Abstract
Numerous theoretical reasons have been posited about why more schooling might improve health. Adult health outcomes and behaviors generally are significantly associated with schooling. However, such associations do not necessarily imply that schooling has causal effects on health outcomes and behaviors. Causal estimates based on schooling variation from policies and from within-MZ (monozygotic) twins have reached mixed conclusions. This study contributed new estimates of cross-sectional associations and within-MZ causal effects using three relatively large US twins samples. The estimates suggested that schooling was significantly associated with numerous health outcomes and behaviors. However, with within-MZ twins control for unobserved factors, schooling was no longer associated with most indicators of better health (with the exception of self-reported health), while it continued to be associated with outcomes such as fertility and spousal schooling. Similar patterns were observed for spousal schooling.
Keywords: Health, schooling, causal effects, identical (monozygotic) twins estimators
Introduction
More-schooled individuals tend to have better adult health outcomes and behaviors. Theoretical models postulate that these associations arise because schooling causally affects adult health through several mechanisms (Figure 1): increasing incomes, and thus enabling purchasing better health care (particularly in the US and similar institutional contexts); improving one’s rank in society, which is associated with better adult health and reduced stress and poor mental health related to actual and/or perceived social deprivation (Rose & Marmot, 1981); increasing interactions with other more-schooled peers, generating health spillovers (Fletcher 2010); lowering discount rates and extending life expectancies, thereby making individuals more patient and increasing their incentives to engage in healthier behaviors (Becker & Mulligan, 1997); increasing productive efficiency to produce more health from given inputs (Grossman, 1972); and increasing allocative efficiency through selecting inputs better for health production (Grossman 1972). While these mechanisms suggest that schooling improves health, some pathways can operate in the opposite direction. For example, higher earnings caused by more schooling increases opportunity costs of time, which may adversely affect health as many health inputs are time-intensive (e.g., doctor visits, exercising). Yet, most theoretical discussions argue that the pathways resulting in positive effects of schooling on health dominate such adverse possibilities.
Given the multitude of mechanisms through which schooling can potentially affect adult health (Figure 1), schooling and schooling-related socioeconomic differences have been characterized as ‘fundamental causes’ of adult health and societal health disparities’ (Link & Phlean, 1995).
However, it is important to emphasize that the frequently-observed empirical associations between schooling and health do not necessarily reflect causal relationships postulated by the above theories. These empirical associations could be confounded by unobserved genetic and social endowments at the bottom of Figure 1 such as (parental/own) preferences, abilities, cognitive functioning, parental economic/social resources that jointly affect schooling and adult health. For example, Cutler and Lleras-Muney (2010) found that 20 percent of the schooling-health gradient was driven by cognitive ability. Moreover, there is also possibly reverse causation, insofar as health (particularly early life health) affects schooling (Currie & Stabile, 2006; Royer, 2009).
Several recent studies have used schooling variation arising from policy changes, e.g. compulsory schooling laws, to estimate causal effects of schooling by using such changes as instrumental variables (IV) or employing regression discontinuity (RD) designs (comparing schooling and health outcomes of individuals just before and just after reforms). These studies, summarized below, have found mixed results; large significant effects in some cases and small or insignificant effects in others.
An alternative strategy to using policy-induced schooling variation is to use variation in schooling within MZ (monozygotic; identical) twin pairs. MZ twins share the same genetic makeup and family-rearing environment. By relating twin-pair differences in health to twin-pair differences in schooling, schooling effects on health are estimated controlling for unobserved factors that affect schooling and health of both twins such as the genetic and social endowments in the bottom of Figure 1. In contrast to a substantial literature using twins data from European countries and Australia, only two published studies applied this within-MZ twins approach to US data. Both Fujiwara and Kawachi (2009) and Lundborg (2013) used 1995 Midlife in the United States Survey (MIDUS) data on 694–701 MZ twins. Both studies found no significant effects of schooling on health outcomes (self-reported health, perceived mental health, number of chronic conditions, body mass index/BMI) and behaviors (smoking, exercise). Lundborg (2013), however, found significant positive impacts of schooling on self-reported health and negative impacts on number of chronic conditions when using dichotomous schooling indicators for high school, some college and college degree instead of continuous schooling measures.
The present study provided more evidence of schooling health-gradients using within-MZ twins methodology and much larger US twin datasets than in MIDUS: the Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry, Minnesota Twin Registry and NAS-NRC Twin Registry of WWII Military Veterans. These new explorations with larger samples permitted assessment of whether the mostly insignificant results reported in the previous US twins studies were in part artifacts of relatively small samples. We found that more schooling was associated cross-sectionally with better health outcomes and behaviors, consistent with much previous literature and most theoretical pathways through which schooling is hypothesized to affect health as discussed above. However, after controlling for unobserved social/genetic endowments in the bottom of Figure 1 through within-MZ twins estimators, there generally was no association between schooling and health with the exception of self-reported health (though not to the extent that cross-sectional associations suggested). There remained associations in within-MZ analyses between schooling and fertility or spousal schooling, indicating that the lack of associations between schooling and health was not primarily driven by inadequate power for within-MZ analyses to detect associations between schooling and important life-course outcomes. Finally, we investigated whether there were positive spillovers of spousal schooling on own health. We found that spousal schooling was associated cross-sectionally with better own health outcomes and behaviors, but not within-MZ twin pairs.
Previous Studies
Several studies examined schooling-mortality gradients in developed economies, with mixed results. Lleras-Muney (2005) used compulsory schooling laws as an IV, and found that an extra schooling grade reduced 10-year mortality rates for US whites by 60 percent. Mazumder (2008), however, showed that this conclusion was not robust to inclusion of state-specific trends. RD estimates showed that schooling had no effect on mortality between age 46–69 in the UK (Clark & Royer, 2013) and on survival rates at ages 50 and 80 in France (Albouy & Lequien, 2009). However, schooling was found to reduce probabilities of dying between age 81–88 by 4 to 6 percent in the Netherlands (van Kippersluis, O’Donnell & van Doorslaer, 2011). Meghir, Palme and Simeonova (2012) found that a Swedish schooling reform led to declines in male mortality at ages 40–49, which was offset by higher mortality at ages 50–59 – but with no effect overall on mortality up to age 60.
Other studies used variation from compulsory schooling laws to identify schooling effects on other health outcomes and behaviors. Schooling improved US and Swedish self-reported health (Mazumder, 2008; Spasojevic, 2010), but had no effect in Denmark (Arendt, 2005). Clark and Royer (2013) found no relationship between schooling and various health outcomes and behaviors in the UK. Some studies found gender differences. Schooling reduced long-term illness of men, but not women in Germany (Kemptner, Jürges & Reinhold, 2011). Grabner (2008), and Brunnello, Fabbri and Fort (2013) found that schooling reduced BMI and probabilities of being overweight for US and European women, but not men. Schooling improved US and UK old-age cognitive functioning (Glymour, Kawachi, Jencks, & Berkman, 2008; Banks & Mazzonna, 2012), and reduced probabilities of reporting poor health and having diabetes among the elderly in Europe (Fonseca & Zheng, 2011).
Compulsory schooling laws that affect high school do not yield insights about health returns to college or other tertiary education. Some studies therefore instrumented men’s college completion using college attainment variation induced by draft avoidance behavior during the Vietnam War. They found college completion reduced mortality, smoking, BMI and overweight (De Walque, 2007; MacInnis, 2006; Buckles, Ofer, Morreil & Wozinak, 2012).
The within-MZ twins approach was applied to twins data from other developed countries, in addition to the two US studies noted above, and resulted in mixed conclusions. Behrman et al. (2011) investigated relations between schooling and hospitalization and mortality using data on 5,294 MZ Danish twins. They found significant cross-sectional associations, but no significant schooling effects with within-MZ twin estimators. Lundborg, Lyttkens and Nysted (2012) used data on 18,696 MZ Swedish twins; their within-MZ twins estimates indicated that an additional schooling grade reduced the mortality hazard by 6 percent. Webbink, Martin and Visscher. (2010) found some indications of gender differences, with schooling reducing being overweight for men but not for women based on Australian MZ twins.
Datasets
The Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry (MATR)
The MATR is a population-based registry of twin pairs ascertained from birth records and school system records of Virginia, North Carolina, and South Carolina (Anderson, Beverly, Corey & Murrelle, 2002). Our analyses were based on the Virginia 30K MATR sample, which were mailed health and lifestyle questionnaires in 1987. Completed questionnaires were obtained from 14,763 twins, approximately 70 percent. We used white MZ twin pairs aged 25–75, with non-missing information on self-reported schooling, reports on co-twin’s schooling, and health. Sample sizes ranged from 3,392 to 4,328.
A question on schooling had options for: (1) 0–7 grades of elementary school; (2) 8 grades of elementary school; (3) 1–3 grades of high school; (4) 4 grades of high school; (5) 1–3 years of college; and (6) 4 or more years of college. These categories were recoded as 5, 8, 10, 12, 14 and 16 schooling grades.
Self-reported health was measured by “how would you rate your health in the past 12 months” with responses of 1=very poor, 2=poor, 3=average, 4=good and 5=very good. BMI was calculated from self-reported height and weight; overweight was defined as BMI ≥ 25. Never-smoked was equal to 1 if the twin had never smoked and 0 otherwise. Twins were asked how many alcoholic drinks they consumed in a typical week with options for 0, 1–3, 4–6, 7–12, 13–18, 19–24, 25–42 and > 42 drinks per week. We recoded these categories as 0, 2, 5, 8.5, 15.5, 21.5, 33.5 and 53. Leisure exercise options were: no exercise; occasional exercise; regular exercise about once a week; exercise a couple of times a week, or jogging, cycling to work or vigorous sport activity at least 3–4 times a week. A dichotomous exercise variable was defined as 1 if the twin reported regular exercise once or more a week and 0 otherwise.
The Minnesota Twin Registry (MTR)
The MTR contains birth records on all twins born in Minnesota in 1936–1955 (Lykken, Bouchard, McGue, & Tellegen, 2002). We used data from the Socioeconomic Survey of Twins, a questionnaire sent in 1994 to 6,638 members of same sex twin pairs. We used samples ranging from 1,290 to 1,310 of white MZ twin pairs, with non-missing information on self-reported schooling, co-twin’s schooling, and health.
Twins were asked to report their own schooling, their co-twin’s schooling, and their spouse’s schooling. We assigned actual grades of schooling if no high school diploma, 11 if GED, 12 if high school diploma, 13 if vocational diploma, 14 if associate degree, 16 if college degree, 18 if masters degree, 19 if JD or MBA and 20 if doctoral degree.
The questionnaire contained a limited number of health questions. Twins were asked “how would you rate your health at the present time?” with responses for 1=bad, 2=poor, 3=fair, 4=good, 5=excellent. BMI was calculated from self-reported height and weight. No information on health behaviors was available.
NAS-NRC Twin Registry of WWII Veterans
The NAS-NRC twin registry consists of 15,924 white male twins born in 1917–1927, both of whom served in the armed forces. Twins were mailed questionnaires in 1967, 1983 and 2000. We used data from the 2000 survey, which contained information on both health and schooling. 2,060 twin pairs responded to the questionnaire (Page, 2002). We used MZ twin pairs, with non-missing information on self-reported schooling and health. Sample sizes ranged from 1,726 to 1,902.
Schooling was measured through a question asking “what is your highest grade level, diploma, degree completed?”, with options for none, 1 grade, 2 grades, …, 11 grades, high school or GED, 1 year of trade school after high school, 1, 2 or 3 years of college, bachelors degree, 1 year of graduate work, masters degree, some doctoral work, doctoral degree. We assigned actual grades if high school was not completed, 12 for graduating from high school, 13 for 1 year of trade school/ 1 year of college, 14 for 2 years of college, 15 for 3 years of college, 16 for a bachelors degree, 17 for 1 year of graduate work, 18 for a masters degree, 19 for some doctoral work and 20 for a doctoral degree.
Self-reported health was measured by “in general would you say your health is” 1=poor, 2=fair, 3=good, 4=very good, 5=excellent. BMI was calculated from self-reported height and weight. Twins were asked whether their health limited (1) vigorous activities, (2) moderate activities, (3) lifting or carrying groceries, (4) climbing several flights of stairs, (5) climbing one flight of stairs, (6) bending, kneeling or stooping, (7) walking more than one mile, (8) walking several blocks, (9) walking one block and (10) bathing or dressing yourself. The questions had 3 possible responses- 1=no, not limited at all, 2=yes limited a little, 3=yes, limited a lot. We created overall measures encapsulating how much health limited activities by summing responses to the 10 questions. We also examined the number of health problems that twins had in old age; twins were asked whether they had the following problems – abdominal or aortic aneurysm, brain aneurysm, rheumatoid arthritis, diverticulitis, emphysema, hemorrhoids or piles, hiatal hernia, kidney stone liver cirrhosis, prostate enlargement, cataracts, glaucoma, macular hole in retina, macular degeneration. For health behaviors, we used an indicator variable for having never smoked.
Statistical Analysis
Figure 2 illustrates how unobserved endowments may determine schooling and health. Schooling of twin i in pair j (Sij) is determined by individual-specific shocks (νij) and unobserved genetic and social endowments (AjS and CjS) that are shared by MZ twins, particularly those reared together. These endowments, for example, reflect abilities, preferences, parental schooling, family income, and childhood neighborhoods, including local schools. Schooling potentially has direct causal effects on the health of twin i in pair j (Hij). However, health is also determined by unobserved genetic and social endowments (AjS , AjH , CjS , CjH ), some of which potentially affect both schooling and health (AjS ,CjS ). Health is also affected by individual-specific shocks (εij). Combining the effect of endowments on health into a single term µj results in a reduced-form relationship between schooling Sij and health Hij that includes both endowments µj and individual-specific shocks εij.
(1) |
The key empirical question addressed in this paper is if the causal pathway β from schooling to health persists once unobserved endowments affecting both schooling and health are controlled. Conventional cross-sectional analyses do not answer this question. OLS regressions of relation (1) provide associations between schooling and health, which are biased estimates of causal effects β of schooling on health if endowments (AjS and CjS in Figure 2) jointly affect schooling and health and result in a correlation between the unobserved term µj and Sij in relation (1).
The importance of endowments that jointly affect schooling and health is suggested by a large theoretical and empirical literature that highlights roles for both schooling and health of parental social/economic backgrounds, in-utero-conditions, and genetic effects on abilities and cognitive function (Almond & Currie, 2011; Becker 1991; Behrman, Taubman & Pollak, 1995; McGue & Bouchard, 1998). As a result, the coefficient β obtained from OLS estimation of relation (1) is biased, upward (downward) if unobserved factors affect both schooling and health in the same (opposite) direction.
Our analyses rest on the insight that unobserved endowments affecting both schooling and health can be controlled through within-MZ twin estimators.
(2) |
In this relation, the influence of unobserved factors µj is differenced out as MZ twins are genetically identical, grow up together, and share many other socioeconomic contexts such as schools and neighborhoods.
We note three potential problems with within-MZ twins estimators. First, attenuation bias due to measurement error is exacerbated by first differencing. To deal with measurement error, we instrumented differences in self-reported schooling with differences in co-twin’s reports (Ashenfelter & Krueger, 1994). Second, estimates are identified by schooling differences within MZ twin pairs. If these differences are small then it may require many observations to identify effects. We therefore examined these differences below and found them large compared to effects of compulsory schooling laws and sufficient to obtain significance for other non-health outcomes. Third, the estimated value of β is upward- (downward-) biased if factors leading to differences in schooling directly affect schooling and health (in addition to any indirect effect on health through schooling) in the same (opposite) direction (Bound and Solon, 1999; Behrman et al. 2011, Kohler, Behrman & Schnittker, 2011).
For example, schooling differences between MZ twins may originate in youth health, that also directly affect adult health. Both the MATR and MTR surveys asked twins whether they have ever suffered from any health conditions such as high blood pressure, asthma, hearing impairment, depression, and if so at what age these conditions were diagnosed. We attempted to control for early life health differences by including an indicator = 1 if the twin was diagnosed with a health condition at age 16 or younger in all cross-sectional and within-MZ twins regressions.
Relation (3) was used to estimate effects of spousal schooling on own health. Relation (3) is the same as relation (1), but also includes schooling of the spouse of twin i in pair j (Spij).
(3) |
In conventional cross-sectional analyses, estimated cross-spouse effects Δ are biased because of unobserved endowments that are correlated with spousal schooling and also directly affect own health. For example, high-ability individuals tend to have better-schooled spouses and better own health. The bias due to unobserved factors is differenced out by contrasting health of MZ twins with different spousal schooling in relation (4), an approach used for the first time to our knowledge in this and the concurrent study by Behrman, Xiong and Zhang (2014).
(4) |
Because within-MZ schooling differences exist over most schooling levels, within-MZ estimates are likely to be closer to average treatment effects (ATE) than local average treatment effects (LATE) (Moffitt, 2009). IV-approaches that rely on compulsory schooling variations to predict schooling yield LATE relevant for individuals affected by instruments used (e.g., close to completing only compulsory schooling) but not ATE for broader populations (Behrman et al. 2011). For understanding the role of schooling differentials for population-level health gradients, and for informing policies that aim at addressing health differences through enhancements of schooling levels, ATE (or approximations thereof) as provided by within-MZ approaches are likely preferable to LATE estimates that based on compulsory schooling regulations.
Results
Table 1 provides descriptive statistics. Average twins ages at the survey times were 46 (MTR), 52 (MATR) and 74 years (NAS-NRC). The majority of MTR and MATR twins were female. Average schooling grades attained were similar in the MTR and NAS-NRC (approximately 14 grades), and lower in the MATR (13.5 grades). Only 2 percent of twins did not graduate from high school in the MTR, compared to 9 and 12 percent in the MATR and NAS-NRC. The proportions of twins that graduated from high school, had some post-high school schooling, and had bachelor degrees were similar across datasets. On average MATR twin pairs had differences of 0.69 grades and 71 percent of twin pairs had no difference in grades. In the MTR and NAS-NRC, 51 and 43 percent of twin pairs had the same grades of schooling, and on average twin pairs had differences of 1.1 and 1.4 grades. The within-MZ twin pair variation in schooling was quite substantial when compared to the estimate that compulsory schooling laws in the US only increased schooling by 0.05 grades (Lleras-Muney, 2005).
Table 1.
MATR | MTR | NAS-NRC | |
---|---|---|---|
(1) | (2) | (3) | |
Background Characteristics |
|||
Age | 52.41 (15.04) | 46.63 (5.49) | 74.16 (2.76) |
25–34 | 0.22 (0.40) | ||
35–44 | 0.12 (0.33) | 0.42 (0.49) | |
45–54 | 0.12 (0.33) | 0.47 (0.50) | |
55–64 | 0.29 (0.45) | 0.11 (0.31) | |
65–75 | 0.26 (0.44) | 0.66 (0.47) | |
76–85 | 0.30 (0.46) | ||
Female | 0.71 (0.45) | 0.65 (0.47) | 0.00 (0.00) |
Grades of schooling | 13.52 (2.10) | 14.03 | 14.02 (3.00) |
Absolute within-twin difference in grades of schooling |
0.69 (1.17) | 1.10 (1.52) | 1.40 (1.87) |
Difference in grades of Schooling |
|||
0 | 0.71 (0.46) | 0.51 (0.50) | 0.43 (0.48) |
1 | 0.19 (0.39) | 0.23 (0.42) | |
2 | 0.25 (0.43) | 0.14 (0.35) | 0.14 (0.35) |
3 | 0.00 (0.05) | 0.05 (0.22) | 0.06 (0.24) |
4 | 0.04 (0.18) | 0.07 (0.25) | 0.07 (0.26) |
5 or more | 0.01 (0.07) | 0.04 (0.18) | 0.06 (0.24) |
Less than 12 grades | 0.09 (0.28) | 0.02 (0.12) | 0.12 (0.32) |
12 grades | 0.34 (0.48) | 0.33 (0.47) | 0.25 (0.43) |
13–15 grades | 0.26 (0.44) | 0.30 (0.46) | 0.25 (0.43) |
16 grades or more | 0.31 (0.46) | 0.35 (0.48) | 0.38 (0.48) |
Co-twin’s grades of schooling |
13.43 (2.12) | 13.91 (2.37) | |
Spouse’s grades of schooling |
13.46 (2.34) | 13.81 (2.47) | |
Early health problem | 0.07 (0.25) | 0.16 (0.36) | |
Health Outcomes | |||
Self-reported health | 4.34 (0.86) | 4.37 (0.67) | 3.48 (0.98) |
BMI | 24.54 (4.27) | 25.74 (4.49) | 25.79 (3.31) |
Overweight | 0.38 (0.47) | 0.50 (0.50) | 0.58 (0.49) |
Limiting activities | 14.54 (4.90) | ||
Number of health problems | 1.97 (1.48) | ||
Health Behaviors | |||
Never Smoked | 0.53 (0.50) | 0.37 (0.48) | |
Exercise | 0.52 (0.50) | ||
Alcohol Consumption | 2.48 (5.81) | ||
Absolute within-twin pair difference in |
|||
Self-reported health | 0.58 (0.76) | 0.48 (0.63) | 0.82 (0.78) |
BMI | 2.08 (2.28) | 2.49 (2.44) | 2.28 (2.01) |
Number of health problems | 1.22 (1.05) | ||
Limiting activities | 3.84 (4.29) | ||
Alcohol consumption | 2.45 (5.68) | ||
Proportion of twin pairs that differ in |
|||
Overweight | 0.19 (0.40) | 0.23 (0.42) | 0.30 (0.45) |
Never Smoked | 0.20 (0.40) | 0.20 (0.40) | |
Exercise | 0.33 (0.47) |
Notes: Standard deviations in parentheses.
Within-MZ twins estimates are closer to ATE if within-MZ twin pair schooling differences are distributed across the schooling distribution. Appendix table 1 summarizes differences in schooling for twin pairs in which at least one twin has (1) 12 schooling grades or lower, (2) 13–15 grades of schooling or (3) 16 grades of schooling or more. The most variation for the MATR and NAS-NRC occurred when at least one twin had 13–15 schooling grades (1.24 and 1.91 grades). For the MTR the most variation occurred in twin pairs where one twin had 16 schooling grades or more (1.94 grades).
Average self-reported health was similar in the MATR and MTR (4.34 and 4.37) but substantially lower in the NAS-NRC (3.48). Average BMI was similar in all datasets. Approximately half of twins were overweight in the MATR and NAS-NRC; in the MTR only 38 percent of twins were overweight. In the MTR 16 percent of twins had an early health problem, more than double the proportion in the MATR.
There was also fairly substantial within-twin pair variation in health measures. The absolute within-twin pair difference in self-reported health was 0.48 in the MTR, 0.58 in the MATR and and much higher in the NAS-NRC (0.84). In all three datasets, absolute within-twin pair differences in BMI were over 2 units, and over 18 percent of twin pairs were discordant on overweight status. 20 percent of twin pairs differed on smoking status in the MATR and NAS-NRC. The absolute within-twin pair difference in number of health problems for the NAS-NRC twins was 1.22.
The main results are in table 2. The MATR and MTR datasets were combined for self-reported health, BMI, overweight to improve precision. For these outcomes, the null hypothesis of equality of coefficients in the within-MZ twins regressions was not rejected at the 5 percent level. Cross-sectional regressions in the MATR-MTR dataset controlled for age, age squared, gender, early health problems and if the twin was from the MATR. Cross-sectional regressions in the NAS-NRC dataset controlled for age and age squared.
Table 2.
Cross-Section | Cross-Section with measurement error |
Within-MZ Twins | Within-MZ Twins for twin pairs where one has ≥16 & other has ≤14 grades |
Within-MZ Twins with measurement error |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | |
MATR-MTR | ||||||||||
Self-reported health | 0.07 | 0.06 to 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.06 to 0.08 | 0.04 | 0.01 to 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.01 to 0.07 | 0.05 | −0.01 to 0.10 |
N | 5638 | 5638 | 2819 | 367 | 2819 | |||||
BMI | −0.23 | −0.27 to −0.17 | −0.28 | −0.34 to −0.21 | −0.02 | −0.11 to 0.07 | 0.03 | −0.11 to 0.17 | −0.03 | −0.22 to 0.16 |
N | 5562 | 5562 | 2781 | 364 | 2781 | |||||
Overweight | −0.02 | −0.03 to −0.02 | −0.03 | −0.04 to −0.02 | −0.00 | −0.01 to 0.01 | −0.00 | −0.01 to 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.04 to 0.02 |
N | 5562 | 5562 | 2781 | 364 | 2781 | |||||
MATR | ||||||||||
Never Smoked | 0.02 | 0.01 to 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.01 to 0.03 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.02 to 0.02 | 0.01 | −0.04 to 0.06 |
N | 4126 | 4126 | 2063 | 256 | 2063 | |||||
Alcohol Consumption | 0.09 | −0.02 to 0.18 | 0.12 | 0.01 to 0.22 | −0.06 | −0.29 to 0.17 | 0.17 | −0.22 to 0.57 | −0.35 | −1.25 to 0.56 |
N | 3392 | 3392 | 1696 | 207 | 1696 | |||||
Exercise | 0.04 | 0.03 to 0.05 | 0.04 | 0.03 to 0.05 | 0.01 | −0.01 to 0.03 | 0.02 | −0.01 to 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.01 to 0.14 |
N | 4240 | 4240 | 2120 | 258 | 2120 | |||||
NAS-NRC | ||||||||||
Self-reported health | 0.07 | 0.05 to 0.08 | 0.03 | −0.00 to 0.07 | 0.02 | −0.02 to 0.08 | ||||
N | 1868 | 934 | 116 | |||||||
BMI | −0.12 | −0.16 to 0.07 | −0.06 | −0.15 to −0.04 | −0.05 | −0.18 to 0.08 | ||||
N | 1844 | 922 | 114 | |||||||
Overweight | −0.01 | −0.21 to −0.01 | −0.00 | −0.02 to 0.01 | −0.00 | −0.02 to 0.22 | ||||
N | 1844 | 922 | 114 | |||||||
Limiting Activities | −0.26 | −0.33 to −0.18 | −0.03 | −0.19 to 0.13 | 0.06 | −0.16 to 0.28 | ||||
N | 1726 | 863 | 107 | |||||||
No. of health problems | −0.01 | −0.03 to 0.02 | −0.03 | −0.08 to 0.06 | −0.02 | −0.09 to 0.05 | ||||
N | 1342 | 671 | 81 | |||||||
Never Smoked | 0.03 | 0.02 to 0.04 | 0.01 | −0.01 to 0.02 | 0.01 | −0.01 to 0.03 | ||||
N | 1902 | 951 | 117 |
Notes: Cross sectional regressions in MATR-MTR control for age, age squared, gender, early health problems and an indicator for MATR. Cross-sectional regressions for MATR control for age, age squared, gender and early health problems. Cross-sectional regressions in NAS-NRC control for age and age squared. For the cross-section corrected for measurement error, twin 1’s schooling is instrumented by twin 2’s report of twin 1’s schooling and vice versa. Within-MZ twins estimates for MATR-MTR and MATR control for early health problems. For within-MZ twins estimates corrected for measurement error, the difference in self-reported schooling is instrumented by the difference in the co-twin’s report of the other’s schooling. 95% confidence intervals based on standard errors clustered by twin pairs.
The cross-sectional estimates indicated that schooling was associated with higher self-reported health, lower BMI and lower probabilities of being overweight. Moreover, for the NAS-NRC twins, more schooling was associated with fewer health-limiting problems. However, there was no relationship between schooling and the total number of health problems in old age. More schooling was associated with better health behaviors in the MATR, in particular with higher probabilities of having never smoked and of undertaking some exercise in leisure time. Surprisingly, more schooling was associated with more alcohol consumption in the MATR. The positive association may be plausible if people believed that there were health benefits of modest alcohol consumption or if alcohol was more affordable to more-schooled individuals due to higher income. These associations increased in magnitude once measurement error was controlled by instrumenting self-reported schooling with reports from co-twins.
Compared to cross-sectional estimates, within-MZ twins estimates indicated that schooling was not associated with BMI, probability of being overweight in the MATR-MTR dataset, or with any health behaviors in the MATR, or with old-age health in the NAS-NRC. In case within-MZ twin pair schooling variation was not sufficient to identify positive schooling effects, we restricted the sample to twin pairs where one twin had 16 grades of schooling or more, and the other twin had 14 grades or less. One might expect it more likely to find schooling effects within twin pairs with these larger schooling differences. However, within-MZ twins estimates on this restricted sub-sample still showed no associations between schooling and health.
The only exception was that schooling was still significantly associated with better self-reported health within-MZ twins in the MATR-MTR dataset. An extra schooling grade increased self-reported health by 0.04 units, on a scale of 0–5 (95% CI 0.01 to 0.06). When self-reported schooling was instrumented using co-twins’ reports of the others’ schooling to control for random measurement error the point estimate increased to 0.05 but no longer was statistically significant (95% CI −0.01 to 0.10).
Appendix table 2 reports results by gender. There were some indications of gender differences, with schooling associated with larger declines in BMI for women, and higher probability of never smoking for men in cross-sectional estimates. No significant gender differences were indicated in the within-MZ twins estimates. Appendix table 3 shows results by different age groups (25–44, 45–65, 65–75). There were significant cross-sectional associations between schooling and better health outcomes across age groups, which were similar in magnitude in the MATR-MTR dataset. The within-MZ twin pair analyses indicated smaller associations that were not significant. For health behaviors, schooling was associated with higher probabilities of exercising for all age groups. Positive associations between schooling and never smoking (alcohol consumption) were only for those aged 25–44 (65–75). Within-MZ twins estimates, as in table 2, indicated no significant associations.
Table 3 explores nonlinear effects of having (1) some post-high school schooling (13–15 grades) and (2) bachelors degree or higher (16 grades or more) relative to having graduated high school or lower (12 grades or lower). No measurement-error-corrected IV estimates are presented because with binary variables measurement error is non-classical. The cross-sectional results indicated that those with some post-high school schooling and those with bachelors degrees had better health outcomes and behaviors compared to those who graduated from high school or lower. The coefficient on having bachelors degrees or higher was almost twice the coefficient on some post-high school schooling, suggesting that there were larger health returns from completing college. Controlling for unobserved factors using within-MZ twins estimators produced estimates that generally were smaller in magnitude and statistically insignificant, both in the full sample of MZ twins and in the sub-sample with large schooling differences. Also note, as before there was still some indication that schooling led to better self-reported health in the MATR-MTR dataset, even after controlling for endowments. Further note that the null hypothesis of equal coefficients between the MATR and MTR datasets in the within-MZ twins regression for overweight status was rejected. This was driven by the surprising finding that in the MTR, individuals with college degrees were 11 percentage points more likely to be overweight relative to high school graduates or lower, whereas they were 6 percentage points less likely to be overweight in the MATR.
Table 3.
Cross-Section | Within-MZ Twins | Within-MZ Twins for twin pairs where one twin has ≥16 and other twin has ≤14 grades |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | |
MATR-MTR | ||||||
Self-reported health | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.16 | 0.10 to 0.22 | 0.06 | −0.03 to 0.15 | −0.03 | −0.25 to 0.19 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.30 | 0.24 to 0.34 | 0.19 | 0.07 to 0.31 | 0.12 | −0.07 to 0.32 |
N | 5638 | 2819 | 367 | |||
BMI | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.38 | −0.66 to −0.09 | −0.10 | −0.44 to 0.24 | 0.18 | −0.88 to 1.24 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −1.03 | −1.29 to −0.77 | −0.02 | −0.52 to 0.47 | 0.18 | −0.80 to 1.18 |
N | 5562 | 2781 | 364 | |||
MTR | ||||||
Overweight | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.03 | −0.09 to 0.03 | 0.05 | −0.03 to 0.13 | −0.06 | −0.23 to 0.11 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −0.06 | −0.13 to 0.00 | 0.11 | 0.01 to 0.21 | 0.05 | −0.09 to 0.19 |
1290 | 645 | 106 | ||||
MATR | ||||||
Overweight | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.03 | −0.07 to 0.01 | −0.03 | −0.08 to 0.02 | −0.02 | −0.14 to 0.10 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −0.12 | −0.16 to −0.09 | −0.06 | −0.13 to −0.001 | −0.05 | −0.15 to 0.05 |
N | 4272 | 2136 | 258 | |||
Never Smoked | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.02 | −0.02 to 0.05 | 0.02 | −0.03 to 0.07 | 0.03 | −0.08 to 0.15 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.10 | 0.06 to 0.12 | 0.01 | −0.05 to 0.07 | 0.02 | −0.07 to 0.12 |
N | 4126 | 2063 | 256 | |||
Alcohol consumption | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.39 | −0.10 to 0.88 | −0.66 | −1.51 to 0.20 | −0.31 | −2.84 to 2.23 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.30 | −0.17 to 0.78 | −0.14 | −1.35 to 1.06 | 0.13 | −2.14 to 2.41 |
N | 3392 | 1696 | 207 | |||
Exercise | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.09 | 0.05 to 0.13 | 0.00 | −0.07 to 0.08 | −0.13 | −0.31 to 0.04 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.18 | 0.14 to 0.21 | 0.07 | −0.02 to 0.16 | −0.04 | −0.20 to 0.12 |
N | 4240 | 2120 | 258 | |||
NAS-NRC | ||||||
Self-reported health | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.23 | 0.11 to 0.34 | 0.05 | −0.12 to 0.23 | −0.09 | −0.55 to 0.31 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.42 | 0.32 to 0.51 | 0.16 | −0.18 to 0.40 | 0.05 | −0.35 to 0.45 |
N | 1868 | 934 | 144 | |||
BMI | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.56 | −0.95 to −0.16 | −0.37 | −0.85 to −0.16 | −0.64 | −1.92 to 0.64 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −1.02 | −1.21 to −0.38 | −0.57 | −1.21 to 0.07 | −0.77 | −1.92 to 0.37 |
N | 1844 | 932 | 141 | |||
Overweight | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.05 | −0.11 to 0.01 | −0.03 | −0.11 to 0.06 | −0.15 | −0.36 to 0.06 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −0.07 | −0.12 to −0.02 | −0.04 | −0.15 to 0.06 | −0.13 | −0.32 to 0.05 |
N | 1844 | 932 | 141 | |||
Limiting Activities | ||||||
Some post high school education | −0.69 | −1.31 to −0.07 | −0.10 | −1.02 to 0.82 | 1.46 | −0.75 to 3.67 |
Bachelors degree or higher | −1.58 | −2.11 to −1.05 | 0.01 | −1.19 to 1.20 | 1.18 | −0.71 to 3.06 |
N | 1726 | 863 | 133 | |||
Never Smoked | ||||||
Some post high school education | 0.04 | −0.01 to 0.10 | −0.02 | −0.09 to 0.05 | 0.05 | −0.11 to 0.21 |
Bachelors degree or higher | 0.21 | 0.17 to 0.27 | 0.05 | −0.04 to 0.14 | 0.08 | −0.05 to 0.21 |
N | 1902 | 951 | 117 |
Notes: The omitted schooling category is high school graduate or lower. Cross sectional regressions in MATR-MTR control for age, age squared, gender, early health problems and an indicator for MATR. Cross-sectional regressions for the single MATR and MTR datasets control for age, age squared, gender and early health problems. Cross-sectional regressions in NAS-NRC control for age and age squared. Within-MZ twins estimates for combined MATR-MTR and single MATR, MTR datasets control for early health problems. 95% confidence intervals based on standard errors clustered by twin pairs.
Table 4 reports estimates of spousal schooling on own health. The sample was restricted to MZ twin pairs in which both were currently married, reported spouses’ schooling and had reported health measures. Individuals who were divorced, widowed, or separated may have been different than currently-married individuals, in ways that were related to spousal schooling and health, which would bias the estimates. For example, individuals who provided less health benefits to their spouses may have been less likely married, and therefore including only married individuals may have overstated cross-spouse health benefits. The cross-sectional estimates in column 1 show that spousal schooling was associated with better health outcomes and behaviors. The absolute magnitudes of estimated associations with spousal schooling decreased in column 2, when own schooling was introduced. Spousal schooling still had positive associations with health outcomes, but no longer had significant associations with health behaviors with the exception of alcohol consumption. In contrast, the within-MZ twins estimates were smaller in absolute magnitudes and statistically insignificant, suggesting that associations between spousal schooling and health were due to unobserved factors.
Table 4.
Cross-Section | Cross-Section | Within-MZ Twins | Within-MZ Twins | Within-MZ Twins for twin pairs where one twin has ≥16 & other has ≤14 grades |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | |
MATR-MTR | ||||||||||
Self-reported health | ||||||||||
Spousal schooling | 0.05 | 0.04 to 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.02 to 0.04 | 0.01 | −0.01 to 0.03 | 0.01 | −0.01 to 0.03 | 0.05 | −0.00 to 0.10 |
Own schooling | 0.04 | 0.03 to 0.06 | 0.03 | 0.01 to 0.06 | 0.03 | −0.01 to 0.07 | ||||
N | 3204 | 3204 | 1602 | 1602 | 217 | |||||
BMI | ||||||||||
Spousal schooling | −0.29 | −0.36 to −0.23 | −0.25 | −0.33 to −0.17 | −0.02 | −0.09 to 0.04 | −0.01 | −0.07 to 0.05 | −0.07 | −0.27 to 0.12 |
Own schooling | −0.11 | −0.19 to −0.03 | −0.09 | −0.22 to 0.03 | −0.03 | −0.24 to 0.19 | ||||
N | 3174 | 3174 | 1587 | 1587 | 218 | |||||
Overweight | ||||||||||
Spousal schooling | −0.03 | −0.03 to −0.02 | −0.02 | −0.03 to −0.01 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.10 | −0.00 | −0.03 to 0.02 |
Own schooling | −0.01 | −0.02 to −0.00 | 0.00 | −0.02 to 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.03 to 0.01 | ||||
N | 3174 | 3174 | 1587 | 1587 | 128 | |||||
MATR | ||||||||||
Never Smoked | 0.01 | 0.00 to 0.02 | 0.01 | −0.00 to 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.01 | 0.00 | −0.02 to 0.02 |
Spousal schooling | 0.02 | 0.00 to 0.03 | −0.00 | −0.02 to 0.01 | −0.01 | −0.03 to 0.01 | ||||
Own Schooling | ||||||||||
N | 2306 | 2306 | 1153 | 1153 | 148 | |||||
Exercise | ||||||||||
Spousal schooling | 0.02 | 0.01 to 0.03 | 0.01 | −0.00 to 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.02 | 0.00 | −0.01 to 0.02 | −0.02 | −0.07 to 0.02 |
Own schooling | 0.02 | 0.01 to 0.04 | 0.01 | −0.02 to 0.03 | 0.02 | −0.02 to 0.06 | ||||
N | 2282 | 2281 | 1141 | 1141 | 145 | |||||
Alcohol Consumption | 0.22 | 0.10 to 0.33 | 0.15 | 0.01 to 0.30 | 0.08 | −0.10 to 0.26 | 0.09 | −0.10 to 0.29 | 0.35 | 0.01 to 0.69 |
Spousal schooling | 0.15 | −0.01 to 0.30 | −0.11 | −0.42 to 0.19 | 0.02 | −0.44 to 0.49 | ||||
Own Schooling | ||||||||||
N | 1844 | 1844 | 922 | 922 | 120 |
Notes: Cross sectional regressions control for age, age squared, gender, early health problems, spousal age categories (20–29 reference category, 30–39, 40–49, 50–59, 60–69 and 70 plus) and an indicator for MATR in MATR-MTR dataset. Within-MZ twins regressions control for early health problems and spousal age categories. 95% confidence intervals based on standard errors clustered by twin pairs.
Appendix table 4 shows estimates of spousal schooling and own health by gender. The cross-sectional associations indicated that husband’s schooling had greater effects in reducing (increasing) BMI (alcohol consumption) than wife’s schooling, whereas wives’ schooling had greater effects in increasing probabilities of never smoking compared to husbands’ schooling. The within-MZ twins estimates showed no evidence of differences between effects of husbands’ and wives’ schooling.
Discussion
As noted in the introduction, there are many theoretical reasons why more schooling may improve health (Figure 1): improving productive and allocative efficiency, lowering discount rates, extending life expectancy, improving social rank, increasing interactions with other more-schooled peers generating health spillovers, affecting access to resources (money, knowledge, prestige, power, social networks) that impact health, among other possibilities. But the extent to which schooling improves health and health-related behaviors is an empirical, not a theoretical, question. And (perhaps) surprisingly, despite robust associations between schooling and health that are frequently interpreted to support theoretical pathways, empirical evidence about causal effects of schooling on health from IV and within-MZ twins estimates is mixed. One reason for these mixed findings is the challenge of controlling roles of social and genetic endowments that jointly affect schooling and health (Figures 1 and 2), which cause biases in many conventional analyses of schooling-health relationships.
To address this issue, this study used three large US MZ twins datasets. We found almost no associations between schooling and adult health once unobserved confounders were controlled with within-MZ twins estimators. We only found some evidence that schooling was still associated with better self-reported health, with the beneficial effect mainly driven by completing college. We also found that spousal schooling was associated with better health outcomes and behaviors, but the associations disappeared within MZ twin pairs.
Our estimates imply that policies aimed at increasing schooling attainment will not have much, if any, health benefits. This conclusion, though controversial, is consistent with some, though not all, previous studies that attempted to control for unobserved confounders to obtain estimates of the causal effects of schooling on health (e.g., Behrman et al. 2011; Lundborg, 2013; Clark & Royer 2013; Mazumder 2008).
Our conclusions rest on the ability of within-MZ twins estimators to identify causal relations. The within-MZ twin pair schooling differences used to identify schooling effects were much larger than the increased schooling attainment caused by US compulsory schooling laws. Moreover, the results were robust for a sub-sample with relatively large schooling differences. Further, we found statistically-significant and substantively-relevant within-MZ associations between schooling and some non-health non-market outcomes, namely completed fertility (reduced by 0.10 child per additional schooling grade) and spousal schooling (increased by 0.34 grades per additional grade of own schooling) (appendix table 5), suggesting that our results on schooling effects on health do not reflect sample sizes too small to detect any possible schooling effects or peculiarities of our schooling measures.
We also note that our study permits inferences for broader populations than just twins. It is true that twins are a small proportion of the population and being a twin is often thought to be different from being a singleton. However our within-MZ twins estimates controlled for unobservable ways in which twins were different from singletons. On the other hand, our results focus only on one country, and generalizing to other contexts may not be valid. Also within the US schooling-health associations differ by race (Liu & Hummer, 2008), and our data did not permit us to address racial differences.
In summary, our results align with some other recent studies that have found small or non-significant schooling impacts on health once there was control for unobserved confounders. Nevertheless, given theoretical arguments for causal relationships between schooling and health and that some studies that control for unobserved confounders have also found significant effects on health, more research is needed for fully understanding these mixed findings and to what extent they reflect differences in empirical strategies or in empirical contexts.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Schooling is associated with adult health behaviors and outcomes.
Schooling is not associated with adult health within-MZ twin pairs.
Spouses’ schooling has little or no significant causal effects on own health
Associations between schooling and adult health are misleading regarding effects
Acknowledgements
The National Institute of Child Health and Development grant RO1 HD046144-01 on “Causal Effects of Schooling on Adult and Child Health” provided background support for this study.
Role of Funder: The funder had no role in study design; in collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in writing the article; and in the decision to submit it for publication
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest regarding this study.
References
- Albouy V, Lequien L. Does compulsory education lower mortality? Journal of Health Economics. 2009;28(1):155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Almond D, Currie J. Human Capital Development before Age Five. In: Ashenfelter O, Card D, editors. Handbook of Labor Economics. New York: North Holland; 2011. pp. 1315–1486. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson LS, Beverly WT, Corey LA, Murrelle L. The Mid-Atlantic twin registry. Twin Research. 2002;5(5):449–455. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arendt JN. Does education cause better health? A panel data analysis using school reforms for identification. Economics of Education Review. 2005;24(2):149–160. [Google Scholar]
- Ashenfelter O, Krueger A. Estimates of the economic return to schooling from a new sample of twins. American Economic Review. 1994;84(5):1157–1173. [Google Scholar]
- Bank J, Mazzonna F. The effect of education on old age cognitive abilities: Evidence from a regression discontinuity design. The Economic Journal. 2012;122(560):418–448. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2012.02499.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS. A treatise on the family. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Becker GS, Mulligan C. The endogenous determination of time preference. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1997;112(3):729–758. [Google Scholar]
- Behrman JR, Pollak RA, Taubman P. From parent to child: intrahousehold allocations and intergenerational relations in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Behrman JR, Kohler H-P, Jensen VM, Pedersen D, Petersen I, Bingley P, Christensen K. Does schooling reduce hospitalization and delay mortality?. New evidence based on Danish twins. Demography. 2011;48(4):1347–1375. doi: 10.1007/s13524-011-0052-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrman JR, Xiong Y, Zhang J. Schooling-health associations misrepresent the causal effects of schooling on health: Evidence from Chinese twins. Unpublished Manuscript: Chinese University of Hong Kong; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Bound J, Solon G. Double trouble: on the value of twins-based estimation of the returns to schooling. Economics of Education Review. 1999;18(2):169–182. [Google Scholar]
- Brunello G, Fabbri D, Fort M. The causal effect of education on body mass: Evidence from Europe. Journal of Labor Economics. 2013;31(1):195–223. [Google Scholar]
- Buckles K, Ofer M, Morril MS, Wozniak A. Institute for the Study of Labor. 2012. The effect of college education on health (Discussion Paper Number 6659) [Google Scholar]
- Clark D, Royer H. The effect of education on adult mortality and health: Evidence from Britain. American Economic Review. 2013;103(6):2087–2120. doi: 10.1257/aer.103.6.2087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Currie J, Stabile M. Child mental health and human capital accumulation: the case of ADHD. Journal of health economics. 2006;25(6):1094–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A. Understanding differences in health by education. Journal of Health Economics. 2010;29(1):1–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Walque D. Does education affect smoking behaviors?: Evidence using the Vietnam draft as an instrument for college education. Journal of Health Economics. 2007;26(5):877–895. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2006.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher JM. Social interactions and smoking: Evidence using multiple student cohorts, instrumental variables, and school fixed effects. Health Economics. 2010;19(4):466–484. doi: 10.1002/hec.1488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonseca R, Zheng Y. The effect of education on health: Cross-country evidence (Working Paper Number WR-864) Rand Corporation. 2011 [Google Scholar]
- Fujiwara T, Kawachi I. Is education causally related to better health? A twin fixed-effect study in the USA. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2009;38(13):1310–1322. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyp226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossman M. On the concept of health capital and demand for health. Journal of Political Economy. 1972;80(2):223–255. [Google Scholar]
- Grabner M. The causal effect of education on obesity. Evidence from compulsory schooling laws. Unpublished Manuscript: University of California at Davis; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Glymour MM, Kawachi I, Jencks CS, Berkman LF. Does childhood schooling affect old age memory or mental status? Using state schooling laws as natural experiments. Journal of Epidemiology Community Health. 2008;62(6):532–537. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.059469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemptner D, Jurges H, Reinhold S. Changes in compulsory schooling and the causal effect of education on health: Evidence from Germany. Journal of Health Economics. 2011;30(2):340–354. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler H-P, Behrman JR, Schnittker J. Social science methods for twins data: Integrating causality, endowments and heritability. Journal of Biodemography and Social Biology. 2011;57(1):88–141. doi: 10.1080/19485565.2011.580619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Link BG, Phelan J. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995:80–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu H, Hummer RA. Are educational differences in U.S. self-reported health increasing? An examination by gender and race. Social Science and Medicine. 2008;67(11):1898–1906. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lleras-Muney A. The relationship between education and adult mortality in the United States. Review of Economic Studies. 2005;72(1):189–221. [Google Scholar]
- Lundborg P. “The health returns to education- What can we learn from twins?”. Journal of Population Economics. 2013;26(2):673–701. [Google Scholar]
- Lundborg P, Lyttkens CH, Nysted P. Evidence from 50,000 twins (Working Paper Number WP12/19) Health, Econometrics and Data Group; University of York: 2012. Human capital and longevity. [Google Scholar]
- Lykken D, Bouchard T, McGue M, Tellegen A. The Minnesota twin family registry - some initial findings. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae. 1990;39(1):35–70. doi: 10.1017/s0001566000005572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacInnis B. The long-term effects of college education on morbidities: New evidence from the pre-lottery Vietnam draft. Unpublished Manuscript: University of Michigan; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Mazumder B. Does education improve health? A reexamination of the evidence from compulsory schooling laws. Economic Perspectives. 2008;33(2):2–16. [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Bouchard TJ., Jr Genetic and environmental influences on human behavioral differences. Annual review of neuroscience. 1998;21(1):1–24. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meghir C, Palme M, Simeonova E. Education, health and mortality: Evidence from a social experiment (Discussion Paper Number 6462) Institute for the Study of Labor. 2012 [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt RA. Issues in the estimation of causal effects in population research, with an application to the effects of teenage childbearing. In: Engelhardt H, Kohler H-P, Fürnkranz-Prskawetz A, editors. Causal Analysis in Population Studies. Berlin: Springer Verlag; 2009. pp. 9–29. [Google Scholar]
- Page WF. The NAS-NRC twin registry of WWII military veteran twins. Twin Research. 2002;5(5):493–496. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose G, Marmot M. Social class and coronary heart disease. British heart journal. 1981;45(1):13–9. doi: 10.1136/hrt.45.1.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Royer H. Separated at girth: US twin estimates of the effects of birth weight. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 2009;1(1):49–85. [Google Scholar]
- Spasojevic J. Effects of education on adult health in Sweden: Results from a natural experiment. In: Slottje D, Tchernis R, editors. Current Issues in Health Economics Contributions to Economic Analysis. 2010. pp. 179–199. Emerald Group Publishing Limited. [Google Scholar]
- van Kippersluis H, O’Donnell O, van Doorslaer E. Long-Run returns to education: Does schooling lead to an extended old age? Journal of Human Resources. 2011;46(4):695–721. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webbink D, Martin N, Visscher P. Does education reduce the probability of being overweight? Journal of Health Economics. 2010;29(1):29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.