Summary
During olfactory learning in fruit flies, dopaminergic neurons assign value to odor representations in the mushroom body Kenyon cells. Here we identify a class of downstream glutamatergic mushroom body output neurons (MBONs) called M4/6, or MBON-β2β′2a, MBON-β′2mp, and MBON-γ5β′2a, whose dendritic fields overlap with dopaminergic neuron projections in the tips of the β, β′, and γ lobes. This anatomy and their odor tuning suggests that M4/6 neurons pool odor-driven Kenyon cell synaptic outputs. Like that of mushroom body neurons, M4/6 output is required for expression of appetitive and aversive memory performance. Moreover, appetitive and aversive olfactory conditioning bidirectionally alters the relative odor-drive of M4β′ neurons (MBON-β′2mp). Direct block of M4/6 neurons in naive flies mimics appetitive conditioning, being sufficient to convert odor-driven avoidance into approach, while optogenetically activating these neurons induces avoidance behavior. We therefore propose that drive to the M4/6 neurons reflects odor-directed behavioral choice.
Highlights
-
•
Glutamatergic mushroom body output neurons are required for memory expression
-
•
Training bidirectionally alters relative odor drive to output neurons
-
•
Blocking glutamatergic mushroom body output neurons mimics appetitive conditioning
-
•
Optogenetic activation drives avoidance behavior
Fruit fly olfactory memory involves mushroom body plasticity. Owald et al. identified glutamatergic mushroom body output neurons that are critical for memory expression. Conditioning bidirectionally alters odor drive to these outputs. Blocking them mimics appetitive conditioning, whereas activation induces avoidance behavior.
Introduction
Learning permits animals to convert innate reflexive stimulus-driven behavioral responses into meaningful stimulus-guided actions. Understanding how such sensory-motor transformations are implemented and altered in the nervous system is a subject of great interest.
In Drosophila, innate behavioral responses to odors can be redirected toward approach or avoidance by a learning session that couples odor exposure with rewarding sugar or punitive electric shock, respectively (Tempel et al., 1983; Tully and Quinn, 1985). Recently, substantial progress has been made in understanding the neural mechanisms of odorant coding and learning in the fly (Wilson, 2013; Masse et al., 2009; Perisse et al., 2013; Busto et al., 2010; Dubnau and Chiang, 2013). However, it remains unclear how peripheral odor responses are transformed into behavioral performance and how learning redirects the transformation.
Flies detect airborne odors using unique collections of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) housed in their antennae and maxillary palps (de Bruyne et al., 1999, 2001). The tuning of each OSN type is determined by the expression of a single odorant receptor gene (Dobritsa et al., 2003; Hallem and Carlson, 2004, 2006; Vosshall, 2000). Axons from OSNs expressing the same receptor converge onto the same glomerulus in each antennal lobe (Vosshall, 2000; Gao et al., 2000; Couto et al., 2005; Fishilevich and Vosshall, 2005), where their activity is relayed to excitatory and inhibitory projection neurons (Olsen et al., 2010; Kazama and Wilson, 2008; Parnas et al., 2013; Liang et al., 2013). Excitatory projection neurons deliver odor information to the calyces of the mushroom bodies (MBs) and to neurons in the lateral horn (LH), whereas inhibitory PN activity is exclusively relayed to the LH (Jefferis et al., 2001; Wong et al., 2002; Fişek and Wilson, 2014; Wang et al., 2014). The LH is largely believed to be responsible for driving innate behavioral responses to odors, since blocking all mushroom body neuron output has little consequence on these behaviors (Heimbeck et al., 2001; Parnas et al., 2013). In contrast, disrupting the mushroom body has long been known to impair learned responses (Heisenberg et al., 1985; Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2002), consistent with the MB being critical for odor memory (Heisenberg, 2003).
Each MB is comprised of 2,000 intrinsic Kenyon cells (KCs), and an individual odor is represented as activity in a sparse subset of these cells (Wang et al., 2004; Honegger et al., 2011). Value can be assigned to these odor representations during learning by the action of reinforcing dopaminergic neurons whose presynaptic terminals are confined to discrete zones along the lobes of the MB (Schwaerzel et al., 2003; Riemensperger et al., 2005; Claridge-Chang et al., 2009; Aso et al., 2012; Mao and Davis, 2009; Liu et al., 2012; Burke et al., 2012; Waddell, 2013). This anatomy and a requirement for dopamine receptor in MB neurons (Kim et al., 2007; Qin et al., 2012) is consistent with a model that olfactory memories are represented in the presynaptic output synapses from mushroom body KCs onto relevant downstream neurons (Heisenberg, 2003).
Anatomical work suggests that fewer than 40 output neurons collect synaptic outputs from the 2,000 KCs (Tanaka et al., 2008; Aso et al., 2014). This substantial convergence indicates that information may be lost, and raises the question of what information is represented as changes in synaptic efficacy from KCs to downstream output neurons. Prior work suggests that the MB is involved in motor gating (Huber, 1967; Martin et al., 1998) and that an element of memory valence is differentially coded between subclasses of the αβ KCs (Perisse et al., 2013). How such information is represented in the connections between KCs and particular downstream neurons is currently unclear.
Physiological changes after training have been reported in two sets of memory-relevant cholinergic output neurons that have dendritic fields within the vertical lobes of the mushroom body (Séjourné et al., 2011; Pai et al., 2013; Plaçais et al., 2013). However, the behavioral consequence of synaptic modification at these sites is unclear. Here we identify a small set of glutamatergic output neurons whose dendrites lie within the tip regions of the horizontal mushroom body lobes and in close spatial proximity to presynaptic terminals of reinforcing dopaminergic neurons (Burke et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012). Blocking these output neurons impairs conditioned odor approach and avoidance. Strikingly, the activation of these output neurons by the conditioned odor is depressed by reward learning and potentiated by aversive learning. Moreover, directly inhibiting these neurons in naive flies converts odor avoidance into attraction, whereas flies are repelled by their activation. Our data therefore suggest that a critical element of learning-induced plasticity within the MB manifests as a bidirectional change in the relative odor drive to specific types of MB output neurons.
Results
GAL4 Control of Glutamatergic M4/6 MBONs
Identified dopaminergic neurons in the PAM (protocerebral anterior medial) cluster in the Drosophila brain convey rewarding reinforcement (Burke et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2012). Blocking the output from a subset of these that are labeled by the 0104-GAL4 driver impairs short-term sweetness-reinforced and longer-term nutrient-reinforced sugar memory (Burke et al., 2012). Furthermore, pairing thermogenetic activation of these neurons with odor presentation formed appetitive odor memories (Burke et al., 2012). The presynaptic terminals from 0104-labeled dopaminergic neurons densely innervate the β′ and γ lobe tips of the horizontal mushroom body lobes, which suggests that appetitive olfactory memories may be represented as changes in the efficacy of synaptic outputs in these regions from the odor-activated KCs onto as-yet-unidentified downstream neurons.
By visually screening available GAL4 collections (Jenett et al., 2012; Bidaye et al., 2014), we identified three fly lines that labeled candidate postsynaptic neurons with arbors in the tip regions, β2, β′2, and γ5, of the horizontal mushroom body lobes (Figure 1). Neurons innervating β′2 and γ5 have been described as MB-M4 and MB-M6 (Tanaka et al., 2008). We therefore named the cells that predominantly innervate either the tip of the β, β′, or γ lobe as M4β, M4β′, and M6, respectively. A very recent study has renamed these neurons as MBON-β2β′2a (M4β), MBON-β′2mp (M4β′), and MBON-γ5β′2a (M6) (Aso et al., 2014). We use both names here for clarity. R21D02-GAL4 expresses in all M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons per hemisphere (Figure 1A, Movie S1). VT1211-GAL4 expresses in M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a, but not in the β tip projecting M4β/MBON-β2β′2a (Figure 1B, Movie S2). Lastly, R66C08-GAL4 only expresses in the M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons that mostly innervate the γ lobe tip and the anterior zone of β′2 (Figure 1C, Movie S3). We determined the polarity of the M4/6 neurons using expression of established neural compartment marker proteins. The dendritic marker DenMark (Nicolaï et al., 2010) localized exclusively to the horizontal MB lobe tips, while the presynaptic active zone protein Syd-1 (Owald et al., 2010) localized to the processes of the M4/6 neurons that lie outside of the MB in the superior medial protocerebrum (SMP) and the crepine region (Ito et al., 2014) (Figure 1D). This polarity suggests that the dendritic field of the M4/6 neurons lies within the MB lobes and is consistent with a role as potential output neurons that pool KC synaptic weights. The genomic fragment used to create the VT1211-GAL4 line (Bidaye et al., 2014) comes from a region that is proximal to the gene for the vesicular glutamate transporter (DVGlut) (Daniels et al., 2008; Mahr and Aberle, 2006). We immunostained the fly brain with an anti-DVGlut antibody (Mahr and Aberle, 2006) to determine whether the M4/6 neurons might be glutamatergic. DVGlut labeling perfectly overlapped with the GFP-marked presynaptic field of the M4/6 neurons (Figure 1E). This is most evident at higher resolution where, in addition, individual M4/6 presynaptic boutons can be seen to be large and spherical (Figure 1E, inserts). We also used GRASP (Feinberg et al., 2008; Gordon and Scott, 2009) to test whether the processes of the M4/6 neurons are close to those of the dopaminergic PAM neurons (Figures 1F and S1B). This analysis revealed strong GFP fluorescence at two locations: the tips of the horizontal MB lobes, where the M4/6 dendrites and dopaminergic presynapses reside, and in the SMP between M4/6 presynaptic terminals and the dendrites of dopaminergic neuron. Although GRASP is most reliably a proximity marker, it is notable that the GRASP visible in the SMP appears to preferentially label terminals of M4/6 neurons rather than the non-synaptic neurites, suggesting that the points of contact may be genuinely synaptic.
M4/6 Neurons Are Required for Appetitive and Aversive Memory Expression
We tested whether output from M4/6 neurons was required for behavioral expression of memory performance by using the R21D02, VT1211, and R66C08 GAL4 drivers to express the dominant temperature-sensitive UAS-shibirets1 (shits1) transgene (Kitamoto, 2001). In each experiment we compared the performance of flies with M4/6 neural blockade to control flies carrying only the GAL4 or UAS-shits1 transgene. We first tested immediate memory performance following sucrose-reinforced appetitive conditioning (Tempel et al., 1983; Krashes and Waddell, 2008). All flies were trained and tested for 3 min memory at the restrictive temperature of 32°C. Blocking the M4/6 neurons caused an impairment in memory performance. R21D02;shits1, VT1211;shits1 and R66C08;shits1 flies displayed performance that was statistically different to that of shits1 and their respective GAL4 control flies (Figures 2A1, 2B1, and 2C1). We also restricted the blockade of M4/6 neurons to the time of memory retrieval by training flies at the permissive 23°C and raising the temperature to 32°C 30 min before and during testing 24 hr appetitive memory. These analyses again uncovered a significant defect in flies with impaired M4/6 neurons, demonstrating a clear requirement for M4/6 neural output for the expression of conditioned approach (Figures 2A2, 2B2, and 2C2). We similarly tested the role of M4/6 neurons in electric-shock-reinforced aversive short-term memory. Memory performance of R21D02;shits1, VT1211;shits1 and R66C08;shits1 flies was again statistically different to that of shits1 and their respective GAL4 control flies (Figures 2A3, 2B3, and 2C3). In both the appetitive and aversive memory experiments, the observed defect appeared more pronounced when simultaneously blocking M4β′/MBON-β′2mp or M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M4β/MBON-β2β′2a neurons with M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons, using VT1211 or R21D02, than blocking M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons alone with R66C08. Importantly, control experiments performed at permissive 23°C did not reveal significant differences between the relevant groups (Figure S2). Output from the M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons is therefore required for the expression of appetitive and aversive memory performance and we propose that the three cell types may function together.
Odors Evoke Activity in MBON Dendrites in the β′ Lobe
To further understand the role of the M4/6 neurons in shaping a behavioral response, we used VT1211-GAL4 to express GCaMP6m (Chen et al., 2013) in the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons and performed two-photon functional calcium imaging to monitor odor-evoked activity in living flies. We exposed flies to 5 s pulses of methylcyclohexanol (MCH) and octanol (OCT), the same odors used in training, and monitored changes in GCaMP fluorescence in the dendrites of M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a in the MB lobe tips (Figure 3A). The magnitude of the dendritic odor-evoked responses was smaller (and with our experimental settings below the level of noise) in MBON dendrites in the γ lobe (Figure S3) than dendrites in the β′ lobe (Figures 3B and 3C). Since the behavioral data indicated that M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons are both required for memory performance (Figure 2B versus Figure 2C), we concentrated further analysis on the MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe. Exposing flies to MCH or OCT elicited robust calcium transients throughout the MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe (Figures 3B and 3C). Furthermore, odor-evoked calcium transients were also evident in the presynaptic boutons of the output neurons (Figure 3D), suggesting the odor-driven input to the dendrites is propagated to the release sites. The MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe also responded when flies were exposed to other odors such as 6-methyl-5-hepten-2-one and pentyl acetate (Figure S4A). In addition, memory performance was impaired when M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons were blocked after flies were trained with these odors (Figure S4B), suggesting that the role of the M4/6 neurons in memory retrieval is not specific to OCT and MCH.
Learning Bidirectionally Alters Relative Odor Drive to M4β′ Neurons
We next determined whether the odor-evoked activity of the MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe was modified by training. Flies were trained using either an appetitive or an aversive conditioning protocol and were subsequently captured and prepared for live-imaging of odor-evoked activity within a window of 1–2 hr after training. Importantly, blocking M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON- γ5β′2a neurons impaired both appetitive and aversive memory 2 hr after training (Figures 3E, 3F, and S5 for permissive temperature controls). We monitored the calcium responses evoked in MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe (example traces shown in Figures 3G and 3H) by exposing the flies to either the odor that had been previously paired with sugar reward or electric-shock punishment (the CS+), or to the non-reinforced odor (the CS−). We also compared the odor-evoked responses in flies that were mock-trained—subjected to the full conditioning regimen of odor presentation but without reinforcement delivery. In addition, we performed both the aversive and appetitive protocols using either MCH as the CS+ and OCT as the CS−, or OCT as the CS+ and MCH as the CS−. Strikingly, in both appetitive conditioning experiments the response to the CS+ relative to the CS− was decreased when compared to the responses in mock trained flies (Figures 3I and 3K). Moreover, this relationship was reversed in each experiment following aversive training, with the relative CS+ to CS− evoked response being increased when compared to the responses in mock trained flies (Figures 3J and 3L). These data suggest that the relative odor drive to the MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe is bidirectionally tuned by olfactory conditioning, and they are consistent with the relative conditioned odor drive being depressed by appetitive learning and potentiated by aversive learning.
Direct Manipulation of M4/6 Neurons Can Mimic Learning in Naive Flies
We reasoned that if a reduced conditioned-odor drive to MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe was an important element of appetitive learning, we might be able to mimic conditioned approach by directly inhibiting the M4/6 neurons. In general, naive flies are repelled by high concentrations of odor when presented in a choice with a clean air stream (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Heimbeck et al., 2001). We therefore used UAS-shits1 to test whether M4/6 neuron block altered naive odor avoidance behavior. Control flies that were either heterozygous for the R21D02-GAL4, VT1211-GAL4, or the UAS-shits1 effector transgene showed robust avoidance of MCH when presented at 100-, 1,000-, and 4,000-fold dilutions. Strikingly, at the two lower concentrations, blocking M4/6 neurons converted naive odor avoidance behavior into significant odor approach (Figure 4A). A similar abolishment and reversal of avoidance was also observed with OCT, although the effective concentration range appears to be different (Figure 4B). Interestingly, blocking only the M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons blunted the aversion but did not induce behavioral reversal (Figures 4C and 4D), indicating that the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp neurons play a particularly prominent role. Furthermore, no significant effects on odor avoidance were observed when the same flies were tested at the permissive temperature (Figure S8) or when blocking the previously described MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc and MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3 (Séjourné et al., 2011; Aso et al., 2014) or MB-V3/MBON-α3 (Pai et al., 2013; Plaçais et al., 2013; Aso et al., 2014) output neurons that are dendritic to the vertical lobes of the mushroom body (Figures 4C and 4D). We speculate that the loss of the phenotype at high MCH concentration reflects either a ceiling effect or a significant role for the LH. Nevertheless, these data indicate that the observed behavioral reversals are specific to blocking M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON- γ5β′2a neurons and that inhibiting these output pathways can convert odor avoidance into odor attraction in a manner that reflects appetitive conditioning.
Our imaging data also indicate that the relative CS+ to CS− odor drive to MBON dendrites in the β′ lobe is increased after aversive conditioning. We therefore tested whether activation of M4/6 neurons promoted avoidance behavior. We expressed a UAS-ReaChR red-light-activated channelrhodopsin transgene (Inagaki et al., 2014) in M4/6 neurons using R21D02-GAL4 and allowed flies to choose between an unlit arm and a red-light-illuminated arm in a T-maze. Whereas all control flies distributed evenly between the tubes, a significant fraction of R21D02;ReaChR flies avoided the illuminated arm (Figure 4E), consistent with M4/6 neuron activity driving avoidance behavior. Therefore, both the imaging of odor-evoked responses after training and the behavioral experiments reveal bidirectional phenotypes that are consistent with the KC-M4/6 junction being a key site that provides direction to odor-driven behavior after aversive and appetitive training.
Discussion
Many prior studies, including our own, have concluded that mushroom body neurons are dispensable for naive odor-driven behavior and subsets are either required or are dispensable for particular memory functions (Heimbeck et al., 2001; Heisenberg et al., 1985; Dubnau et al., 2001; McGuire et al., 2001; Schwaerzel et al., 2002; Krashes et al., 2007; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013; Isabel et al., 2004; Huang et al., 2012; Perisse et al., 2013; Xie et al., 2013). However, these experiments simultaneously blocked all the outputs from a given population of KCs using cell-wide expression of shits1. Our results here suggest that these models should be reconsidered. Blocking the specific M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a output from the mushroom body, as opposed to blocking all outputs, has a radical effect on naive odor-driven behavior. We propose that ordinarily, in naive flies, the multiple mushroom body output channels are ultimately pooled and contribute a net zero to odor-driven behavior. Therefore, if one uses a mushroom body neuron-driven UAS-shits1 that simultaneously blocks all outputs, there is no apparent effect on naive behavior. If, however, one blocks only one channel, or alters its efficacy by learning, the odor-driven behavior can be changed. A similar logic could also account for why we observe clear memory retrieval defects when blocking M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons that presumably pool outputs from the tip of the γ and β′ lobe, yet blocking all α′β′ neuron outputs did not demonstrably disrupt later memory retrieval (Krashes et al., 2007; Krashes and Waddell, 2008). Others have shown a role for α′β′ neuron output to retrieve earlier forms of memory (Wang et al., 2008; Cervantes-Sandoval et al., 2013).
Both our physiological and behavioral results are consistent with a depression of the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a output being sufficient to code learned approach. Learning-related plasticity has been reported at the β-lobe outputs in both bees (Okada et al., 2007) and locusts (Cassenaer and Laurent, 2012), although the importance of these synaptic connections in the behavior of these insects is not known. At this stage we cannot be sure that our observed decrease in the relative odor drive reflects plasticity of the synapses between odor-specific KCs and the M4/6 neurons. However, it seems plausible, because this synaptic junction is addressed by the relevant rewarding dopaminergic neurons (Burke et al., 2012). Given that blocking M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons converts avoidance to approach, other mushroom body output channels, perhaps some of which lie on the vertical α-lobe projection (Séjourné et al., 2011; Plaçais et al., 2013), must drive the approach behavior. It is therefore conceivable that a similar plasticity of odor drive to these putative approach outputs could be critical for aversive conditioning. Such an idea is consistent with several prior reports of aversive memory traces that are specific to the vertical α-branch of the mushroom body (Yu et al., 2005, 2006; Cervantes-Sandoval and Davis, 2012). In addition, aversive learning has been reported to depress odor drive in the vertical lobe of downstream MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc and MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3 neurons (Séjourné et al., 2011; Aso et al., 2014) and to potentiate odor drive of MB-V3/MBON-α3 output neurons (Pai et al., 2013; although Plaçais et al. [2013] reported potentiation after appetitive learning). However, it is notable that blocking either the MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc and MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3 neurons or MB-V3/MBON-α3 neurons did not affect naive odor avoidance behavior in our experiments or those of others (Séjourné et al., 2011; Pai et al., 2013; Plaçais et al., 2013). Therefore, although MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc, MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3, and MB-V3/MBON-α3 neurons are required for memory expression, it is not currently known which reinforcing neurons address MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc, MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3, and MB-V3/MBON-α3 connections and how these outputs specifically contribute to odor-guided behavior.
Our physiological analyses suggest bidirectional plasticity of odor-evoked responses, with aversive learning increasing the relative conditioned odor drive to the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp neurons. This could account for why output from M4/6 neurons is also required for expression of aversive memory. Moreover, whereas blocking the M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons converts odor avoidance into approach, activation of M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons drives avoidance. It therefore seems likely that plasticity of the relative odor drive to M4β′/MBON-β′2mp neurons is also part of the aversive memory engram. Again, we do not know that the increased odor drive after training reflects synaptic potentiation between odor-specific KCs and the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp neurons. Increased odor drive to M4β′/MBON-β′2mp neurons could, for example, also result from plasticity elsewhere in the KCs that enhances signal propagation along the horizontal KC arbor. Nevertheless, the MB-M3 dopaminergic neurons that are required to reinforce aversive memory also innervate the tips of the β and β′ lobe (Aso et al., 2012). In addition, a recent study reported that aversive learning specifically decreased unconditioned odor-evoked neurotransmission from the γ neurons (Zhang and Roman, 2013), a result that presumably would mirror a relative increase in the response to the reinforced odor. Lastly, aversive conditioning using relative shock intensity utilizes the rewarding dopaminergic neurons (Perisse et al., 2013) that occupy the same zones on the mushroom body as the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neuron dendrites. With the caveat that GRASP is only an indicator of proximity, our anatomical studies suggest that dendrites of rewarding dopaminergic neurons may connect to the M4β′/MBON-β′2mp and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neuron presynaptic terminals, forming a potential feedback or forward loop that could serve such a relative-judgment function.
It is perhaps noteworthy that KC outputs in the vertical lobe are onto excitatory cholinergic MB-V2α/MBON-α2sc and MB-V2α′/MBON-α′3 (Séjourné et al., 2011) and MB-V3/MBON-α3 (Pai et al., 2013; Plaçais et al., 2013) neurons, whereas the horizontal outputs are onto glutamatergic, potentially inhibitory (Liu and Wilson, 2013), M4β/MBON-β2β′2a, M4β′/MBON-β′2mp, and M6/MBON-γ5β′2a neurons. This suggests that distinct signaling modes may be driven from the bifurcated collaterals of KCs. It will be crucial to understand how these outputs from the different branches, and those from discrete lobes, are ultimately pooled to guide appropriate behavior.
Experimental Procedures
Fly Strains
All flies were reared on standard cornmeal-agar food at either 25°C or 18°C. The driver lines used were R21D02-GAL4, R66D08-GAL4, R48B04-LexA (Jenett et al., 2012; Lin et al., 2014), R58E02-LexA (Liu et al., 2012), G0239-GAL4 (Pai et al., 2013), NP2492-GAL4 (Séjourné et al., 2011), and VT1211-GAL4 (Bidaye et al., 2014). GAL4 driver lines were crossed to UAS-shits1 (Kitamoto, 2001) or UAS-ReaChR (Inagaki et al., 2014). GAL4 driver lines, UAS-shits1, or UAS-ReaChR were crossed to Canton-S flies as controls. For anatomy, driver lines were combined with UAS-mCD8-GFP (Lee and Luo, 1999), 247-LexA (Pitman et al., 2011), LexAop-mCD2-mRFP (Lai and Lee, 2006), UAS-DenMark-mRFP (Nicolaï et al., 2010), or UAS-GFP-Syd-1 (Owald et al., 2010). GRASP experiments were performed as described (Gordon and Scott, 2009; Pitman et al., 2011).
Confocal Imaging and Immunostaining
All confocal images were acquired on a Leica SP5 at manually adjusted laser intensity and gain. Brains were dissected on ice and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde. For native fluorophore imaging, samples were incubated and washed in PBT (0.1% Triton) and PBS before mounting. For immunostainings, brains were incubated in PBT (0.1% Triton) supplemented with a rabbit anti-DVGlut primary antibody (Mahr and Aberle, 2006) (1:500 dilution), followed by incubation with secondary antibodies (Alexa 647, Sigma).
Two-Photon Calcium Imaging
We imaged 3- to 8-day-old UAS-GCaMP6m; VT1211-GAL4 female flies 1–2 hr after training. Flies were trained using either 4-MCH or 3-OCT as the CS+ and the reciprocal odor as the CS− in a T-maze (see below). Mock trained flies were exposed to MCH and OCT with no sugar or shock reinforcement. For imaging, flies were briefly anesthetized < 10 s on ice and mounted in a custom-made chamber. The head capsule was opened under room temperature sugar-free HL3-like saline (Yoshihara, 2012). The legs and proboscis were immobilized with wax. Fluorescence was excited using 70 fs pulses, 80 MHz repetition rate, centered on 910 nm generated by a Ti-Sapphire laser (Chameleon Ultra II, Coherent). Images of 256 × 128 pixels were acquired at 11.5 Hz using two-photon microscopy (Scientifica) with a 40X, 0.8 NA water-immersion objective, controlled by ScanImage 3.8 software (Pologruto et al., 2003). Odors were delivered on a clean air carrier stream using a custom-designed system (Shang et al., 2007), which also synchronizes the timing of odor delivery and the two-photon image acquisition. Two-photon fluorescence images were manually segmented using ImageJ. Movement of the animal was small enough such that images did not require registration. The fluorescence over the defined region of interest was summed at each frame to yield one fluorescence trace, F(t). Where possible, each hemisphere was separately evaluated and treated as an independent “n.” All subsequent analyses utilized custom-written Matlab routines. Flies were exposed to two consecutive 5 s clean air puffs with 30 s intervals. First responses were discarded and second responses were defined as the “no odor response.” After brief rest, flies were exposed to 5 s MCH (air stream passing over 10−2 odor dilution in mineral oil, and then further blended 1:9 with a clean air stream), then 30 s clean air, followed by 5 s OCT pulse. This odor stimulation protocol was delivered twice. Baseline fluorescence (F) corresponds to the average fluorescence signal across an 8 s window starting 9 s after scan onset and terminating 3 s before the first air or odor exposure. The baseline was then used to compute the relative change in fluorescence (ΔF(t)/F = (F(t) − F)/F). Responses were determined to start 2.5 s after the instrumentation odor delivery command and to end within 12.5 s. This delayed onset accounts for the computational, electronic, mechanical, and fluid flow lag. The response curves were normalized and averaged over the two paired odor presentations:
(Equation 1) |
are the ΔF(t)/F response curves of the “n”th experiment to the “i”th odor stimulation protocol. The normalization factor was chosen to be the average of the total CS+ and CS− response to avoid bias toward one or the other and was calculated as the sum over the acquisition time points of the ΔF(t)/F curves multiplied by the sampling interval. We then computed the odor response difference for each n, Dn(t) = CSn+ − CSn−. To quantify the difference between the trained and mock groups, the area under the peak of each curve (defined as 5 ± 0.5 s after odor delivery) was computed and expressed as a percentage difference to the mean of the mock response curves:
(Equation 2) |
< D > mock is the mean of the odor response difference curves in the corresponding mock group:
(Equation 3) |
where the summation is over the experiments in the relevant mock group and Nmock is the number of experiments in that group. We note that by inspection of Equation 2, the average of the Peakn values for each mock group will be zero. The Peak values obtained from each trained group were compared with those of the corresponding mock group using the Mann-Whitney U-test (see Figure S6 for normalized odor response traces and Figure S7 for analysis overview). The learning-induced difference curve, L(t), is the difference between the mean ± SEM of the Dn(t) curves of the trained and corresponding mock groups. The errors were combined in the usual way, i.e., error in . Graphs were created in Prism 6 (GraphPad Software).
Behavior
For appetitive and aversive memory testing, flies were reared at 25°C and 4- to 9-day-old mixed-sex populations were tested together in all experiments. Flies were starved for 21–24 h prior to appetitive training (Krashes and Waddell, 2008). Flies were also starved after training for 2 hr and 24 hr memory testing. Aversive and appetitive training was performed as described (Perisse et al., 2013). Briefly, for appetitive conditioning flies were exposed to the CS− for 2 min followed by 30 s of air and then to the CS+ in the presence of dry sucrose for 2 min. For aversive conditioning flies were exposed to the CS+ for 1 min with twelve 90 V electric shocks at 5 s intervals followed by 45 s of air and the CS− for 1 min. For testing flies were given 2 min to choose between the CS+ and CS− in a T-maze. Performance index (PI) was calculated as the number of flies approaching (appetitive memory) or avoiding (aversive memory) the conditioned odor, minus the number of flies going the other direction, divided by the total number of flies in the experiment. A single PI value is the average score from flies of the identical genotype tested with the reciprocal reinforced/non-reinforced odor combination (Tully and Quinn, 1985; Perisse et al., 2013). Permissive temperature was 23°C and restrictive 32°C. Odor dilutions were adjusted between experiments and odor batches to minimize bias (MCH 5–8 μl in 8 ml mineral oil and OCT 7–8 μl in 8 ml mineral oil). All memory experiments utilized a transgenic line with UAS-shibirets1 on the X and III chromosome.
To assay naive odor choice, 5-day-old flies were starved for 21–24 hr prior to testing. Flies were allowed to choose between MCH or OCT (1:100, 1:1,000, or 1:4,000 dilution in mineral oil) and mineral oil-suffused air streams for 2 min. Preference index was calculated as the number of flies approaching the odor minus the number approaching mineral oil, divided by the total number of flies in the experiment. All naive odor choice experiments utilized a transgenic line with UAS-shits1 on the III chromosome, and crosses were reared at 18°C. One “n” corresponds to a single test trial.
For optogenetic experiments flies were kept on food supplemented with 1 mM retinal for 2 days prior to testing. Three high-power LEDs (700 mA, centered at 630 nm) were mounted on one arm of the T-maze and triggered for 100 ms at 5 Hz. Flies were given 1 min to choose between the illuminated or non-illuminated arm.
Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed using Matlab and Prism 6. All behavioral data were analyzed with a one-way ANOVA followed by a Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD) post-hoc test. Imaging data were analyzed using a Mann-Whitney U-test. Definition of statistical significance is set at p < 0.05.
Author Contributions
D.O. and S.W. conceived this project and designed all experiments. J.F., G.D., E.P., and D.O. performed all behavioral experiments. Live-imaging was performed by D.O. using custom apparatus and software designed by D.O. and C.B.T. and constructed and programmed by C.B.T. Imaging data were analyzed by D.O and C.B.T. using software programmed by C.B.T. and designed by D.O. and C.B.T. GAL4 lines were visually screened and selected by W.H. and D.O. Anatomical data were produced by W.H. and D.O. The manuscript was written by S.W. and D.O.
Acknowledgments
We thank Gero Miesenböck and members of the Waddell and Miesenböck labs for support and discussion and Paola Cognigni for help with the ReaChR experiments. We thank H. Aberle for the DVGlut antibody, G. Rubin and the Janelia Farm Project, B. Dickson and the VDRC, and the Bloomington stock center for fly lines. D.O. was supported by an EMBO Long-Term Fellowship and a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship. E.P. thanks the Philippe and Bettencourt-Schueller Foundations. S.W. is funded by a Wellcome Trust Senior Research Fellowship in the Basic Biomedical Sciences and by funds from the Gatsby Charitable Foundation and Oxford Martin School.
Footnotes
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Supplemental Information
References
- Aso Y., Herb A., Ogueta M., Siwanowicz I., Templier T., Friedrich A.B., Ito K., Scholz H., Tanimoto H. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 2012;8:e1002768. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aso Y., Hattori D., Yu Y., Johnston R.M., Iyer N.A., Ngo T.T., Dionne H., Abbott L.F., Axel R., Tanimoto H., Rubin G.M. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. eLife. 2014;3:e04577. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bidaye S.S., Machacek C., Wu Y., Dickson B.J. Neuronal control of Drosophila walking direction. Science. 2014;344:97–101. doi: 10.1126/science.1249964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke C.J., Huetteroth W., Owald D., Perisse E., Krashes M.J., Das G., Gohl D., Silies M., Certel S., Waddell S. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492:433–437. doi: 10.1038/nature11614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busto G.U., Cervantes-Sandoval I., Davis R.L. Olfactory learning in Drosophila. Physiology (Bethesda) 2010;25:338–346. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00026.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassenaer S., Laurent G. Conditional modulation of spike-timing-dependent plasticity for olfactory learning. Nature. 2012;482:47–52. doi: 10.1038/nature10776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cervantes-Sandoval I., Davis R.L. Distinct traces for appetitive versus aversive olfactory memories in DPM neurons of Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:1247–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cervantes-Sandoval I., Martin-Peña A., Berry J.A., Davis R.L. System-like consolidation of olfactory memories in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:9846–9854. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0451-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen T.W., Wardill T.J., Sun Y., Pulver S.R., Renninger S.L., Baohan A., Schreiter E.R., Kerr R.A., Orger M.B., Jayaraman V. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claridge-Chang A., Roorda R.D., Vrontou E., Sjulson L., Li H., Hirsh J., Miesenböck G. Writing memories with light-addressable reinforcement circuitry. Cell. 2009;139:405–415. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couto A., Alenius M., Dickson B.J. Molecular, anatomical, and functional organization of the Drosophila olfactory system. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1535–1547. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniels R.W., Gelfand M.V., Collins C.A., DiAntonio A. Visualizing glutamatergic cell bodies and synapses in Drosophila larval and adult CNS. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;508:131–152. doi: 10.1002/cne.21670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruyne M., Clyne P.J., Carlson J.R. Odor coding in a model olfactory organ: the Drosophila maxillary palp. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:4520–4532. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04520.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Bruyne M., Foster K., Carlson J.R. Odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Neuron. 2001;30:537–552. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00289-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobritsa A.A., van der Goes van Naters W., Warr C.G., Steinbrecht R.A., Carlson J.R. Integrating the molecular and cellular basis of odor coding in the Drosophila antenna. Neuron. 2003;37:827–841. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00094-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubnau J., Chiang A.S. Systems memory consolidation in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:84–91. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubnau J., Grady L., Kitamoto T., Tully T. Disruption of neurotransmission in Drosophila mushroom body blocks retrieval but not acquisition of memory. Nature. 2001;411:476–480. doi: 10.1038/35078077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feinberg E.H., Vanhoven M.K., Bendesky A., Wang G., Fetter R.D., Shen K., Bargmann C.I. GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners (GRASP) defines cell contacts and synapses in living nervous systems. Neuron. 2008;57:353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fişek M., Wilson R.I. Stereotyped connectivity and computations in higher-order olfactory neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:280–288. doi: 10.1038/nn.3613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fishilevich E., Vosshall L.B. Genetic and functional subdivision of the Drosophila antennal lobe. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1548–1553. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao Q., Yuan B., Chess A. Convergent projections of Drosophila olfactory neurons to specific glomeruli in the antennal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 2000;3:780–785. doi: 10.1038/77680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon M.D., Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. 2009;61:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallem E.A., Carlson J.R. The odor coding system of Drosophila. Trends Genet. 2004;20:453–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2004.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallem E.A., Carlson J.R. Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell. 2006;125:143–160. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heimbeck G., Bugnon V., Gendre N., Keller A., Stocker R.F. A central neural circuit for experience-independent olfactory and courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:15336–15341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011314898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg M. Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:266–275. doi: 10.1038/nrn1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heisenberg M., Borst A., Wagner S., Byers D. Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. J. Neurogenet. 1985;2:1–30. doi: 10.3109/01677068509100140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honegger K.S., Campbell R.A., Turner G.C. Cellular-resolution population imaging reveals robust sparse coding in the Drosophila mushroom body. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11772–11785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1099-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang C., Zheng X., Zhao H., Li M., Wang P., Xie Z., Wang L., Zhong Y. A permissive role of mushroom body α/β core neurons in long-term memory consolidation in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:1981–1989. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber F. Central control of movements and behavior of invertebrates. In: Wiersma C.A.G., editor. Invertebrate Nervous Systems. University of Chicago Press; Chicago: 1967. pp. 333–351. [Google Scholar]
- Inagaki H.K., Jung Y., Hoopfer E.D., Wong A.M., Mishra N., Lin J.Y., Tsien R.Y., Anderson D.J. Optogenetic control of Drosophila using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin reveals experience-dependent influences on courtship. Nat. Methods. 2014;11:325–332. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isabel G., Pascual A., Preat T. Exclusive consolidated memory phases in Drosophila. Science. 2004;304:1024–1027. doi: 10.1126/science.1094932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito K., Shinomiya K., Ito M., Armstrong J.D., Boyan G., Hartenstein V., Harzsch S., Heisenberg M., Homberg U., Jenett A., Insect Brain Name Working Group A systematic nomenclature for the insect brain. Neuron. 2014;81:755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jefferis G.S., Marin E.C., Stocker R.F., Luo L. Target neuron prespecification in the olfactory map of Drosophila. Nature. 2001;414:204–208. doi: 10.1038/35102574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenett A., Rubin G.M., Ngo T.T., Shepherd D., Murphy C., Dionne H., Pfeiffer B.D., Cavallaro A., Hall D., Jeter J. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Rep. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kazama H., Wilson R.I. Homeostatic matching and nonlinear amplification at identified central synapses. Neuron. 2008;58:401–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y.C., Lee H.G., Han K.A. D1 dopamine receptor dDA1 is required in the mushroom body neurons for aversive and appetitive learning in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:7640–7647. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1167-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J. Neurobiol. 2001;47:81–92. doi: 10.1002/neu.1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krashes M.J., Waddell S. Rapid consolidation to a radish and protein synthesis-dependent long-term memory after single-session appetitive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:3103–3113. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krashes M.J., Keene A.C., Leung B., Armstrong J.D., Waddell S. Sequential use of mushroom body neuron subsets during drosophila odor memory processing. Neuron. 2007;53:103–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai S.L., Lee T. Genetic mosaic with dual binary transcriptional systems in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:703–709. doi: 10.1038/nn1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee T., Luo L. Mosaic analysis with a repressible cell marker for studies of gene function in neuronal morphogenesis. Neuron. 1999;22:451–461. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80701-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang L., Li Y., Potter C.J., Yizhar O., Deisseroth K., Tsien R.W., Luo L. GABAergic projection neurons route selective olfactory inputs to specific higher-order neurons. Neuron. 2013;79:917–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin S., Owald D., Chandra V., Talbot C., Huetteroth W., Waddell S. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1536–1542. doi: 10.1038/nn.3827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu W.W., Wilson R.I. Glutamate is an inhibitory neurotransmitter in the Drosophila olfactory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:10294–10299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220560110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu C., Plaçais P.Y., Yamagata N., Pfeiffer B.D., Aso Y., Friedrich A.B., Siwanowicz I., Rubin G.M., Preat T., Tanimoto H. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;488:512–516. doi: 10.1038/nature11304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahr A., Aberle H. The expression pattern of the Drosophila vesicular glutamate transporter: a marker protein for motoneurons and glutamatergic centers in the brain. Gene Expr. Patterns. 2006;6:299–309. doi: 10.1016/j.modgep.2005.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao Z., Davis R.L. Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Front Neural Circuits. 2009;3:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin J.R., Ernst R., Heisenberg M. Mushroom bodies suppress locomotor activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Learn. Mem. 1998;5:179–191. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masse N.Y., Turner G.C., Jefferis G.S. Olfactory information processing in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2009;19:R700–R713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire S.E., Le P.T., Davis R.L. The role of Drosophila mushroom body signaling in olfactory memory. Science. 2001;293:1330–1333. doi: 10.1126/science.1062622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolaï L.J., Ramaekers A., Raemaekers T., Drozdzecki A., Mauss A.S., Yan J., Landgraf M., Annaert W., Hassan B.A. Genetically encoded dendritic marker sheds light on neuronal connectivity in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:20553–20558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010198107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okada R., Rybak J., Manz G., Menzel R. Learning-related plasticity in PE1 and other mushroom body-extrinsic neurons in the honeybee brain. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:11736–11747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2216-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen S.R., Bhandawat V., Wilson R.I. Divisive normalization in olfactory population codes. Neuron. 2010;66:287–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owald D., Fouquet W., Schmidt M., Wichmann C., Mertel S., Depner H., Christiansen F., Zube C., Quentin C., Körner J. A Syd-1 homologue regulates pre- and postsynaptic maturation in Drosophila. J. Cell Biol. 2010;188:565–579. doi: 10.1083/jcb.200908055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pai T.P., Chen C.C., Lin H.H., Chin A.L., Lai J.S., Lee P.T., Tully T., Chiang A.S. Drosophila ORB protein in two mushroom body output neurons is necessary for long-term memory formation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:7898–7903. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216336110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnas M., Lin A.C., Huetteroth W., Miesenböck G. Odor discrimination in Drosophila: from neural population codes to behavior. Neuron. 2013;79:932–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perisse E., Yin Y., Lin A.C., Lin S., Huetteroth W., Waddell S. Different kenyon cell populations drive learned approach and avoidance in Drosophila. Neuron. 2013;79:945–956. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitman J.L., Huetteroth W., Burke C.J., Krashes M.J., Lai S.L., Lee T., Waddell S. A pair of inhibitory neurons are required to sustain labile memory in the Drosophila mushroom body. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:855–861. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plaçais P.Y., Trannoy S., Friedrich A.B., Tanimoto H., Preat T. Two pairs of mushroom body efferent neurons are required for appetitive long-term memory retrieval in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 2013;5:769–780. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2013.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pologruto T.A., Sabatini B.L., Svoboda K. ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes. Biomed. Eng. Online. 2003;2:13. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-2-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin H., Cressy M., Li W., Coravos J.S., Izzi S.A., Dubnau J. Gamma neurons mediate dopaminergic input during aversive olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:608–614. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riemensperger T., Völler T., Stock P., Buchner E., Fiala A. Punishment prediction by dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1953–1960. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwaerzel M., Heisenberg M., Zars T. Extinction antagonizes olfactory memory at the subcellular level. Neuron. 2002;35:951–960. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00832-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwaerzel M., Monastirioti M., Scholz H., Friggi-Grelin F., Birman S., Heisenberg M. Dopamine and octopamine differentiate between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:10495–10502. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-33-10495.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Séjourné J., Plaçais P.Y., Aso Y., Siwanowicz I., Trannoy S., Thoma V., Tedjakumala S.R., Rubin G.M., Tchénio P., Ito K. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:903–910. doi: 10.1038/nn.2846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shang Y., Claridge-Chang A., Sjulson L., Pypaert M., Miesenböck G. Excitatory local circuits and their implications for olfactory processing in the fly antennal lobe. Cell. 2007;128:601–612. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.12.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka N.K., Tanimoto H., Ito K. Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom body. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;508:711–755. doi: 10.1002/cne.21692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tempel B.L., Bonini N., Dawson D.R., Quinn W.G. Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1983;80:1482–1486. doi: 10.1073/pnas.80.5.1482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tully T., Quinn W.G. Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1985;157:263–277. doi: 10.1007/BF01350033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vosshall L.B. Olfaction in Drosophila. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2000;10:498–503. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00111-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddell S. Reinforcement signalling in Drosophila; dopamine does it all after all. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013;23:324–329. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Guo H.F., Pologruto T.A., Hannan F., Hakker I., Svoboda K., Zhong Y. Stereotyped odor-evoked activity in the mushroom body of Drosophila revealed by green fluorescent protein-based Ca2+ imaging. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:6507–6514. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3727-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Mamiya A., Chiang A.S., Zhong Y. Imaging of an early memory trace in the Drosophila mushroom body. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:4368–4376. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2958-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang K., Gong J., Wang Q., Li H., Cheng Q., Liu Y., Zeng S., Wang Z. Parallel pathways convey olfactory information with opposite polarities in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:3164–3169. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1317911111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson R.I. Early olfactory processing in Drosophila: mechanisms and principles. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;36:217–241. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong A.M., Wang J.W., Axel R. Spatial representation of the glomerular map in the Drosophila protocerebrum. Cell. 2002;109:229–241. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(02)00707-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xie Z., Huang C., Ci B., Wang L., Zhong Y. Requirement of the combination of mushroom body γ lobe and α/β lobes for the retrieval of both aversive and appetitive early memories in Drosophila. Learn. Mem. 2013;20:474–481. doi: 10.1101/lm.031823.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshihara M. Simultaneous recording of calcium signals from identified neurons and feeding behavior of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Vis. Exp. 2012 doi: 10.3791/3625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu D., Keene A.C., Srivatsan A., Waddell S., Davis R.L. Drosophila DPM neurons form a delayed and branch-specific memory trace after olfactory classical conditioning. Cell. 2005;123:945–957. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu D., Akalal D.B., Davis R.L. Drosophila alpha/beta mushroom body neurons form a branch-specific, long-term cellular memory trace after spaced olfactory conditioning. Neuron. 2006;52:845–855. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S., Roman G. Presynaptic inhibition of gamma lobe neurons is required for olfactory learning in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:2519–2527. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.