Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Health Psychol. 2013 Oct 22;20(4):369–379. doi: 10.1177/1359105313502564

Are child anxiety and somatization associated with pain in pain-related functional gastrointestinal disorders?

Amy E Williams 1, Danita I Czyzewski 2,3, Mariella M Self 2,3, Robert J Shulman 2,3,4
PMCID: PMC4418969  NIHMSID: NIHMS682632  PMID: 24155191

Abstract

This study investigated individual and incremental contributions of somatization and trait anxiety to pain report in children with pain-related functional gastrointestinal disorders. Eighty children (7–10 years) with pain-related functional gastrointestinal disorders completed the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children, the Children’s Somatization Inventory, and 2-week pain diaries (assessing pain frequency and maximum pain). Hierarchical regressions indicated that both trait anxiety and somatization were significantly related to maximum pain and pain frequency, with somatization explaining more variance. Trait anxiety did not significantly add to prediction above somatization. Assessment of somatization may assist with treatment planning for children with functional abdominal pain.

Keywords: anxiety, functional abdominal pain, pediatric pain, somatization

Introduction

Abdominal pain is one of the most common pain complaints in childhood (Saps et al., 2009). For a large proportion of these complaints, no known medical condition will account for these pains, and no medical treatment is indicated (Subcommittee on Chronic Abdominal Pain, 2005; Thakkar et al., 2007). The Rome diagnostic criteria categorize these pain conditions broadly as pain-related functional gastrointestinal (GI) disorders (painF-GID) (Drossman et al., 2006). Children with painFGID have functional impairments similar in extent to children with organic GI disorders (Varni et al., 2006; Youssef et al., 2006) with many developing chronic symptoms (Walker et al., 2004). Cognitive–behavioral interventions for painFGIDs are increasingly recognized as the front line of treatment (Palermo et al., 2010; Sprenger et al., 2011), and therefore innovations in treatment will be facilitated through better delineation of psychological characteristics that are the most salient targets of cognitive–behavioral interventions.

In adults with medically unexplained symptoms, including painFGID, anxiety and/or somatization are frequently identified as predictors of enhanced pain experience and increased medical treatment-seeking (Boyd et al., 2005; Henningsen et al., 2003; Koloski et al., 2003; Vandvik et al., 2004; Whitehead et al., 1997). Cognitive–behavioral interventions addressing anxiety and/or somatization have been effective at decreasing symptom severity, disability, and healthcare utilization (Allen et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2007). Additionally, greater improvement in functional ability following treatment was found to be correlated with larger reductions in somatization (Creed et al., 2005).

In contrast to the long history of research on anxiety, efforts to understand somatization are more recent. Somatization can be defined as increased perception, awareness, and/or pathological interpretation of unpleasant physiological sensations. Prospective and epidemiological studies and measurement advances (Walker et al., 2009) have provided growing empirical support for the construct of somatization. Somatization has been described in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood and is stable across developmental stages (Dhossche et al., 2001; Steinhausen and Winkler Metzke, 2007). Research provides evidence for cognitive (attribution, attention) as well as perceptual (sensitivity, lowered threshold) models of somatization (Rief and Broadbent, 2007). Evidence of somatization (distress intolerance and increased body vigilance) has been observed to a greater extent in adults with irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) compared to controls (Keough et al., 2011).

As with adults, higher anxiety in children with painFGID is associated with increased pain severity (Saps et al., 2009), frequency, duration (Hyams et al., 1996), chronicity (Mulvaney et al., 2006), and disability (Walker and Greene, 1991; Wendland et al., 2010). Higher somatization is related to increased pain report in healthy children (Rocha et al., 2003), and children with painFGID have higher somatization scores compared to healthy peers and peers with mood and anxiety disorders (Czyzewski et al., 2007; Tsao et al., 2009; Walker et al., 1993). However, less is known about the relation of somatization to pain in within the group of children with painFGIDs.

Psychological treatment of children’s pain-FGIDs typically targets manifestations of anxiety (body tension, worry, behavioral avoidance) with both cognitive and behavioral strategies (Levy and Walker, 2005; Palermo et al., 2010), while manifestations of somatization (catastrophic thinking about pain, dysfunctional beliefs about pain, misattribution of sensations) are much less frequently targeted. In comparison to the child literature, adult interventions have more examples of direct targeting of somatization (“Sometimes my stomach just feels this way and it doesn’t mean anything is wrong”) (Allen et al., 2006; Boyce, 2001; Martin et al., 2007).

Associations between psychological variables and painFGID can be understood in the context of motivational priming theory (MPT) which states that affective experience emanates from two opponent motive systems, appetitive (reward or positive affect) and defensive (threat or negative affect) (Bradley et al., 2001; Lang, 1995). Appetitive activation inhibits and defensive activation facilitates pain or nociception (Rhudy et al., 2005). Thus, persons who tend to experience greater defensive activation, such as during anxiety or somatization, are likely to also experience more pain. Preparedness theory states that a negative association is more easily developed with a fear-relevant stimulus (Seligman, 1970, 1971). A fear-relevant stimulus associated with pain may over time form a stronger association and cause greater defensive activation. Thus, MPT predicts that anxiety and somatization likely cause pain facilitation, and preparedness theory predicts that somatization (which is by definition more fear relevant to pain than is anxiety) may lead to greater defensive activation and pain facilitation; however, research has not assessed this prediction.

Research clarifying relations of anxiety and somatization to pain in children with painFGID will begin to address the question of whether somatization is undertreated compared to anxiety, help identify children at risk for poor outcomes, and further tailor treatments to specifically target psychological variables most related to greater morbidity. This study sought to determine independent contributions of child somatization and trait anxiety to pain in children with painFGID, and to determine if trait anxiety has a significant contribution to pain experience beyond that explained by somatization. We hypothesized that somatization would have greater contributions to pain.

Methods

Participants

Participants were part of a larger study of physiological and psychological characteristics of children with painFGID (Shulman et al., 2007). Children aged 7–10 years with painFGID were recruited from primary and tertiary care clinics in a large academically affiliated pediatric healthcare network. This age range was selected to avoid confounds with the effects of puberty on pain and because children at this age are cognitively able to complete study questionnaires. Chart reviews were conducted by a trained research coordinator to identify potential participants with diagnoses of abdominal pain or irritable bowel syndrome (International Classification of Diseases (ICD)-9 codes 789.0 and 564.00). Parents were contacted by mail and advised to call the research coordinator if interested in participating. Interested participants were screened by telephone to assess inclusion or exclusion criteria. Children were included if, according to parent report, for at least the previous 2 months they had abdominal pain one or more times per week of moderate to severe (≥3/10) intensity or causing interference with activities (Apley and Naish, 1958; Von Baeyer and Walker, 1999). A chart review was completed to ensure no medical cause for abdominal pain had been identified. Children were excluded due to presence of organic GI illness (or if organic GI illness remained in the differential), other significant chronic health condition (requiring daily medication or specialty follow-up care), decreased growth velocity, GI blood loss, unexplained fever, vomiting, chronic severe diarrhea, weight loss ≥5 percent of their body weight within a 3-month period, current use of anti-inflammatory medications, or previous use of GI medication that provided complete relief of symptoms. Children were also excluded if English language fluency or developmental disabilities would interfere with questionnaire completion.

Participants included 114 children and their parents. Twenty patients were excluded for missing questionnaire data and 14 because their diaries (described below) were missing 30 percent or more of the pain ratings. The final sample included 80 participants. An a priori power analysis with G-Power (Faul et al., 2009) indicated that a sample size of 74 participants would provide adequate power. All procedures were approved by the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board. A trained research coordinator provided an explanation of study procedures and reviewed consent forms prior to obtaining written consent from parents and informed assent from the child.

Procedure

Study procedures were completed during a home visit. Parents provided demographic information and children completed questionnaires in a room independent from their parent. A research assistant explained questionnaires and answered any questions. Questionnaires were read to 7-year-old children to ensure reading difficulties did not affect responses. Parents and children were then instructed (verbally and in writing) regarding completion of the pain diary (kept for 2 weeks following the home visit) and collection of a stool sample (done within 1–2 days of the home visit). A research coordinator contacted each participant weekly to ensure diary completion.

Measures

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAIC) (Spielberger, 1973) is a measure of general anxiety symptoms designed to be relevant to most children including those with and without physical symptoms. The child-response questionnaire consists of two 20-item sub-scales: state anxiety (context specific) and trait anxiety (stable individual difference in anxiety proneness). This study used trait anxiety, which inquires about how the child “usually” feels. Items consist of emotional statements (e.g. “I worry too much”) and children indicate how often the statement is true for them on a 3-point rating (“hardly ever” = 1 point, “sometimes” = 2 points, and “often” = 3 points). Responses are summed with total scores ranging between 20 and 60 and higher scores indicating greater trait anxiety. T-scores were used for data analyses. The trait anxiety subscale has demonstrated good test–retest (6-week interval; rs > .65) and internal consistency reliability (αs > .78) (Spielberger, 1973). Criterion-related validity has been demonstrated via high correlations with other measures of child anxiety (rs > .63) (Spielberger, 1973).

Children’s Somatization Inventory

The Children’s Somatization Inventory (CSI) (Walker and Garber, 1993) assesses somatization symptoms in pediatric patients and was originally designed for use with children with recurrent abdominal pain. The original child-report version (35 items) was used. Each item lists a symptom (e.g. “difficulty swallowing,” “headache”) and children indicate on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = “not at all,” 1 = “a little,” 2 = “some,” 3 = “a lot,” and 4 = “a whole lot”) how much they were “bothered by each symptom” during the preceding 2 weeks. Scores range between 0 and 140 with increasing scores indicating greater levels of somatization. The questionnaire has demonstrated good test–retest (r = .66) and internal consistency reliability (α = .92) (Walker and Garber, 1993). The CSI is correlated with the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) Somatic Complaints scale (r = .42) suggesting good criterion-related validity (Walker and Garber, 1993).

Pain diary

Participants completed a 2-week paper diary to track abdominal pain. Children were instructed to complete the diary without parental input; however, parents were asked to prompt children to complete it. Three diary ratings were made daily (upon awakening, after lunch, and at bedtime). Children made pain ratings by placing a mark on a 100-mm horizontal visual analog scale (VAS) with anchors of “no pain at all” and “worst pain you can imagine.” Pain intensity was established by measuring the distance in millimeters from the left end of the line to the mark made by the child. Maximum pain rating was the highest pain rating (1–100 mm) given during the 2-week period. Pain frequency was defined as the number of times a child rated pain as ≥10 mm on a 100-mm VAS. This cutoff was selected because pain ≥ 10 mm has been accepted in the literature as representing clinically significant mild pain (Jensen et al., 2003; Kelly, 2001).

GI inflammation

Subclinical levels of inflammation have been identified in children with painFGID (Shulman et al., 2008), and positive correlations between GI inflammation and somatization in children with functional GI symptoms have been reported (Schurman et al., 2010). Fecal calprotectin concentration has been found to reliably reflect GI inflammation identified during more invasive and extensive testing (Fagerberg et al., 2005). In the absence of a medical diagnosis, pain symptoms caused by GI inflammation may be wrongly attributed solely to psychological factors. Because GI inflammation is a potential confound in our assessment of the association between psychological variables and pain, this study included GI inflammation in the initial step of regression models. A stool was obtained from children by placing a plastic “hat” over the toilet. Fecal calprotectin levels are expressed as micrograms per gram of stool and were analyzed using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) technique (Genova Diagnostics, Asheville, NC, USA).

Data analysis

Preliminary analyses

Independent samples t-tests were used to compare included and excluded participants on trait anxiety, somatization, GI inflammation, maximum pain rating, and pain frequency. Pearson correlations assessed relations between predictor and outcome variables. Descriptive analyses were conducted for somatization, trait anxiety, and fecal calprotectin.

Primary analyses

Pain outcome data were not normally distributed, and thus, these analyses were completed with Log(e) transformed data. To assess the independent contributions of the psychological variables, two parallel hierarchical regressions were conducted for each pain outcome (maximum pain rating, pain frequency), one with trait anxiety and another with somatization as a predictor. We included sex as a covariate in analyses since research indicates that girls experience more chronic pain, greater pain intensity, and more pain-related disability compared to boys (Hakala et al., 2002; Merlijn et al., 2003; Roth-Isigkeit et al., 2005). Sex was entered first and fecal calprotectin was entered second in regressions. Trait anxiety or somatization was entered last. A final hierarchical regression was conducted for each pain outcome to assess the additive effect of trait anxiety. Sex and fecal calprotectin were entered in Step 1; somatization was entered in Step 2, followed by trait anxiety in Step 3.

Results

Demographics

Seventy-one percent (n = 57) of the participants were girls. The mean age of the participating children was 8.50 years (standard deviation (SD) = 1.11 years). Participants identified themselves as Caucasian (68%), Hispanic American (15%), Black or African American (10%), and Asian American (1%).

Preliminary analyses

There were no significant differences between included and excluded participants on trait anxiety, somatization, GI inflammation, maximum pain rating, or pain frequency (ts < .68, ps > .49). Table 1 presents correlations between predictor and outcome variables. The mean trait anxiety t-score was 46.93 (SD = 13.42). Fifteen participants (19%) had a trait anxiety score at or above 1 SD above the mean (t ≥ 60). Thus, the trait anxiety scores were generally consistent with the normative sample (Spielberger, 1973). The mean somatization (CSI) score was 28.09 (SD = 21.42). Median calprotectin concentration was 38.00 μg/g of stool (range = 15–403 g) with 25 participants (31.25%) having concentrations at or above 50 μg/g of stool, which has been suggested as the upper limit of normal for both adults (Ton et al., 2000) and children (Fagerberg et al., 2003). A post hoc power analysis with G-Power (Faul et al., 2009) indicated an achieved power of .93 or greater for regression analyses.

Table 1.

Correlations between predictor and outcome variables.

GI inflammation Trait anxiety CSI Maximum pain
Trait anxiety −.01
CSI .23* .43*
Maximum pain .11 .31* .38*
No. of pain episodes .02 .28* .40* .74*

GI: gastrointestinal; CSI: Children’s Somatization Inventory.

*

p < .05.

To ensure pain-related items or symptoms (i.e. headache, lower back pain, stomach pain) on the CSI were not solely responsible for relations between the CSI and pain outcomes, the analyses were also conducted with the eight pain-related CSI items removed from CSI scores. The same pattern of results emerged; results using the full CSI questionnaire are reported to maintain the integrity of the questionnaire.

Prediction of maximum pain rating

Table 2 presents outcomes for regression analyses, including the unique variance accounted for by trait anxiety and somatization in the respective final regression models.

Table 2.

Hierarchical regressions of individual and incremental contributions of somatization and trait anxiety.

Variable R2 R2 change F β t % Unique variance
Regressions assessing individual contributions of variables
Maximum pain report (0–100 mm VAS)
Trait anxiety regression 0.13 3.82*
Step 1 Sex 0.16 1.44
Step 2 GI inflammation 0.13 1.19
Step 3 Trait anxiety 0.09 0.30 2.75* 8.64
Somatization regression 0.17 5.25*
Step 1 Sex 0.17 1.64
Step 2 GI inflammation 0.04 0.38
Step 3 Somatization 0.13 0.37 3.41* 12.67
Number of pain episodes
Trait anxiety regression 0.11 3.09*
Step 1 Sex 0.17 1.53
Step 2 GI inflammation 0.03 0.31
Step 3 Trait anxiety 0.07 0.27 2.50* 7.29
Somatization regression 0.20 6.16*
Step 1 Sex 0.18 1.75
Step 2 GI inflammation −0.06 −0.59
Step 3 Somatization 0.16 0.41 3.89* 16.00
Regressions assessing incremental value of trait anxiety
Maximum pain report (0–100 mm VAS) 0.20 4.56*
Step 1 Sex 0.17 1.66
GI inflammation 0.06 0.56
Step 2 Somatization 0.21 0.29 2.43* 6.30
Step 3 Trait anxiety 0.01 0.17 1.50 2.40
Number of pain episodes 0.21 4.86*
Step 1 Sex 0.18 1.76
GI inflammation −0.05 −0.47
Step 2 Somatization 0.21 0.36 3.05* 9.86
Step 3 Trait anxiety 0.00 0.11 0.99 1.04

VAS: visual analog scale; GI: gastrointestinal.

R2 change is provided for trait anxiety and somatization.

*

p < .05.

Trait anxiety

The final model was significant and explained 13 percent of the variance in maximum pain ratings. Significant effects were yielded for trait anxiety (β = .30, p < .01).

Somatization

The final model was significant and explained 17 percent of the variance. Significant effects were yielded for somatization (β = .37, p < .01).

Prediction of pain frequency

Trait anxiety

The final model was significant and explained 11 percent of the variance in pain frequency. Significant effects were yielded for trait anxiety (β = .27, p = .02).

Somatization

The final model was significant and explained 20 percent of the variance in pain frequency. Significant effects were yielded for somatization (β = .41, p < .01).

Incremental effect of anxiety

Individual models indicated that somatization accounted for more unique variance than anxiety for both pain outcomes. A combined model assessed whether anxiety provided incremental value to the variance accounted for by somatization. Results for the final models are presented in Table 2 and summarized below.

Maximum pain rating

Sex and GI inflammation included in Step 1 (p = .17) accounted for 5 percent of variance and yielded no significant correlates of maximum pain ratings. Step 2 was significant (p < .01) and yielded a significant effect for the added variable of somatization (β = .37, p < .01). The model accounted for 17 percent of variance in maximum pain ratings. Step 3 was significant (p < .01) and accounted for 20 percent of the variance in maximum pain ratings, but did not yield a significant change in variance accounted for (p = .14). A significant effect was yielded for somatization (β = .29, p = .02).

Pain frequency

Sex and GI inflammation included in Step 1 (p = .25) accounted for 4 percent of variance and yielded no significant correlates of pain frequency. Step 2 was significant (p < .01) and yielded a significant effect for the added variable of somatization (β = .41, p < .01). The model accounted for 20 percent of variance in pain frequency. Step 3 was significant (p < .01) and accounted for 21 percent of the variance in pain frequency, but did not yield a significant change in variance accounted for (p = .33). A significant effect was yielded for somatization (β = 36, p < .01).

Discussion

With awareness of the need to continually improve targets of cognitive–behavioral pain treatment for children, this study compared the contributions of trait anxiety and somatization to pain complaints in a group of young children with painFGID. After controlling for sex and GI inflammation, somatization, compared to trait anxiety, was more strongly associated with pain in children with painFGID, suggesting that children who are prone to report multiple somatic complaints have more intense and frequent abdominal pain. Not only did somatization explain a greater amount of variance in pain than did trait anxiety, when both constructs were entered into a combined model, trait anxiety did not significantly add to prediction above and beyond somatization. These data are consistent with findings from a study in adults which found somatization but not trait anxiety was related to outcomes in irritable bowel syndrome (Boyd et al., 2005). While research has demonstrated elevated levels of somatization in children with painFGID compared to peers without painFGID (Czyzewski et al., 2007; Walker et al., 1993), this study extends those findings by demonstrating that within a group of children with painFGID greater somatization is related to increased pain. The present findings are consistent with MPT and preparedness theory which together predict that trait anxiety and somatization should lead to defensive activation and resulting pain facilitation with a stronger relation between somatization, compared to anxiety, and pain.

Psychological variables are believed to influence pain via supraspinal pathways (periaqueductal gray (PAG), rostroventromedial medulla (RVM), and dorsal horn of spinal cord) which can cause both facilitation and inhibition of pain. Imaging studies demonstrate that distraction from pain leads to pain inhibition via activation of PAG–RVM pain inhibitory pathways (Valet et al., 2004). Furthermore, pain itself is a physiological stressor which may itself increase pain facilitation (Rhudy et al., 2005). Thus, to the extent a child with high somatization is overattending to physiological sensations, they are likely to have decreases in supraspinal inhibition of pain and increases in supraspinal facilitation of pain, and therefore augmented pain experience overall.

This study has implications for treatment of children with painFGID, especially within a medical setting. Trait anxiety in this population typically does not reach clinically significant thresholds, as was the case in this study. Although anxiety may sometimes be more readily apparent, and is a concept more familiar to medical and lay-persons, somatization may provide a more specific representation of how psychological factors exacerbate pain. Failure to recognize a child’s tendency to overattend to and make negative attributions about bodily sensations could result in inadvertent reinforcement of this characteristic by attention (even through repeated reassurance) from healthcare professionals—counter to what may be most helpful for pain management. Cognitive strategies targeting somatic focus and dysfunctional beliefs about physical sensations may improve therapeutic outcomes above those achieved by treatment of anxiety and pain management strategies alone. Furthermore, measures of somatization may help identify children with abdominal pain who are at risk for more impairing or chronic symptoms.

An increased tendency toward somatization has been recognized in mothers of children with painFGID (Czyzewski et al., 2007; Van der Veek et al., 2011; Walker and Greene, 1989), suggesting these mothers may be prone to attending to their children’s symptoms. A healthcare provider’s counsel to the parent to pay less attention to their child’s symptoms is likely to be an important component of intervention and requires that the physician be aware of the relation between pain complaints and somatization, not just the relation between pain complaints and anxiety.

While some evidence supports that persons with GI inflammation report increased pain compared to those without GI inflammation (Schurman et al., 2010), this was not supported by the present data. Thirty-one percent of participants had GI inflammation, but this was not a significant contributor to pain report. Similarly, while gender differences in pain are often found, gender did not contribute unique variance in predicting these outcomes. The relatively young age of our sample may account for this lack of difference since most pediatric functional abdominal pain samples in the literature include both children and adolescents.

Use of daily diaries to gather data regarding pain is a strength of this study. The diary helps overcome potential inaccuracies inherent in retrospective self-report data, particularly with children who may be poor reporters of past symptoms. However, any child self-report data, including diaries, should be interpreted with caution as they rely on a young child’s ability to accurately and reliably report symptoms.

Limitations of this study include a relatively small sample size with more female than male participants. Although inclusion of only 7- to 10-year-old children limits the generalizability to children outside this age range, it does provide a more focused understanding of prepubescent children with painFGID, a less studied population. Finally, future research should seek to replicate these results with multiple methods of assessment since reliance only on children’s report of symptoms is a potential limitation.

The results of this study suggest several avenues for future research to better understand and treat painFGID in children. Future studies should compare the efficacy of psychological treatments targeting somatization versus anxiety in treatment of children with painFGID to determine if specifically targeting somatization leads to better outcomes. Future research should seek to understand whether somatization and/or anxiety are predictive of long-term pain outcomes, predispose children to painFGID, or are the result of frequent pain experiences. Regardless, these data illustrate the importance of including the assessment of somatization symptoms in research and treatment of patients with abdominal pain.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This research was supported by grant number R01-NR05337 to R.J.S. from the National Institutes of Health, the Daffy’s Foundation, the USDA/ARS under Cooperative Agreement No. 6250-51000-043, and P30-DK56338 which funds the Texas Medical Center Digestive Disease Center.

Footnotes

Declaration of conflicting interests

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This work is a publication of the USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine and Texas Children’s Hospital, Houston, TX. The contents of this publication do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the USDA, nor does mention of trade names, commercial products, or organizations imply endorsement by the US Government.

References

  1. Allen LA, Woolfolk RL, Escobar JI, et al. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for somatization disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2006;166:1512–1518. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.14.1512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apley J, Naish N. Recurrent abdominal pains: A field survey of 1,000 school children. Archives of Disease in Childhood. 1958;33:165–170. doi: 10.1136/adc.33.168.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boyce P. Psychologic therapies for irritable bowel syndrome. Current Treatment Options in Gastroenterology. 2001;4(4):323–331. doi: 10.1007/s11938-001-0058-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Boyd C, Abraham S, Kellow J. Psychological features are important predictors of functional gastrointestinal disorders in patients with eating disorders. Scandinavian Journal of Gastroenterology. 2005;40:929–935. doi: 10.1080/00365520510015836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, et al. Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion. 2001;1:276–298. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Creed F, Guthrie E, Ratcliffe J, et al. Does psychological treatment help only those patients with severe irritable bowel syndrome who also have a concurrent psychiatric disorder? The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;39(9):807–815. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Czyzewski DI, Eakin MN, Lane MM, et al. Recurrent abdominal pain in primary and tertiary care: Similarities and differences. Children’s Health Care. 2007;36(2):137–153. doi: 10.1080/02739610701334970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dhossche D, Ferdinand R, van der Ende J, et al. Outcome of self-reported functional-somatic symptoms in a community sample of adolescents. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 2001;13:191–199. doi: 10.1023/a:1014686318060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Drossman DA, Corazziari E, Delvaux M, et al. ROME III: The Functional Gastrointestinal Disorders. McLean, VA: Degnon Associates; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  10. Fagerberg UL, Loof L, Merzoug RD, et al. Fecal calprotectin levels in healthy children studied with an improved assay. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 2003;37:468–472. doi: 10.1097/00005176-200310000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fagerberg UL, Loof L, Myrdal U, et al. Colorectal inflammation is well predicted by fecal calprotectin in children with gastrointestinal symptoms. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 2005;40:450–455. doi: 10.1097/01.mpg.0000154657.08994.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41:1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hakala P, Rimpela A, Salminen JJ, et al. Back, neck, and shoulder pain in Finnish adolescents: National cross sectional surveys. British Medical Journal. 2002;325:743. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7367.743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Henningsen P, Zimmermann T, Sattel H. Medically unexplained physical symptoms, anxiety, and depression: A meta-analytic review. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(4):528–533. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000075977.90337.e7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hyams JS, Burke G, Davis PM, et al. Abdominal pain and irritable bowel syndrome in adolescents: A community-based study. Journal of Pediatrics. 1996;129:220–226. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(96)70246-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Jensen MP, Chen C, Brugger AM. Interpretation of visual analog scale ratings and change scores: A reanalysis of two clinical trials of postoperative pain. Journal of Pain. 2003;4(7):407–414. doi: 10.1016/s1526-5900(03)00716-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kelly A-M. The minimum clinically significant difference in visual analogue scale pain score does not differ with severity of pain. Emergency Medicine Journal. 2001;18:205–207. doi: 10.1136/emj.18.3.205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Keough ME, Timpano KR, Zawilinski LL, et al. The association between irritable bowel syndrome and the anxiety vulnerability factors: Body vigilance and discomfort tolerance. Journal of Health Psychology. 2011;16:91–98. doi: 10.1177/1359105310367689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Koloski NA, Talley NJ, Boyce PM. Does psychological distress modulate functional gastrointestinal symptoms and health care seeking? A prospective, community cohort study. American Journal of Gastroenterology. 2003;98:789–797. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2003.07388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lang PJ. The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist. 1995;50(5):372–385. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.50.5.372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Levy RL, Walker LS. Cognitive behavioral therapy for treatment of recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly. 2005;19(2):137–149. [Google Scholar]
  22. Martin A, Rauh E, Fichter M, et al. A one-session treatment for patients suffering from medically unexplained symptoms in primary care: A randomized clinical trial. Psychosomatics. 2007;48(4):294–303. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.48.4.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Merlijn VPBM, Hunfeld JAM, van der Wouden JC, et al. Psychosocial factors associated with chronic pain in adolescents. Pain. 2003;101:33–43. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00289-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mulvaney S, Lambert EW, Garber J, et al. Trajectories of symptoms and impairment for pediatric patients with functional abdominal pain: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:737–744. doi: 10.1097/10.chi.0000214192.57993.06. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Palermo TM, Eccleston C, Lewandowski AS, et al. Randomized controlled trials of psychological therapies for management of chronic pain in children and adolescents: An updated meta-analytic review. Pain. 2010;148(3):387–397. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe KM, et al. Affective modulation of nociception at spinal and supraspinal levels. Psychophysiology. 2005;42:579–587. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00313.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rief W, Broadbent E. Explaining medically unexplained symptoms-models and mechanisms. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:821–841. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rocha EM, Prkachin KM, Beaumont SL, et al. Pain reactivity and somatization in kindergarten-aged children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2003;28:47–57. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/28.1.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Roth-Isigkeit A, Thyen U, Stoven H, et al. Pain among children and adolescents: Restrictions in daily living and triggering factors. Pediatrics. 2005;115:e152–e162. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Saps M, Seshadri P, Sztainbert M, et al. A prospective school-based study of abdominal pain and other common somatic complaints in children. Journal of Pediatrics. 2009;154:322–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.09.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Schurman JV, Singh M, Singh V, et al. Symptoms and subtypes in pediatric functional dyspepsia: Relation to mucosal inflammation and psychological functioning. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 2010;51:298–303. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0b013e3181d1363c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Seligman ME. On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review. 1970;77:406–418. [Google Scholar]
  33. Seligman ME. Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy. 1971;2:307–320. [Google Scholar]
  34. Shulman RJ, Eakin MN, Czyzewski DI, et al. Increased gastrointestinal permeability and gut inflammation in children with functional abdominal pain and irritable bowel syndrome. Journal of Pediatrics. 2008;153:646–650. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.04.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Shulman RJ, Eakin MN, Jarrett M, et al. Characteristics of pain and stooling in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 2007;44:203–208. doi: 10.1097/01.mpg.0000243437.39710.c0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children: STAIC. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  37. Sprenger L, Gerhards F, Goldbeck L. Effects of psychological treatment on recurrent abdominal pain in children—A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31:1192–1197. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Steinhausen H-C, Winkler Metzke C. Continuity of functional-somatic symptoms from later childhood to young adulthood in a community sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:508–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01721.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Subcommittee on Chronic Abdominal Pain. Chronic abdominal pain in children. Pediatrics. 2005;115(3):812–815. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Thakkar K, Gilger MA, Shulman RJ, et al. EGD in children with abdominal pain: A systematic review. American Journal of Gastroenterology. 2007;102(3):654–661. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2007.01051.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Ton H, Brandsnes O, Dale S, et al. Improved assay for fecal calprotectin. Clinica Chimica Acta. 2000;292:41–54. doi: 10.1016/s0009-8981(99)00206-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Tsao JCI, Allen LB, Evans S, et al. Anxiety sensitivity and catastrophizing: Associations with pain and somatization in non-clinical children. Journal of Health Psychology. 2009;14:1085–1094. doi: 10.1177/1359105309342306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Valet M, Sprenger T, Boecker H, et al. Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulofrontal cortex and the midbrain during pain—An fMRI analysis. Pain. 2004;109:399–408. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Van der Veek SMC, Derkx HHF, Haan ED, et al. Do parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric functional abdominal pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology. 2011;17:258–272. doi: 10.1177/1359105311410513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Vandvik PO, Wilhelmsen I, Ihleb C, et al. Comorbidity of irritable bowel syndrome in general practice: A striking feature with clinical implications. Alimentary Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2004;20:1195–1203. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2036.2004.02250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Varni JW, Lane MM, Burwinkle TM, et al. Health-related quality of life in pediatric patients with irritable bowel syndrome: A comparative analysis. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2006;27:451–458. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200612000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Von Baeyer CL, Walker LS. Children with recurrent abdominal pain: Issues in the selection and description of research participants. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 1999;20:307–313. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199910000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Walker LS, Garber J. Children’s Somatization Inventory: Preliminary Manual. Nashville: TN: Vanderbilt University Medical Center; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  49. Walker LS, Greene JW. Children with recurrent abdominal pain and their parents: More somatic complaints, anxiety, and depression that other patient families? Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1989;14:231–243. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/14.2.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Walker LS, Greene JW. The Functional Disability Inventory: Measuring a neglected dimension of child health status. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1991;16:39–58. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/16.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Walker LS, Beck JE, Garber J, et al. Children Somatization Inventory: Psychometric properties of the revised from (CSI-24) Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2009;34:430–440. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Walker LS, Garber J, Greene JW. Psychosocial correlates of recurrent childhood pain: A comparison of pediatric patients with recurrent abdominal pain, organic illness, and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:248–258. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.2.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Walker LS, Lipani T, Greene JW, et al. Recurrent abdominal pain: Symptom subtypes based on the Rome II criteria for pediatric functional gastrointestinal disorders. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition. 2004;38:187–191. doi: 10.1097/00005176-200402000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wendland M, Jackson Y, Stokes LD. Functional disability in paediatric patients with recurrent abdominal pain. Child: Care, Health, and Development. 2010;36:516–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01087.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Whitehead WE, Crowell MD, Davidoff AL, et al. Pain from rectal distension in women with irritable bowel syndrome. Digestive Diseases and Sciences. 1997;42:796–804. doi: 10.1023/a:1018820315549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Youssef NN, Murphy TG, Langseder AL, et al. Quality of life for children with functional abdominal pain: A comparison study of patients’ and parents’ perceptions. Pediatrics. 2006;117:54–59. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES