW, Morison SJ. Attachment security and indiscriminately friendly behavior in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7(2):283–294. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400006507. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D. An ecological-transactional perspective on child maltreatment: Failure of the average expectable environment and its influence on child development. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen D, editors. Developmental Psychopathology (2nd Ed), Vol 3: Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 2006. pp. 129–201. [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti D, Carlson V, editors. Child maltreatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Clark S, Symons D. A longitudinal study of Q-sort attachment security and self-processes at age five. Infant and Child Development. 2000;9:91–104. [Google Scholar]
- Dobrova-Krol NA, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. The importance of quality of care: effects of perinatal HIV infection and early institutional rearing on preschoolers' attachment and indiscriminate friendliness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51(12):1368–1376. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02243.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Bellis MD. The psychobiology of neglect. Child maltreatment. 2005;10(2):150–72. doi: 10.1177/1077559505275116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gjerde PF. The interpersonal structure of family interaction settings: Parent-adolescent relations in dyads and triads. Developmental Psychology. 1986;22(3):297–304. [Google Scholar]
- Goldfarb W. Psychological privation in infancy and subsequent adjustment. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1945;15:247–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1945.tb04938.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Harlow HF, Suomi SJ. Induced psychopathology in monkeys. Engineering and Sciences. 1970;33:8–14. [Google Scholar]
- Hawk B, McCall RB. CBCL behavior problems of post-institutionalized international adoptees. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2010;13:199–211. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0068.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodges J, Tizard B. Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1989;30:77–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00770.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR. A neurological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(6):726–735. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.6.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffari-Bimmel N, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mooijaart A. Social development from infancy to adolescence: Longitudinal and concurrent factors in an adoption sample. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:1143–1153. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juffer F, Palacios J, LeMare L, Sonuga-Barke E, Tieman W, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, et al. Verhulst F. Development of adopted children with histories of early adversity. In: McCall RB, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F, Groza VK, Groark CJ, editors. Children without Permanent Parents: Research, practice, and policy Monographs of the Society for Research of Child Development. 4. Vol. 76. 2011. pp. 31–61. [Google Scholar]
- MacLean K. The impact of institutionalization on child development. Development and psychopathology. 2003;15:853–884. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Main M, Hesse E. Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism? In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 161–182. [Google Scholar]
- Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM, editors. Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1990. pp. 121–160. [Google Scholar]
- Marcovitch S, Goldberg S, Gold A, Washington J, Wasson C, Krekewich K, Handley-derry M. Determinants of behavioural problems in Romanian children adopted in Ontario. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1997;20(1):17–31. doi: 10.1080/016502597385414. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- O'Connor TG, Marvin RS, Rutter M, Olrick JT, Britner PA. Child-parent attachment following early institutional deprivation. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15(1):19–38. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park KA, Waters E. Security of attachment and preschool friendships. Child Development. 1989;60:1076–1081. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb03538.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Colvert E, Kreppner J, Beckett C, Castle J, Groothues C, Sonuga-Barke EJS. Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I: disinhibited attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(1):17–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01688.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M the ERA Study Team. Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39(4):465–476. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smyke AT, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Guthrie D. Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment among young institutionalized children. Child Development. 2010;81(1):212–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01390.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sroufe LA, Waters E. Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development. 1977;48:1184–1199. [Google Scholar]
- Stams GJM, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH. Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:806–821. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele M, Steele H, Jin X, Archer M, Herreros F. Effects of lessening the level of deprivation in Chinese orphanage settings: Decreasing disorganization and increasing security; Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; Denver, CO. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Stovall KC, Dozier M. The development of attachment in new relationships: Single subject analyses for ten foster infants. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:133–156. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400002029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stovall–McClough KC, Dozier M. Forming attachments in foster care: Infant attachment behaviors during the first 2 months of placement. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:253–271. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404044505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Symons D, Clark S, Isaksen C, Marshall J. Stability of Q-sort attachment security from age two to five. Infant Behavior and Development. 1998;21(4):785–791. [Google Scholar]
- The St. Petersburg-USA Orphanage Research Team. The effects of early social emotional and relationship experience on the development of young orphanage children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2008;73:1–262. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2008.00483.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tinbergen N. The study of instinct. New York: Oxford University Press; 1951/1974. [Google Scholar]
- Tizard B, Hodges J. The effect of early institutional rearing on the development of eight year old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1978;19:99–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1978.tb00453.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tizard B, Rees J. A comparison of the effects of adoption, restoration to the natural mother, and continued institutionalization on the cognitive development of four-year-old children. Child Development. 1974;45:92–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tizard B, Rees J. The effect of early institutional rearing on the behavior problems and affectional relationships of four-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1975;16:61–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1975.tb01872.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Dries L, Juffer F, van IJzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Fostering security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review. 2009;31(3):410–421. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2008.09.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van den Dries L, Juffer F, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Alink LRA. Infants' responsiveness, attachment, and indiscriminate friendliness after international adoption from institutions or foster care in China: Application of Emotional Availability Scales to adoptive families. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24(1):49–64. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van IJzendoorn MH, Sagi-Schwartz A. Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In: Shaver PR, Cassidy J, editors. Handbook of Attachment: Theory, research, and clinical application. New York: Guilford; 2008. pp. 890–905. [Google Scholar]
- van IJzendoorn MH, Vereijken CMJL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Riksen-Walraven JM. Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development. 2004;75(4):1188–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorria P, Papaligoura Z, Dunn J, van Ijzendoorn MH, Steele H, Kontopoulou A, Sarafidou Y. Early experiences and attachment relationships of Greek infants raised in residential group care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:1208–1220. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorria P, Rutter M, Pickles A, Wolkind S, Hobsbaum A. A comparative study of Greek children in long-term residential group care and in two-parent families: I. Social, emotional, and behavioural differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39(2):225–36. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wampler KS, Halverson CF, Jr, Moore JJ, Walters LH. The Georgia Family Q-Sort: An observational measure of family functioning. Family Process. 1989;28:223–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1989.00223.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waters E, Deane KE. Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-Methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. In: Bretherton I, Waters E, editors. Growing Points of Attachment Theory and Research Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1-2, Serial No. 209. Vol. 50. 1985. pp. 41–65. [Google Scholar]
- Waters E, Kondo-Ikemura K, Posada G, Richters J. Learning to love: Mechanisms and milestones. In: Gunnar M, Sroufe LA, editors. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, Vol 23: Self Processes and Development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1991. pp. 217–255. [Google Scholar]
- Wismer Fries AB, Pollak SD. Emotion understanding in postinstitutionalized Eastern European children. Development and Psychopathology. 2004;16:355–369. doi: 10.1017/S0954579404044554. doi:10.10170S0954579404044554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Dumitrescu A. Attachment disturbances in young children. II: Indiscriminate behavior and institutional care. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(8):983–9. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Carlson EA The BEIP Group. Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania. Child Development. 2005;76(5):1015–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00894.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]