Skip to main content
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience
. 2015 May 12;9:116. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00116

Mutation-related differences in exploratory, spatial, and depressive-like behavior in pcd and Lurcher cerebellar mutant mice

Jan Tuma 1,2, Yaroslav Kolinko 1,3, Frantisek Vozeh 1,2, Jan Cendelin 1,2,*
PMCID: PMC4429248  PMID: 26029065

Abstract

The cerebellum is not only essential for motor coordination but is also involved in cognitive and affective processes. These functions of the cerebellum and mechanisms of their disorders in cerebellar injury are not completely understood. There is a wide spectrum of cerebellar mutant mice which are used as models of hereditary cerebellar degenerations. Nevertheless, they differ in pathogenesis of manifestation of the particular mutation and also in the strain background. The aim of this work was to compare spatial navigation, learning, and memory in pcd and Lurcher mice, two of the most frequently used cerebellar mutants. The mice were tested in the open field for exploration behavior, in the Morris water maze with visible as well as reversal hidden platform tasks and in the forced swimming test for motivation assessment. Lurcher mice showed different space exploration activity in the open field and a lower tendency to depressive-like behavior in the forced swimming test compared with pcd mice. Severe deficit of spatial navigation was shown in both cerebellar mutants. However, the overall performance of Lurcher mice was better than that of pcd mutants. Lurcher mice showed the ability of visual guidance despite difficulties with the direct swim toward a goal. In the probe trial test, Lurcher mice preferred the visible platform rather than the more recent localization of the hidden goal.

Keywords: Lurcher, olivocerebellar degeneration, pcd, spatial learning, water maze

Introduction

Neurodegenerative disorders affecting the olivo-cerebellar system are manifested by well-characterized motor disorders. Nevertheless, the cerebellum is also involved in cognitive and behavioral processes, abnormalities of which have been described in humans (Schmahmann and Sherman, 1997; Cooper et al., 2010; Fancellu et al., 2013; Marien and Beaton, 2014), as well as in a wide spectrum of cerebellar mutant mice (for review see Manto and Marmolino, 2009; Cendelin, 2014). Cerebellar mutants are variable relative to the feature and extent of the cerebellar and extra-cerebellar neuronal degeneration. Moreover, the mutations appear in different mouse strains and these mutants retain specific phenotypic traits of the original strains. The importance of the genetic background for behavioral manifestation has been shown in gain-of-function (Cendelin et al., 2014) as well as loss-of-function mutations (Lalouette et al., 2001). Furthermore, the review by D'hooge and de Deyn (2001) showed that sex differences, age, nutrition, stress, infections as well as experimental protocol, apparatus, and data analysis could markedly influence results in the Morris water maze task (Morris, 1984). With respect to these facts, it is therefore difficult to compare the behavioral phenotype of various mutations in mice of different background strains from different studies. On the other hand, the identification and understanding of specific impairments related to a particular mutation should be of interest regarding the variability of human hereditary cerebellar degenerations (Manto, 2005) and the use of mouse models for the development of disease-targeted therapeutic approaches.

In the present study, the behavioral phenotype of two of the most frequently used mouse models of olivocerebellar degeneration, Lurcher and Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd), were studied. Lurcher mice (Phillips, 1960) constitute the semi-dominant gain-of-function mutation in the δ2 glutamate receptor (GluRδ2) gene that changes the receptor into a leaky membrane channel, which chronically depolarizes the cell membrane (Zuo et al., 1997). GluRδ2 is expressed predominantly by Purkinje cells (Araki et al., 1993) and therefore, cell-autonomous degeneration of Purkinje cells is a primary effect of the mutation (Wetts and Herrup, 1982a,b). Virtually all Purkinje cells disappear by 3 months of age (Caddy and Biscoe, 1979). Fast reductions of cerebellar interneurons and inferior olive neuron numbers are due to secondary target-related cell death (Caddy and Biscoe, 1979; Wetts and Herrup, 1982a,b; Zanjani et al., 2006). Lurchers are characterized by ataxia (Fortier et al., 1987), spatial orientation impairments (Lalonde et al., 1988; Cendelin et al., 2008), and alterations of anxiety-related behaviors (Hilber et al., 2004). Lurcher mutation exists in two phenotypically undistinguishable alleles, the original one, Grid2Lc (Zuo et al., 1997), and Grid2Lc−J (de Jager et al., 1997). For experiments, Grid2Lc mutants have been used, e.g., in B6CBA and C3H (Caddy and Biscoe, 1979; Cendelin et al., 2014) strain backgrounds.

Pcd mice (Mullen et al., 1976) carry a recessive loss-of-function mutation in the gene encoding the cytosolic ATP/GTP binding protein 1 (Agtpb1), a.k.a. Nna1 (Fernandez-Gonzalez et al., 2002). Nna1 is expressed throughout the brain and retina with prominence in cerebellar Purkinje cells (Mullen et al., 1976; Baltanas et al., 2011), mitral cells of the olfactory bulb (Greer and Shepherd, 1982), thalamic neurons (O'Gorman, 1985; O'Gorman and Sidman, 1985), and retinal photoreceptors (Blanks et al., 1982; Lavail et al., 1982). Histopathological analysis of pcd mice revealed rapid Purkinje cell loss between the third and fourth postnatal week (Baltanas et al., 2013), slowly progressive cerebellar granule cell degeneration, moderate reduction of the deep cerebellar nuclei, and slow degeneration of inferior olivary neurons that are supposed to be secondary to the loss of Purkinje cells (Ghetti et al., 1987; Triarhou et al., 1987). The photoreceptor decrease progresses slowly and even after 9 months of life, some photoreceptors are retained (Marchena et al., 2011). Pcd mice suffer from ataxia (Mullen et al., 1976; Goodlett et al., 1992) and a deterioration of spatial navigation learning (Goodlett et al., 1992). The pcd mutation exists in several different alleles (Wang and Morgan, 2007). Pcd mice carrying the original allele Agtpbp1pcd have been used for experiments, e.g., in B6.BR (Vinueza Veloz et al., 2014), C57BL/6J (Zhang et al., 1996), or B6C3Fe (Rotter et al., 2000) strains.

Both mutants constitute a distinct type of mutation affecting the olivo-cerebellar system either exclusively (Lurcher) or inclusively (pcd) and determining a strong pathological phenotype. Distinct histopathological similarities predestine them to frequent mutual comparisons, mostly often indirect (Furuya et al., 1994; Lalonde and Thifault, 1994; Le Marec and Lalonde, 1997, 2000), but none of these studies have involved systematic experiments. Therefore, the aim of this study was to test the behavioral phenotype of pcd and Lurcher mice with particular attention paid to cognitive and emotional disturbances under the same environmental conditions. We also aimed to assess the comparability of the mutants, which are not commercially available in identical strains. Thus, healthy littermates were also tested to assess the role of the genetic backgrounds.

Materials and methods

Animals

Two cohorts of adult (3 months) B6.BR pcd1J and B6CBA Lurcher mutants and their healthy wild type littermates of both sexes were used (for n, see Table 1). Both B6.BR pcd and wild type mice were obtained by crossing heterozygous males and females. Both B6CBA Lurcher and wild type mice were obtained by crossing wild type females with heterozygous Lurcher males. All animals were housed in the same breeding facility under standard laboratory conditions in a temperature and humidity controlled room with a 12/12 h light/dark cycle (6 a.m. to 6 p.m.). The tests were performed during the light phase of the cycle. Animals were kept in plastic cages with wooden shavings and maintained with a standard commercial pellet diet and water ad libitum. All experimental procedures were performed in compliance with the EU Guidelines for Scientific Experimentation on Animals and with the permission of the Ethical Commission of the Faculty of Medicine in Pilsen.

Table 1.

Mean ± SEM (n) body weight of 3-month-old pcd and Lurcher mice and their healthy littermate controls (separately for both cohorts).

B6.BR B6CBA
pcd wild type Lurcher wild type
COHORT 1
Females (g) 15.96±1.262 (16) 21.57±0.986 (22) 20.49±1.847 (17) 22.00±1.887 (21)
Males (g) 19.56±2.350 (16) 26.51±2.509 (17) 24.79±1.847 (19) 27.89±1.885 (18)
COHORT 2
Females (g) 15.53±2.911 (14) 22.32±1.052 (13) 20.01±1.507 (14) 22.92±1.168 (12)
Males (g) 19.03±3.199 (11) 27.81±1.519 (16) 24.56±1.272 (12) 28.47±1.574 (15)

Experimental design

To eliminate the influence of the tests on behavior, two cohorts of mice were used. Cohort A was used for analysis of the explorative behavior in the open field and spatial learning, orientation and navigation in the Morris water maze. Cohort B was used for assessment of motivation and depressive-like behavior in the water environment. The body weight of mice from both cohorts was measured on the first day of the experiment before the tests. For behavioral tests and body weight evaluation, male and female mice were considered separate experimental groups. Since pcd mice are known to suffer from retinal degeneration (Blanks et al., 1982; Lavail et al., 1982), the retinas of samples of pcd mutants were examined stereologically and compared with those from their B6.BR wild type littermates as well as with retinas of B6CBA Lurcher and wild type mice to assess the presence and extent of photoreceptor degeneration at the time of finishing the behavioral testing.

Behavioral testing

Open field

Explorative behavior and spontaneous motor activity were analyzed using the open field test. The apparatus consisted of a white open top plastic box (50 × 50 × 50 cm) with an illumination intensity of 20 lux at the box floor level. The subject was placed in the center of the open field and left undisturbed for 5 min. The apparatus was cleaned with 70% ethanol between subjects. The activity was recorded using EthoVision® XT 7.1 (Noldus Information Technology b.v., Netherlands). The locomotion activity (% of the test duration), distance moved (cm), thigmotaxis (% moved distance in the 3 cm border zone), and mean walking speed (cm/s) were evaluated.

Morris water maze task

The goal-directed navigation and spatial learning were evaluated using a Morris water maze task (Morris, 1984). The apparatus consisted of a circular white plastic pool (100 cm in diameter × 55 cm in height), with the water level set at a height of 35 cm above the base. The pool was filled with water (26 ± 2°C) and illuminated with 70 lux at the water surface. Escape from the water was provided by a transparent circular PlexiGlass platform (7.5 cm in diameter; 0.5 cm below the water level). Four starting points around the circumference of the pool were arbitrarily designated: North (N), South (S), West (W), and East (E). Each animal performed four trials per day-session with 16 min inter-trial intervals. The subject was introduced into the pool facing the wall in one of four starting positions. The maximal time for the platform location was 60 s. If the mouse did not locate the platform within the allotted time, it was manually placed on the platform. After each trial, the mouse was left on the platform for 30 s.

The water maze test consisted of 12 consecutive day-sessions arranged into three phases: visible platform test (day-sessions 1–5), reversal hidden platform test (day-sessions 6–11), and probe trial (day-session 12). For the visible platform test, the hidden escape platform position was highlighted by a cylindrical label (3 cm in diameter; 5 cm in height) with vertical black and white stripes mounted 12 cm above the submerged platform. The label served as a cue for visual goal-directed navigation. Platform position and starting point order is schematically depicted in Figure 1. For the probe trial, the escape platform was removed, and each mouse was allowed to swim freely for 60 s per trial.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Scheme of the Morris water maze protocol indicating time schedule, platform position and starting point sequence. The red circle indicates a visible platform position. An empty circle indicates a hidden platform position. Dashed lines represent imaginary quadrant borders.

The movement of the mice in the maze was recorded using EthoVision® XT 7.1. Escape latencies (s) and distance moved (cm) were measured as the basic parameters of the performance in the Morris water maze task. Swimming speed during periods of activity (i.e., excluding floating periods) was calculated to assess the swimming ability of the mice and to evaluate the relationship between latency and distance moved. Mouse navigation and orientation relative to the escape platform position was determined as the heading angle error and direct swim percentage. The heading angle error was measured as a deviation from a direct line from the starting point to the center of the platform. As direct swim, those trials with a shorter distance moved than the length of a direct line connecting the starting point and the platform multiplied by 1.3 were considered (Cendelin et al., 2014). The exploration strategy was evaluated using thigmotaxis (% moved distance in the 10 cm margin zone of the maze). Floating (% of time spent inactive) was assessed as a specific behavioral event. Spatial learning and memory were assessed using the preference for the NW or SW quadrant, respectively (% of distance moved), for the first 30 s of the first start of the probe trial only to avoid the effect of adaptation on the missing platform.

Forced swimming test

The motivation to swim and depressive-like behavior were analyzed using the Porsolt's forced swimming test (Porsolt et al., 1979). Mice were immersed in a glass water tank (diameter: 18 cm; height: 26 cm; water depth: 19 cm). The water was maintained at 26 ± 2°C and illuminated with 70 lux at the water surface. The mouse was left to swim without any possibility of escape for 15 min per day-session for three consecutive days. Immobility periods were recorded using EthoVision® XT 7.1, and relative immobility (% of total time) was calculated. To assess the development of depressive-like behavior within a day-session, immobility periods were evaluated separately in three 5-min time-bouts for each day-session.

Quantitative histology of the retina

The presence and extent of retinal degeneration in pcd mutants (n = 8) compared with their healthy littermates (n = 8) and B6CBA mice (Lurcher: n = 8; WT: n = 8) was assessed using stereological analysis. Paraformaldehyde-fixed right eyes of four females and four males per group were processed into 10 μm thick serial sections with random orientation. Every fifteenth section was stained with Gill's hematoxylin and scanned as a stack of four 2.5 μm optical sections using an Olympus C-5060 digital camera coupled to an Olympus CX31 microscope (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) using an 60× objective with a numerical aperture of 1.35. To count the retinal photoreceptor cell nuclei, nine dissector-counting frames were randomly imposed on each stack (Glaser et al., 2007), taking into account only those optical dissectors located in the outer nuclear layer (ONL) of the retina (352 ± 15 dissectors for each animal). The volume of the retina and total number of photoreceptor nuclei were estimated using the fractionator method. Finally, the number of photoreceptor nuclei was related to the retina volume and numerical density was determined (Gundersen, 1986; Boyce et al., 2010). The mean coefficient of sampling error (CE) was 4.7% for the ONL volume and 4.4% for the retina volume (Gundersen and Jensen, 1987).

Statistical analysis

Data were analyzed using traditional statistical tests extended with a non-parametric permutational approach (Pesarin and Salmaso, 2010). Three-Way ANOVA or Three-Way ANOVA with repeated measurements were evaluated, and the following factors were analyzed: type—cerebellar mutant (CM)/wild type (WT), strain—B6CBA/B6.BR, sex—female/male, and within-group factors day-session and/or time-bout (session, bout; if applicable). Interactions of these factors were also assessed. All ANOVA-tests were followed by planned comparisons performed using t-tests with a Bonferroni correction for repeated measurements (day-session and/or time-bout). The data ordered in a paired design were analyzed using the paired t-test. The preference for the selected quadrants was verified using the one-sample t-test against a value of 25%, which represents a random occurrence. The data are presented as mean ± SEM. p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Reported F and t-values are considered as F0 and t0, respectively, before the start of permutational tests. ANOVAs and t-tests were performed with maximal 5000 and 10,000 permutations, respectively. Statistical analyses were conducted using the R version 3.1.2 for Mac OS.

Results

Body weight

The mean body weight of mice is presented in Table 1. Both pcd and Lurcher mutants showed significantly reduced body weights compared to their healthy counterparts. Moreover, pcd mice showed significantly lower body weights compared to Lurchers, even though the wild type mice for both groups were not different (for statistics see Supplementary Table 1).

Open field

Spatial distribution of the exploratory activity in the open field is presented in Figure 2A. Despite an evident preference for corners of the square arena in all experimental groups, B6CBA mice, and especially B6CBA Lurchers, showed a higher tendency to explore the entire arena. The significance of individual parameters measured in the open field on individual factors (type, strain, sex) and their interactions are shown in Table 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Open field test: (A) Superposition of trajectories (frequency of animal presence) for females and males in the arena; (B) Total distance moved (cm); (C) Percentage of time spent with locomotion activity (%); (D) Percentage of thigmotaxis (% moved distance in the 3 cm border zone) and (E) Mean walking speed (cm/s) during locomotion activity periods. Statistical significance was evaluated using a permutational t-test. Within-strain comparison: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001. Between-strain comparison: # p < 0.05, ## p < 0.01, and ### p < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Table 2.

Open field test: statistical significances of the between-group factors (type, strain, and sex) and their interactions.

Between-group factors Distance Locomotion Thigmotaxis Walking speed
F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) P
Type 3.24 0.045 18.12 <0.001 6.32 0.007 55.21 <0.001
Strain 48.45 <0.001 37.04 <0.001 15.63 <0.001 49.22 <0.001
Sex 4.14 n.s. 2.80 n.s. 1.48 n.s. 4.28 0.020
Type:Strain 21.06 <0.001 6.12 0.005 0.54 n.s. 31.48 <0.001
Type:Sex 0.66 n.s. 1.76 n.s. 0.70 n.s. 0.30 n.s.
Strain:Sex 2.08 n.s. 0.00 n.s. 4.41 0.032 4.77 0.023
Type:Strain:Sex 12.46 <0.001 8.38 0.004 0.33 n.s. 13.01 <0.001

Permutational Three-Way ANOVA.

Distance moved is shown in Figure 2B. While pcd females moved a shorter distance in the open field than B6.BR wild type females, Lurcher females walked longer distances than B6CBA wild type females. In males, no significant differences were found between mutant and wild type mice. Wild type females and pcd and wild type males of the B6.BR strain had longer distances moved than did their B6CBA counterparts. Pcd males moved longer distances than pcd females (t = −3.68, p < 0.001) and B6CBA wild type males moved longer distances than females (t = −2.40, p < 0.020).

Locomotion activity is shown in Figure 2C. The activity was higher in Lurcher females than in B6CBA wild type females and in pcd males than in B6.BR wild type males. Strain comparison showed higher activity in B6.BR wild type females, mutant and wild type males than in their B6CBA counterparts. Pcd males had higher locomotion activity than pcd females (t = −3.76, p < 0.001), and B6CBA wild type males were more active than females (t = −2.52, p < 0.013).

Thigmotaxis is displayed in Figure 2D. Thigmotaxis was significantly lower in Lurcher males than in B6CBA wild type males. B6.BR males showed higher thigmotaxis than B6CBA males. There was no effect of sex on thigmotaxis in the open field.

The parameter walking speed is shown in Figure 2E. Walking speed in the open field arena was lower in pcd females than in B6.BR wild type females, in pcd males than in B6.BR wild type males, and in Lurcher males than in B6CBA wild type males. Strain comparison showed that pcd females walked slower than Lurcher females, but pcd males were faster than Lurcher males. Both B6.BR wild type females and males achieved a higher walking speed than B6CBA wild type mice. Pcd males showed higher walking speed than pcd females (t = −3.59, p < 0.001), and B6CBA wild type males walked faster than females (t = −2.02, p < 0.048).

Morris water maze

Parameters measured in the Morris water maze are displayed in Figures 3–5. For the significance of the effect of individual factors (type, strain, sex, day-session) and their interactions on parameters measured in the Morris water maze, see Tables 3, 4. Typical examples of trajectory shapes observed during the experiment are shown in Figures 6A–F.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Morris water maze: (A) Mean escape latency (s); (B) Total distance moved (cm) and (C) Mean swimming speed during periods of active swimming (cm/s). Statistical significance was evaluated using permutational t-test with Bonferroni correction for the repeated measurement for within-strain comparisons (pcd vs. B6.BR wild type, Lurcher vs. B6CBA wild type) as well as for between-strain comparisons of cerebellar mutants (CM; pcd vs. Lurcher) and wild types (WT; B6.BR WT vs. B6CBA WT). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Table 3.

Morris water maze—escape latency, distance moved, and swimming speed: statistical significances of the between-group factors (type, strain, and sex) and within-group factors (session) as well as their interactions.

VISIBLE PLATFORM TASK
Between-group factors Latency Distance moved Swimming speed
F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p
Type 665.03 <0.001 424.95 <0.001 15.29 <0.001
Strain 100.00 <0.001 0.01 n.s. 148.94 <0.001
Sex 0.70 n.s. 1.18 n.s. 2.41 n.s.
Type:Strain 31.43 <0.001 15.00 <0.001 58.87 <0.001
Type:Sex 0.11 n.s. 0.03 n.s. 1.62 n.s.
Strain:Sex 4.91 n.s. 0.07 n.s. 3.88 0.043
Type:Strain:Sex 0.57 n.s. 0.17 n.s. 0.01 n.s.
Within-group factors F(4, 552) p F(4, 552) p F(4, 552) p
Session 221.38 <0.001 251.08 <0.001 13.52 <0.001
Type:Session 15.50 <0.001 17.14 <0.001 2.22 n.s.
Strain:Session 1.66 n.s. 0.47 n.s. 7.08 <0.001
Sex:Session 1.62 n.s. 0.95 n.s. 0.90 n.s.
Type:Strain:Session 14.23 <0.001 11.25 <0.001 0.95 n.s.
Type:Sex:Session 0.68 n.s. 2.79 <0.001 0.09 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Session 2.01 n.s. 0.80 n.s. 0.21 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Session 2.62 0.037 1.50 n.s. 0.85 n.s.
REVERSAL HIDDEN PLATFORM TASK
Between-group factors Latency Distance moved Swimming speed
F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p
Type 291.22 <0.001 192.35 <0.001 57.66 <0.001
Strain 6.71 0.005 89.06 <0.001 163.72 <0.001
Sex 0.66 n.s. 3.50 n.s. 2.72 n.s.
Type:Strain 11.03 0.008 176.79 <0.001 60.70 <0.001
Type:Sex 0.07 n.s. 0.48 n.s. 0.14 n.s.
Strain:Sex 4.45 0.025 0.01 n.s. 2.48 0.048
Type:Strain:Sex 0.05 n.s. 1.50 n.s. 0.43 n.s.
Within-group factors F(5, 690) p F(5, 690) p F(5, 690) p
Session 28.67 <0.001 43.91 <0.001 16.82 <0.001
Type:Session 15.68 <0.001 20.09 <0.001 5.42 <0.001
Strain:Session 1.13 n.s. 1.33 n.s. 0.47 n.s.
Sex:Session 1.09 n.s. 1.11 n.s. 0.62 n.s.
Type:Strain:Session 1.21 n.s. 1.75 0.025 0.45 n.s.
Type:Sex:Session 0.13 n.s. 0.82 n.s. 0.92 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Session 2.07 n.s. 0.90 n.s. 1.05 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Session 1.79 n.s. 1.26 n.s. 0.18 n.s.

Permutational Three-Way ANOVA with repeated measurements.

Table 4.

Morris water maze—heading deviation, direct swim percentage, thigmotaxis, and floating: statistical significances of the between-group factors (type, strain, and sex) and within-group factors (session) as well as their interactions.

VISIBLE PLATFORM TASK
Between-group factors Heading Direct swim Thigmotaxis Floating
F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p
Type 314.21 <0.001 553.17 <0.001 101.37 <0.001 0.29 n.s.
Strain 1.45 n.s. 80.97 <0.001 0.39 n.s. 16.35 <0.001
Sex 0.02 n.s. 2.87 n.s. 1.89 n.s. 12.13 <0.001
Type:Strain 1.17 n.s. 37.56 <0.001 1.80 n.s. 5.82 0.037
Type:Sex 2.08 n.s. 2.46 n.s. 1.78 n.s. 1.11 n.s.
Strain:Sex 0.52 n.s. 0.03 n.s. 0.73 n.s. 13.43 <0.001
Type:Strain:Sex 0.10 n.s. 2.21 n.s. 0.05 n.s. 0.42 n.s.
Within-group factors F(4, 552) p F(1, 138) p F(4, 552) p F(4, 552) p
Session 43.71 <0.001 75.07 <0.001 214.95 <0.001 7.79 <0.001
Type:Session 11.01 <0.001 52.12 <0.001 60.72 <0.001 3.11 0.003
Strain:Session 0.96 n.s. 7.20 <0.001 1.65 n.s. 3.61 <0.001
Sex:Session 0.23 n.s. 0.46 n.s. 3.58 n.s. 1.44 n.s.
Type:Strain:Session 2.57 0.034 3.13 <0.001 3.61 <0.001 6.90 <0.001
Type:Sex:Session 0.91 n.s. 0.19 n.s. 3.39 <0.001 0.45 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Session 0.14 n.s. 0.61 n.s. 1.20 n.s. 6.03 <0.001
Type:Strain:Sex:Session 1.44 n.s. 0.81 n.s. 0.73 n.s. 0.55 n.s.
REVERSAL HIDDEN PLATFORM TASK
Between-group factors Heading Direct swim Thigmotaxis Floating
F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p F(1, 138) p
Type 174.80 <0.001 113.05 <0.001 124.96 <0.001 0.07 n.s.
Strain 0.90 n.s. 16.52 <0.001 6.76 0.008 13.07 <0.001
Sex 0.36 n.s. 0.51 n.s. 0.62 n.s. 5.87 0.018
Type:Strain 8.64 <0.001 13.26 <0.001 10.45 <0.001 6.64 <0.001
Type:Sex 0.30 n.s. 0.16 n.s. 2.31 n.s. 0.00 n.s.
Strain:Sex 6.36 0.014 1.40 n.s. 1.39 n.s. 4.40 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex 0.02 n.s. 0.39 n.s. 0.00 n.s. 0.01 n.s.
Within-group factors F(5,690) p F(5,690) p F(5,690) p F(5,690) p
Session 16.00 <0.001 12.51 <0.001 3.28 n.s. 2.14 0.022
Type:Session 5.30 <0.001 9.32 <0.001 7.53 <0.001 0.51 n.s.
Strain:Session 2.77 <0.001 1.96 <0.001 1.77 n.s. 1.28 n.s.
Sex:Session 3.62 0.005 0.72 n.s. 2.07 <0.001 1.36 n.s.
Type:Strain:Session 0.82 n.s. 0.83 n.s. 0.94 n.s. 1.27 n.s.
Type:Sex:Session 1.65 n.s. 1.26 n.s. 2.65 0.024 1.23 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Session 2.78 0.013 1.26 n.s. 0.43 n.s. 1.17 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Session 0.15 n.s. 2.55 0.005 3.01 0.004 0.62 n.s.

Permutational Three-Way ANOVA with repeated measurements.

Escape latencies

Escape latencies in the Morris water maze (Figure 3A) were significantly longer in both types of cerebellar mutants than in their wild type littermates during the test with the visible as well as the hidden platform. The only day on which the difference was low (for the B6.BR mice, it was insignificant) was the first day with the hidden platform moved into the opposite quadrant (D6). Strain comparison showed smaller differences in both mutant as well as wild type mice. Compared with Lurchers, pcd mice (both males and females) had longer latencies in the visible platform task, while no differences between the mutants were found during the hidden platform task. In addition, B6.BR wild type mice achieved worse results than their B6CBA counterparts did (for females, only at the beginning of the visible platform task, but for males also at the end of the hidden platform task). The direct comparison of females and males showed differences in B6.BR wild type mice only on the day-session 3 (t = −5.28, p < 0.001) and day-session 5 (t = −2.72, p = 0.023).

Distance moved

Distance moved (Figure 3B) was significantly longer in Lurcher mice compared with wild type B6CBA mice on all days of the test. On the other hand, pcd mice showed a markedly longer trajectory than wild type B6.BR mice only in the visible platform task, except for on the first day, while the difference appeared only occasionally in the next phase. In wild type mice, mild strain differences appeared only in the visible platform task. Nevertheless, in the mutants, a significant difference appeared in the hidden platform task when the distance moved was markedly longer in Lurchers than in pcd mice. Sex differences were found only on the day-session 3 in B6.BR wild type mice (t = −2.72, p = 0.037).

Swimming speed

Swimming speed (Figure 3C) was significantly lower in pcd mice than in other mice.

On the other hand, Lurchers did not swim slower than their wild type littermates. Lurcher males were even significantly faster than wild type ones on day-session 1. Strain comparison showed a slower swimming speed in B6.BR wild type mice than in B6CBA ones, namely in the visible platform phase. There were no significant sex differences in swimming speed.

Heading deviation

Heading deviation error (Figure 4A) was significantly higher in mutant mice than in their wild type littermates. In the B6.BR strain, the differences were mainly seen in the visible platform task, while, in the B6CBA strain, they were significant for almost the entire course of the experiment. In females, no strain differences were seen. B6.BR wild type males were occasionally worse than B6CBA males. The only sex difference in heading deviation error was found on the day-session 10 in B6.BR wild type mice (t = −3.42, p = 0.011).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Morris water maze: (A) Heading deviation (°) from the direct swim to the platform and (B) Percentage of direct swim trials (%). Statistical significance was evaluated using permutational t-test with Bonferroni correction for the repeated measurement for within-strain comparisons (pcd vs. B6.BR wild type, Lurcher vs. B6CBA wild type) as well as for between-strain comparisons of cerebellar mutants (CM; pcd vs. Lurcher) and wild types (WT; B6.BR WT vs. B6CBA WT). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Direct swim

The percentage of direct swim trials (Figure 4B) was high in wild type mice of both strains in the visible platform task except for the first day session. Also, on some days of the hidden platform task, wild type mice showed a significantly higher percentage of direct swim trials than their mutant littermates. In mutant mice, direct swim trials were rare in both phases of the test. Strain differences showing better performance in B6CBA mice were only seen for a few day-sessions and mainly for wild type mice. Males and females did not differ in direct swim percentage.

Thigmotaxis

Thigmotaxis (Figure 5A) was significantly higher in both cerebellar mutants than in wild type animals on most days of the water maze test. Strain differences, on the other hand, were poor. The only difference between males and females was found on the day-session 5 in B6.BR wild type mice (t = −3.19, p = 0.010).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Morris water maze: (A) Percentage of thigmotaxis (% of distance moved in 10 cm border zone) and (B) Percentage of floating (% of time spent without swimming). Statistical significance was evaluated using permutational t-test with Bonferroni correction for the repeated measurement for within-strain comparisons (pcd vs. B6.BR wild type, Lurcher vs. B6CBA wild type) as well as for between-strain comparisons of cerebellar mutants (CM; pcd vs. Lurcher) and wild types (WT, B6.BR WT vs. B6CBA WT). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Floating analysis

Floating analysis (Figure 5B) revealed almost no differences in the percentage of time spent without activity between mutant and wild type mice. Nevertheless, in cerebellar mutants, a strain difference was observed, since floating behavior was very rare in Lurcher mutants. B6.BR wild type males spent significantly more time floating than females on the day-session 3 (t = −3.47, p = 0.003), day-session 4 (t = −2.86, p = 0.015), day-session 5 (t = −2.87, p = 0.013), and day-session 7 (t = −2.87, p = 0.035).

Morris water maze task acquisition

Significance of the within factor (day-session) indicated the importance of development of the parameters during the course of the Morris water maze test (Tables 3, 4). Learning process, which was manifested as a shortening of escape latencies (Figure 3A, Table 5) and distance moved (Figure 3B, Table 5), was detectable for the visible platform task in all groups of mice and for the hidden platform task for both B6CBA and B6.BR wild type mice and B6CBA Lurcher males. Lurcher females and both pcd males and females did not learn the hidden platform task (Table 5). Wild type mice of both strains also showed a significant decrease of heading deviation error (Figure 4A, Table 5) and an increase of the direct swim percentage (Figure 4B, Table 5) during both visible and hidden platform tasks. Lurcher mice improved heading deviation and direct swim percentage (males only) during the visible platform task, but not during the hidden platform task. Pcd mice did not improve their heading deviation or direct swim percentage in either of the tasks.

Table 5.

Morris water maze: statistical significances of change between or during individual phases.

VISIBLE PLATFORM TASK (DAY-SESSION 1 VS. DAY-SESSION 5)
Latency Distance moved Heading Direct swim
Females t p t p t p t p
pcd B6.BR 4.90 <0.001 7.43 <0.001 1.39 n.s. −1 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR 14.25 <0.001 13.28 <0.001 9.46 <0.001 −7.51 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA 8.82 <0.001 7.83 <0.001 3.06 0.007 −0.81 n.s.
Wild type B6CBA 11.44 <0.001 9.52 <0.001 4.93 <0.001 −9.55 <0.001
Males t p T p t p t p
pcd B6.BR 3.80 0.002 7.99 <0.001 2.06 n.s. 0 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR 9.54 <0.001 9.26 <0.001 5.50 <0.001 −6.67 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA 9.23 <0.001 8.74 <0.001 3.68 0.001 −2.96 0.016
Wild type B6CBA 7.27 <0.001 6.68 <0.001 5.11 <0.001 −14.75 <0.001
REVERSAL HIDDEN PLATFORM TASK (DAY-SESSION 6 VS. DAY-SESSION 11)
Latency Distance moved Heading Direct swim
Females t p T p t p t p
pcd B6.BR 1.14 n.s. 0.22 n.s. 1.17 n.s. −0.56 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR 6.00 <0.001 7.52 <0.001 4.09 <0.001 4.55 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA −0.58 n.s. −0.15 n.s. −1.47 n.s. 1.73 n.s.
Wild type B6CBA 6.23 <0.001 7.27 <0.001 4.35 <0.001 11.12 <0.001
Males t p t p t p t p
pcd B6.BR −0.43 n.s. 1.12 n.s. 1.96 n.s. 0 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR 3.63 0.004 4.38 <0.001 3.66 0.002 7.32 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA 3.16 0.004 3.65 0.002 1.41 n.s. 1.68 n.s.
Wild type B6CBA 5.62 <0.001 6.22 <0.001 4.50 <0.001 7.62 <0.001
PLATFORM TRANSITION (DAY-SESSION 5 VS. DAY-SESSION 6)
Latency Distance moved Heading Direct swim
Females t p t p t p t p
pcd B6.BR −3.23 0.006 −2.93 0.011 −1.78 n.s. 0 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR −13.71 <0.001 −13.16 <0.001 −15.43 <0.001 7.8 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA −8.76 <0.001 −9.28 <0.001 −2.51 0.025 1.14 n.s.
Wild type B6CBA −10.42 <0.001 −12.56 <0.001 −8.48 <0.001 11.78 <0.001
Males t p t p t p t p
pcd B6.BR −2.64 0.020 −2.19 0.042 −2.80 0.016 −1.46 n.s.
Wild type B6.BR −11.81 <0.001 −12.63 <0.001 −10.13 <0.001 7.91 <0.001
Lurcher B6CBA −8.55 <0.001 −8.35 <0.001 −4.55 <0.001 2.96 0.016
Wild type B6CBA −8.36 <0.001 −9.60 <0.001 −6.91 <0.001 13.83 <0.001

Permutational paired t-test.

Change of the platform position and its concealment (compare day-sessions 5 and 6) led to significant prolongation of both latencies and distance moved in all groups of mice (Figures 3A,B, Table 5), an increase in heading deviation error in all groups except pcd females (Figure 4A, Table 5) and a decrease in the direct swim percentage in wild type mice and Lurcher males (Figure 4B, Table 5). The effect of change of the platform position and its concealment is also shown in Supplementary Figure 1.

Probe trial

Probe trial on the last day of the Morris water maze test showed a mild preference for the NW quadrant in which the hidden platform was localized for the previous 6 day-sessions in B6CBA and B6.BR wild type mice. Surprisingly, both types of cerebellar mutants showed a significant preference for the SE quadrant, where the visible platform was localized during the first phase of the water maze test (Figure 6G). These findings were confirmed by the measurement of latency of the first occurrence in the former position of the visible and hidden platform (Supplementary Figure 2).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Examples of typical trajectories (A–F) of cerebellar mutants and wild type mice. (A) Thigmotaxis (e.g., B6.BR WT female, day-session 1, trial 1) was typical for Lurcher (Lc) and both B6.BR and B6CBA wild type (WT) mice on the first day-session. (B) Rotation (e.g., pcd male, day-session 1, trial 1) was an abnormal trajectory pattern that occurred in both cerebellar mutants in all day-sessions. (C) Focal search (e.g., B6.BR WT male, day-session 6, trial 1) in area of previous platform position appeared in WT and Lc mice immediately after change of the platform position (day-session 6). (D) Scanning (e.g., B6.BR WT male, day-session 6, trial 3) or (E) circling (e.g., B6.BR WT female, day-session 7, trial 1) strategy was used by WT mice before they learned the new platform position. (F) Direct swim (e.g., B6CBA WT male, day-session 11, trial 3) typical for WT mice in day-sessions 2–5 (visible platform) and appearing also in WT mice at the end of hidden platform task, but not in cerebellar mutants. Hatched circles indicate position of visible platform empty circles indicate position of hidden platform. (G) Morris water maze (probe trial): Percentage of distance moved through individual quadrants during the first 30 s of the first trial (E-starting position) of day-session 12. Statistical significance was evaluated using one-sample permutational t-test against 25% value, which represents a random occurrence: *p < 0.05 and **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Forced swimming test

Depressive-like behavior, which manifested as a state of immobility in the Porsolt's forced swimming test, is presented in Figure 7. The analysis showed a significant between-group effect of the type and strain, but not their interaction (Table 6). Nevertheless, a Three-Way ANOVA showed a significant effect of type:sex as well as type:strain:sex factor interactions (Table 6). A repeated measurement ANOVA also showed a significant within-group factor effect of the time-bout and day-session (Table 6).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Forced swimming test: percentage of immobility state (% of time spent without moving). Statistical significance was evaluated using a permutational t-test with a Bonferroni correction for the repeated measurement for within-strain comparisons (pcd vs.B6.BR wild type, Lurcher vs. B6CBA wild type) and for between-strain comparisons of cerebellar mutants (CM; pcd vs. Lurcher) and wild types (WT; B6.BR WT vs. B6CBA WT). Sex differences were significant in pcd mutants only from day-session 1, time-bout 2 until day-session 3, time-bout 1 (t = 3.13, p = 0.011; t = 4.05, p = 0.003; t = 3.89, p = 0.004; t = 2.90, p = 0.030; t = 3.67; p = 0.005; t = 2.93, p = 0.019, respectively). Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Table 6.

Forced swimming test: statistical significances of the between-group factors (type, strain, and sex) and within-group factors (bout, session) as well as their interactions.

Immobility
Between-group factors F(1, 99) p
Type 39.45 <0.001
Strain 76.79 <0.001
Sex 0.03 n.s.
Type:Strain 0.43 n.s.
Type:Sex 18.15 <0.001
Strain:Sex 2.10 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex 7.17 0.013
Within-group factors F(2, 198) p
Bout 23.02 0.011
Type:Bout 2.65 0.025
Strain:Bout 1.69 n.s.
Sex:Bout 1.37 n.s.
Type:Strain:Bout 17.60 <0.001
Type:Sex:Bout 2.87 0.023
Strain:Sex:Bout 1.13 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Bout 1.67 n.s.
Within-group factors F(1, 99) p
Session 29.48 <0.001
Type:Session 12.91 <0.001
Strain:Session 5.00 0.005
Sex:Session 0.72 n.s.
Type:Strain:Session 0.24 n.s.
Type:Sex:Session 0.27 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Session 0.10 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Session 0.03 n.s.
Within-group factors F(2, 198) p
Bout:Session 1.83 n.s.
Type:Bout:Session 1.08 n.s.
Strain:Bout:Sesion 1.22 n.s.
Type:Strain:Bout:Session 1.15 n.s.
Sex:Bout:Session 0.30 n.s.
Type:Sex:Bout:Sesion 3.56 n.s.
Strain:Sex:Bout:Sesion 0.41 n.s.
Type:Strain:Sex:Bout:Session 2.63 n.s.

Permutational Three-Way ANOVA with repeated measurements.

Total length of immobility in pcd females did not significantly differ from that in B6.BR wild type females (except the first 5 min time-bout in day-session 3). On the contrary, pcd males showed less immobility than B6.BR wild type males (Figure 7). In B6CBA mice, both Lurcher females and males had a shorter duration of immobility than did wild type mice in most time-bouts of day-session 2 and 3 (Figure 7). The occurrence of immobility periods was very low in Lurcher mice; thus, their immobility state duration was significantly shorter than in pcd mice in each day-session for females and the last 2 day-sessions for males. B6CBA wild type females showed less immobility than B6.BR wild type females in the first time-bout for day-session 1 and 2 as well as the first two time-bouts on day-session 3. The sex differences were found in pcd mice; males showed less immobility than females (Figure 7).

Furthermore, while the immobility was permanently rare in Lurchers, its duration increased from the day-session 1 to 3 in other mice (Table 7). For pair comparison of time-bout 1 vs. time-bout 3 for each day-session, see Supplementary Table 2.

Table 7.

Forced swimming test: paired comparison of day-session 1 and 3 for each time-bout.

Groups Time-bout (min) Females Males
t p t p
pcd B6.BR 00-05 −4.60 <0.001 −0.86 n.s.
05-10 −0.53 n.s. −1.44 n.s.
10-15 0.01 n.s. −3.22 0.006
Wild type B6.BR 00-05 −4.07 0.003 −7.48 <0.001
05-10 −3.30 0.012 −2.46 0.028
10-15 −5.17 0.001 −2.93 0.011
Lurcher B6CBA 00-05 −1.22 n.s. 1.04 n.s.
05-10 0.14 n.s. 0.66 n.s.
10-15 −0.51 n.s. 0.60 n.s.
Wild type B6CBA 00-05 −2.93 0.011 −4.24 <0.001
05-10 0.14 n.s. 0.66 n.s.
10-15 −1.36 n.s. −2.41 0.033

Permutational paired t-test.

Quantitative histology

Stereological analysis showed only an insignificant reduction in the density of retinal photoreceptors nor ONL volume relative to whole retina volume in pcd mutants compared with their healthy littermates, or with B6CBA Lurcher and wild type mice (Figure 8).

Figure 8.

Figure 8

(A) Histology of the retina in pcd and B6.BR wild type (WT) mice stained with Gill's hematoxylin. The bars indicate 50 μm. (B) Mean number of photoreceptors in outer nuclear layer (ONL) per mm3 of the retina and (C) mean volume of ONL expressed as percentage of whole retina volume in pcd, B6.BR WT, Lurcher (Lc) and B6CBA WT mice. Values of individual animals are indicated by Δ for females and ◯ for males.

Discussion

In this study, we have shown specific features of spatial performance and behavioral differences in response to the Morris water maze task in pcd and Lurcher mice, the most frequently used mouse models of olivocerebellar degeneration (for review, see Lalonde and Strazielle, 2007; Cendelin, 2014). Although it has been described that the neurodegenerative process disrupts spatial learning in both mutants, specific features of their spatial performance, which are presented here, have not been reported and sufficiently explained before, and even contradict some earlier opinions (Goodlett et al., 1992; Lalonde and Thifault, 1994). With regard to many factors that could influence the performance of the mice in behavioral tests, e.g., specific mutations, genetic background, sex, and the environment (for review, see Wolfer and Lipp, 2000; D'hooge and de Deyn, 2001), we performed a detailed comparative behavioral analysis of spatial navigation, learning and memory in pcd and Lurcher mutants. In order to assess specific behavioral abnormalities that could influence performance in the spatial navigation task, open field and forced swimming tests were done.

Behavior of Pcd and Lurcher mutants in the open field and forced swimming test

The type of the mutation, background strain and sex influenced behavior in both open field and forced swimming tests. The effect of sex of experimental animals was relatively stronger in these tests than in the Morris water maze and was, in this case, sufficient to completely invert the differences. In the open field, B6.BR mice showed typical preference to the corners of the arena, while, in B6CBA mice, the activity was more dispersed through the arena. Such higher dispersion of the activity was more marked in Lurchers than in their wild type littermates. Abnormal exploration has been reported also by Caston et al. (1998) who found significantly reduced exploratory behavior in Lurchers despite an increase in spontaneous activity. The most obvious phenomenon observed in the forced swimming test was the absence of floating even during later phases of each day-session of the forced swimming test and the absence of an increase of floating duration across the day-sessions in Lurcher mice. While tendency toward inactivity and depressive-like behavior were observed in pcd mice, Lurchers showed rather inadequate high activity.

Features of behavior seen in both open field and forced swimming tests comply with behavioral disinhibition affecting Lurcher mice (Frederic et al., 1997; Lalonde, 1998; Hilber et al., 2004; Porras-Garcia et al., 2005). The discrepancy between less fear-related behavior and elevated levels of corticosterone during stressful situations (Frederic et al., 1997; Hilber et al., 2004; Lorivel et al., 2014), a lack of prepulse inhibition and an inability to produce the immobility response suggest that Lurcher mice have a reduced capacity to inhibit selective components of natural behaviors due to an affection of the sensorimotor gating mechanism (Lalonde, 1998; Porras-Garcia et al., 2005). In pcd mice, only indirect evidence suggesting the possibility of some level of behavioral disinhibition and perseveration were reported in studies of spontaneous alternation, exploration and habituation (Lalonde et al., 1987, 1989). Since more dispersed activity in the open field and less frequent immobility in the forced swimming test were also in B6CBA wild type mice as compared with B6.BR wild type mice, these phenomena are not only due to the Lurcher phenotype, but might be at least a partially strain-related phenomenon.

Performance of Pcd and Lurcher mutants in the morris water maze tests

Both pcd and Lurcher cerebellar mutants showed poor performance in the Morris water maze. Despite finding a marked improvement in Lurcher mice during the visible platform task, the results were worse than in wild type controls, and there were only a few trials with a direct swim toward the goal. The results for learning the hidden platform position were much worse, and among cerebellar mutants, it was detectable only in Lurcher males. This complies with earlier findings (Lalonde and Thifault, 1994; Cendelin et al., 2014), and may support the hypothesis that Lurcher mice have impaired visuomotor integration suggested by Lalonde and Thifault (1994). Nevertheless, visuomotor integration ability seems to be partially preserved in Lurchers, since they are able to learn the visual platform task. Preference for the zone of the previous platform position during the first trial after changing the platform position (analogy of probe trial) also supports the idea that Lurcher mutants have some level of spatial learning ability that seems to be strongly dependent on the possibility of visual guidance training, which was constituted in our study by the 5 day-sessions of the visual platform task. Poor performance in the hidden platform task, on the other hand, may suggest a severe spatial learning deficit.

Although it has been reported in pcd mice that their performance in the visual platform task is not impaired compared to wild type controls (Goodlett et al., 1992), we observed poor performance of pcd mice in both visual and hidden platform tasks. The first study of spatial navigation in pcd mice used quite small experimental groups of male mice (Goodlett et al., 1992). In the present study, the results are based on larger samples, and males and females were analyzed separately.

An interesting phenomenon seen in the probe trial was the marked preference for the quadrant in which the visible platform was localized, and the omitting of the quadrant of the more recent localization of the hidden platform in both types of cerebellar mutants. In pcd mice, this is an artifact of spending a long time in the proximity of the starting point due to low activity. Furthermore, the reversal hidden platform task seemed to be extremely difficult for Lurchers. The preference of the original target quadrant could be explained by the behavioral inflexibility of Lurcher mice (Dickson et al., 2010). Behavioral flexibility, inhibitory response, and working memory are high-level cognitive skills, which enable the effective execution of goal-directed behaviors (Dalley et al., 2004). These skills have consistently been shown to be dependent on the prefrontal cortex (Dalley et al., 2004; Robbins and Arnsten, 2009). It has been demonstrated that the cerebellum modulates the prefrontal cortex activity (Strick et al., 2009; Rogers et al., 2013). Behavioral inflexibility, as well as behavioral disinhibition, which are closely related to inhibitory response (Young et al., 2009), suggested the affection of higher cognitive skills in Lurcher mutants. Thus, the poor performance of cerebellar mutants in the water maze task could be caused by at least four types of factors or their combinations: (1) Cognitive disorders, (2) Sensory disorders, (3) Motivation and behavioral abnormalities, and (4) Motor deficits. The performance is further modified by differences in manifestation of the mutations, strain, and sex.

Role of cognition

The Morris water maze task requires at least two types of non-motor learning. First, association between the platform and escape from the maze must be created. Second, the animal must start to learn the position of the platform. Associative learning processes are supposed to be strongly related to the cerebellum (Gruart et al., 1997; Jimenez-Diaz et al., 2004; for review, see Thompson and Steinmetz, 2009; Perciavalle et al., 2013) and its abnormalities have been described in both Lurcher and pcd mutants (Chen et al., 1996; Porras-Garcia et al., 2005, 2010; Brown et al., 2010). Thus, delayed association between the platform and water escape could affect the motivation to learn its position in cerebellar mutants.

Traditionally, spatial cognition is related to the hippocampus (O'Keefe and Nadel, 1978). Nevertheless, the cerebellum participates in the construction of hippocampal spatial representation and, thus, plays an important role in goal-directed navigation (Rochefort et al., 2011, 2013; Onuki et al., 2013). Therefore, it could be assumed that the absence of Purkinje cells in both pcd and Lurcher mice might have a strong impact on the hippocampal processes involved in solving spatial tasks.

Role of sensory impairments

Since good vision is crucial for spatial navigation, retinal degeneration could be an important factor that strongly influences behavior, namely spatial performance in pcd mice. Nevertheless, the retinal degeneration in pcd mice is only slowly progressive (Blanks et al., 1982; Lavail et al., 1982; Blanks and Spee, 1992; Marchena et al., 2011), and we have found only an insignificant reduction of photoreceptor density in the retinas of pcd mice at the age at which they were tested for spatial orientation. Despite this, some impact of vision problems on behavior during spatial tasks could not be excluded due to the possible functional imperfection of a degenerating retina even before a reduction of photoreceptor number becomes evident (Marchena et al., 2011).

However, in addition, the cerebellar disorder itself may lead to severe sensory dysfunctions by at least two mechanisms—affection of perceptual processes and oculomotor abnormalities. The cerebellum is associated with perceptual systems including vision, proprioception and self-motion perception, and cerebellar lesions lead to a wide range of sensory impairments (for review, see Baumann et al., 2015). Therefore, cerebellar disorders may severely affect spatial orientation ability due to the inappropriate acquisition and processing of information necessary for space navigation.

Control of oculomotor function is important for sighting fixation and for the visual following of an object by a moving individual. In Lurchers, abnormalities of the optokinetic and vestibuloocular reflexes were described by van Alphen et al. (2002). Since cerebellar Purkinje cells control oculomotor coordination, including optokinetic and vestibuloocular reflexes (for review, see Angelaki and Hess, 2005; Yakusheva et al., 2007), oculomotor problems could be expected also in pcd mice, in which, however, the vestibuloocular reflex has been found to be almost normal (Killian and Baker, 2002).

For all of these reasons, sensory problems can be expected to play a significant role in navigation difficulties in cerebellar mutants. In the visible goal task, these problems may be less important than in the case of the hidden goal task, since the goal represents a single and marked intramaze object of interest instead of multiple extramaze landmarks necessary for hidden goal location.

Role of motivation and behavioral abnormalities

Paradoxically, pcd mice showed short distances moved. This fact can be explained by low swimming activity and longer periods of floating compared with Lurchers. Therefore, their trajectory was relatively short, even in the case where they did not reach the platform and spent the entire trial slowly swimming with floating periods, while Lurcher mice spent this time intensively swimming. Higher tendency of inactivity in pcd mice and higher swimming activity in Lurcher mice were also seen in the forced swimming test. Floating is a behavioral phenomenon that may substantially influence the results of the water maze tasks (Llano Lopez et al., 2010) or may be a response to a difficult task as a manifestation of depressive-like behavior and learned helplessness (Porsolt et al., 1979). Potential sight impairment due to retinal degeneration and poor fitness related to low body weight may make the spatial task too difficult for pcd mice, which might induce learned helplessness.

Role of motor impairment

For performance in the water maze task, swimming and direction maintenance abilities are required. Motor impairment has been shown many times in cerebellar mutants (Fortier et al., 1987; Lalonde et al., 1996; Le Marec and Lalonde, 1997, 1998; Cendelin et al., 2008, 2014). On the other hand, Fortier et al. (1987) showed a normal EMG pattern in swimming Lurcher mice, but not in walking ones, suggesting that swimming is not as affected by the ataxia as gait. Furthermore, Lurcher mice achieved the same swimming speed as wild type mice. In pcd mice, low swimming speed could account for their abnormal swimming pattern (Goodlett et al., 1992), but also for lower activity or worse fitness. Nevertheless, in both Lurcher and pcd mice, we have observed a high incidence of rotating, but almost no direct swim trials. A low frequency of direct swim was even seen in Lurcher mice at the end of the visible platform task when they showed an improving ability to reach the visible goal. Therefore, motor deficiency does not seem to affect swimming ability, but rather, could influence trajectory shape and disable the maintenance of a straight course toward the goal in cerebellar mutants.

Role of the mutation, strain background and sex

Poor spatial performance is a strong phenotypic manifestation of particular mutations in pcd and Lurcher mice. These symptoms are easily detectable by the tests, and other factors, such as strain and sex, seem to only slightly modulate performance. Sex dimorphism as a function of brain structures related to both behavioral processes and motor control has been described (Arvidsson et al., 2014), and significant sex differences were even reported in neurological manifestations of mutations in mice (Walton et al., 2012; Truong et al., 2013).

More problematic is the comparison of the manifestation of mutations. Despite the main features and extent of cerebellar degeneration being similar, pcd and Lurcher mice differ in a number of aspects. The overall performance of pcd mice in the Morris water maze was worse than in Lurchers. Grid2Lc and Agtpb1pcd mutations not only differ in the mechanism of cell death activation, but the spectrum of extracerebellar brain damage and the affection of other tissues was wider with the Agtpb1pcd mutation. Therefore, in this case, modifying factors have a broader range of targets. Particularly, retinal degeneration (Blanks et al., 1982; Lavail et al., 1982; Blanks and Spee, 1992; Marchena et al., 2011) and expression of Nna1 in the skeletal muscles (Harris et al., 2000) are important. Since strain differences between B6CBA and B6.BR wild type mice were also observed in the present study, genetic background plays a role. Pcd and Lurcher mice are not commercially available on the same strain background. Therefore, it is difficult to unambiguously distinguish a specific mutation effect from the modifying effect of strain-specific phenotypic traits, the importance of which was particularly shown for floating behavior.

Recently, we have shown that, despite the Lurcher mutation having a strong manifestation, the phenotype could be modulated by genetic background (Cendelin et al., 2014). Considering the magnitude of differences between the same mutants in different strains and between different mutants, we could conclude that strain differences could be sufficient to cover or mitigate some of the mutation-related differences.

Conclusion

We have confirmed the severe impairments in cognitive and behavioral tests in both pcd and Lurcher mutant mice. Contrary to previous studies (Goodlett et al., 1992; Lalonde and Thifault, 1994), we found that visuomotor integration is only partially disabled in Lurchers, and that pcd mice failed in both visual and hidden goal tests, using large samples of mice. Overall performance in the Morris water maze test was better in Lurcher mutants than in pcd mice. The effect of the mutation as well as of the genetic background was seen. The deficit of spatial performance in cerebellar mutants may potentially arise from a combination of cognitive, sensory, emotional, and motor disturbances, all of which are expected to be of different importance in various mutants. Mutation-related differences could be potentiated by specific phenotypic traits of different strains of origin than these mutants.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Helena Geciova. Irena Pojarova, and Dr. Pavel Ostasov for their technical assistance and Dr. Milena Kralickova for institutional support. COST Grant No. LD12057 of the Ministry of Education, Youth and Sport of the Czech Republic.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00116/abstract

References

  1. Angelaki D. E., Hess B. J. (2005). Self-motion-induced eye movements: effects on visual acuity and navigation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 966–976. 10.1038/nrn1804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Araki K., Meguro H., Kushiya E., Takayama C., Inoue Y., Mishina M. (1993). Selective expression of the glutamate receptor channel delta 2 subunit in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 197, 1267–1276. 10.1006/bbrc.1993.2614 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arvidsson E., Viereckel T., Mikulovic S., Wallen-Mackenzie A. (2014). Age- and sex-dependence of dopamine release and capacity for recovery identified in the dorsal striatum of C57/Bl6J mice. PLoS ONE 9:e99592. 10.1371/journal.pone.0099592 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baltanas F. C., Berciano M. T., Valero J., Gomez C., Diaz D., Alonso J. R., et al. (2013). Differential glial activation during the degeneration of Purkinje cells and mitral cells in the PCD mutant mice. Glia 61, 254–272. 10.1002/glia.22431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baltanas F. C., Casafont I., Lafarga V., Weruaga E., Alonso J. R., Berciano M. T., et al. (2011). Purkinje cell degeneration in pcd mice reveals large scale chromatin reorganization and gene silencing linked to defective DNA repair. J. Biol. Chem. 286, 28287–28302. 10.1074/jbc.M111.246041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baumann O., Borra R. J., Bower J. M., Cullen K. E., Habas C., Ivry R. B., et al. (2015). Consensus paper: the role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum 14, 197–220. 10.1007/s12311-014-0627-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blanks J. C., Mullen R. J., Lavail M. M. (1982). Retinal degeneration in the pcd cerebellar mutant mouse. II. Electron microscopic analysis. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 231–246. 10.1002/cne.902120303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Blanks J. C., Spee C. (1992). Retinal degeneration in the pcd/pcd mutant mouse: accumulation of spherules in the interphotoreceptor space. Exp. Eye Res. 54, 637–644. 10.1016/0014-4835(92)90019-O [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boyce R. W., Dorph-Petersen K. A., Lyck L., Gundersen H. J. (2010). Design-based stereology: introduction to basic concepts and practical approaches for estimation of cell number. Toxicol. Pathol. 38, 1011–1025. 10.1177/0192623310385140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown K. L., Agelan A., Woodruff-Pak D. S. (2010). Unimpaired trace classical eyeblink conditioning in Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 93, 303–311. 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Caddy K. W., Biscoe T. J. (1979). Structural and quantitative studies on the normal C3H and Lurcher mutant mouse. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 287, 167–201. 10.1098/rstb.1979.0055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caston J., Chianale C., Delhaye-Bouchaud N., Mariani J. (1998). Role of the cerebellum in exploration behavior. Brain Res. 808, 232–237. 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00847-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cendelin J. (2014). From mice to men: lessons from mutant ataxic mice. Cerebellum Ataxias 1:4 10.1186/2053-8871-1-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cendelin J., Korelusova I., Vozeh F. (2008). The effect of repeated rotarod training on motor skills and spatial learning ability in Lurcher mutant mice. Behav. Brain. Res. 189, 65–74. 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cendelin J., Tuma J., Korelusova I., Vozeh F. (2014). The effect of genetic background on behavioral manifestation of Grid2 mutation. Behav. Brain. Res. 271C, 218–227. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chen L., Bao S., Lockard J. M., Kim J. K., Thompson R. F. (1996). Impaired classical eyeblink conditioning in cerebellar-lesioned and Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice. J. Neurosci. 16, 2829–2838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cooper F. E., Grube M., Elsegood K. J., Welch J. L., Kelly T. P., Chinnery P. F., et al. (2010). The contribution of the cerebellum to cognition in spinocerebellar ataxia type 6. Behav. Neurol. 23, 3–15. 10.1155/2010/724861 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dalley J. W., Cardinal R. N., Robbins T. W. (2004). Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in rodents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 771–784. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. de Jager P. L., Zuo J., Cook S. A., Heintz N. (1997). A new allele of the lurcher gene, lurcherJ. Mamm. Genome 8, 647–650. 10.1007/s003359900530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. D'hooge R., de Deyn P. P. (2001). Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning and memory. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 36, 60–90. 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00067-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dickson P. E., Rogers T. D., Del Mar N., Martin L. A., Heck D., Blaha C. D., et al. (2010). Behavioral flexibility in a mouse model of developmental cerebellar Purkinje cell loss. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 94, 220–228. 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.05.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fancellu R., Paridi D., Tomasello C., Panzeri M., Castaldo A., Genitrini S., et al. (2013). Longitudinal study of cognitive and psychiatric functions in spinocerebellar ataxia types 1 and 2. J. Neurol. 260, 3134–3143. 10.1007/s00415-013-7138-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fernandez-Gonzalez A., La Spada A. R., Treadaway J., Higdon J. C., Harris B. S., Sidman R. L., et al. (2002). Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) phenotypes caused by mutations in the axotomy-induced gene, Nna1. Science 295, 1904–1906. 10.1126/science.1068912 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fortier P. A., Smith A. M., Rossignol S. (1987). Locomotor deficits in the mutant mouse, Lurcher. Exp. Brain Res. 66, 271–286. 10.1007/BF00243304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frederic F., Chautard T., Brochard R., Chianale C., Wollman E., Oliver C., et al. (1997). Enhanced endocrine response to novel environment stress and endotoxin in Lurcher mutant mice. Neuroendocrinology 66, 341–347. 10.1159/000127257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Furuya S., Irie F., Hashikawa T., Nakazawa K., Kozakai A., Hasegawa A., et al. (1994). Ganglioside GD1 alpha in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Its specific absence in mouse mutants with Purkinje cell abnormality and altered immunoreactivity in response to conjunctive stimuli causing long-term desensitization. J. Biol. Chem. 269, 32418–32425. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ghetti B., Norton J., Triarhou L. C. (1987). Nerve cell atrophy and loss in the inferior olivary complex of “Purkinje cell degeneration” mutant mice. J. Comp. Neurol. 260, 409–422. 10.1002/cne.902600307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Glaser J., Greene G., Hendricks S. (2007). Stereology for Biological Research with a Focus on Neuroscience. Williston, VT: MBF Press. [Google Scholar]
  29. Goodlett C. R., Hamre K. M., West J. R. (1992). Dissociation of spatial navigation and visual guidance performance in Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice. Behav. Brain. Res. 47, 129–141. 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80119-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Greer C. A., Shepherd G. M. (1982). Mitral cell degeneration and sensory function in the neurological mutant mouse Purkinje cell degeneration (PCD). Brain Res. 235, 156–161. 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90206-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gruart A., Pastor A. M., Armengol J. A., Delgado-Garcia J. M. (1997). Involvement of cerebellar cortex and nuclei in the genesis and control of unconditioned and conditioned eyelid motor responses. Prog. Brain Res. 114, 511–528. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)63383-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gundersen H. J. (1986). Stereology of arbitrary particles. A review of unbiased number and size estimators and the presentation of some new ones, in memory of William R. Thompson. J. Microsc. 143, 3–45. 10.1111/j.1365-2818.1986.tb02764.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gundersen H. J., Jensen E. B. (1987). The efficiency of systematic sampling in stereology and its prediction. J. Microsc. 147, 229–263. 10.1111/j.1365-2818.1987.tb02837.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Harris A., Morgan J. I., Pecot M., Soumare A., Osborne A., Soares H. D. (2000). Regenerating motor neurons express Nna1, a novel ATP/GTP-binding protein related to zinc carboxypeptidases. Mol. Cell Neurosci. 16, 578–596. 10.1006/mcne.2000.0900 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hilber P., Lorivel T., Delarue C., Caston J. (2004). Stress and anxious-related behaviors in Lurcher mutant mice. Brain Res. 1003, 108–112. 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jimenez-Diaz L., Navarro-Lopez Jde D., Gruart A., Delgado-Garcia J. M. (2004). Role of cerebellar interpositus nucleus in the genesis and control of reflex and conditioned eyelid responses. J. Neurosci. 24, 9138–9145. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2025-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Killian J. E., Baker J. F. (2002). Horizontal vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) head velocity estimation in Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd/pcd) mutant mice. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1159–1164. 10.1152/jn.00219.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lalonde R. (1998). Immobility responses in Lurcher mutant mice. Behav. Genet. 28, 309–314. 10.1023/A:1021627631721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lalonde R., Filali M., Bensoula A. N., Lestienne F. (1996). Sensorimotor learning in three cerebellar mutant mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 65, 113–120. 10.1006/nlme.1996.0013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lalonde R., Lamarre Y., Smith A. M. (1988). Does the mutant mouse lurcher have deficits in spatially oriented behaviours? Brain Res. 455, 24–30. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90109-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lalonde R., Manseau M., Botez M. I. (1987). Spontaneous alternation and habituation in Purkinje cell degeneration mutant mice. Brain Res. 411, 187–189. 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90699-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lalonde R., Manseau M., Botez M. I. (1989). Exploration and habituation in Purkinje cell degeneration mutant mice. Brain Res. 479, 201–203. 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91354-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lalonde R., Strazielle C. (2007). Spontaneous and induced mouse mutations with cerebellar dysfunctions: behavior and neurochemistry. Brain Res. 1140, 51–74. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lalonde R., Thifault S. (1994). Absence of an association between motor coordination and spatial orientation in lurcher mutant mice. Behav. Genet. 24, 497–501. 10.1007/BF01071563 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lalouette A., Lohof A., Sotelo C., Guenet J., Mariani J. (2001). Neurobiological effects of a null mutation depend on genetic context: comparison between two hotfoot alleles of the delta-2 ionotropic glutamate receptor. Neuroscience 105, 443–455. 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00193-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lavail M. M., Blanks J. C., Mullen R. J. (1982). Retinal degeneration in the pcd cerebellar mutant mouse. I. Light microscopic and autoradiographic analysis. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 217–230. 10.1002/cne.902120302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Le Marec N., Lalonde R. (1997). Sensorimotor learning and retention during equilibrium tests in Purkinje cell degeneration mutant mice. Brain Res. 768, 310–316. 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00666-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Le Marec N., Lalonde R. (1998). Treadmill performance of mice with cerebellar lesions: 1. Purkinje cell degeneration mutant mice. Behav. Neurosci. 112, 225–232. 10.1037/0735-7044.112.1.225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Le Marec N., Lalonde R. (2000). Treadmill performance of mice with cerebellar lesions: 2. Lurcher mutant mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 73, 195–206. 10.1006/nlme.1999.3926 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Llano Lopez L., Hauser J., Feldon J., Gargiulo P. A., Yee B. K. (2010). Evaluating spatial memory function in mice: a within-subjects comparison between the water maze test and its adaptation to dry land. Behav. Brain. Res. 209, 85–92. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lorivel T., Roy V., Hilber P. (2014). Fear-related behaviors in Lurcher mutant mice exposed to a predator. Genes Brain Behav. 13, 794–801. 10.1111/gbb.12173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Manto M., Marmolino D. (2009). Animal models of human cerebellar ataxias: a cornerstone for the therapies of the twenty-first century. Cerebellum 8, 137–154. 10.1007/s12311-009-0127-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Manto M. U. (2005). The wide spectrum of spinocerebellar ataxias (SCAs). Cerebellum 4, 2–6. 10.1080/14734220510007914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Marchena M., Lara J., Aijon J., Germain F., de La Villa P., Velasco A. (2011). The retina of the PCD/PCD mouse as a model of photoreceptor degeneration. A structural and functional study. Exp. Eye Res. 93, 607–617. 10.1016/j.exer.2011.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Marien P., Beaton A. (2014). The enigmatic linguistic cerebellum: clinical relevance and unanswered questions on nonmotor speech and language deficits in cerebellar disorders. Cerebellum Ataxias 1, 1–12 10.1186/2053-8871-1-12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Morris R. (1984). Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods 11, 47–60. 10.1016/0165-0270(84)90007-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mullen R. J., Eicher E. M., Sidman R. L. (1976). Purkinje cell degeneration, a new neurological mutation in the mouse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 73, 208–212. 10.1073/pnas.73.1.208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. O'Keefe J., Nadel L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  59. O'Gorman S. (1985). Degeneration of thalamic neurons in “Purkinje cell degeneration” mutant mice. II. Cytology of neuron loss. J. Comp. Neurol. 234, 298–316. 10.1002/cne.902340303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. O'Gorman S., Sidman R. L. (1985). Degeneration of thalamic neurons in “Purkinje cell degeneration” mutant mice. I. Distribution of neuron loss. J. Comp. Neurol. 234, 277–297. 10.1002/cne.902340302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Onuki Y., van Someren E. J., de Zeeuw C. I., van der Werf Y. D. (2013). Hippocampal-cerebellar interaction during spatio-temporal prediction. Cereb. Cortex 25, 313–321. 10.1093/cercor/bht221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Perciavalle V., Apps R., Bracha V., Delgado-Garcia J. M., Gibson A. R., Leggio M., et al. (2013). Consensus paper: current views on the role of cerebellar interpositus nucleus in movement control and emotion. Cerebellum 12, 738–757. 10.1007/s12311-013-0464-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pesarin F., Salmaso L. (2010). Permutation Tests for Complex Data: Theory, Applications and Software. Chippenham: A John Wiley and Sons, Ltd. [Google Scholar]
  64. Phillips R. J. S. (1960). ‘Lurcher’, a new gene in linkage group XI of the house mouse. J. Genet. 57, 35–42 10.1007/BF02985337 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Porras-Garcia E., Cendelin J., Dominguez-Del-Toro E., Vozeh F., Delgado-Garcia J. M. (2005). Purkinje cell loss affects differentially the execution, acquisition and prepulse inhibition of skeletal and facial motor responses in Lurcher mice. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 979–988. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03940.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Porras-Garcia E., Sanchez-Campusano R., Martinez-Vargas D., Dominguez-Del-Toro E., Cendelin J., Vozeh F., et al. (2010). Behavioral characteristics, associative learning capabilities, and dynamic association mapping in an animal model of cerebellar degeneration. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 346–365. 10.1152/jn.00180.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Porsolt R. D., Bertin A., Blavet N., Deniel M., Jalfre M. (1979). Immobility induced by forced swimming in rats: effects of agents which modify central catecholamine and serotonin activity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 57, 201–210. 10.1016/0014-2999(79)90366-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Robbins T. W., Arnsten A. F. (2009). The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 267–287. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rochefort C., Arabo A., Andre M., Poucet B., Save E., Rondi-Reig L. (2011). Cerebellum shapes hippocampal spatial code. Science 334, 385–389. 10.1126/science.1207403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rochefort C., Lefort J. M., Rondi-Reig L. (2013). The cerebellum: a new key structure in the navigation system. Front. Neural Circuits 7:35. 10.3389/fncir.2013.00035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rogers T. D., Dickson P. E., McKimm E., Heck D. H., Goldowitz D., Blaha C. D., et al. (2013). Reorganization of circuits underlying cerebellar modulation of prefrontal cortical dopamine in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder. Cerebellum 12, 547–556. 10.1007/s12311-013-0462-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rotter A., Rath S., Evans J. E., Frostholm A. (2000). Modulation of GABA(A) receptor subunit mRNA levels in olivocerebellar neurons of purkinje cell degeneration and weaver mutant mice. J. Neurochem. 74, 2190–2200. 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.0742190.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schmahmann J. D., Sherman J. C. (1997). Cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 41, 433–440. 10.1016/S0074-7742(08)60363-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Strick P. L., Dum R. P., Fiez J. A. (2009). Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 413–434. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thompson R. F., Steinmetz J. E. (2009). The role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of discrete behavioral responses. Neuroscience 162, 732–755. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Triarhou L. C., Norton J., Ghetti B. (1987). Anterograde transsynaptic degeneration in the deep cerebellar nuclei of Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mutant mice. Exp. Brain Res. 66, 577–588. 10.1007/BF00270691 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Truong D. T., Bonet A., Rendall A. R., Rosen G. D., Fitch R. H. (2013). A behavioral evaluation of sex differences in a mouse model of severe neuronal migration disorder. PLoS ONE 8:e73144. 10.1371/journal.pone.0073144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. van Alphen A. M., Schepers T., Luo C., de Zeeuw C. I. (2002). Motor performance and motor learning in Lurcher mice. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 978, 413–424. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07584.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Vinueza Veloz M. F., Zhou K., Bosman L. W., Potters J. W., Negrello M., Seepers R. M., et al. (2014). Cerebellar control of gait and interlimb coordination. Brain Struct. Funct. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1007/s00429-014-0870-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Walton J. C., Schilling K., Nelson R. J., Oberdick J. (2012). Sex-dependent behavioral functions of the Purkinje cell-specific Galphai/o binding protein, Pcp2(L7). Cerebellum 11, 982–1001. 10.1007/s12311-012-0368-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wang T., Morgan J. I. (2007). The Purkinje cell degeneration (pcd) mouse: an unexpected molecular link between neuronal degeneration and regeneration. Brain Res. 1140, 26–40. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.07.065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wetts R., Herrup K. (1982a). Cerebellar Purkinje cells are descended from a small number of progenitors committed during early development: quantitative analysis of lurcher chimeric mice. J. Neurosci. 2, 1494–1498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Wetts R., Herrup K. (1982b). Interaction of granule, Purkinje and inferior olivary neurons in lurcher chimeric mice. II. Granule cell death. Brain Res. 250, 358–362. 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90431-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wolfer D. P., Lipp H. P. (2000). Dissecting the behaviour of transgenic mice: is it the mutation, the genetic background, or the environment? Exp. Physiol. 85, 627–634. 10.1111/j.1469-445X.2000.02095.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yakusheva T. A., Shaikh A. G., Green A. M., Blazquez P. M., Dickman J. D., Angelaki D. E. (2007). Purkinje cells in posterior cerebellar vermis encode motion in an inertial reference frame. Neuron 54, 973–985. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Young S. E., Friedman N. P., Miyake A., Willcutt E. G., Corley R. P., Haberstick B. C., et al. (2009). Behavioral disinhibition: liability for externalizing spectrum disorders and its genetic and environmental relation to response inhibition across adolescence. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 117–130. 10.1037/a0014657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Zanjani S. H., Selimi F., Vogel M. W., Haeberle A. M., Boeuf J., Mariani J., et al. (2006). Survival of interneurons and parallel fiber synapses in a cerebellar cortex deprived of Purkinje cells: studies in the double mutant mouse Grid2Lc/+;Bax(–/–). J. Comp. Neurol. 497, 622–635. 10.1002/cne.21017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zhang W., Lee W. H., Triarhou L. C. (1996). Grafted cerebellar cells in a mouse model of hereditary ataxia express IGF-I system genes and partially restore behavioral function. Nat. Med. 2, 65–71. 10.1038/nm0196-65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zuo J., de Jager P. L., Takahashi K. A., Jiang W., Linden D. J., Heintz N. (1997). Neurodegeneration in Lurcher mice caused by mutation in delta2 glutamate receptor gene. Nature 388, 769–773. 10.1038/42009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES