Abstract
This study evaluated pet affinity as a buffer between ambivalence over emotional expression (AEE) and social support. AEE occurs when one desires to express emotions but is reluctant to do so and is related to negative psychological outcomes. Individuals high in AEE may have difficulty receiving social support and thus may not gain accompanying benefits. Social support has been associated with positive health outcomes, and pet support is positively associated with human social support. The present study explores the potential protective effect of pet affinity. One hundred ninety-eight undergraduate dog owners completed measures assessing perceived social support, pet affinity, and AEE. AEE was expected to be negatively associated with social support, and pet affinity was expected to buffer the negative effects of AEE on social support. We found that AEE was negatively associated with perceived social support. An interaction between pet affinity and AEE emerged such that the negative association between AEE and social support was weaker among those higher in pet affinity. Thus, at high levels of AEE, those who felt a close connection with their pets reported more perceived social support than those less connected with their pets. Overall, these findings emphasize the potential benefits of pet affinity.
Keywords: emotion, pet ownership, disclosure
1. Introduction
Pets are an integral part of many people's lives, and much research has been done on the positive health benefits of engaging with pets, whether it is only for a few minutes or throughout a lifetime. The positive outcomes that interactions with pets have been shown to demonstrate have been mostly supportive in nature; the mere presence of a pet can decrease a person's mental stress (Allen, Shykoff, & Izzo, 2001), elevate their mood (Coakley & Mahoney, 2009), and increase confidence in a caregiver (Schneider & Harley, 2006). In at least one study, the supportive benefits of pet affinity were demonstrated to go beyond even that of close relationships with humans (Allen, Blascovich, & Mendes, 2002). In light of the supportive role of pets, the present research seeks to explore how pet affinity might benefit those who have trouble expressing emotions and gaining social support. Specifically, we will examine how pet affinity might moderate the negative relationship between ambivalence over emotional expression (AEE) and social support.
Social support is defined by Thoits (2010) as “emotional, informational, or practical assistance from significant others, such as family members, friends, or coworkers; support actually may be received from others or simply perceived to be available when needed” (p. S46). Past research has demonstrated that social support has been associated with positive benefits for both physical and mental health in relation to depression, anxiety, cancer, AIDS/HIV, and daily stressors (Cobb, 1976; Nurullah, 2012). Those who feel a lack of social support suffer from negative consequences in well-being and mental and physical health. Research has shown that a lack of social support predicts stress, depression (Pauley & Hesse, 2009), and an increased likelihood of developing coronary heart disease (Barth, Schneider, & Von Känel, 2010).
One population that seems to be particularly vulnerable to a lack of social support is individuals who are high in ambivalence over emotional expression (AEE). AEE results from a conflict in which one wants to express one's feelings, but is afraid of the consequences that may result (King & Emmons, 1990). Those who are high in AEE report a whole host of negative outcomes such as: psychological distress (Katz & Campbell, 1994; King, 1998; Tucker, Winkelman, Katz, & Bermas, 1999), depression, obsessive-compulsive tendencies, anxiety, paranoid ideation, psychoticism (King & Emmons, 1990; King & Emmons, 1991), poorer interpersonal functioning (e.g., less marital satisfaction; King, 1993), and greater fear of intimacy (Emmons & Colby, 1995). Similar to social support, the effects are not limited to mental well-being; those who are high in AEE also demonstrate negative physical side effects. High AEE patients report more physical symptomatology in general (King & Emmons, 1990; King & Emmons, 1991), and gastrointestional cancer patients high in AEE reported more pain, poorer quality of life and emotional well-being, lower social functioning, and engaged in more pain catastrophizing (Porter, Keefe, Lipkus, & Hurwitz, 2005).
The negative link between AEE and social support is well-documented in a variety of populations. Emmons and Colby (1995) found that college students who were low in AEE tended to also be low in social support. A large national study found that AEE was negatively linked to social support among postmenopausal women (Michael et al., 2006), and a European study found that high levels of AEE were associated with lower social functioning (including support) in Dutch rheumatoid arthritis patients (van Middendorp et al., 2005).
The conceptual basis for this negative relationship between AEE and social support has been the subject of speculation by many researchers. King and Emmons (1990) state that those who are high in AEE tend to overread and overthink others' emotions. This excessive rumination over others' emotions often leads to psychological distress. Furthermore, Lu, Uysal, and Teo (2011) hypothesized that those with high levels of AEE may feel helpless about this distress, and given their relative inability to express their emotions, they have little recourse to resolve the situation. It may also be that those with high levels of AEE are confused about their own emotions, and therefore experience conflict over whether to express them or not. A third possibility is that AEE prevents people from using social support as a coping mechanism, which leaves them with fewer strategies to manage stressful life events. Emmons and Colby (1995) found that those high in AEE tended to utilize avoidant coping styles, and also tended to report negative attitudes toward social support.
The inability to predict how other people will react to self-expression can lead to a hesitancy to disclose emotions to others, as well as a tendency to regret self-disclosure that was perceived to be too revealing. Past research has negatively linked self-disclosure with closeness for people with high social anxiety (Kashdan, Volkmann, Breen, & Han, 2007), which may function in a similar way to AEE in regards to close relationships. Along with the poorer interpersonal functioning (King, 1993) and fear of intimacy (Emmons & Colby, 1995) mentioned previously as negative outcomes for those who are high in AEE, it was also found that self-authenticity moderated the negative association between relationship satisfaction and emotion suppression. This research demonstrated that the incongruence between one's self and his or her emotional expression was the key aspect of the internal conflict (English & John, 2013). Thus, other means of deriving social support should be explored in domains in which a person can act completely authentically, without fear of social repercussions from emotional expression.
The particular domain that this study aims to explore is the supportive role of pets, and whether they can provide a source of non-judgmental social support. There is consistent evidence in current literature of positive benefits resulting from the presence of pets across a variety of populations. Several studies show physical benefits such as improved cardiovascular health and decreased physiological stress from interactions with animals and pets (dogs especially; Brown, 1999; Zasloff, 1996; Albert & Bulcroft, 1988; Giaquinto & Valentini, 2009). In addition, the presence of dogs during psychotherapy sessions has been shown to increase patients' positive views of the therapist and their willingness to disclose information (Schneider & Harley, 2006). Another study demonstrated that hypertensive stockbrokers who adopted a pet cat or dog experienced reduced physiological reactions to mental stress, compared to their control counterparts who did not adopt a pet (Allen, Shykoff, & Izzo, 2001). Furthermore, hospitalized patients experienced an increase in vitality, better mood, and a decrease in pain and respiratory rates when they were visited by dogs (Coakley & Mahoney, 2009).
Researchers have also shown that in some cases, pets can fulfill a supportive role similar to the role typically filled by other people. In one study, pet owners were found to have lower blood pressure, lower heart rate, lower cardiovascular reactivity, and faster recovery when their pets were present during a stressful math task or a cold presser task. Of particular interest, when participants performed the math task in front of their spouse, their blood pressure and heart rate increased; however, when their pet was brought in, their reactivity significantly decreased (Allen, Blascovich, & Mendes, 2002). This demonstrates that, in some cases, pets can provide non-judgmental social support, even greater than close others. Similarly, a university study revealed that college freshmen felt they would benefit from pet therapy specifically because of the associated social support. The students reported viewing their pets as family members that would provide support and comfort in stressful times (Adamle, Riley, & Carlson, 2009).
Given all of this evidence of the supportive, non-judgmental role of pets in emotional well-being and social support, the present study was designed to evaluate the relationship between AEE and social support by considering pet affinity (operationalized in this study as the degree to which people value interactions with pets, derived from the Pet Attitude Scale; Templer, Salter, Dickey, Baldwin, & Veleber, 1981) as a potential moderator. The first and second hypotheses predicted (respectively) that AEE would be negatively associated with social support, and that pet affinity would be positively associated with social support. The third hypothesis predicted that pet affinity would moderate the association between AEE and social support such that the negative relationship between AEE and social support would be weaker among those high in pet affinity.
2.2.2 The Medical Outcomes Study (MOS) Social Support Scale (Sherbourne & Stewart, 1991)
The 19-item MOS was used to evaluate participants' perceived access to emotional and informational support (eight items; e.g., “You have someone to give you good advice about a crisis”), tangible support (four items; e.g., “You have someone to take you to the doctor if you needed it”), and affectionate support (three items; e.g., “You have someone to love you and make you feel wanted”). An additional three items evaluated positive social interactions (e.g., “You have someone to do something enjoyable with”). The total score was the sum of all 19 items (α = .96).
2.2.3 Ambivalence over emotional expression (AEQ; King & Emmons, 1990)
The AEQ includes 28 items (α = .94) measuring expression of positive emotions, negative emotions, and expression of intimacy. Participants respond to items on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = frequently indicating how often they feel what each statement suggests. Sample items include “I'd like to talk about my problems with others, but at times I just can't” and “I often cannot bring myself to express what I am really feeling.”
2.2.4 Pet affinity
Pet affinity was measured using the Pet Attitude Scale a validated and recently revalidated scale (Templer et al., 1981: Morovati, Steinberg, & Taylor, 2008). The scale is comprised of 18 items on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree). Sample items include “House pets add happiness to my life”, and reverse coded “Having pets is a waste of money” (α = .93).
2.3 Procedure
Participants were recruited via email, flyers, and classroom advertisements to participate in a study of perceptions of pet owners. Students who met eligibility criteria (e.g., a dog owner, at least 18 years of age, and a registered student) could sign up for the study. Once students signed up for the study, they each submitted pictures of their dogs to the researchers. Participants then scheduled a time to come in to the laboratory for their assessment, during which they completed study materials. Participants received course extra credit in exchange for participation in this study.
3. Results
3.1 Descriptives
Table 1 presents means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for each of the major variables in the study. Social support was significantly and negatively related to AEE. Conversely, social support was significantly and positively associated with pet affinity. Pet affinity was not associated with AEE.
Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations among Variables.
1. | 2. | 3. | |
---|---|---|---|
1. Social Support | -- | ||
2. Ambivalence over Emotional Expression | -.41*** | -- | |
3. Pet Affinity | .25** | .07 | -- |
Mean | 63.58 | 2.65 | 6.11 |
Standard Deviation | 13.85 | .82 | .58 |
Note. N = 198
p < .001.
p < .01.
3.2 Primary analyses
Results of the Step 1 regression analysis supported our first hypothesis that AEE would negatively predict social support, β = -.453, p = .001. In addition, our second hypothesis that pet affinity would positively predict social support, was also supported, β = .294, p < .001.
A hierarchical regression analysis was performed to examine the role of AEE and pet affinity in predicting social support. Each predictor was mean centered. In Step 1, we evaluated social support as a function of AEE and pet affinity. At Step 2, we added the two-way product term between AEE and pet affinity in predicting social support. The Step 2 regression analysis addressed our third hypothesis that AEE and pet affinity would interact to predict social support such that AEE would be negatively related to social support; however, this effect would be less pronounced for those high in pet affinity. Consistent with Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken (2003), the interaction was graphed using parameter estimate values from a regression equation such that low and high values were calculated by using one standard deviation below and above the means for each of the predictors. Results revealed a significant two-way interaction between AEE and pet affinity such that AEE was negatively associated with perceived social support, β = .128, p = < .05.; however, this relationship was attenuated among those higher in pet affinity (see Figure 1).
4. Discussion
The current study evaluated the relationship between AEE, social support, and pet affinity in a sample of dog owners. This study builds upon previous research by emphasizing the benefits of pet affinity, particularly for those high in AEE. Consistent with prior research, AEE was negatively associated with perceived social support, while pet affinity was positively associated with social support. Further, pet affinity moderated the association between AEE and social support such that the negative relationship between AEE and social support was weaker among those high in pet affinity. The present findings suggest that pet affinity may serve as a buffer for dog owners against the negative effects of AEE on social support.
The negative relationship between AEE and social support has been demonstrated repeatedly in the literature (Emmons & Colby, 1995; Michael et al., 2006; van Middendorp et al., 2005). This negative relationship likely exists because those high in AEE are confused about their own emotions and feel distress over whether or not to express these emotions (Lu et al., 2011). Individuals high in AEE are also prone to confusion about the emotions of others, which can lead to misunderstanding others' emotions (King & Emmons, 1990). This lack of understanding may deter those high in AEE from cultivating or utilizing social support networks, which may lead to psychological distress.
Past research has found that people high in AEE have difficulty expressing themselves to other people because of their fear of negative consequences (King & Emmons, 1990). Therefore, a potential explanation for our findings is that those high in AEE may be able to fulfill their social support needs through communication with pets. Pets may provide an easily accessible and non-judgmental audience for emotional disclosure. This works in conjunction with pets' unconditional positive regard toward their owners to create an environment in which owners feel safe to communicate and openly express their feelings. Interestingly, research has shown that the mere presence of a dog can lead to this “safe communication” effect. For example, the presence of dogs during therapy sessions has been demonstrated to increase willingness to disclose information among patients who were previously low in willingness to disclose (Schneider & Harley, 2006). One possible explanation for this effect is that dogs provide a non-judgmental, safe environment in which patients can freely express themselves.
In a similar vein, pet affinity may attenuate the need for social support by both lowering stress levels as well as buffering against stressful situations. Studies have found that petting a dog raises parasympathetic activity, a physiological reaction that is associated with general stress reduction (Motooka, Koike, Yokoyama, & Kennedy, 2006). Also, when participants were faced with stressful situations, they exhibited less symptoms of stress and recovered faster in the presence of pets (Allen, Blascovich, & Mendes, 2002). In addition, a recent study found that individuals high in pet affinity reported feeling comforted and supported by their pets in times of stress (Adamle, Riley, & Carlson, 2009). Therefore pet affinity and interactions with pets may enact a de-stressing effect that is comparable to social support. This may be particularly important for individuals high in AEE who perceive a lack of social support, as they may instead turn to their pets to fulfill this need.
Another potential explanation for these findings is that those who are high in pet affinity may experience more positive emotions, and thus may rely on human social support less than those low in pet affinity. Past research has found that pet owners reported laughing more than those who did not own a pet, and that this laughter was most frequently the result of an incident related to the pet (Valeri, 2006). Perhaps pets can provide both support and increase positive emotions in a way that mimics the positive benefits of social support and/or alleviates the need for traditional social support.
4.1 Conclusions, limitations and future directions
The strengths of the study should be considered in light of some limitations. One such limitation is the use of undergraduate students in our sample, which offers limited generalizability. However, the sample used is ethnically diverse and maps onto US population characteristics. Furthermore, our sample consisted of dog owners specifically. Our study chose to examine dog owners because the literature has found mixed results based on the type of pet owned. For example, Valeri (2006) demonstrated that dog owners tended to laugh more than cat owners. Future research might evaluate whether this effect is also found with owners of other types of pets. Another limitation is that our study was a cross-sectional design, which does not allow for observation of trajectories. Future research might consider examining how these variables relate to one another over time.
This study contributes to the social support literature by seeking to understand and identify potential buffers for the detrimental effects of AEE on social support. We evaluated the potential protective effect of pet affinity on social support. Findings were consistent with expectations in that AEE was negatively associated with social support, whereas pet affinity was positively associated with social support. Furthermore, we found that the negative association between AEE and social support was weaker among those high in pet affinity compared to those low in pet affinity. Thus, individuals who felt a close connection with their pets reported more perceived social support than those who were less connected with their pets, despite high levels of AEE. These results suggest that feeling emotionally connected with pets can serve as a coping mechanism for those high in AEE. Future research might examine the role that pets may play in buffering the association between high AEE and depressive symptomatology. The current study contributes to the growing literature by identifying individual differences in social support and suggests that encouraging interaction with pets might be considered as a target for treatment and prevention efforts to reduce the negative impact of AEE.
Table 2. Hierarchical regression analysis for variables predicting social support from ambivalence over emotional expression (AEE), and pet affinity (PA).
Predictor | B | SE B | β | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Social Support | Step 1 | AEE | -7.677 | 1.076 | -.453*** |
PA | 6.980 | 1.473 | .294*** | ||
Step 2 | AEE * PA | 3.825 | 1.896 | .128* |
Note. N = 198
p < .001
p < .01
p < .05.
Acknowledgments
Thank you to Lisa Caraway and Richard Rodriguez for your help in running this study. A special thank you to our furry friends for your support: Calvin Joseph, Rizzi Marie, Ranger, Lady, Captain Maxwell, and Alfee.
This study was completed at the Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204-5022.
References
- Adamle KN, Carlson T, Riley TA. Evaluating college student interest in pet therapy. 2009 Mar;57:545+. doi: 10.3200/JACH.57.5.545-548. 2013/9. Retrieved from http://go.galegroup.com.ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA196440054&v=2.1&u=txshracd2588&it=r&p=HRCA&sw=w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albert A, Bulcroft K. Pets, families, and the life course. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1988;50(2):543–552. [Google Scholar]
- Allen K, Blascovich J, Mendes WB. Cardiovascular reactivity and the presence of pets, friends, and spouses: The truth about cats and dogs. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2002;64(5):727–739. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000024236.11538.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen K, Shykoff BE, Izzo JL., Jr Pet ownership, but not ACE inhibitor therapy, blunts home blood pressure responses to mental stress. Hypertension. 2001;38(4):815–820. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth J, Schneider S, Von Känel R. Lack of social support in the etiology and the prognosis of coronary heart disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2010;72(3):229–238. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d01611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown JM. Childhood attachment to a companion animal and social development of incarcerated male juvenile delinquents (Ph D, California School of Professional Psychology - Fresno) ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. 1999 Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/docview/304560201?accountid=7107. (304560201)
- Coakley AB, Mahoney EK. Creating a therapeutic and healing environment with a pet therapy program. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice. 2009;15(3):141–146. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2009.05.004. doi: http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/10.1016/j.ctcp.2009.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobb S. Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1976;38(5):300–314. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197609000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3rd. Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Emmons RA, Colby PM. Emotional conflict and well-being: Relation to perceived availability, daily utilization, and observer reports of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;68(5):947–959. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.5.947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- English T, John OP. Understanding the social effects of emotion regulation: The mediating role of authenticity for individual differences in suppression. Emotion. 2013;13(2):314–329. doi: 10.1037/a0029847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giaquinto S, Valentini F. Is there a scientific basis for pet therapy? Disability & Rehabilitation. 2009;31(7):595–598. doi: 10.1080/09638280802190735. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=s3h&AN=37140999&site=ehost-live. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashdan TB, Volkmann JR, Breen WE, Han S. Social anxiety and romantic relationships: The costs and benefits of negative emotion expression are context-dependent. Journal Of Anxiety Disorders. 2007;21(4):475–492. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz IM, Campbell JD. Ambivalence over emotional expression and well-being: Nomothetic and idiographic tests of the stress-buffering hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67(3):513–524. [Google Scholar]
- King LA. Emotional expression, ambivalence over expression, and marital satisfaction. Journal Of Social And Personal Relationships. 1993;10(4):601–607. doi: 10.1177/0265407593104008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- King LA. Ambivalence over emotional expression and reading emotions in situations and faces. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74(3):753–762. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA, Emmons RA. Conflict over emotional expression: Psychological and physical correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1990;58(5):864–877. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA, Emmons RA. Psychological, physical, and interpersonal correlates of emotional expressiveness, conflict, and control. European Journal of Personality. 1991;5(2):131–150. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=12077251&site=ehost-live. [Google Scholar]
- Lu Q, Uysal A, Teo I. Need satisfaction and catastrophizing: Explaining the relationship among emotional ambivalence, pain, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Health Psychology. 2011;16(5):819–827. doi: 10.1177/1359105310392092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michael YL, Wisdom JP, Perrin N, Bowen D, Cochrane BB, Brzyski R, Ritenbaugh C. Expression and ambivalence over expression of negative emotion: Cross-sectional associations with psychosocial factors and health-related quality of life in postmenopausal women. Journal of Women and Aging. 2006;18(2):25–40. doi: 10.1300/J074v18n02_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morovati DR, Steinberg AL, Taylor LC, Lee HB. Further validation evidence for the Pet Attitude Scale. North American Journal Of Psychology. 2008;10(3):543–552. [Google Scholar]
- Motooka M, Koike H, Yokoyama T, Kennedy NL. Effect of dog-walking on autonomic nervous activity in senior citizens. Medical Journal of Australia. 2006;184(2):60–63. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2006.tb00116.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nurullah AS. Received and provided social support: A review of current evidence and future directions. American Journal of Health Studies. 2012;27(3):173–188. [Google Scholar]
- Pauley PM, Hesse C. The effects of social support, depression, and stress on drinking behaviors in a college student sample. Communication Studies. 2009;60(5):493–508. doi: 10.1080/10510970903260335. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Porter LS, Keefe FJ, Lipkus I, Hurwitz H. Ambivalence over emotional expression in patients with gastrointestinal cancer and their caregivers: Associations with patient pain and quality of life. Pain. 2005;117(3):340–348. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.06.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider MS, Harley LP. How dogs influence the evaluation of psychotherapists. Anthrozoos. 2006;19(2):128–142. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/login?url=http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=a9h&AN=22213602&site=ehost-live. [Google Scholar]
- Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL. The MOS social support survey. Social Science & Medicine. 1991;32(6):705–714. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90150-B. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Templer DI, Salter CA, Dickey S, Baldwin R, Veleber DM. The construction of a pet attitude scale. The Psychological Record. 1981;31:343–348. [Google Scholar]
- Thoits PA. Stress and health: Major findings and policy implications. Journal of Health & Social Behavior. 2010;51(1):S41–S53. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker JS, Winkelman DK, Katz JN, Bermas BL. Ambivalence over emotional expression and psychological well-being among rheumatoid arthritis patients and their spouses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1999;29(2):271–290. [Google Scholar]
- Valeri RM. Tails of laughter: A pilot study examining the relationship between companion animal guardianship (pet ownership) and laughter. Society & Animals. 2006;14(3):275–293. doi: 10.1163/156853006778149190. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Van Middendorp H, Geenen R, Sorbi MJ, Hox JJ, Vingerhoets AJJM, Van Doornen LJP, Bijlsma JWJ. Styles of emotion regulation and their associations with perceived health in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2005;30(1):44–53. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3001_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zasloff RL. Measuring attachment to companion animals: A dog is not a cat is not a bird. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 1996;47(1–2):43–48. doi: http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.lib.uh.edu/10.1016/0168-1591(95)01009-2. [Google Scholar]