Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Dev Perspect. 2015 Apr 1;9(2):96–100. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12112

Development of Maladaptive Coping: A Functional Adaptation to Chronic, Uncontrollable Stress

Martha E Wadsworth 1
PMCID: PMC4442090  NIHMSID: NIHMS661917  PMID: 26019717

Abstract

Health disparities are rooted in childhood and stem from adverse early environments that damage physiologic stress-response systems. Developmental psychobiological models of the effects of chronic stress account for both the negative effects of a stress-response system calibrated to a dangerous and unpredictable environment from a health perspective, and the positive effects of such an adaptively calibrated stress response from a functional perspective. Our research suggests that contexts that produce functionally adapted physiologic responses to stress also encourage a functionally adapted coping response—coping that can result in maladjustment in physical and mental health, but enables children to grow and develop within those contexts. In this article, I highlight the value of reframing maladaptive coping as functional adaptation to understand more completely the development of children’s coping in different contexts, and the value of such a conceptual shift for coping-based theory, research, and intervention.

Keywords: functional adaptation, environmental context, allostatic load, coping, stress reactivity, self-regulation

Developmental Pathways to Psychopathology

Mental and physical health problems are stratified along a socioeconomic gradient and across racial divides. Racial minorities and those with fewer financial resources have higher levels of mental and physical disease and premature mortality (1, 2). These health disparities are rooted in childhood (3) and stem in part from exposure to the chronic, uncontrollable stress endemic in impoverished environments (4). Research has begun to unpack the ways that stress directly affects biological systems and leads to disorder and disease, and how it is associated with cognitive, emotional, and behavioral patterns that further contribute to disease and disorder (e.g., 5). Developmental psychobiological models that emphasize the sensitivity of the brain and associated systems to environmental inputs have explained the first set of pathways—how chronic stress and environmental adversity get under the skin to contribute to physical and mental health problems (e.g., 68). Research on at-risk youth suggests that the other pathway—cognitive, emotional, behavioral—also transmits risk (or confers protection) from adverse environments (5), but little theoretical or empirical work has delineated how or why problematic coping styles develop from chronic stress. In this article, I propose that atypical patterns of emotional, cognitive, and behavioral coping and self-regulation stem directly from the same early experiences of chronic uncontrollable stress that contribute to dysregulated biological stress reactivity.

Development of Children’s Coping

Some theory and research have addressed how children develop their characteristic ways of coping, whether adaptive or maladaptive. According to the Responses to Stress model (9), children’s earliest attempts to regulate themselves begin in infancy in symbiotic coregulation with a caregiver. Early self-soothing (thumb sucking) and regulation behaviors (looking away) emerge during the first year, setting in motion a process that unfolds over the next two decades (and beyond) wherein children become increasingly independent in their ability to manage difficult situations and soothe themselves (10). The ingredients necessary for children to develop a healthy repertoire of coping skills apparently are the presence of mild and moderate stress, positive coping models, and age-appropriate scaffolding (11, 12).

During the toddler and preschool years, children typically progress from crying and seeking physical comfort to seeking help and avoiding sources of stress. As cognitive abilities develop in the elementary school years and children improve their emotional awareness, metacognition, and executive functions, more complex cognitive and behavioral coping strategies such as problem solving and cognitive reframing emerge, and coping repertoires are refined well into adolescence and beyond (e.g., 13). By mid- to late adolescence, youth typically have a coping repertoire that contains a range of strategies (e.g., active and accommodative; cognitive, emotional and behavioral) and the ability to flexibly match coping strategies to important characteristics of a stressful situation (e.g., its controllability and urgency).

However, some youth have unhealthy or maladaptive coping repertoires that rely too heavily on a single strategy such as avoidance, tend to be inflexible and applied rigidly, and can include behaviors with serious negative consequences. Such underdeveloped or immature coping styles have been identified as proximal causal mechanisms that connect stressful childhoods to psychopathology (e.g., 1416).

The Origins of Maladaptive Coping

What happens in a child’s life to disrupt the normative developmental sequence and cause children to get stuck at an immature level of coping? Zimmer-Gembeck and Skinner’s integrative review showed that older children and adolescents may continue to use less mature or primitive forms of coping (e.g., escaping and seeking contact with a caregiver) when they face extremely stressful events. Research on child trauma and post-traumatic stress disorder suggests that children develop problematic coping to protect themselves from overwhelming stress such as maltreatment (17). Similarly, cognitive theories of depression suggest that children develop negative coping and thinking patterns from early invalidating interactions with caregivers. Thus, repeated use of developmentally primitive coping, lack of exposure to healthy alternatives, or repeated exposure to overwhelming stress may solidify a maladaptive style of coping—one that relies too much on primitive strategies such as avoidance and denial (e.g., 18). Despite recognition that maladaptive coping apparently stems from adverse childhood experiences (e.g., those associated with poverty), and predicts psychopathology across the lifespan and mediates the association between adversity and psychopathology (e.g., 19), little research has addressed how maladaptive styles of coping develop.

Maladaptive Coping as Functional Adaptation

Allostatic load (AL; 20) theory consolidates clinical and research findings into a cohesive explanation of how toxic early environments lead to disparities in physical health—via damage to physiologic stress-response systems resulting from the body’s attempts to maintain homeostasis in the face of chaos. The AL model focuses on damage to and dysregulation of the sympathetic-adrenomedullary (SAM) system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA; 4) caused by early uncontrollable stress. Repeated, excessive activations of these systems as a result of chronic and uncontrollable stress lead to wear and tear on the body. When chronically engaged, HPA and SAM systems overactivate critical physical organs and systems involving physical organs (e.g., cardiovascular, immunologic, metabolic), sometimes damaging them and leading to physical diseases (7). Research informed by AL illustrates the sensitivity of the brain and other organ systems to environmental stress in childhood, as well as the potential costs of this neural plasticity (20). Although AL theory recognizes the functional reason why human physiology adapts to early uncontrollable stress, it emphasizes the damage to stress-response systems and underscores the physical and mental health problems that result.

In contrast to AL theory, the adaptive calibration model (ACM; 21) emphasizes the necessary and beneficial functions of biological and behavioral adaptations to chronically stressful environments. For example, highly reactive biological stress responses, and vigilant and avoidant coping are critical for adapting to harsh, inhospitable environments. Given its functionalist perspective, the ACM acknowledges but does not emphasize that these atypical responses also lead to higher rates of physical and mental health problems among disadvantaged groups and thereby contribute to health disparities.

Uniting the insights of AL and ACM, experiential canalization theory (6) emphasizes that functional adaptations of the biological and behavioral systems come with tradeoffs. The physiologic systems that develop to protect the child—and presumably succeed in doing so—also lead to undesirable processes and outcomes in other domains. For example, the dysregulated HPA is hypervigilant in all situations, not just dangerous ones. Therein lies the crux of the problem—a hypervigilant stress response maximizes an at-risk child’s ability to grow and develop in his or her assigned environmental niche, and simultaneously fatigues related metabolic, cardiovascular, immunologic, and endocrine systems, leading to premature disease (e.g., hypertension and midline adiposity), early aging, and risk for psychopathology. By extension, a rigidly applied avoidant coping response protects a child growing up amid violence and victimization, but can also lead to clinical levels of anxiety or depression.

Extending each of these theories to include coping would lead to the prediction that coping and self-regulation patterns would also be shaped by the demands of the inhospitable rearing environment (e.g., that associated with poverty). Developmental cascade theories explain that such adaptations to the stress response result from the body’s efforts to align person and environment systems (e.g. 22, 23). These complex chains of person-environment transactions calibrate the child’s biological, behavioral, and socioaffective systems so that the child can function within his or her environmental context. Thus, coping behaviors that protect the child from danger to the physical or psychological self and promote status and inclusion would be highly developed (e.g., 21), quite likely at the expense of a wide, flexible coping repertoire that would be adaptive in a range of settings.

Support for such a phenomenon is accumulating. The conditions that lead to a hyperreactive or hyporeactive physiologic stress response apparently affect the development of self-regulation and coping skills similarly (4, 24). Because of their chronic and uncontrollable nature, poverty and exposure to violence are exceedingly difficult for children to cope with using active primary control strategies such as problem solving (e.g., 25, 24). As a result, coping strategies that keep children from danger and protect them physically and psychologically are favored, and can become established because of developmental cascade processes (e.g., 23). Children experiencing chronic stress (e.g., family conflict, economic hardship, exposure to violence and victimization) tend to rely on cognitive and behavioral avoidance (e.g., 26, 27). Moreover, while avoidance generally predicts poor functioning (e.g., 19), avoidant coping in the context of uncontrollable stress such as conflict and violence is sometimes associated with psychological problems in the long term but not in the short term (e.g., 27, 28). Thus, early or primitive strategies may help children cope with extremely or chronically stressful events (13).

Children facing chronic and uncontrollable stress less often cope by actively attempting to solve problems and managing their emotions, a type of coping typically associated with more optimal psychological functioning. In fact, active attempts to intervene in uncontrollably stressful situations such as interparental conflict often lead to undesirable psychosocial outcomes (e.g., 16), including more frequent emotional and behavioral problems. Efforts to accommodate to stress through acceptance, cognitive reframing, and distraction tend to be more effective in coping with uncontrollable stress because they predict less frequent symptoms even in children exposed to chronic stress. However, children facing extreme stress use less of this type of coping, as well (29).

Therefore, this pattern of findings (of reliance on and efficacy of avoidance in the context of adversity) is consistent with the functional adaptation models outlined earlier (e.g., 6). However, while adaptive for chronic stress, reliance on avoidance does not equip children to cope with the circumstances they will encounter in life and places them at risk for mental health problems (19). In summary, my research and that of others (24, 25) support the notion that living with chronic stress shapes the development of a child’s coping repertoire in a way that is simultaneously adaptive and maladaptive, depending on the context (dangerous or normative) and the outcome (safety or illness).

What is to be gained by applying these developmental models to theory and research on how children cope? Conceptualizing maladaptive coping from the perspective of functional adaptation discourages simplistic thinking about “good” and “bad” coping in favor of attending to more complex questions about how, when, and for whom different types of coping are adaptive. Additionally, recognizing that some of the so-called negative outcomes associated with different types of coping represent tradeoffs incurred by successful adaptation to suboptimal environments should encourage new research questions (e.g., about the origins of psychopathology and health disparities). Coping-based intervention can also be transformative: Teaching at-risk children how to use different coping strategies and expand their coping repertoires may be beneficial as children encounter complex and novel environments as they grow older (30). Additionally, some normative developmental contexts will present challenges that can benefit from children using a range of coping strategies. Finally, coping interventions may also represent our best hope for repairing and recalibrating physiologic stress systems (e.g., the HPA) that have adapted to chronic stress in ways that increase physical and mental health problems.

The Biology of Coping

Research from my lab supports the proposition that coping is malleable, different types of coping look different physiologically, and experimentally manipulated coping yields physiological differences (31). For example, experimentally induced secondary control coping is associated with less HPA activity and quicker HPA down-regulation than is avoidant coping (32). Coupled with evidence that psychosocial interventions lead to predicted changes in biological markers of the HPA (e.g., 33, 34), this suggests that coping-based intervention may improve a youth’s coping and recalibrate his or her psychobiological stress-response system. The positive implications of remediation of the psychobiological stress-response system are many and varied given the system’s role in physical and mental health problems. Hence, I propose that improving children’s abilities to cope with stress and regulate their reactivity can break the cycle of damage from chronic uncontrollable stress to health disparities, especially if the coping and regulatory processes that are targeted have effects at many levels, including the physiologic level (35).

Furthermore, research on brain plasticity in regions such as the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala, coupled with our emerging evidence of physiologic coping malleability, suggests that learning new ways to cope with stress may lay new neural pathways that could prevent further damage to these systems (20). Behavioral interventions ranging from training in stress inoculation to environmental enrichment can stimulate neurogenesis in key areas such as the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and amygdala, even into late adulthood (e.g., 36). In addition, brain regions critical for coping and self-control, such as the prefrontal cortex, have lengthy maturational periods extending into young adulthood. This prolonged development may functionally extend the period of malleability and responsivity of this system to psychosocial intervention (e.g., 37). Thus, psychosocial interventions that promote skills to cope with stress, increase a sense of control, and lead to reduced stress levels may not only affect physiologic stress systems (38), but also restore damaged systems that could otherwise result in pathology and dysfunction (e.g., 39). In fact, repairing these systems may help eliminate health disparities.

However, in all of this work we must not lose sight of the essential message of functional adaptation. The stress-response systems of children facing extreme stress have adapted to survive in harsh environments. Therefore, given that children living in poverty cannot be magically transported to safe and nurturing environments, they need to be able to survive despite harsh environments while they acquire skills to cope with developmentally normative types of stress. Such children may need a wider repertoire of coping skills to counteract both chronic, uncontrollable stress and more routine daily stress. Thus, interventions should target opening new coping pathways, but critically, not necessarily closing off early or primitive coping pathways. As implied by research on developmental coping, achieving a balance of approach and avoidance, mature and primitive will likely be most adaptive for the widest array of contexts children will encounter as they develop.

Conclusion

I have chronicled an expanded conceptualization of contextually adapted and adaptive coping that is consistent with developmental psychobiology theories of how chronic, uncontrollable stress during childhood gets under the skin. Bringing coping into the conversation illuminates psychosocial pathways to repair damaged biological stress systems, thereby providing clear and practical directions for intervention. An intervention that teaches children about active, avoidant, and accommodative coping skills with which to tackle both controllable and uncontrollable stress and addresses when to use each type of coping or a combination of strategies may help counteract health disparities. In addition, an intervention informed by the functional adaptation perspective would recognize that many children’s behaviors that lead to serious negative consequences (e.g., delinquency and substance use) are the tradeoffs that accompany adapting successfully to a childhood marked by adversity (e.g., poverty and violence). Thus, my intervention may have even wider implications in that it offers at-risk youth socially desirable ways to adapt to their environments. Hence, by viewing coping through this contextually rich framework, researchers can identify and foster skills and abilities to buffer at-risk youth from stress. This perspective also illuminates possible pathways to repair and restore damaged physiologic stress-response systems that could otherwise result in socioeconomic or racial health disparities (38).

Acknowledgments

The work described in this article was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD078753-01).

References

  • 1.Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Social Science and Medicine. 2013;90:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Williams DR. Miles to go before we sleep: Racial inequities in health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2012;53:279–295. doi: 10.1177/0022146512455804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022146512455804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:14716–14721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902971106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0902971106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives. 2013;7:43–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12013. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Miller GE, Chen E, Parker KJ. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:959–997. doi: 10.1037/a0024768. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Blair C, Raver CC. Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist. 2012;67:309–318. doi: 10.1037/a0027493. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Danese A, McEwen BS. Adverse childhood experiences, allostasis, allostatic load, and age-related disease. Physiology and Behavior. 2012;106:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35:1562–1592. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Connor-Smith J, Compas BE, Wadsworth ME, Thomsen AH, Saltzman H. Responses to stress in adolescence: Measurement of coping and involuntary stress responses. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:976–992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.6.976. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Compas BE, Connor-Smith JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:87–127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Abaied JL, Rudolph KD. Mothers as a resource in times of stress: Interactive contributions of socialization of coping and stress to youth psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38:273–289. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9364-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9364-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Compas BE. Coping, regulation, and development during childhood and adolescence. New Directions in Child and Adolescent Development. 2009;124:87–99. doi: 10.1002/cd.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zimmer-Gembeck M, Skinner EA. Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2011;35:1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025410384923. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Auerbach RP, Abela JRZ, Zhu X, Yao S. Understanding the role of coping in the development of depressive symptoms: Symptom specificity, gender differences, and cross-cultural applicability. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2010;49:547–561. doi: 10.1348/014466509X479681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466509X479681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gerstein ED, Pedersen YA, Crnic KA, Ryu E, Baker BL, Blacher J. Developmental risk and young children’s regulatory strategies: Predicting behavior problems at age five. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2011;39:351–364. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9471-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9471-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shelton KH, Harold GT. Pathways between interparental conflict and adolescent psychological adjustment: Bridging links through children’s cognitive appraisals and coping strategies. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2008;28:555–582. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ullman SE, Peter-Hagene L. Social reactions to sexual assault disclosure, coping, perceived control, and PTSD symptoms in sexual assault victims. Journal of Community Psychology. 2014;42:495–508. doi: 10.1002/jcop.21624. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.21624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Segrin C, Woszidlo A, Givertz M, Montgomery N. Parent and child traits associated with overparenting. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2013;32:569–595. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2013.32.6.569. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Seiffge-Krenke I. Causal links between stressful events, coping style, and adolescent symptomatology. Journal of Adolescence. 2000;23:675–691. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McEwen BS. The brain on stress: Toward an integrative approach to brain, body, and behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8:673–675. doi: 10.1177/1745691613506907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ellis BJ, Del Guidice M, Shirtcliff EA. Beyond allostatic load. In: Beauchaine T, Hinshaw S, editors. Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. 3. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2013. pp. 251–284. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cox MJ, Mills-Koonce R, Propper C, Gariépy J. Systems theory and cascades in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:497–506. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology. 2010;22:491–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wadsworth ME, Compas BE. Coping with family conflict and economic strain: The adolescent perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2002;12:243–274. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gaylord-Harden N, Elmore CA, Campbell CL, Wethington A. An examination of the tripartite model of depressive and anxiety symptoms in African American youth: Stressors and coping strategies as common and specific correlates. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2011;40:360–374. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.563467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2011.563467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Evans GW, Gonnella C, Marcynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychological Science. 2005;16:560–565. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Santiago CD, Wadsworth ME. Coping with family conflict: What’s helpful and what’s not for low-income adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2009;18:192–202. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Reid-Quiñones K, Kliewer W, Shields BJ, Goodman K, Ray MH, Wheat E. Cognitive, affective, and behavioral responses to witnessed versus experienced violence. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2011;81:51–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01071.x. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01071.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wadsworth ME, Santiago CD. Risk and resiliency processes in ethnically diverse families in poverty. Journal of Family Psychology. 2008;22:399–410. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Raviv T, Wadsworth ME. The efficacy of a pilot prevention program for children and caregivers coping with economic strain. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2010;34:216–228. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wadsworth ME, Bendezú JJ, Loughlin-Presnal JE, Ahlkvist JA, Tilghman-Osborne EM, Bianco H, Rindlaub L, Hurwich-Reiss E. Unlocking the black box: A multi-level analysis of children’s coping. 2015 doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1141356. Unpublished manuscript. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bendezú JJ, Wadsworth ME, Ahlkvist JA, Bianco HB. Children’s physiological responses to social stress: Associations with self-reported coping and parent coping socialization. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development; Seattle, WA. Apr, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau J, Levine S. Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:845–859. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Luecken LJ, Hagan MJ, Sandler IN, Tein J, Ayers TS, Wolchik SA. Longitudinal mediators of a randomized prevention program effect on cortisol for youth from parentally bereaved families. Prevention Science. 2014;15:224–232. doi: 10.1007/s11121-013-0385-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11121-013-0385-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;301:2252–2259. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hölzel BK, Carmody J, Evans KC, Hoge EA, Dusek JA, Morgan L, Lazar SW. Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010;5:11–17. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp034. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lourenco F, Casey BJ. Adjusting behavior to changing environmental demands with development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2013;37:2233–2242. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplasticity: Stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:689–695. doi: 10.1038/nn.3093. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.3093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Katz M, Liu C, Schaer M, Parker KJ, Ottet M, Epps A, Lyons DM. Prefrontal plasticity and stress inoculated-induced resilience. Developmental Neuroscience. 2009;31:293–299. doi: 10.1159/000216540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000216540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES