Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: JAMA Neurol. 2015 May 1;72(5):500–502. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.33

A Call for New Thoughts About What Might Influence Human Brain Aging

Aging, Apolipoprotein E, and Amyloid

Charles DeCarli 1
PMCID: PMC4428918  NIHMSID: NIHMS683501  PMID: 25775040

The wiser mind mourns less for what age takes away than what it leaves behind.”

William Wordsworth, The Fountain

As a greater proportion of the world’s population live beyond age 65 years, there is increasing awareness of age-related differences in cognitive ability and a rising interest in finding ways to maintain healthy brain aging with the hope to avoid dementia. Although conventional wisdom stresses the inevitable decline of cognitive ability with age, seminal work by Wilson et al1 finds that individual trajectories of cognitive ability vary greatly, suggesting that at least some of the age-related differences in cognitive ability are due to incipient disease. Conventional wisdom also stresses that incipient or clinically expressed Alzheimer disease (AD) explains most cognitive decline and incident dementia among older individuals. The negative impact of incipient AD on cognitive ability among apparently cognitively normal older individuals is further supported by the long prodromal period of the AD process. The advent of in vivo amyloid imaging (and even eventually tau imaging) has stimulated intense interest regarding the role of incipient AD in relationship to cognitive aging. Early reports suggest a strong relationship between memory performance and cortical amyloid retention in a group of individuals with various degrees of cognitive ability. Later studies also found that increased cerebral amyloid burden, which is highly associated with age and the apolipoprotein E ε4 (APOE ε4) genotype2 among cognitively normal individuals, is associated with subtle declines in cognitive performance3 and increased risk for future dementia.4 This work and the increasing availability of biological markers of AD pathology have led to a proposed biological cascade model of AD5 and reevaluation of diagnostic criteria for AD.6 If one ascribes religiously to the concept that a large proportion of cognitive differences with age are driven by incipient disease, then one might expect that memory performance–a cognitive ability that changes most dramatically with age and is common to AD–would follow increasing levels of associated cerebral amyloid and be strongly associated with hippocampal atrophy. In their article, Jack et al7 present new information that challenges the notion that amyloid accumulation explains memory performance across the entire age range. Importantly, this work does not only address the likely highly significant impact of cerebral amyloid accumulation on dementia risk, but also extends current knowledge relating to the impact of the aging process across the spectrum of ages 30 to 95 years to brain structure, amyloid accumulation, and memory performance among cognitively normal individuals.

Their study7 details cross-sectional associations between age; memory performance using the auditory-verbal learning test, a common memory task; hippocampal volume; and the extent of cerebral amyloidosis for a group of more than 1200 men and women, ranging from age 30 to 95 years. A number of important findings come from this seminal study. One particularly striking result was the rather dramatic changes in both hippocampal volume and memory performance seen before age 65 years and before the age when the prevalence of extensive cortical amyloid burden is increasing. Moreover, the apparent slope of memory decline appears to increase after age 65 years, in conjunction with or slightly before an inflection and increasing prevalence of extensive cerebral amyloid burden. While the authors remind us that we cannot infer causality from cross-sectional studies, this remarkable study prompts the question as to what may cause hippocampal and memory changes in early life.

If one tenaciously holds to the notion that the insidious consequences of other diseases may be contributing to these earlier differences, vascular brain injury is an obvious candidate. Vascular risk factors, such as diabetes mellitus, are associated with subtle cognitive impairment among individuals aged 47 to 57 years8 and hypertension is associated with significantly greater cerebral atrophy among individuals 40 years on average.9 Moreover, numerous studies of middle-life vascular risk factors have found that they increase the risk for later-life dementia and are associated with accelerated brain injury.10 In addition, measures of vascular brain injury significantly increase the risk for later-life cognitive impairment, dementia, stroke, and even mortality.11 These data support the notion that vascular brain injury may begin early in life in a subset of affected individuals that affects both cognition and brain structure. While the high prevalence of hypertension, which is approximately 11% for individuals between 18 and 40 years but increases to 40% of US adults 45 to 64 years,12 is highly suggestive as a cause for the observed differences in cognition and hippocampal volume. However, reported studies have found that vascular risk factors are more likely to lead to generalized cerebral atrophy9 and nonmemory cognitive impairments,8 which contrast with the findings of Jack et al7 in this issue of JAMA Neurology. Therefore, while vascular risk factors likely contribute to some of the cognitive impairments known to occur with normal aging, they do not fully overlap with the findings reported by Jack et al.7

To what other processes then can we ascribe the differences in hippocampal volume and memory performance found before age 60 years? Genetic influences certainly play a role. Sex has an overwhelming genetic influence and findings shown in this study are consistent with other published findings of sex differences in brain structure and cognition. Consistent with these other studies, Jack et al7 show significant sex differences in memory and hippocampal volume across the age span, with differences in memory performance being particularly substantial earlier in life with men doing more poorly on tests of memory performance and having smaller hippocampal volumes in proportion to head size than women. However, the risk for late-life dementia does not favor a higher prevalence of dementia among men, raising further interesting questions about the role of early aging sex differences in memory performance and brain atrophy and the risk for later-life dementia. Apolipoprotein E is another major genetic factor and the results from the study by Jack et al7 and others confirm the profound effect of APOE ε4 genotype on amyloid retention beyond age 70 years. Yet, unlike the effect of the APOE ε4 genotype on gray matter metabolism throughout the life span,13 the impact of APOE ε4 on hippocampus and memory performance appear more directly related to co-occurring AD degeneration14 with the same later age at onset. In addition to the evidence of major gene effects, twin and family studies have found high heritability coefficients for memory performance and brain and hippocampal volume among older as well as younger individuals. Genome-wide association studies in large community-based studies of patients ranging from age 30 to 90 years have found significant associations between hippocampal volume and non-AD genes, suggesting independent genetic effects15 that may indicate other biological pathways that could explain age-related differences. Similar genetic association studies with memory performance among cognitively normal older individuals also have been significant.16 However, the impact of these genetic polymorphisms is relatively small (eg, 0.2 SD for the hippocampus17) and, again, cannot fully explain the findings seen by Jack et al.7

As is often the case for new scientific findings, we are left with more questions than clear answers. Cognitive neuroscientists have established a host of theories that describe differences in cognitive performance in young and middle-aged individuals,18 which provide a strong foundation for mechanistic research related to the early aging of the brain. Further genetic and animal model studies, particularly of nonhuman primates,19 are likely to elucidate specific cell types and cortical networks that change most dramatically during the first 40 to 50 years of life. Understanding the basic biology of these early processes are likely to substantially inform us about ways in which we can maintain cognitive health and optimize resistance to late-life dementia. However, such work requires the necessary motivation found by seminal work, such as that of Jack et al,7 which tell us where and when to investigate these processes. Establishing what is normal20 creates avenues for new research, increasing the likelihood of discovering novel therapeutics for late-life disease states, which is a laudable goal indeed!

Acknowledgments

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant P30 AG010129 from the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The National Institutes of Health had no role in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript, and the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References

  • 1.Wilson RS, Gilley DW, Bennett DA, Beckett LA, Evans DA. Person-specific paths of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease and their relation to age. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(1):18–28. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Morris JC, Roe CM, Xiong C, et al. APOE predicts amyloid-beta but not tau Alzheimer pathology in cognitively normal aging. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(1):122–131. doi: 10.1002/ana.21843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Pietrzak RH, Lim YY, Ames D, et al. Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle (AIBL) Research Group. Trajectories of memory decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: results from the Australian Imaging, Biomarkers and Lifestyle Flagship Study of Ageing [published online December 20, 2014] Neurobiol Aging. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Morris JC, Roe CM, Grant EA, et al. Pittsburgh compound B imaging and prediction of progression from cognitive normality to symptomatic Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2009;66(12):1469–1475. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jack CR, Jr, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12(2):207–216. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jack CR, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, et al. Age, sex, and APOE ε4 effects on memory, brain structure, and β-amyloid across the adult life span [published online March 16, 2015] JAMA Neurol. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.4821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, et al. Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study Investigators. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology. 2001;56(1):42–48. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Maillard P, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Effects of systolic blood pressure on white-matter integrity in young adults in the Framingham Heart Study: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(12):1039–1047. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70241-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, et al. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77(5):461–468. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Debette S, Beiser A, Decarli C, et al. Association of MRI markers of vascular brain injury with incident stroke, mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and mortality: the Framingham Offspring Study. Stroke. 2010;41(4):600–606. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.570044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Jones DW, Hall JE. Seventh report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure and evidence from new hypertension trials. Hypertension. 2004;43(1):1–3. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000110061.06674.ca. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Caselli RJ, Hentz JG, Osborne D, et al. Apolipoprotein E and intellectual achievement. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50(1):49–54. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50007.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mormino EC, Kluth JT, Madison CM, et al. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Episodic memory loss is related to hippocampal-mediated beta-amyloid deposition in elderly subjects. Brain. 2008;132(Pt 5):1310–1323. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stein JL, Medland SE, Vasquez AA, et al. Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative; EPIGEN Consortium; IMAGEN Consortium; Saguenay Youth Study Group; Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology Consortium; Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis Consortium. Identification of common variants associated with human hippocampal and intracranial volumes. Nat Genet. 2012;44(5):552–561. doi: 10.1038/ng.2250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Barral S, Cosentino S, Christensen K, et al. Long Life Family Study. Common genetic variants on 6q24 associated with exceptional episodic memory performance in the elderly. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(12):1514–1519. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Taal HR, St Pourcain B, Thiering E, et al. Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genetic Epidemiology Consortium; Early Genetics & Lifecourse Epidemiology Consortium; Early Growth Genetics Consortium. Common variants at 12q15 and 12q24 are associated with infant head circumference [published correction appears in Nat Genet. 2013;45(6):713] Nat Genet. 2012;44(5):532–538. doi: 10.1038/ng.2238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Park DC. Cognitive Aging: A Primer. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rapp PR, Amaral DG. Individual differences in the cognitive and neurobiological consequences of normal aging. Trends Neurosci. 1992;15(9):340–345. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90051-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.DeCarli C, Massaro J, Harvey D, et al. Measures of brain morphology and infarction in the Framingham Heart Study: establishing what is normal. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26(4):491–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES