Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2015 Mar 24;47:38–41. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.03.015

Testing a Model of Caffeinated Alcohol-specific Expectancies

Ashley N Linden-Carmichael a,c, Cathy Lau-Barraco a,b,d, Amy L Stamates a,e
PMCID: PMC4417404  NIHMSID: NIHMS675064  PMID: 25864133

Abstract

Introduction

The present study sought to further understand the association between caffeinated alcoholic beverage (CAB) use and alcohol-related risks. In particular, we focused on the role of two identified expectancies specific to CAB use: intoxication enhancement and avoidance of negative consequences. Although outcome expectancies are consistent predictors of substance use, limited research has examined expectancies related to CAB use and their association with alcohol-related behaviors, such as protecting themselves from alcohol-related harms. Consequently, the present study examined CAB-specific expectancies and protective behavioral strategies (PBS) as mediators of CAB use and negative consequences.

Methods

Participants were 322 (219 women) college drinkers who completed self-report measures of typical CAB and alcohol use, CAB-specific expectancies, PBS use, and alcohol-related harms.

Results

Structural equation modeling revealed, after controlling for typical non-CAB heavy alcohol use, a significant indirect effect of CAB use to alcohol-related problems through avoidance of negative consequences CAB expectancies and PBS use. However, intoxication enhancement expectancies did not mediate this association.

Conclusions

Thus, our findings indicate that heavier CAB use was associated with stronger expectations that drinking CABs can help avoid negative consequences. These beliefs were related to using fewer PBS when drinking and a greater likelihood of experiencing problems. Given that these expectancies may be underlying mechanisms of CAB use, their inclusion in existing alcohol interventions may be beneficial.

Keywords: Caffeinated alcohol, expectancies, protective behavioral strategies, risky drinking, college students

1. Introduction

Caffeinated alcoholic beverages (CABs), or self-mixed beverages containing caffeine (e.g., Red Bull) and alcohol (e.g., vodka), are popular among college students (Berger, Fendrich, & Fuhrmann, 2013) and are associated with negative outcomes (see Linden & Lau-Barraco, 2014 for a review). Experimental evidence suggests drinking CABs reduces one’s perception of intoxication (e.g., headache, motor coordination failures) without reducing one’s actual level of impairment (e.g., Marczinski & Fillmore, 2006). Thus, CAB consumption may heighten an individual’s vulnerability to experiencing harms. Efforts are needed to identify factors that contribute to or underlie the relationship between CAB use and harms.

Social learning theory (SLT; Bandura, 1969, 1977; Maisto, Carey, & Bradizza, 1999) could provide a backdrop for identifying constructs related to CAB consumption and problems. According to SLT, an individual’s beliefs about the effects of a substance, or outcome expectancies, can impact their drug use. Indeed, expectancies predict problem-drinking development (Christiansen, Smith, Roehling, & Goldman, 1989) and mediate the influence of antecedents and drinking behavior (Sher, Walitzer, Wood, & Brent, 1991). Outcome expectancies also are related to an individual’s use of strategies to protect themselves from alcohol-related harms. Protective behavioral strategies (PBS) are cognitive-behavioral techniques one can use in order to limit alcohol use and problems (Martens, Ferrier, Sheehy, Corbett, Anderson, & Simmons, 2005). PBS use is negatively associated with drinking (Linden, Lau-Barraco, & Milletich, 2014) and drinking-related harms (Martens, Taylor, Damann, Page, Mowry, & Cimini, 2004). Stronger endorsement of positive alcohol expectancies has been shown to relate to less PBS use (Linden et al., 2014) and more negative drinking consequences (Madson, Moorer, Zeigler-Hill, Bonnell, & Villarosa, 2013). These findings support PBS as a relevant factor to consider in understanding the link between expectancies and drinking.

While some studies have examined caffeine or alcohol expectancies related to CAB consumption (e.g., Heinz, Kassel, & Smith, 2009; Lau-Barraco & Linden, 2014; Lau-Barraco, Milletich, & Linden, 2014), there have been few investigations of CAB-specific expectancies (e.g., Mallett, Marzell, Scaglione, Hultgren, & Turrisi, 2014; Varvil-Weld, Marzell, Turrisi, Mallett, & Cleveland, 2013). One study developed a validated measure of CAB-specific expectancies and identified two expectancy factors: intoxication enhancement (IE; e.g., having more energy to party) and avoidance of negative consequences (ANC; e.g., allowing one to drive safer; MacKillop, Howland, Rohsenow, Few, Amlung, Metrik, & Calise, 2012). IE expectancies were found to be associated with more frequent CAB use while ANC expectancies were not. These preliminary relationships suggest that CAB use is propelled by one’s expectations that these beverages will enhance their drunkenness rather than avoid harms. The association between subtypes of CAB expectancies and other drinking-related behaviors (e.g., alcohol-related harms; PBS use) awaits empirical investigation.

In the present study, we sought to test types of CAB-specific expectancies as mediating factors in a conceptual model that encapsulates CAB use, CAB-specific expectancies, PBS use, and alcohol-related problems. We hypothesized that heavier CAB use would be positively associated with stronger IE expectancies, but not ANC expectancies. Further, we hypothesized that heavier CAB use would predict stronger CAB-specific expectancies, which would relate to less frequent PBS use and, in turn, more alcohol-related harms experienced.

2. Material and Methods

2.1 Participants and Procedure

Participants were 322 (219 women) drinkers recruited through an undergraduate psychology research pool at a mid-size East Coast university. Mean age was 20.55 (SD = 3.84) years. Ethnicity was 44.7% Caucasian, 38.8% African American, 6.2% Hispanic, 2.8% Asian, 0.9% Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, 0.3% Native American/Alaskan Native, 5.6% self-identified as “other”, and 0.6% did not respond. Class standing was 46.0% freshmen, 24.2% sophomores, 14.9% juniors, 13.7% seniors, 0.9% self-identified as “other”, and 0.3% did not respond.

After providing informed consent, participants completed self-report questionnaires in small groups on campus that took approximately 1 hour to complete. This study was approved by the university’s college committee on human subjects research and followed APA (2002) guidelines. Participants were provided course credit for participating.

2.2 Measures

CAB and alcohol use was assessed with the Daily Drinking Questionnaire (DDQ; Collins, Parks, & Marlatt, 1985) in which participants reported the number of CABs and alcoholic beverages consumed during a typical week. Typical weekly drinking quantity was used as a measure of CAB use. Heavy episodic drinking frequency was used as a measure of alcohol use. CAB-specific expectancies were measured with the 9-item Caffeine plus Alcohol Combined Effects Questionnaire (CACEQ; MacKillop et al., 2012). Participants reported the extent to which they agreed with statements relating to CAB use ranging from 1 to 5. Subscales include IE (α = .80) and ANC (α = .84). PBS was assessed with the Protective Behavioral Strategies Survey (PBSS; Martens et al., 2005). This 15-item scale asks the extent to which participants use each item when using alcohol/partying ranging from 1 to 5. Of the original three subscales, only two subscales were used: limiting/stopping drinking and manner of drinking, as research indicates they are most proximal to a drinking situation (e.g., Frank, Thake, & Drake, 2012). Internal consistency was .85. Alcohol-related problems in the past 12 months was assessed with the 48-item Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (YAACQ; Read, Kahler, Strong, & Colder, 2006) with response options yes and no. Higher scores indicate greater likelihood of experiencing problems. Internal consistency was .93.

3. Results

Prior to conducting analyses, outliers were Winsorized (Barnett & Lewis, 1994) and missing values were inspected. Missing data ranged from 0% to 2.5% across study variables. The results of Little’s (1988) omnibus test for the pattern missing completely at random (MCAR) was non-significant, χ2(62) = 80.56, p = .057, suggesting the data were MCAR. Thus, expectation maximization algorithms were used to impute missing data. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations are presented in Table 1. Overall, CAB use was associated with greater alcohol-related problems, more frequent heavy episodic drinking, and less frequent PBS use. Regarding expectancies, CAB use was associated with stronger CAB-specific ANC expectancies but unrelated to IE expectancies.

Table 1.

Intercorrelations and Descriptive Statistics for Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. CAB use quantity --
2. CAB IE expectancies .10 --
3. CAB ANC expectancies .11* .33*** --
4. PBSS-L −.15** −.18** −.14* --
5. PBSS-M −.24*** −.32*** −.21*** .62*** --
6. YAACQ .23*** .22*** .18** −.35*** −.41*** --
7. Heavy episodic drinking .27*** .12* .05 −.25*** −.33*** .44*** --
M 3.76 10.39 12.80 20.45 14.51 11.16 1.2
SD 5.40 3.03 4.68 7.58 4.73 9.19 41.17

Note. Correlations are based on expectation maximization imputations. CAB = caffeinated alcoholic beverage; IE = intoxication enhancement; ANC = avoidance of negative consequences; PBSS = Protective Behavioral Strategies Survey (M = manner of drinking subscale; L = limiting/stopping drinking subscale); YAACQ = Young Adults Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire.

***

p < .001.

**

p < .01.

*

p < .05.

Structural equation modeling tested the hypothesized model in Mplus 6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010). Bootstrapping was used to address non-normality within the data. The chi-square goodness of fit statistic (χ2), comparative fit index (CFI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were used to evaluate model fit. Prior research supports that CAB users tend to be heavier drinkers in general (e.g., Brache & Stockwell, 2011), which could account for some of the risks associated with CAB use; thus, we controlled for typical heavy episodic drinking behavior. One latent variable (i.e., PBS) was created with the subscale scores, “limiting/stopping drinking” and “manner of drinking”. Results indicated that the hypothesized model (see Figure 1) provided an acceptable fit to the data, χ2(7) = 9.78, p = .201, CFI = .993, RMSEA = .035, SRMR = .023 (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Avoidance of negative consequences (ANC) and intoxication enhancement (IE) caffeinated alcohol-specific expectancies as mediators of the association between caffeinated alcoholic beverage (CAB) use and outcomes (i.e., protective behavioral strategies latent factor and alcohol-related problems). Typical heavy episodic non-caffeinated alcohol consumption was included as a control but is not displayed here for simplicity. Statistical significance levels pertain to unstandardized estimates based on 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals generated from 10,000 bootstrap samples. Standardized estimates are enclosed in parentheses. *p < .05.

ANC and IE expectancies were tested as separate mediators in the pathways of CAB use on alcohol-related problems through its relationship with expectancies and PBS. Results revealed that CAB ANC expectancies significantly mediated the pathway, B = 0.01 with 95% BC CI [0.00, 0.03], such that greater CAB use was associated with stronger ANC expectancies, less frequent PBS use and more harms. CAB-specific IE expectancies did not emerge as a significant mediator, B = 0.01 with 95% BC CI [−0.01, 0.04].

4. Discussion

The current study tested a conceptual model that partially explained the pathway between CAB use and alcohol-related problems. Our model was able to account for 31% of the variance in negative consequences. We also examined the link between CAB consumption and harms through their association with CAB-specific expectancies and PBS use in drinking situations. Contrary to our hypotheses, ANC expectancies was a more relevant mediator than IE expectancies. Specifically, our results indicated a significant pathway of CAB use, CAB ANC expectancies, PBS use, and problems. That is, heavier CAB users have stronger perceptions that drinking CABs can help prevent or minimize negative outcomes from drinking. These beliefs, however, appear to be linked to a chain of negative consequences that include failing to protect themselves from harm and experiencing drinking-related problems. Findings did not support a significant pathway of CAB use, IE expectancies, PBS use, and problems. In other words, perceptions that drinking CABs can allow them to stay alert for longer and become intoxicated faster was not a key factor in the relationship between CAB use and experience of harm.

We also examined the relationship between both types of CAB-specific expectancies and the number of CABs consumed. We found that stronger ANC expectancies were positively associated with CAB use but IE expectancies were not. This is in contrast to MacKillop and colleagues’ (2012) findings that only IE expectancies were associated with CAB use. One possibility for the divergent results may be the assessment of CAB consumption across both investigations. Our study inquired about CAB use quantity but MacKillop and colleagues assessed frequency. Perhaps IE expectancies are more related to how often one drinks whereas ANC is salient for how many CABs are consumed. It is possible that perceptions of CABs’ energizing properties better determine whether someone will drink CABs on any drinking occasion, but perceiving that CABs can protect against harms may give a false sense that they can drink more.

Regarding associations between CAB-specific beliefs and PBS use, we found that both expectancies were related to less frequent use of PBS. This is consistent with prior research examining alcohol-only outcome expectancies, such that positive beliefs about the effects of alcohol were negatively associated with PBS use (Bonar et al., 2012; Linden et al., 2014; Madson et al., 2013). In the case of CAB use, a similar pattern is observed with ANC expectancies. Particularly, the negative association between these expectancies and PBS use suggests that when college students perceive that drinking CABs protects them from deleterious effects of alcohol, they may not see a need to use PBS. PBS use also was negatively associated with IE expectancies, such that perceiving energy enhancing outcomes from drinking CABs may make one less inclined to use PBS. Perhaps students drinking CABs to get “buzzed” more quickly are unlikely to be concerned with using PBS, as PBS would prevent them from achieving the desired effects.

Findings from the current study offer implications for incorporating information on problematic CAB use in brief motivational interventions (BMIs; see Larimer & Cronce, 2002 for a review). CAB users may benefit from BMIs that address factors relevant to CAB outcomes, including those found in the present study (e.g., PBS, ANC expectancies), as well as including CAB-specific information in the personalized feedback given in the BMI. Prior investigations suggest that receiving beverage-specific information within an intervention can help decrease the use of certain beverages (e.g., malt liquor; Werch, Jobli, Moore, DiClemente, Dore, & Brown, 2005). Thus, including CAB-specific information may increase the efficacy of reducing heavy CAB consumption.

There are several limitations that should be noted. Our study is cross-sectional and therefore prevents causal inferences. Additionally, our study relied on self-report estimates, which may be subject to recall biases or social desirability. Furthermore, the majority of our sample consisted of females, which may limit our ability to generalize to men. Lastly, our findings did not control for impulsivity, which can be related to CAB outcomes (Brache & Stockwell, 2011). Some research does, however, suggest that the link between CAB use and consequences exists after considering risk-taking propensity (Brache & Stockwell, 2011).

5. Conclusions

Our findings contributed to the existing literature by further illuminating factors associated with CAB consumption. We found preliminary evidence that heavier CAB consumption and stronger CAB-specific expectancies were related to less PBS use. Our findings also revealed that heavier CAB use was associated with stronger expectations that CABs can help avoid negative consequences and, consequently, a failure to use PBS when drinking and greater likelihood of experiencing problems. The underlying influence of ANC expectancies and PBS may indicate their relevance for inclusion in existing alcohol interventions.

Highlights.

  • Developed a conceptual model of caffeinated alcoholic beverage (CAB) use and harms.

  • Expectancies and protective behavioral strategies mediated study associations.

  • CAB-specific expectancies may warrant inclusion in alcohol interventions.

Acknowledgments

Role of Funding Sources

Ashley N. Linden-Carmichael, M.S. is supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) under a Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F31-AA023118-01A1). NIAAA had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Contributors

All authors significantly contributed to and approved the final manuscript. Ashley N. Linden-Carmichael designed the study, conceptualized the research aims and hypotheses, conducted statistical analyses, and wrote the Introduction, Results, and Discussion sections. Cathy Lau-Barraco helped further conceptualize the research aims and hypotheses and provided feedback on the manuscript. Amy L. Stamates wrote the Method section and assisted in proof-reading the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

All authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest for the current paper.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychological Association. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist. 2002;57:1060–1073. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.12.1060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bandura A. Principles of behavior modification. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bandura A. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnett V, Lewis T. Outliers in statistical data. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester; New York: 1994. p. 608. [Google Scholar]
  5. Berger L, Fendrich M, Fuhrmann D. Alcohol mixed energy drinks: Are there associated negative consequences beyond hazardous drinking in college students? Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38:2428–2432. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bonar EE, Hoffman E, Rosenberg H, Kryszak E, Young KM, Ashrafioun L, Bannon EE. Development of a questionnaire to assess university students’ intentions to use behavioral alcohol-reduction strategies. Journal of American College Health. 2012;60:395–402. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2012.663842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brache K, Stockwell T. Drinking patterns and risk behaviors associated with combined alcohol and energy drink consumption in college drinkers. Addictive Behaviors. 2011;36:1133–1140. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Christiansen BA, Smith GT, Roehling PV, Goldman MS. Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;57:93–99. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Collins RL, Parks GA, Marlatt GA. Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self-administration of alcohol. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1985;53:189–200. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.53.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Frank C, Thake J, Davis CG. Assessing the protective value of protective behavioral strategies. Journal of Studies of Alcohol and Drugs. 2012;73:839–843. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Heinz AJ, Kassel JD, Smith EV. Caffeine expectancy: Instrument development in the Rasch measurement framework. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009;23:500–511. doi: 10.1037/a0016654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Larimer ME, Cronce JM. Identification, prevention, and treatment: A review of individual-focused strategies to reduce problematic alcohol consumption by college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002;14:148–163. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lau-Barraco C, Linden AN. Caffeinated alcohol use and expectancies for caffeine versus alcohol. Substance Use & Misuse. 2014;49:1241–1249. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2014.891619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lau-Barraco C, Milletich RJ, Linden AN. Caffeinated alcohol consumption profiles and associations with use severity and outcome expectancies. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39:308–315. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Linden AN, Lau-Barraco C. A qualitative review of psychosocial risk factors associated with caffeinated alcohol use. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2014;22:144–153. doi: 10.1037/a0036334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Linden AN, Lau-Barraco C, Milletich RJ. Protective behavioral strategies, alcohol expectancies, and drinking motives in a model of college student drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2014;28:952–959. doi: 10.1037/a0037041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Little R. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1988;83:1198–1202. doi: 10.2307/2290157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. MacKillop J, Howland J, Rohsenow DJ, Few LR, Amlung MT, Metrik J, Calise TV. Initial development of a measure of expectancies for combinations of alcohol and caffeine: The Caffeine + Alcohol Combined Questionnaire (CACEQ) Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2012;20:466–472. doi: 10.1037/a0030539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Madson MB, Moorer KD, Zeigler-Hill V, Bonnell MA, Villarosa M. Alcohol expectancies, protective behavioral strategies, and alcohol-related outcomes: A moderated mediation study. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy. 2013;20:286–296. doi: 10.3109/09687637.2013.766788. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Maisto SA, Carey KB, Bradizza CM. Social learning theory. In: Leonard KE, Blaine HT, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 106–163. [Google Scholar]
  22. Mallett KA, Marzell M, Scaglione N, Hultgren B, Turrisi R. Are all alcohol and energy drink users the same? Examining individual variation in relation to alcohol mixed with energy drink use, risky drinking, and consequences. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2014;28:97–104. doi: 10.1037/a0032203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Marczinski CA, Fillmore MT. Clubgoers and their trendy cocktails: Implications of mixing caffeine into alcohol on information processing and subjective reports of intoxication. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2006;14:450–458. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.4.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Martens MP, Ferrier AG, Sheehy MJ, Corbett K, Anderson DA, Simmons A. Development of the protective behavioral strategies survey. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2005;66:698–705. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Martens MP, Taylor KK, Damann KM, Page JC, Mowry ES, Cimini MD. Protective behavioral strategies when drinking alcohol and their relationship to negative alcohol-related consequences in college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2004;18:390–393. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.4.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 6. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2010. [Google Scholar]
  27. Read JP, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Colder CR. Development and preliminary validation of the young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:169–177. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sher KJ, Walitzer KS, Wood PK, Brent EE. Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1991;100:427–448. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Varvil-Weld L, Marzell M, Turrisi R, Mallett KA, Cleveland MJ. Examining the relationship between alcohol-energy drink risk profiles and high-risk drinking behaviors. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2013;37:1410–1416. doi: 10.1111/acer.12102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Werch C, Jobli E, Moore MJ, DiClemente CC, Dore H, Brown CH. A brief experimental alcohol beverage-tailored program for adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66:284–290. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES