Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2014 Dec 17;45(8):1751–1763. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714002918

Hypersomnia Subtypes, Sleep and Relapse in Bipolar Disorder

Katherine A Kaplan 1, Eleanor L McGlinchey 2, Adriane Soehner 3, Anda Gershon 1, Lisa S Talbot 4, Polina Eidelman 5, June Gruber 6, Allison G Harvey 7
PMCID: PMC4412779  NIHMSID: NIHMS659857  PMID: 25515854

Abstract

Background

Though poorly defined, hypersomnia is associated with negative health outcomes, and new-onset and recurrence of psychiatric illness. Lack of definition impedes generalizability across studies. The present research clarifies hypersomnia diagnoses in bipolar disorder by exploring possible subgroups and their relationship to prospective sleep data and relapse into mood episodes.

Methods

A community sample of 159 adults (ages 18–70) with bipolar spectrum diagnoses, euthymic at study entry, were included. Self-report inventories and clinician-administered interviews determined features of hypersomnia. Participants completed sleep diaries and wore wrist actigraphy at home to obtain prospective sleep data. Approximately seven months later, psychiatric status was reassessed. Factor analysis and latent profile analysis explored empirical groupings within hypersomnia diagnoses.

Results

Factor analyses confirmed two separate subtypes of hypersomnia (‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’) that were uncorrelated. Latent profile analyses suggested a four-class solution, with ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’ again representing two separate classes. Prospective sleep data suggested that the sleep of ‘long sleepers’ is characterized by long time in bed, not long sleep duration. Longitudinal assessment suggested that ‘excessive sleepiness’ at baseline predicted mania/hypomania relapse.

Conclusions

This study is the largest of hypersomnia to include objective sleep measurement, and refines our understanding of classification, characterization and associated morbidity. Hypersomnia appears to be comprised of two separate subgroups, long sleep and excessive sleepiness. Long sleep is characterized primarily by long bedrest duration. Excessive sleepiness is not associated with longer sleep or bedrest, but predicts relapse to mania/hypomania. Understanding these entities has important research and treatment implications.

Introduction

Evidence is accruing for the impact of hypersomnia on health and quality of life across the lifespan. Adolescents with hypersomnia report more emotional disturbance, unhappiness and interpersonal problems (Roberts et al., 2001), adults with hypersomnia are 13.4 times more likely to abuse substances (Breslau et al., 1996), and older adults with excessive daytime sleepiness report significant impairment in daily activities and productivity (Gooneratne et al., 2003). Individuals with hypersomnia are more likely to be taking medications, spending more on healthcare, and receiving government subsidies (Jennum and Kjellberg, 2010). A recent meta-analysis of 16 prospective studies documented that long habitual sleep was associated with increased rates of all-cause mortality, with long sleep conferring a 1.3× increased risk in the rate of subsequent death (Cappuccio et al., 2010).

Hypersomnia is common in the mood disorders and portends poorer illness course. Hypersomnia is present in approximately 30% of individuals with major depressive disorder (Kaplan and Harvey, 2009) and is associated with longer, more severe and more treatment-resistant depressions (Matza et al., 2003). Hypersomnia itself is a treatment-resistant symptom (Worthington et al., 1995, Iovieno et al., 2011) and a chief complaint of those not achieving remission from depression (Zimmerman et al., 2005). Prospective epidemiologic studies suggest that individuals with hypersomnia are 2.4 to 2.9 times more likely to develop a subsequent depressive episode (Breslau et al., 1996, Ford and Cooper-Patrick, 2001). In bipolar depression, hypersomnia is even more prevalent (estimated at 38–78% across studies; Kaplan et al., 2011) and highly recurrent (Leibenluft et al., 1995). Even outside of depressive episodes, roughly 25% of euthymic bipolar individuals experience hypersomnia, and this hypersomnia is associated with future depressive symptoms (Kaplan et al., 2011).

The present research focuses on hypersomnia in individuals with bipolar disorder for two reasons. First, among psychiatric disorders, hypersomnia appears to be most common in bipolar disorder (Akiskal and Benazzi, 2005, Bowden, 2005, Benazzi, 2006, Kaplan and Harvey, 2009). Second, hypersomnia persists into the inter-episode period of bipolar disorder at a relatively high rate (Kaplan et al., 2011). Because reports of hypersomnia may be confounded with other symptoms of depression such as anergia, avolition, or psychomotor retardation (Billiard et al., 1994, Dolenc et al., 1996), as well as mood-congruent biases in reporting, we chose to explore hypersomnia in an inter-episode sample. To our knowledge, this is the largest investigation of hypersomnia in a clinical sample using clinician-guided and self-reports of hypersomnia along with subjective and objective measures of sleep.

Despite the evidence for the importance of hypersomnia, problems related to definition and diagnosis abound (Kaplan and Harvey, 2009). Diagnostic manuals, along with the empirical research they inform, appear divided in characterizing psychiatric hypersomnia by either long sleep duration or by excessive daytime sleepiness. The DSM-5, which renames hypersomnia “hypersomnolence disorder,” states that the disorder is characterized by excessive sleepiness evidenced by excessive need for sleep, long sleep, or excessive sleep inertia (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A handful of published studies define psychiatric hypersomnia via long sleep (Avery et al., 1991, Tam et al., 1997, Williamson et al., 2000, Roberts et al., 2001, Parker et al., 2006, Soehner et al., 2014). By contrast, the International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition (World Health Organization, 1993) and the International Classification of Sleep Disorders, Second Edition (American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 2005) describe hypersomnia as excessive sleepiness, or the propensity of falling asleep during the daytime that is not solely accounted for by an inadequate amount of sleep. Consistent with this definition, Ohayon et al. (2012) investigated excessive sleepiness in the general population, identifying self-reported excessive sleepiness symptoms most strongly associated with functional impairment. These researchers proposed a definition of ‘hypersomnia disorder’ characterized exclusively by excessive sleepiness, not long sleep.

Indeed, accruing evidence suggests that these two constructs—excessive sleepiness and long sleep—may not overlap. Ohayon (2012) found no relationship between the frequency of excessive sleepiness complaints and self-reported long sleep (though see (Ohayon et al., 2013) for alternate interpretation). Nofzinger et al. (1991) evaluated individuals diagnosed with bipolar disorder reporting hypersomnia (as defined by long sleep) on a daytime multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) and observed no excessive sleepiness in this long sleeping group. Additional studies have confirmed that excessive sleepiness, as measured objectively by daytime MSLTs, is not present in individuals with psychiatric disorders who complain of long-sleep-defined hypersomnia (Billiard et al., 1994, Dolenc et al., 1996, Vgontzas et al., 2000). In sum, those who complain of long sleep at night do not demonstrate excessive sleepiness during the daytime, and there is a suggestion in the literature that long sleep and excessive sleepiness are uncorrelated. One goal of the present paper is to examine if two separate groups are emerging under the umbrella term ‘hypersomnia’ – a group complaining of long sleep and a group complaining of excessive daytime sleepiness. At present, lack of an operational definition limits comparisons between studies and amalgamation of research findings. Correct classification of hypersomnia is essential to understanding its etiology, sequelae, and treatment.

At least two important such sequelae of hypersomnia are understudied. First, the relationship between self-reported hypersomnia and actual sleep obtained is unclear. Studies that have measured nighttime sleep objectively suggest that individuals with hypersomnia actually sleep no longer than their non-hypersomnic counterparts. One study utilizing polysomnography found that a psychiatric hypersomnia group slept only 7.68 hours on average, and only 14% slept beyond nine hours (Billiard et al., 1994). This finding has subsequently been replicated by multiple groups using polysomnography, actigraphy and sleep diary (Nofzinger et al., 1991, Dolenc et al., 1996, Vgontzas et al., 2000, Kaplan et al., 2011). Individuals with insomnia have long been known to overestimate wakefulness and underestimate total sleep time (Mercer et al., 2002, Harvey and Tang, 2012); it is unclear if individuals with hypersomnia similarly misperceive their sleep (Attarian et al., 2004, Trajanovic et al., 2007). Second, as noted above, studies have established that hypersomnia confers increased likelihood of developing psychiatric illness (Breslau et al., 1996, Ford and Cooper-Patrick, 2001). The extent to which excessive sleepiness, as compared to long sleep, differentially impacts rates of relapse into illness episodes is not known.

This paper addresses the diagnostic confusion and understudied consequences by contributing data relating to hypersomnia classification and sequelae in a relatively large clinical sample. The goal is to identify subtypes of hypersomnia—namely, self-reported long sleep and excessive sleepiness—and to examine the relationship of these subtypes to prospective sleep data and relapse into depression and/or mania. We use a data-driven approach to delineating subtypes of hypersomnia based on a multi-method combination of subjective, objective and clinician-guided instruments. The first aim was to evaluate the independence of self-reported long sleep and self-reported excessive sleepiness (Billiard et al., 1994, Kaplan and Harvey, 2009) via confirmatory factor analysis and latent profile analysis, controlling for the effects of psychotropic medications. The second aim was to investigate the relationship between hypersomnia subtype, prospective sleep data, and episode relapse to better characterize the sleep and psychiatric morbidity of individuals who complain of hypersomnia.

Methods

Sample

The present data is a secondary analysis of data accrued from three separate studies on sleep in bipolar disorder conducted between December 2005 and November 2011 (Talbot et al., 2009, Gershon et al., 2012, Kaplan and Harvey, 2013). Adult participants over age 18 were recruited from advertisements, online bulletins and physician referrals. The final sample included 159 adults ages 18–70 with bipolar spectrum disorder diagnoses (Bipolar I=143, Bipolar II=13 and Bipolar NOS=3) who were inter-episode at study entry. Care was taken to ensure that participants were recruited to reflect Alameda County demographics. A summary of participant characteristics is presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Participant characteristics.

Demographic Variable N=159
Age, mean (SD) 35.8 (11.4)
Women, No. (%) 103 (65.6%)
Race/Ethnicity, No. (%)
  African American 13 (8.4%)
  Asian American 20 (12.9%)
  Caucasian 100 (64.5%)
  Hispanic 10 (6.5%)
  Other/Biracial 12 (7.8%)
Employment Status, No. (%)
  Full Time/Part Time 87 (56.9%)
  Unemployed/Retired/Disability 66 (43.1%)
Marital Status, No. (%)
  Married/Partnered 33 (20.9%)
  Separated/Divorced/Widowed 27 (17.1%)
  Single 98 (62.0%)
Annual Income, No. (%)
  Less than $50,000 95 (73.6%)
  Greater than $50,000 34 (26.4%)
IDS-C Total Score, mean (SD) 11.7 (7.6)
YMRS Total Score, mean (SD) 3.3 (2.9)
Psychotropic Medications, No. (%)
  None 10 (6.3%)
  Monotherapy 35 (22.0%)
  Polytherapy 114 (71.7%)
  Mood Stabilizers/Anticonvulsants 96 (60.4%)
  Antidepressants 85 (53.5%)
  Atypical Antipsychotics 77 (48.4%)
  Typical Antipsychotics 2 (1.3%)
  Anxiolytics 36 (22.6%)
  Stimulants 9 (5.7%)
  Sleep/Hypnotics 37 (23.3%)

Individuals from all studies were eligible to participate if they (a) met DSM-IV criteria for a diagnosis of bipolar disorder type I, II, or NOS, as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID; First et al., 1997); (b) did not meet criteria for a diagnosis of current substance or alcohol abuse or dependence in the past three months; (c) did not meet criteria for narcolepsy, sleep apnea, restless leg syndrome or periodic limb movement disorder based on the Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders (DSISD; Edinger et al., 2004) and (d) did not report history of severe head trauma, stroke, neurological disease, or severe medical illness (e.g., advanced autoimmune disorder). Inter-episode status was confirmed with the SCID and established cutoff scores on the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Version (IDS-C; Rush et al., 1996) and Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; Young et al., 1978). All participants were required to be under the care of a psychiatrist, and information on medication dose and administration was collected upon study entry. Participants were not excluded on the basis of comorbidities or pharmacological treatments, given that comorbidity and polytherapy are common features of bipolar disorder. As medication class and dosing may influence the course and associated features of hypersomnia, however, medication effects were examined using an approach developed by Phillips et al. (Phillips et al., 2008, Almeida et al., 2009) that considers both the number and dose of psychotropic medications in evaluating their impact.

Diagnostic Measures

The SCID (First et al., 1997) is a semi-structured interview designed to assess DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for Axis I disorders. The SCID has good inter-rater reliability for a majority of psychiatric disorders (Skre et al., 1991, Williams et al., 1992). Trained doctoral students and postdoctoral fellows in clinical psychology administered the SCID to all participants to assess current and lifetime Axis I disorders. Diagnostic inter-rater reliability was established by re-scoring a randomly-selected sample of SCID interviews (n=35); diagnoses matched those made by the original interviewer in all cases (k=1.00). The Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders (DSISD; Edinger et al., 2004) is a semi-structured interview that assesses research diagnostic criteria for sleep disorders. The DSISD has been shown to have acceptable reliability and validity (Edinger et al., 2009).

Inter-episode status was confirmed using established cutoffs of IDS-C < 24 and YMRS < 12. The IDS-C (Rush et al., 1996) is a 30-item clinician-guided instrument used to assess severity of depressive symptoms. The measure has demonstrated good reliability and validity (Trivedi et al., 2004). The YMRS (Young et al., 1978) is an 11-item measure used to assess the severity of manic symptoms, also shown to have good reliability and validity.

Hypersomnia Indicators

Six items were selected as hypersomnia indicators. These indicators have been used in previous research on hypersomnia and in bipolar disorder (Frye et al., 2007, Koffel and Watson, 2009, Kaplan et al., 2011, Plante et al., 2012).

The IDS-C (Rush et al., 1996) contains an item designed to assess the presence of hypersomnia based on self-reported sleep length in the past month. This item requires clinicians to probe for typical and maximal sleep length in a 24-hour period, including prompts for napping. The IDS-C classifies its symptom levels according to number of hours slept over a 24-hour period. It has previously been shown to have utility in assessing bipolar hypersomnia (Kaplan et al., 2011, Plante et al., 2012). Inter-rater reliability for this item, established using intra-class correlations (Shrout and Fleiss, 1979) between the original item score and a randomly-selected sample of IDS-C interviews (n=30), was found to be excellent (r=0.97).

The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Self Report (IDS-SR; Rush et al., 1996) mirrors the content of the IDS-C in a self-report format. The hypersomnia item of the IDS-SR asks participants to rate the longest period slept in a 24-hour period over the past month, including naps.

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al., 1989) is a 19-item self-report measure of subjective sleep quality in the last month yielding a global score and seven component scores. The PSQI has been shown to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability (Carpenter and Andrykowski, 1998). Two items from the PSQI were chosen as indicators of hypersomnia. The first was an item assessing self-reported sleep duration in the past month (Question 4), which has been validated against actigraphy and sleep diary in various samples (Backhaus et al., 2002, Grandner et al., 2006) and has been used to estimate habitual sleep duration in previous research (King et al., 1997, Knutson et al., 2006). The second indicator was the Daytime Dysfunction subscale, derived from two questions about excessive sleepiness (Question 8) and daytime impairment (Question 9). This subscale has been validated against other measures of daytime impairment (e.g. Buysse et al., 2008).

The Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS; Johns, 1991) is a self-report measure of excessive daytime sleepiness. This questionnaire assesses the likelihood of falling asleep in 8 different situations, yielding a composite score of sleepiness severity with scores > 10 representing excessive sleepiness. The ESS has shown good internal consistency and high test-retest reliability (Johns, 1992).

Finally, to more directly tap the construct of excessive sleepiness, individuals were queried about the severity of their daytime sleepiness. Mirroring the definition seen in the ICSD-2 and ICD-10, participants were queried “To what extent do you think that you feel sleepy during the daytime?” and asked to rate their response on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from “not at all” to “very much.” This item is subsequently referred to as the “Excessive Sleepiness Item.”

Prospective Sleep Data

All participants kept standard sleep diaries for one week to assess parameters including total sleep time, time in bed and sleep efficiency. The sleep diary has been shown to be a reliable estimate (Gehrman et al., 2002, Morin and Espie, 2003) and is considered the gold standard subjective measure of sleep (Buysse et al., 2006, Carney et al., 2012). Participants completed the log prior to sleep and upon waking, and a subset of participants (N=91) were required to call a voicemail twice daily with their answers to ensure compliance. Total sleep time was calculated by subtracting all time spent awake from all time spent in bed over a 24 hour period including naps. Time in bed was scored by summing all intended sleep periods, excluding periods of reading or television watching in bed. Naps were included in total sleep time and time in bed calculations on the basis that individuals with hypersomnia are reported to experience both extended nighttime and daytime bedrest durations (Billiard et al., 1994).

A subset of participants (N=75) were also equipped with an actigraph (Actiwatch AW-64; Mini Mitter, Philips Respironics Inc., Bend OR) to obtain an objective estimate of sleep for one week. Actigraphs are small wrist watch-like devices that provide an empirical estimate of the sleep/wake cycle via movement. Movement data are recorded and downloaded onto a computer and analyzed to generate various sleep parameters. Actigraphy has been used in previous research focusing on sleep parameters in bipolar disorder (Lam et al., 2003, Millar et al., 2004, Harvey et al., 2005, Jones et al., 2005) and has recently been validated as a reliable measure for sleep length and fragmentation in a bipolar sample (Kaplan et al., 2012). This device features a sensitivity of 0.05g and a bandwidth between 3 Hz and 11 Hz, with a sampling frequency of 32 Hz. Analyses were completed using the medium sensitivity setting and immobile minutes algorithm in Actiware 5.57. Mirroring the variables extracted from sleep diaries, total daily sleep time and time in bed were extracted from actigraph output.

Procedures

All procedures were approved by the University of California, Berkeley, Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects. After completing the initial telephone screen, participants who appeared likely to be eligible were invited to the laboratory for a baseline visit. During this visit participants signed informed consent and were interviewed by trained postdoctoral or doctoral researchers to assess the diagnostic status and symptom severity using the SCID, the DSISD, the YMRS, and the IDS-C. Once eligibility was determined by these measures, participants completed the remaining self-report and clinician-guided indicators of hypersomnia. All eligible participants completed the daily sleep diary and wore the actiwatch for one week. Approximately seven months after this initial visit (222±73 days), participants were invited to the laboratory or contacted via telephone and a trained interviewer re-assessed psychiatric diagnoses over the seven-month period via the SCID. The present analyses examined relapse into mania, hypomania or depression within this seven-month follow-up time period.

Data Analyses

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted using Amos 20.0 (IBM SPSS, Inc.) to test the a priori hypothesis that hypersomnia is composed of two distinct subtypes, long sleep and excessive daytime sleepiness. Following generally-accepted guidelines, sample size to number of indicators was kept above 20 to ensure stability of the model (Marsh et al., 1988, MacCallum et al., 1999). Model fit was evaluated using established standards, including chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (χ2/df ) ≤ 3, comparative fit indices (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis indices (TLI) > 0.85, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.05 (Hu and Bentler, 1995, Hair et al., 1998). Missing data was imputed using the imbedded Full Information Maximum Likelihood algorithm (Enders and Bandalos, 2001), though structural integrity of all models was confirmed by comparing the imputed model to a complete model where missing data were deleted list-wise. Likewise, to evaluate the impact of bipolar spectrum diagnosis (i.e. I, II or NOS) on model stability, model fit for the full sample was compared to a model with Bipolar II and Bipolar NOS (n=16) omitted from analyses. Model fit for a two-factor solution was evaluated against a more parsimonious one-factor model by examining the statistical significance (i.e., p value) associated with the Δχ2/df value (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002).

We evaluated the impact of demographic variables on our CFA using Multiple Indicators Multiple Causes (MIMIC) modeling, a special type of Structural Equation Modeling which allows for the simultaneous detection of associations between covariates and latent variables. Given that females were overrepresented in our sample and rates of bipolar spectrum disorders are not known to differ across genders, we evaluated the impact of gender on our CFA. We also evaluated age as a covariate in our models given previously-established associations between age and long sleep (Kaplan and Harvey, 2009). MIMIC modeling was estimated using Mplus 6.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 2007).

Latent profile analysis (LPA) was used to determine the number and composition of groups into which participants are placed based on maximum likelihood estimation (Muthén, 2004). LPA is a type of cluster analysis that seeks to establish group membership in categorical latent variables (hypersomnia subtypes) using continuous manifest indicators (sleep reports). Unlike traditional cluster analysis, latent profile analysis establishes group membership by probability score, not distance, and is not subject to the same constraints as traditional cluster analyses (Hagenaars and McCutcheon, 2002). LPA was conducted using Mplus 6.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 2007) with the number of latent classes determined by the Bayesian information criteria partismony index (Nylund et al., 2007), the interpretablitiy of clusters, the Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood-ratio test (Lo et al., 2001) and the Bootstrap likelihood-ratio test (McLachlan and Peel, 2000) between the estimated model and a model with one fewer class.

In order to evaluate the association between hypersomnia subtypes and prospective sleep data, we applied a structural equation modeling (SEM) framework to our latent factors, looking for relations between subtype membership and sleep variables (total sleep time and time in bed) using the standard indices of model fit described above. In a similar fashion, SEM was used to evaluate the relationship between hypersomnia subgroup and relapse into depression or mania at follow up. As relapse was a binary variable, Markov chain Monte Carlo methods were used in these path analyses. Correlations among exogenous variables were examined before restricting covariances to zero.

Medication Analyses

To evaluate the potential effect of psychotropic medication on hypersomnia class membership, medication load score (Phillips et al., 2008, Almeida et al., 2009, Kaplan et al., 2011) was then included as a covariate in our CFA models and our LPA analyses. Medication load scores, designed to account for both the number and dose of psychotropic medications taken, involve classifying each medication dose as ‘low’ or ‘high’ and assigning a corresponding score of 1 or 2 based on published parameters for antidepressants and mood stabilizers (Sackeim, 2001), for chlorpromazine-equivalent mean effective daily doses (ED50) of antipsychotics (Davis and Chen, 2004), and for midpoint dosing as recommended in the Physician’s Desk Reference (PDR Staff, 2007) for anxiolytics and hypnotics. A composite measure of medication load is created for each participant by summing across medications (i.e. summing all 1s and 2s), reflecting both dose and diversity of medications taken by each participant (Almeida et al., 2009).

Results

Factor Analyses

The factor loadings from the two-factor model evaluated are presented in Table 2. The overall fit of the model was good (χ2/df=1.48, df=8, p=0.16, CFI=0.97, TLI=0.92, RMSEA=0.05). As expected, all factor loadings onto the ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’ factors were significant (mean loading=0.66). Furthermore, the two factors themselves were uncorrelated (r=−0.09), suggesting independence. Finally, the two-factor model provided a superior fit to the data than a more parsimonious one-factor model (Δχ2=37.04, df=2, p<0.0001).

Table 2.

CFA factor loadings and R2 values, and LPA means and standard deviations (in parentheses) for the six selected indicators of hypersomnia.

CFA
LPA
Factor 1 Factor 2 Class 1 Class 2 Class 3 Class 4

Indicator ‘Long
Sleep’
‘Excessive
Sleepiness’
R2 ‘Long
Sleep’
‘Excessive
Sleepiness’
‘Short
Sleep’
‘Normal
Sleep’
IDS-C Hypersomnia Item (0–3) 0.81 - 0.66 2.19 (0.40) 0.00 (0.00) 0.00 (0.00) 1.00 (0.00)
IDS-SR Hypersomnia Item (0–3) 0.74 - 0.54 2.14 (0.38) 2.00 (0.63) 0.22 (0.42) 1.24 (0.60)
PSQI Sleep Duration Item in hours 0.67 - 0.44 9.22 (1.53) 7.02 (1.81) 6.39 (1.45) 8.23 (1.43)
Epworth Sleepiness Scale Total - 0.81 0.66 6.57 (4.70) 12.00 (4.24) 6.72 (4.34) 6.77 (3.76)
PSQI Daytime Dysfunction Subscale (0–3) - 0.34 0.12 1.43 (0.76) 2.30 (0.67) 1.18 (0.70) 1.44 (0.64)
Excessive Sleepiness Item (0–4) - 0.62 0.38 1.91 (1.30) 3.22 (1.09) 1.93 (1.30) 1.96 (1.12)

Note: CFA, confirmatory factor analysis; LPA, latent profile analysis, IDS-C, Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rated Version; IDS-SR, Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Self-Report Version; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.

To evaluate the influence of bipolar spectrum diagnosis on the model, individuals without Bipolar I Disorder (i.e. Bipolar II and NOS; n=16) were excluded. The fit of the model remained acceptable even after deleting these participants (χ2/df=0.94, df=8, p=0.48, CFI=1.0, TLI=1.01, RMSEA = 0.00). Likewise, the overall fit of the model was largely satisfactory (Hu and Bentler, 1999) after deleting missing data list-wise (χ2/df=2.07, df=8, p=0.04, CFI=0.90, TLI=0.80, RMSEA = 0.14). Thus, confirmatory factor analysis suggested that ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’ were separate and uncorrelated latent factors, and that this superior two-factor solution held even after excluding bipolar spectrum disorders and missing data from analyses.

We evaluated the impact of key demographic variables (gender and age) as covariates in a MIMIC model, a special form of SEM that allows modeling of direct connections between covariates and factors. The overall fit of this MIMIC model was largely satisfactory (χ2/df=1.66, df=16, p=0.05, CFI=0.92, TLI=0.87, RMSEA = 0.07). None of the factor loadings between the covariates and the factors were significant except for age, where a significant effect of age on the ‘long sleep’ factor was observed (p<.01).

Latent Profile Analyses

Comparison of BIC values from two- (BIC=3531.87), three-(BIC=3318.98), four- (BIC=3296.88) and five- (BIC=3303.34) class LPA models provided evidence for the four-class model’s superior relative fit. Class 1 (n=17, ‘Long Sleep’) consisted of individuals with long sleep duration and a relative absence of excessive sleepiness complaints. These individuals reported sleeping over 9 hours on average per night and feeling “a little” to “somewhat” sleepy during the daytime, with ESS scores in a non-clinical (<10) range. Class 2 (n=14, ‘Excessive Sleepiness’) consisted of individuals reporting clinically-significant ESS scores and rating daytime sleepiness between “a lot” and “very much,” but with average sleep duration of 7.1 hours. Class 3 (n=88, ‘Short Sleep’) and Class 4 (n=39, ‘Normal Sleep’) was comprised of individuals who did not report excessive sleepiness (ESS scores<10) but self-reported an average sleep duration of 6.4 hours and 8.2 hours, respectively. Table 2 presents response probabilities of each hypersomnia indicator according to latent class membership. In sum, latent profile analyses again suggested that ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’ were best defined as separate classes.

To examine whether bipolar spectrum diagnosis or missing data imputation affected model fit, separate latent class analyses were run excluding non-Bipolar I participants and excluding missing data list-wise. A four class solution continued to illustrate superior fit when excluding Bipolar II and NOS subjects from analyses (BIC for 2, 3, 4, and 5 class solutions were 3234.70, 3016.47, 3006.82, and 3008.66, respectively) and when excluding missing data list-wise (BIC for 2, 3, 4, and 5 class solutions were 1599.30, 1578.24, 1543.88, and 1556.39, respectively).

Medication Analyses

A summary of psychotropic medications taken by the participants in the sample is presented in Table 1. Participants took an average of 2.5±1.5 medications. Mean medication load score for all participants taking medications was 3.4±2.1. When entered as a covariate in our CFA models, medication load score did not significantly improve model fit (Δχ2=6.88, df=5, p=0.23). Likewise, there was no difference in medication load scores between the ‘Long Sleep’ class (4.2±2.5) and the ‘Excessive Sleepiness’ class (3.9±2.0) as determined by LPA analyses (t(27)=0.4, p=0.69). We also evaluated the impact of each of the seven individual medication classes (e.g. stimulants, anxiolytics) listed in Table 1. The ‘Long Sleep’ and the ‘Excessive Sleepiness’ class did not differ by rates of any medication class taken (χ2≤1.00, p>0.4 for all). We also did not observe differences in any one class of medication taken between those who did and did not relapse to hypomania/mania by follow-up (χ2≤3.06, p≥0.07 for all). Thus, medications did not appear to influence confirmatory factor analyses and medication load or type did not differ between the ‘Long Sleep’ and ‘Excessive Sleepiness’ classes.

Relationship to Prospective Sleep Data

We evaluated the relationship between hypersomnia subtype and prospective sleep data (average total sleep time and average time in bed) for both sleep diary and actigraphy within an SEM framework. Considering sleep diary data first, we found an acceptable model fit (χ2/df=1.12, CFI=0.99, TLI=0.98, RMSEA=0.03) for average diary total sleep time with significant factor loading onto ‘long sleep’ (p<0.001) but with nonsignificant loading onto ‘excessive sleepiness’ (p=0.75). Likewise, an acceptable model fit (χ2/df=2.0, CFI=0.98, TLI=0.97, RMSEA=0.04) was observed for average diary time in bed with significant factor loading onto ‘long sleep’ (p<0.001) but with nonsignificant loading onto ‘excessive sleepiness’ (p=0.73). In contrast, average actigraphy total sleep time revealed acceptable model fit (χ2/df=1.12, CFI=0.99, TLI=0.98, RMSEA=0.03) but no significant factor loadings onto either ‘long sleep’ (p=0.22) or ‘excessive sleepiness’ (p=0.08). Actigraphy time in bed analyses showed acceptable model fit (χ2/df=1.45, CFI=0.96, TLI=0.90, RMSEA=0.05) with a significant factor loading onto ‘long sleep’ (p=0.05) but with nonsignificant loading onto ‘excessive sleepiness’ (p=0.15). Thus sleep diary data suggested that ‘long sleep’, but not ‘excessive sleepiness’, was related to total sleep time and time in bed. Actigraphy further suggested ‘long sleep’ was characterized by time in bed, not total sleep time.

Relationship to Illness Relapse

The relationship between hypersomnia subtype and SCID-determined bipolar relapse was also investigated. Of the 121 individuals who were re-assessed at follow-up, 21 individuals (17.5%) had developed a hypomanic/manic episode and 37 individuals (30.1%) had developed a major depressive episode between baseline and follow-up. Considering relapse to depression within the seven-month evaluation period, neither ‘long sleep’ nor ‘excessive sleepiness’ predicted relapse into depression (p>0.05 for both). In examining relapse into hypomania/mania, ‘excessive sleepiness’ predicted relapse into hypomania/mania (p<0.01) while ‘long sleep’ was not a significant predictor of manic relapse (p=0.78). The relationship between excessive sleepiness and manic relapse did not appear to be driven by insufficient sleep, as no relationship between excessive sleepiness and prospective sleep length was observed in CFA models (all ps > 0.05). Likewise, LPA analyses suggested the ‘excessive sleepiness’ class slept significantly more than the ‘short sleep’ class (471 vs 411 minutes; t(90)=−2.6, p=0.01) but not differently from the ‘normal sleep’ class (471 vs 487 minutes; t(49)=−.72, p=ns), again suggesting the relationship between excessive sleepiness and manic relapse was not driven by short or insufficient sleep.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the largest study of hypersomnia in a clinical sample using clinician-administered and self-reports of hypersomnia, as well as the largest to evaluate hypersomnia using subjective and objective measures of sleep. This paper makes four important contributions to our understanding of hypersomnia. First, we found that hypersomnia is comprised of two separate subgroups, ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive sleepiness’. Second, neither of these subgroups appears differentially impacted by medications. Third, the objective sleep of ‘long sleepers’ is characterized by long time in bed, not long sleep duration. Forth, excessive sleepiness predicts relapse to mania.

Our data clearly suggest evidence for subtypes of hypersomnia. Factor analyses point to long sleep and excessive sleepiness as separate constructs. Likewise, latent profile analyses, which are considered an appropriate method for identifying symptom structure when no ‘gold-standard’ diagnosis is available (Garrett et al., 2002), also identified long sleep and excessive sleepiness as belonging to separate classes. Though this has been suggested in the literature (Nofzinger et al., 1991, Billiard et al., 1994, Ohayon et al., 2012), our investigation was the first to provide empirical data indicating that long sleep and excessive sleepiness are indeed separate and uncorrelated. It should be noted that this finding stands in contrast to a recent population-based telephone survey (Ohayon et al., 2013), in which a positive correlation was noted between sleep length and excessive sleepiness. Differences in studies are likely to be accounted for by differences in sampling, methodology, and participants. The telephone-based survey did not focus on a clinical sample, relied exclusively on self-report, and did not exclude participants based on presence of sleep disorders such as obstructive sleep apnea or periodic limb movement disorder. As these sleep disorders involve disrupted sleep continuity, individuals are likely to experience both excessive sleepiness (from insufficient nocturnal sleep) and long sleep (reflecting a homeostatic compensatory process). The present investigation offered a clearer picture of hypersomnia by excluding such confounding sleep disorders. It further utilized clinician interviews along with self-report to make hypersomnia determinations, thereby addressing a finding in the literature that individuals with hypersomnia tend to overestimate their sleep when asked to estimate via self-report alone (Attarian et al., 2004, Trajanovic et al., 2007).

Neither long sleep nor excessive sleepiness appeared to be differentially influenced by medications. Those with ‘long sleep’ and ‘excessive daytime sleepiness’ did not differ in number, dose or class of medication taken, and medication load was not a significant covariate in our models. Even so, medications present a challenge in research with clinical samples. Sleep-related side effects are present in 4% to 37% of patients with bipolar disorder (GlaxoSmithKline, 2005, Staff, 2007), and atypical antipsychotics in particular are known for their sedative properties (Kane and Sharif, 2008). As research in medication-free bipolar samples is neither representative nor generalizable, we did not exclude on the basis of medications and instead assessed effects of medications in careful post-hoc analyses. We did not control for medication changes over the seven-month interval between baseline evaluation and follow-up. Hence, a limitation is that medication changes during this period may have influenced rates of relapse. Even so, medications remain the first line treatment of bipolar disorder (Sachs et al., 2000) and dose changes and medication augmentations are quite common in the course of treatment.

Viewing hypersomnia as comprised of subtypes has important implications for researching etiology and understanding mechanisms. Long sleep has multiple proposed etiologies, including both biological [decreased slow wave activity at night (Plante et al., 2012), a slower circadian pacemaker (Aeschbach et al., 2003)], and psychological [anergia or avolition (Nofzinger et al., 1991, Billiard et al., 1994), avoidance coping (Jacobson et al., 2001)] mechanisms. Likewise recent research suggests that excessive sleepiness may be related to decreased cerebrospinal fluid histamine levels (Kanbayashi et al., 2009) and differences in the human leukocyte antigen (HLA) DQB1*0602 (Goel et al., 2010). The degree to which these proposed mechanisms might overlap—or differentiate—long sleep from excessive sleepiness is still unknown. Treatment of long sleep and excessive sleepiness is also likely to be quite different based on understanding of these mechanisms.

One key finding emerged from prospective sleep measurement: actigraphy data collected over a 24-hour period suggested that long sleepers displayed longer time in bed but not longer total sleep time. In other words, the sleep of self-reported long sleepers is characterized by excessive bedrest duration. This finding has been observed in studies utilizing polysomnography (Nofzinger et al., 1991, Dolenc et al., 1996, Vgontzas et al., 2000, Kaplan et al., 2011) but has not been examined in the home environment using actigraphy. A relatively recent publication by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine on practice parameters for actigraphy commented on the lack of studies employing actigraphy in hypersomnia, noting that “there were no studies identified that compared actigraphy versus the clinical history plus sleep logs (or another reference standard) to estimate mean sleep time or sleep pattern when evaluating patients with hypersomnia as a complaint” (Morgenthaler et al., 2007). The present study is the first to use actigraphy expressly to evaluate sleep in hypersomnia and provides evidence for its utility in characterizing the sleep disturbance. It should also be noted that excessive sleepiness was not associated with increased total sleep time or time in bed in either diaries or actigraphy, adding further support for long sleep and excessive sleepiness as separate subtypes of hypersomnia.

Multiple studies suggest that sleep disturbances such as hypersomnia predict first episode and recurrence of depression (Breslau et al., 1996, Ford and Cooper-Patrick, 2001, Cho et al., 2008). Surprisingly, our findings did not support this relationship, as we observed neither long sleep nor excessive sleepiness predicted relapse to a depression at 7-month follow-up. We offer two possible explanations as to why this may be the case. First, our follow-up period was seven months, whereas other prospective research on hypersomnia and depression had follow-up durations of anywhere from one to three years; as such, had the interval between baseline and follow-up assessment been longer, a positive relationship might have emerged. Second, we defined relapse as a SCID-assessed depressive episode, though it may be the case that a dimensional measure of depressive symptomatology (e.g. the IDS-C) would have shown elevation at follow-up.

A novel finding to emerge from the present study was that excessive sleepiness predicted relapse into hypomania/mania, an association that has not received much attention. As mentioned above, this relationship did not appear to be attributable to medication administration. We also examined the prospective sleep of the excessive sleepiness group to see if they were sleeping less, as associations between decreased sleep and increased mania are established in the literature (Colombo et al., 1999), but we did not find supportive evidence. There may be an as-yet unexplained biological marker contributing to both excessive sleepiness dysfunction and the circadian instability at the core of bipolar illness episodes (Harvey, 2008). Alternatively, a homeostatic regulatory process may underlie the relationship between excessive sleepiness in the euthymic period and reduced sleep need in hypomania/mania, though little is known about mechanisms of sleep regulation in either state (Wehr et al., 1987, Plante and Winkelman, 2008).

The present findings should be considered in light of several important limitations. First, the study included only individuals with bipolar spectrum diagnoses given their strong relationship to hypersomnia, and results may lack generalizability to other psychiatric populations. Even so, the objective sleep data here are consistent with other published reports of dysthymia (Billiard et al., 1994, Dolenc et al., 1996), and one recent study exploring sleep disturbance in Seasonal Affective Disorder also suggested hypersomnia may be characterized by long bedrest duration (Roecklein et al., 2013). An important agenda for future research will be to explore hypersomnia subtypes and their relationship to illness course in other mood disorders. It should also be noted that our findings were derived from a relatively small, predominantly female and Caucasian sample, though it was a sizeable sample given the inclusion of the clinician-administered interview and prospective sleep monitoring. Our prospective sleep monitoring included one week of sleep diary, though more than one week of diary may be needed to adequately characterize sleep disturbance (Wohlgemuth et al., 1999). Finally, we did not assess for medication changes or hypersomnia at follow-up assessment, so we are unable to comment on the chronicity of hypersomnia complaints or the impact of medication changes on mood episodes. We also did not assess for caffeine or other non-prescription stimulant use in the period between baseline and follow-up, and use of these substances may have impacted the relationship between excessive sleepiness and mania observed in our sample.

The present findings have implications for the newly-published DSM-5 and set an agenda for future research. As currently stated, DSM-5 criteria for hypersomnolence disorder (what was previously referred to as hypersomnia in the DSM-IV) hinge on “a complaint of excessive sleepiness” defined via excessive sleep drive, long sleep, or excessive sleep inertia. The present research clearly demonstrates that excessive sleepiness and long sleep are separate subgroups. As long as they remain together under the umbrella term hypersomnolence, excessive sleepiness and long sleep will continue to confuse the literature and impede generalizability. Future research that investigates characteristics and consequences of each group separately is needed to better understand hypersomnia, and both population-based and clinical investigations that utilize semi-structured interviews along with self report are useful in preventing sleep estimation bias. Additionally, treatment research is needed to identify interventions tailored for each subtype. For example, considering behavioral treatment options, interventions focusing on behavioral activation (Jacobson et al., 2001) or social rhythms stabilization (Frank et al., 2000) may be successful for long sleep, whereas interventions focused on light (Kaida et al., 2006) or minimizing sleep inertia (Hayashi et al., 2003) may differentially impact excessive sleepiness.

Table 3.

Descriptive statistics for actigraphy and sleep diary by LPA-determined hypersomnia subtype

Overall
Sample
Long Sleep
Subtype
Excessive
Sleepiness
Subtype
Sleep Diary, mean (SD)
  Total Sleep Time 7.52 (1.33) 8.80 (1.44) 7.86 (0.85)
  Time in Bed 8.92 (1.42) 10.39 (1.55) 9.16 (1.32)
Actigraphy, mean (SD)
  Total Sleep Time 7.10 (1.45) 7.69 (1.60) 7.02 (0.72)
  Time in Bed 9.09 (1.62) 10.05 (1.11) 9.09 (1.62)

Note: LPA, latent profile analysis.

Acknowledgments

Financial Support. This project was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health Grant No. R34 MH080958 awarded to AGH and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Grant awarded to KAK.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest. All authors report no conflicts of interest.

Ethical Standards. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008

References

  1. Aeschbach D, Sher L, Postolache TT, Matthews JR, Jackson MA, Wehr TA. A longer biological night in long sleepers than in short sleepers. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2003;88:26–30. doi: 10.1210/jc.2002-020827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akiskal HS, Benazzi F. Atypical depression: a variant of bipolar II or a bridge between unipolar and bipolar II? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;84:209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Almeida JR, Akkal D, Hassel S, Travis MJ, Banihashemi L, Kerr N, Kupfer DJ, Phillips ML. Reduced gray matter volume in ventral prefrontal cortex but not amygdala in bipolar disorder: significant effects of gender and trait anxiety. Psychiatry Research. 2009;171:54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. 2nd Ed. Westchester: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  6. Attarian HP, Duntley S, Brown KM. Reverse sleep state misperception. Sleep Medicine. 2004;5:269–272. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2003.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Avery DH, Khan A, Dager SR, Cohen S, Cox GB, Dunner DL. Morning or evening bright light treatment of winter depression? The significance of hypersomnia. Biological Psychiatry. 1991;29:117–126. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90040-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Broocks A, Riemann D, Hohagen F. Test-retest reliability and validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in primary insomnia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;53:737–740. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00330-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Benazzi F. Symptoms of depression as possible markers of bipolar II disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2006;30:471–477. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Billiard M, Dolenc L, Aldaz C, Ondze B, Besset A. Hypersomnia associated with mood disorders: a new perspective. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1994;(38) Suppl 1:41–47. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90134-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowden CL. A different depression: clinical distinctions between bipolar and unipolar depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2005;84:117–125. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00194-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, Andreski P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;39:411–418. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buysse D, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Lichstein KL, Morin CM. Recommendations for a standard research assessment of insomnia. Sleep. 2006;29:1155–1173. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.9.1155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buysse DJ, Hall ML, Strollo PJ, Kamarck TW, Owens J, Lee L, Reis SE, Matthews KA. Relationships between the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), and clinical/polysomnographic measures in a community sample. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2008;4:563–571. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research. 1989;28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cappuccio FP, D’Elia L, Strazzullo P, Miller MA. Sleep duration and all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Sleep. 2010;33:585–592. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.5.585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carney CE, Buysse DJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Edinger JD, Krystal AD, Lichstein KL, Morin CM. The consensus sleep diary: standardizing prospective sleep self-monitoring. Sleep. 2012;35:287–302. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carpenter JS, Andrykowski MA. Psychometric evaluation of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1998;45:5–13. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00298-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2002;9:233–255. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cho HJ, Lavretsky H, Olmstead R, Levin MJ, Oxman MN, Irwin MR. Sleep disturbance and depression recurrence in community-dwelling older adults: a prospective study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:1543–1550. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Colombo C, Benedetti F, Barbini B, Campori E, Smeraldi E. Rate of switch from depression into mania after therapeutic sleep deprivation in bipolar depression. Psychiatry Research. 1999;86:267–270. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00036-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davis JM, Chen N. Dose response and dose equivalence of antipsychotics. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004;24:192–208. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000117422.05703.ae. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dolenc L, Besset A, Billiard M. Hypersomnia in association with dysthymia in comparison with idiopathic hypersomnia and normal controls. Pflugers Archiv: European Journal of Physiology. 1996;431:R303–R304. doi: 10.1007/BF02346389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Edinger JD, Bonnet MH, Bootzin RR, Doghramji K, Dorsey CM, Espie CA, Jamieson AO, McCall WV, Morin CM, Stepanski EJ. Derivation of research diagnostic criteria for insomnia: report of an American Academy of Sleep Medicine Work Group. Sleep. 2004;27:1567–1596. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.8.1567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Edinger JD, Wyatt JK, Olsen MK, Stechuchak KM, Carney CE, Chiang A, Krystal AD, Lineberger MD, Means MK, Radtke RA, Knauss F. Reliability and validity of the Duke Structured Interview for Sleep Disorders for insomnia screening. Sleep. 2009;32:A265. [Google Scholar]
  26. Enders CK, Bandalos DL. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2001;8:430–457. [Google Scholar]
  27. First M, Spitzer R, Gibbons M, Williams J. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM IV Axis 1 Disorders: Clinician Version. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  28. Ford DE, Cooper-Patrick L. Sleep disturbances and mood disorders: an epidemiologic perspective. Depression and Anxiety. 2001;14:3–6. doi: 10.1002/da.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Frank E, Swartz HA, Kupfer DJ. Interpersonal and social rhythm therapy: managing the chaos of bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:593–604. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00969-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Frye MA, Grunze H, Suppes T, McElroy SL, Keck PE, Jr, Walden J, Leverich GS, Altshuler LL, Nakelsky S, Hwang S, Mintz J, Post RM. A placebo-controlled evaluation of adjunctive modafinil in the treatment of bipolar depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:1242–1249. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06060981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Garrett ES, Eaton WW, Zeger S. Methods for evaluating the performance of diagnostic tests in the absence of a gold standard: a latent class model approach. Statistics in Medicine. 2002;21:1289–1307. doi: 10.1002/sim.1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gehrman P, Matt GE, Turingan M, Dinh Q, Ancoli-Israel S. Towards an understanding of self-reports of sleep. Journal of Sleep Research. 2002;11:229–236. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gershon A, Thompson WK, Eidelman P, McGlinchey EL, Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Restless pillow, ruffled mind: sleep and affect coupling in interepisode bipolar disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2012;121:863–873. doi: 10.1037/a0028233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. GlaxoSmithKline. Psychotropic Prescribing Guide (Physician's Desk Reference) New York: Thompson PDR; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  35. Goel N, Banks S, Mignot E, Dinges DF. DQB1*0602 predicts interindividual differences in physiologic sleep, sleepiness, and fatigue. Neurology. 2010;75:1509–1519. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f9615d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gooneratne NS, Weaver TE, Cater JR, Pack FM, Aner HM, Greenberg AS, Pack AI. Functional outcomes of excessive daytime sleepiness in older adults. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2003;51:642–649. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0579.2003.00208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grandner MA, Kripke DF, Yoon IY, Youngstedt SD. Criterion validity of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: Investigation in a non-clinical sample. Sleep and Biological Rhythms. 2006;4:129–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8425.2006.00207.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hagenaars JA, McCutcheon AL. Applied Latent Class Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC. Multivariate Data Analysis with Readings. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  40. Harvey AG. Sleep and circadian rhythms in bipolar disorder: Seeking synchrony, harmony, and regulation. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:820–829. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Harvey AG, Schmidt DA, Scarna A, Semler CN, Goodwin GM. Sleep-related functioning in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder, patients with insomnia, and subjects without sleep problems. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:50–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Harvey AG, Tang NK. (Mis)perception of sleep in insomnia: a puzzle and a resolution. Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138:77–101. doi: 10.1037/a0025730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hayashi M, Masuda A, Hori T. The alerting effects of caffeine, bright light and face washing after a short daytime nap. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2003;114:2268–2278. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00255-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Evaluating model fit. In: Hoyle RH, editor. Structural Equation Modeling: Concepts, Issues and Applications. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1995. pp. 76–99. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
  46. Iovieno N, van Nieuwenhuizen A, Clain A, Baer L, Nierenberg AA. Residual symptoms after remission of major depressive disorder with fluoxetine and risk of relapse. Depression and Anxiety. 2011;28:137–144. doi: 10.1002/da.20768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jacobson N, Martell C, Dimidjian S. Behavioral activation treatment for depression: returning to contextual roots. Clinial Psychology Science and Practice. 2001;8:255–270. [Google Scholar]
  48. Jennum P, Kjellberg J. The socio-economical burden of hypersomnia. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. 2010;121:265–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01227.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–545. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep. 1992;15:376–381. doi: 10.1093/sleep/15.4.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jones SH, Hare DJ, Evershed K. Actigraphic assessment of circadian activity and sleep patterns in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders. 2005;7:176–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00187.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kaida K, Takahashi M, Haratani T, Otsuka Y, Fukasawa K, Nakata A. Indoor exposure to natural bright light prevents afternoon sleepiness. Sleep. 2006;29:462–469. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.4.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kanbayashi T, Kodama T, Kondo H, Satoh S, Inoue Y, Chiba S, Shimizu T, Nishino S. CSF histamine contents in narcolepsy, idiopathic hypersomnia and obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep. 2009;32:181–187. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.2.181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kane JM, Sharif ZA. Atypical antipsychotics: sedation versus efficacy. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2008;(69) Suppl 1:18–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kaplan KA, Gruber J, Eidelman P, Talbot LS, Harvey AG. Hypersomnia in inter-episode bipolar disorder: does it have prognostic significance? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;132:438–444. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Hypersomnia across mood disorders: A review and synthesis. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2009;13:275–285. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Behavioral treatment of insomnia in bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;170:716–720. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12050708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kaplan KA, Talbot LS, Gruber J, Harvey AG. Evaluating sleep in bipolar disorder: comparison between actigraphy, polysomnography, and sleep diary. Bipolar Disorders. 2012;14:870–879. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. King AC, Oman RF, Brassington GS, Bliwise DL, Haskell WL. Moderate-intensity exercise and self-rated quality of sleep in older adults. A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1997;277:32–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Knutson KL, Ryden AM, Mander BA, Van Cauter E. Role of sleep duration and quality in the risk and severity of type 2 diabetes mellitus. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2006;166:1768–1774. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.16.1768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Koffel E, Watson D. The two-factor structure of sleep complaints and its relation to depression and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:183–194. doi: 10.1037/a0013945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lam DH, Watkins ER, Hayward P, Bright J, Wright K, Kerr N, Parr-Davis G, Sham P. A randomized controlled study of cognitive therapy for relapse prevention for bipolar affective disorder: outcome of the first year. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:145–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Leibenluft E, Clark CH, Myers FS. The reproducibility of depressive and hypomanic symptoms across repeated episodes in patients with rapid-cycling bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1995;33:83–88. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)00077-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001;88:767–778. [Google Scholar]
  65. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods. 1999;4:84–99. [Google Scholar]
  66. Marsh HW, Balla JR, McDonald RP. Goodness-of-fit indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: The effect of sample size. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;103:391–410. [Google Scholar]
  67. Matza LS, Revicki DA, Davidson JR, Stewart JW. Depression with atypical features in the National Comorbidity Survey: classification, description, and consequences. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:817–826. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McLachlan GJ, Peel D. Finite Mixture Models. New York: John Wiley; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mercer JD, Bootzin RR, Lack LC. Insomniacs’ perception of wake instead of sleep. Sleep. 2002;25:564–571. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Millar A, Espie CA, Scott J. The sleep of remitted bipolar outpatients: A controlled naturalistic study using actigraphy. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2004;80:145–153. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00055-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Morgenthaler T, Alessi C, Friedman L, Owens J, Kapur V, Boehlecke B, Brown T, Chesson A, Jr, Coleman J, Lee-Chiong T, Pancer J, Swick TJ. Practice parameters for the use of actigraphy in the assessment of sleep and sleep disorders: an update for 2007. Sleep. 2007;30:519–529. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.4.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Morin CM, Espie CA. Insomnia : A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  73. Muthén B. Latent Variable Analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  74. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  75. Nofzinger EA, Thase ME, Reynolds 3rd CF, Himmelhoch JM, Mallinger A, Houck P, Kupfer DJ. Hypersomnia in bipolar depression: A comparison with narcolepsy using the multiple sleep latency test. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1991;148:1177–1181. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.9.1177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Nylund KL, Asparoutiov T, Muthen BO. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
  77. Ohayon MM, Dauvilliers Y, Reynolds CF., 3rd Operational definitions and algorithms for excessive sleepiness in the general population: implications for DSM-5 nosology. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:71–79. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Ohayon MM, Reynolds CF, 3rd, Dauvilliers Y. Excessive sleep duration and quality of life. Annals of Neurology. 2013;73:785–794. doi: 10.1002/ana.23818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Parker G, Malhi G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Parker K. Sleeping in? The impact of age and depressive sub-type on hypersomnia. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2006;90:73–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. PDR Staff. Physicians' Desk Reference 2008: Hospital/Library Version. Stamford: Thomson Healthcare; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  81. Phillips ML, Travis MJ, Fagiolini A, Kupfer DJ. Medication effects in neuroimaging studies of bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:313–320. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Plante DT, Landsness EC, Peterson MJ, Goldstein MR, Wanger T, Guokas JJ, Tononi G, Benca RM. Altered slow wave activity in major depressive disorder with hypersomnia: a high density EEG pilot study. Psychiatry Research. 2012;201:240–244. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Plante DT, Winkelman JW. Sleep disturbance in bipolar disorder: therapeutic implications. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:830–843. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Roberts R, Roberts C, Chen I. Functioning of adolescents wtih symptoms of disturbed sleep. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2001;30:1–18. [Google Scholar]
  85. Roecklein KA, Carney CE, Wong PM, Steiner JL, Hasler BP, Franzen PL. The role of beliefs and attitudes about sleep in seasonal and nonseasonal mood disorder, and nondepressed controls. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;150:466–473. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.04.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Rush AJ, Gullion CM, Basco MR, Jarrett RB, Trivedi MH. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties. Psychological Medicine. 1996;26:477–486. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sachs GS, Printz DJ, Kahn DA, Carpenter D, Docherty JP. The expert consensus guideline series: medication treatment of bipolar disorder. Postgraduate Medicine. 2000;1:1–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sackeim HA. The definition and meaning of treatment-resistant depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2001;(62) Suppl 16:10–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;86:420–428. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Skre I, Onstad S, Torgersen S, Kringlen E. High interrater reliability for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Axis I (SCID-I) Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1991;84:167–173. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb03123.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Soehner AM, Kaplan KA, Harvey AG. Prevalence and clinical correlates of co-occurring insomnia and hypersomnia symptoms in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2014;167:93–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.05.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Staff P. Physicians’ Desk Reference : PDR. Oradell: Medical Economics Co.; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  93. Talbot LS, Hairston IS, Eidelman P, Gruber J, Harvey AG. The effect of mood on sleep onset latency and REM sleep in interepisode bipolar disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:448–458. doi: 10.1037/a0016605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tam EM, Lam RW, Robertson HA, Stewart JN, Yatham LN, Zis AP. Atypical depressive symptoms in seasonal and non-seasonal mood disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1997;44:39–44. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)01447-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Trajanovic NN, Radivojevic V, Kaushansky Y, Shapiro CM. Positive sleep state misperception - a new concept of sleep misperception. Sleep Medicine. 2007;8:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Trivedi MH, Rush AJ, Ibrahim HM, Carmody TJ, Biggs MM, Suppes T, Crismon ML, Shores-Wilson K, Toprac MG, Dennehy EB, Witte B, Kashner TM. The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rating (IDS-C) and Self-Report (IDS-SR), and the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, Clinician Rating (QIDS-C) and Self-Report (QIDS-SR) in public sector patients with mood disorders: a psychometric evaluation. Psychological Medicine. 2004;34:73–82. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703001107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Kales A, Criley C, Vela-Bueno A. Differences in nocturnal and daytime sleep between primary and psychiatric hypersomnia: diagnostic and treatment implications. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2000;62:220–226. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200003000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wehr TA, Sack DA, Rosenthal NE. Sleep reduction as a final common pathway in the genesis of mania. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1987;144:201–204. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.2.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Williams JB, Gibbon M, First MB, Spitzer RL, Davies M, Borus J, Howes MJ, Kane J, Pope HG, Jr, Rounsaville B, et al. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). II. Multisite test-retest reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1992;49:630–636. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820080038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Brent DA, Balach L, Dahl RE, Ryan ND. Atypical symptoms of depression in a sample of depressed child and adolescent outpatients. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:1253–1259. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200010000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Wohlgemuth WK, Edinger JD, Fins AI, Sullivan RJ., Jr How many nights are enough? The short-term stability of sleep parameters in elderly insomniacs and normal sleepers. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:233–244. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  103. Worthington J, Fava M, Davidson K, Alpert JE, Nierenger A, Rosenbaum J. Patterns of improvement in depressive symptoms with fluoxetine treatment. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1995;31:223–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Young R, Biggs J, Ziegler V, Meyer D. A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity and sensitivity. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1978;133:429–425. doi: 10.1192/bjp.133.5.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Zimmerman M, McGlinchey JB, Posternak MA, Friedman M, Boerescu D, Attiullah N. Differences between minimally depressed patients who do and do not consider themselves to be in remission. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2005;66:1134–1138. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v66n0908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES