Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2015 May;27(2):411–423. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000061

Early life nutrition and neural plasticity

Michael K Georgieff 1, Katya E Brunette 2, Phu V Tran 3
PMCID: PMC4443711  NIHMSID: NIHMS658298  PMID: 25997762

Abstract

The human brain undergoes a remarkable transformation during fetal life and the first postnatal years from a relatively undifferentiated but pluripotent organ to a highly specified and organized one. The outcome of this developmental maturation is highly dependent on a sequence of environmental exposures that can have either positive or negative influences on the ultimate plasticity of the adult brain. Many environmental exposures are beyond the control of the individual, but nutrition is not. An ever-increasing amount of research demonstrates that nutrition not only shapes the brain and affects its function during development, but that several nutrients early in life have profound and long-lasting effects on the brain. Nutrients have been shown to alter opening and closing of critical and sensitive periods of particular brain regions. This paper discusses the roles that various nutrients play in shaping the developing brain, concentrating specifically on recently explicated biological mechanisms by which particularly salient nutrients influence childhood and adult neural plasticity.

I. Introduction

The human brain grows rapidly and differentiates extensively during the late fetal period and the first 3 postnatal years (Thompson & Nelson, 2001). While brain development continues across the lifespan and its form and function is modified by experience, the opportunity to influence its later function appears to be far greater during early life than in adulthood. The concept of neural plasticity is necessarily interwoven into the discussion of how malleable the brain is during different times of life. While multiple definitions of neural plasticity exist, fundamentally it can be thought of how readily the brain responds to either positive or negative stimuli, and as a consequence, whether long-term effects remain after the stimulus is removed.

Many external factors affect early brain development. Among those factors, nutrition is key for two reasons. First, neurodevelopment is a highly metabolically taxing process. The brain consumes 60% of the energy utilized by the newborn human, a figure far greater than other mammals (Kuzawa, 1998). Thus, optimal brain development is dependent on key nutrients such as glucose, branched chain amino acids, oxygen and iron that directly support cellular metabolism and ultimately cell differentiation (Wullschleger, Lewith & Hall, 2006). Second, nutrition is a factor that can be willfully altered. Thus, there is a golden opportunity to improve human brain formation and development outcome by leveraging knowledge about which nutrients to provide at which particular time(s).

Before embarking on a discussion of neural plasticity as it relates to nutrition during brain development, this paper will first consider the interactions among three concepts: critical/sensitive periods, neural plasticity, and developmental origins of adult (mental) health and disease (DOHaD). The biology of critical/sensitive periods and their influence on enhancing or reducing neural plasticity will then be discussed in order to anchor the discussion of nutrient effects on neural plasticity in neuroanatomy and neurometabolism. The pre-clinical and clinical evidence for critical periods for nutrients in early brain development will be presented and followed by a deeper probing of two nutrients, iron and choline, and their roles in neural plasticity and long-term brain function.

II. Critical Periods and Sensitive Periods as Opportunities in Child Neurodevelopment

Critical and sensitive periods during brain development are salient opportunities for environmental stimuli to shape the child’s brain. These periods are typically characterized by a high degree of neuronal plasticity, a fact that contributes to the saliency of that opportunity (Hensch, 2004). They occur early in life, apparently spanning fetal and early postnatal epochs and the events that occur during these periods influence brain function across the lifespan and perhaps transgenerationally; a concept now referred to as DOHaD (Hanson & Gluckman, 2004).

What is the biological advantage of critical or sensitive periods? Teleologically, one might argue that they provide the opportunity for the appearance of new phenotypes emanating from an otherwise relatively common genetic background; thus giving an opportunity for evolution and species development. At an individual level, what happens during these periods differentiates us as individuals and can, to a great extent, determine how resilient or rigid we are across the lifespan.

Much has been made of the terms critical or sensitive periods (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Lerner, 2011; Plato). There is general agreement that critical periods refer to time-periods when the brain’s response to environmental stimuli (either the presence/absence of necessary stimuli or exposure to noxious stimuli) results in irreversible long-term neurobehavioral effects (Bornstein, 1989). One thinks of the classic experiments where lack of visual stimulation results in failure of ocular dominance columns to form properly during development, resulting in permanent visual impairment (Weisel & Hubel, 1963). No amount of later stimulation (outside of the critical period) could restore normalvision (although see below for newer thinking about the biology of this phenomenon). Similarly, normal bird song does not evolve unless the fledgling is provided with the father’s song during a critical time period (Balmer et al, 2009). The harsh reality is that such defined critical periods exist and seem particularly present during the embryologic and anatomic development of the fetal and early postnatal brain. Critical periods appear less prominently in the older developing organism, to some extent replaced by sensitive periods.

Sensitive periods can be conceptualized a “softer version” of critical periods (Armstrong et al, 2006; Bornstein, 1989; Columbo 1982; Johnson, 2005; Michel & Tyler, 2005). Rather than the more strict concept of critical periods where failure to construct the brain normally is irrevocable, sensitive periods describe epochs where the brain is particularly receptive to stimuli usually over a broader period of time. This enchanced receptivity represents an opportunity to construct a “better” (or worse) brain during a time period where one gets more effect for a given stimulus than in other time periods. Nutrient and social stimulation effects operate largely within the sensitive period conceptualization, although there are examples of nutrients early in fetal life that obey critical period laws. Neural plasticity is greater during a sensitive period than before or after the period, but the system remains malleable throughout development. Thus, sensitive periods can be conceptualized as an “enhanced opportunity” for neural plasticity during development as opposed to critical periods, which act with greater determinism with respect to long-term neurobehavioral outcomes.

It is important to maintain definitional integrity when discussing the role of critical or sensitive periods and their role in modulating neural plasticity. Most commonly, critical periods are thought of in terms of the timing of brain events (eg, neurogenesis, differentiation) (Hensch, 2004). As is discussed in section III, the brain is not a homogenous organ; rather, it consists of multiple regions, each with differently timed developmental onsets, trajectories and maturational completion (Thompson & Nelson, 2001). Thus, critical or sensitive periods of growth and differentiation will vary by brain region, by age and by neuronal process (including plasticity). Because environmental factors clearly mold the brain differentially across the lifespan, they can also be conceptualized as demonstrating sensitive (and possibly, critical) periods where they exert more influence. Nutrition, social stimulation, and stress are all important environmental molders of the brain that exhibit critical or sensitive periods of effects (Wachs, et al, 2014).

III. The Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology of Critical Periods in the Brain: Early Life Mechanisms that Inform Adult Neural Plasticity

It has been 20 years since Cicchetti and Tucker (1994) published the landmark issue on neural plasticity in Development and Psychopathology. That forward looking issue summarized many of the important biobehavioral concepts known at the time regarding developmental plasticity and the role of sensitive periods in shaping short and long-term behavioral outcomes. An explosion of research subsequent to that issue has provided substantive cellular and molecular proof of the cellular underpinnings of neural plasticity and provided mechanisms of neurobiologial plasticity. Section III discusses the findings and demonstrates that the seminal observations reported 20 years ago are grounded in a solid and tractable biology.

Development is a time of rapid growth and differentiation. Because the brain is not a homogenous structure the requirements for metabolites/nutrients that support development are distributed regionally to the areas of most rapid growth at a given time in life. Brain regions (eg, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex) begin as highly undifferentiated, but pluripotent regions. In their nascent form, they have low efficiency and poor specificity of function, but are more capable of recovery from insults than the older, more highly differentiated brain (Hensch, 2004). This capacity for recovery has been utilized as one definition of plasticity. As development of a brain region progresses, it becomes more specialized functionally but loses a large amount of its ability to recover from insults. This loss of recoverability can be characterized as a loss of potential plasticity. The period of rapid development during which the region undergoes this maturational process and is most malleable (positively or negatively) is often defined as a critical or sensitive period (see above).

Specifically, the greater potential inherent in phases of early cell determination and early neuronal connectivity confers greater need for appropriate energy substrates, environmental stimuli, and gene expression. A developing area is at risk when any of these three aspects are compromised. The opening of a critical or sensitive period of regional brain development is often marked by an uptick in metabolic demand because of the high-energy cost of growth. Cellular signaling pathways such as the mammalian Target of Rapamycin (mTOR) pathway act as sensors of critical substrate availability and subsequently modulate the neuron’s fate (Wullschleger et al, 2006). The fate ranges from autophagy/apoptosis at the negative end of the spectrum to complexity of neuronal structure on the other (Jaworski et al, 2005). The synaptic efficacy of a hippocampal CA1 neuron is defined (generally) by the complexity of its dendritic structure. mTOR signaling senses the availability of critical nutrients such as specific amino acids, iron, oxygen, and growth factors and uses this information to regulate actin polymerization and protein translation rates (Knox et al, 2007). For example, the period from postnatal day (P) 15 to P25 in the rodent hippocampus is a period of rapid dendritogenesis (Fretham et al, 2011; Pokorny & Yamamoto, 1981) and is heralded by increased energy utilization (Dallman & Schwartz, 1964), iron transporter expression (Siddappa, et al, 2002), growth factor expression (Tran et al, 2008) and maturation of its electrophysiology (Bekenstein & Lothman, 1991).

The understanding of the biology of critical periods and neural plasticity has undergone a tremendous metamorphosis in the past 10 years. Much of the classic work on critical periods has been done in the visual system (Wiesel & Hubel, 1963). However, the general principles are likely similar across other brain areas; therefore much of the following refers to general critical period plasticity with supporting examples drawn from the visual system where there has been more research.

Formation of ocular dominance columns requires plasticity for appropriate function, yet greater plasticity during development also makes this and other systems more vulnerable to potential insults that occur during this period of increased responsiveness. This general principle of increased vulnerability, but also recoverability will be discussed in the context of early iron deficiency and neural plasticity in the latter section of this article.

What neurobiological qualities does a sensitive or critical period have during normal development? How does a developing system compare to an adult system in terms of plasticity? How does early environmental deprivation (including nutrient deprivation) alter plasticity? Neural circuit refinement and onset and duration of critical periods are dependent on electrical activity (both experience-independent and experience-dependent), cross talk between diverse molecular signaling cascades, and inhibition. Structural consolidation also occurs during critical periods but eventually leads to closing of the critical period (Hensch, 2004). The general connections between populations of neurons to connect various brain areas are made early in development. Therefore structural plasticity in adults generally is characterized by relatively subtle refinements of dendrite branching and synapse modulation when compared to earlier in development.

Plasticity is not limited to the developing organism. Adult plasticity may also use similar mechanisms to those used in early development, but is often conceptualized separately from early life plasticity in the literature. New discoveries investigating adult plasticity in the visual system of rodents show re-opening of early critical period plasticity with pharmacologic or genetic modification, suggesting that more remains to be elucidated regarding limits and properties of adult plasticity (Sugiyama & Hensch, 2008).

The visual system has been studied extensively as a model of early life critical period-dependent neural plasticity. Comparisons to this model system have lead our group to the postulate that similar neurobiologic events occur during early life iron deficiency and that these events alter hippocampal plasticity during early development with residual changes extending into adulthood.

During development of the visual system, spontaneous action potentials from cholinergic neurotransmission begin to shape the system with electrical and chemical activity (Wong, 1999). Even when the eyes of newborn animals are closed, in the absence of relevant stimuli, development continues. Based on lack of light stimuli it could be argued that the neurons of the visual system are programmed, through time dependent (versus activity or experience dependent) activation of early gene expression, to begin producing neurites, generating action potentials, and receiving chemical and electrical input. However, it is important to realize that previous events have shaped these actions in a kind of internal ‘experience’. These previous events include expression of genes that determine the anterior/posterior axis of the nervous system, homeobox gene gradients signaling populations of cells to become more specialized and to connect to other specific populations, and neurogenesis of cells bound for proximal or distal locations (as determined by their expression of specific external receptors and the internal and external milieu of signaling molecules). Cell to cell surface receptor interactions help shape cell fate (Artivanis-Tsakonas, et al, 1999). Prior to ‘determination’, the input these cells receive includes varied levels of delta/notch signaling produced by interaction with surrounding cells. Cell ‘fate’ becomes determined due to location-specific interactions and many of the new cells will become support cells for the sensory neurons.

When a given cell migrates and reaches its target area and is committed to becoming a neuron, it must send out neurites to connect with other neurons. Migration and stop signals for reaching and identifying the target location are controlled by growth factors such as brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and guidance cue gradients such as stromal cell derived factor 1(SDF1) (Wong & Ghosh, 2002). After reaching the target location, activity dependent refinement of connections begins. The response to activity may include new gene expression for and relocalization of proteins such as transmembrane receptors, actin binding proteins, and postsynaptic density elements such as Calcium calmodulin Kinase IIα (CaMKIIα) and post synaptic density 95 (PSD-95), all of which will lead to functional changes (Wong & Ghosh, 2002). Early refinement often includes pruning back of exuberant axon and dendrite growth. In the case of the visual cortex, the ocular dominance columns are formed with light stimulated activity to result in segregated and biased (greater) input from one eye (Hensch, 2004). As will be discussed later, the expression of BDNF, SDF-1, CamKIIa, and PSD-95 are acutely and chronically altered by early life ID (Carlson, et al, 2007; Tran, et al, 2009). BDNF is of particular interest because it is a gene that is epigenetically modifiable by early life events (Roth, et al, 2009).

The balance between excitatory and inhibitory tone is an important determinant of the receptivity of the brain to external events during development. For example, the balance is altered during the beginning of the critical period for formation of ocular dominance columns, classically observed by Wiesel and Hubel in 1963 (Wiesel & Hubel, 1963). In the visual system, inhibitory neurotransmission (GABA) increases as the system consolidates and matures functionally (Isaacson & Scanziani, 2011). Eventually, closure of the critical period is accompanied by the appearance of consolidating perineuronal nets, an extracellular matrix that wraps around parvalbumin (PV) secreting basket cells (Celio et al, 1998; Markram, et al, 2004; Morishita & Hensch, 2008; Sugiyama et al, 2008). Thus, the maturity of GABAergic interneurons and the presence of perineuronal nets correspond with the closure of the critical period. The presence of neurite inhibitors in the extracellular milieu, such as myelin-associated glycoprotein (MAG) and Nogo, secreted by glial cells also contribute to closing the critical period and prevent re-establishing plasticity in the adult, such as regrowth after injury (Morishita and Hensch et al. 2008; Sugiyama, et al 2008; Yiu and He, 2006). In experiments using monocular deprivation and re-expression of Otx2, a homeoprotein, which plays a role in PV cell maturation, reactivation of ocular dominance column neural plasticity in adulthood occurs (Sugiyama, et al, 2008). If visual ocular dominance critical periods can be manipulated to reopen in adulthood, restoration of proper ocular dominance after impaired development is the next logical step.

The idea of re-inducing plasticity levels similar to those observed in early life in adult animals has profound implications (Donato, et al, 2013). These include a potential major therapeutic role in rescuing abnormal development and age related deterioration, as well as reorienting how we conceive of experiments using adult animals. Classical adult plasticity, where the animal shows changes in learning paradigms mostly involves dendritic spine changes which may or may not be accompanied by electrical long term potentiation (LTP) changes based on activity of glutamatergic receptors, with strengthening or weakening in a Hebbian manner (Hebb, 1949). Traditionally, by adulthood, the potential for dramatic change is very small. Many recent studies suggest there may be more to adult plasticity than previously thought, particularly in terms of long term changes in gene expression.

Epigenetic modifications of genes involved in synaptic plasticity (eg, BDNF) are another set of mechanisms that underlie greater or lesser neuronal plasticity beyond manipulation of critical period openings and closings (Huang, et al, 1999). Epigenetic modifications include DNA methylation (eg, of CpG islands), histone methylation and histone acetylation. These changes can occur as a result of early life environment stress (Meaney, et al, 2000), but recently has been shown to be induced by learning paradigms in adult animals (Lubin, et al, 2008). These studies demonstrate a view of adult plasticity driven by methylation and acetylation of DNA and histones that alter gene expression adult rat hippocampus following contextual fear conditioning (Lubin, et al, 2008). Early adverse maternal care remodels chromatin of the offspring well into adulthood, and the altered maternal care behavior is passed on to the next generation presumably due to permanently altered gene expression (Roth, et al, 2009).

The classic example of an early life event that causes effects on the developing brain through epigenetic mechanisms is perinatal stress (Meaney, et al, 2000). Chronic stress during sensitive developmental periods alters the regulation of the stress response as an adult. Dams will lick and groom their pups less if under severe stress, and this mothering style is ‘passed on’ to female offspring, presumably through epigenetic modification of genes encoding glucocorticoid receptor genes in the original generation of offspring (Silveira, et al, 2007). Rat mothers who show higher licking and grooming of their pups confer on them a less reactive stress response (Liu, et al, 1997), making them more resilient to stress and often passing on the high licking and grooming parenting behavior to their offspring. It is thought that this maternal care activates serotonergic pathways which increase growth factors that act to increase the GR promoter activity. Increased GR promoter activity results in a more efficient negative feedback and less reactive pups (Meaney, et al 2000; Silveira, et al, 2007). Increased histone acetylation and demethylation may underlie this upregulation of GR genes by facilitating the binding of transcription factors at their promoters. (Carvin, et al, 2003; Encio, et al, 1991;). Interestingly, the hippocampus, particularly CA1 pyramidal neurons, expresses a high concentration of GR receptors (deKloet, et al, 1998); dysregulation of the stress response could have effects on hippocampal function, potentially altering learning and memory (Bagot, et al, 2009). Adult offspring from low licking and grooming (LG) mothers have decreased branch lengths and spine density in CA1 hippocampal neurons, and reduced long term potentiation, a cellular basis of learning and memory, under basal conditions (Champagne, et al, 2008).

In the same way that the neonatal brain is more vulnerable to the positive or negative effects of non-nutritional factors (eg, stress; social support) (for review, see Wachs, et al, 2014), it may also be more responsive to nutritional manipulation. High cell proliferation during the fetal-neonatal period means these new cells will benefit from supplements or increased substrate as they grow, or be negatively affected by substrate deficits. These positive or negative effects appear to affect neural plasticity through three, likely inter-related, mechanisms: 1) direct effects, for example on the complexity of dendritic arbors, which in turn correlate with greater neural capacity and greater synaptic plasticity; 2) modulation of the timing of onset and closure of critical periods of regional brain development; and 3) epigenetic modification of genes involved in synaptic plasticity. The following section discusses which nutrients exhibit one or more of these characteristics in early life, while the final section discusses two nutrients, iron and choline, where there are substantial mechanistic data to support the idea that early life nutrient deficits (eg, iron deficiency) or supplements (eg, choline) regulate adult neural plasticity capacity.

In summary, basic cellular and molecular mechanisms modulate the degree of neural plasticity in developing neural systems. Some of these mechanisms remain active into adulthood, albeit at a lesser activity level. The ability to reactivate these mechanisms in adulthood holds great promise to reverse long-term insults that occurred due to abnormal events during fetal and early postnatal life. Utilization of these principles may prove useful to rescue other long-term developmental deficits, such as altered hippocampal dendrite plasticity induced by early life malnutrition. Indeed, understanding the basic biology of how neural plasticity is modulated during critical or sensitive periods likely will lead to innovative (and previously unconsidered) solutions to solve previously unsolvable long-term deficits. For example, if an early life environmental event reduced neural plasticity through epigenetic mechanisms (vide infra), there is no guarantee that simply restoring the specific environmental stimulus will result in a full reversion. This is observed with early life iron deficiency, where prompt treatment after diagnosis does not prevent all long-term disabilities. However, treatment of the underlying epigenetic modifications with agents that modify DNA methylation (eg, choline) or histone acetylation patterns has shown promise in restoring adult neuronal plasticity and behavioral phenotype (Kennedy et al, 2014; Lucassen, et al, 2013).

IV. Nutrients that exhibit sensitive or critical periods

All nutrients are important for brain development, but some exhibit far more influence than others during late fetal/neonatal life and early childhood. These nutrients affect fundamental processes of early brain development (Table 1). The fetal and early postnatal brain in particular has high nutritional requirements because of its high metabolic rate. The neonatal brain’s oxygen consumption rate is 60% of the body’s total (Kuzawa, 1998). Substrates that support this high basal metabolic rate include glucose, amino acids, iron, zinc and oxygen (Fretham et al., 2011). Thus, a constant flow of these particular nutrients is critical for normal brain development and interruption of this flow typically results in acute, and often chronic, brain dysfunction. Since the brain is not a homogenous organ, but a set of different regions and processes all with different developmental trajectories, the nutrient requirements are not homogenously distributed across the entire brain. At any given time, regions (eg, hippocampus, striatum, prefrontal cortex) are differentially sensitive to the positive or negative effects of a given nutrient based on the regions’ need for the nutrient at that point in development (Rice & Barone, 2000). The effect of a nutrient on brain regional development is a function of the timing of supplementation/deficit of the nutrient and the dose/duration of the perturbation (Kretchmer, et al., 1996). Dose/duration in this context can be thought of as a single entity in the form of an “area under the curve”.

Table 1.

Examples of Nutrients that Affect Early Life Brain Development

Nutrient Brain Requirement for
Nutrient
Affected Brain Region or
Process
Protein-Energy Cell Proliferation,
Cell Differentiation
Synaptogenesis,
Growth Factors
Global
Cortex
Hippocampus
Iron Myelin
Dopamine
Energy
White Matter
Striatal-Frontal
Hippocampal-Frontal
Zinc DNA
Neurotransmitter
release
Autonomic NS
Hippocampus
Cerebellum
LC-PUFAs Synaptogenesis
Myelin
Eye
Cortex

While it should not be surprising that deficits of nutrients that support fundamental brain metabolism result in acute brain dysfunction, it is the occurrence of long-term brain/behavior deficits in spite of treatment of the underlying nutrient deficiency that supports the idea of sensitive or critical periods for particular nutrients and particular brain areas. A number of nutrients demonstrate this property early in life in humans and in pre-clinical models (Table 2). The cost to society of missing critical periods of nutrient administration on long-term brain health is not trivial (Walker et al, 2007). Fully 2 billion people (1/3 of the world’s population) are iron deficient. This most common nutrient deficiency has a particular tropism for pregnant women, their fetuses and their young offspring. Rates of deficiency in low and middle income countries approach 80%. Iron deficiency also induces a hypothyroid state, compounding the potential damage to the developing brain. Similarly, zinc deficiency affects an extraordinary number of people; by some estimates close to 1.8 billion people. This nutrient deficiency is usually co-morbid with protein deficiency, again resulting in a compounding effect. Finally, iodine deficiency affects at least 600 million people world-wide and is the cause of cretinism.

Table 2.

Nutrients that particularly affect early brain development and demonstrate a critical or sensitive period

  • Macronutrients
    • -
      Protein*
    • -
      Specific fats (e.g. LC-PUFAs)*
    • -
      Glucose
  • Micronutrients
    • -
      Zinc*
    • -
      Copper*
    • -
      Iodine (Thyroid)*
    • -
      Iron*
    • -
      Selenium
  • Vitamins/Cofactors
    • -
      B vitamins (B6, B12)
    • -
      Vitamin A
    • -
      Vitamin K
    • -
      Folate*
    • -
      Choline *
*

Denotes nutrient that meets the principles for demonstrating critical or sensitive period during development.

All of these nutrients are particularly at risk for deficiency in the fetus, young infant and child because of the enormous requirements of the developing brain (for review, see Fuglestad, et al, 2010). Elimination of these micronutrient deficiencies has been estimated to have the potential to increase the world’s IQ by 10 points, with the consequent shift of job potential in the positive direction (Walker et al, 2007). Beyond neurocognitive effects, early deficits of several of these nutrients have been associated with significant adult psychopathology. For example, the risk of adult-onset schizophrenia in offspring of iron deficient mothers is directly proportional to the degree of maternal iron deficiency in pregnancy (Insel et al, 2008). Early life iron deficiency also results in greater anxiety and depression in adulthood with consequent loss of job potential (Lukowski et al, 2010).

Attribution of temporally distal neurobehavioral outcomes to early nutritional deficits (thus confirming a critical or sensitive period effect on neural plasticity) remains a difficult problem in human populations. The main concern is the lack of certainty that a behavioral abnormality documented some years after the nutritional deficit is truly due to the nutrient and not to other confounding variables that are co-morbid with the deficit (eg, poverty, stress) or that occurred subsequent to the deficit but before the assessment. Common early life nutrient deficiencies are rarely fatal and thus demonstration of an effect at the brain tissue level is not possible. Instead, a layered, multi-disciplinary approach utilizing developmentally appropriate pre-clinical models is used to provide a plausible biological proof of long-term effects since anatomic, spectroscopic or functional neuroimaging techniques are typically not sensitive enough to detect specific nutrient deficits in the developing brain. An example of this multi-layered approach confirming the effect of early life iron deficiency on the developing declarative learning and memory system is provided later in this paper.

With that caveat in mind, studies of young humans that suggest critical periods for nutrients exist can be sorted into two groups: prevention/supplementation trials and observational/interventional trials. The latter are far more common than the former. Prevention/supplementation trials involve randomization of a nutritionally at risk population to receive a nutrient supplement or placebo. While some of these trials show positive results in the supplemented group (Christian et al, 2010), others do not (Christian et al, 2011). The temptation to dismiss the possibility that early nutrition affects long-term brain development in the face of null studies is great. However, when considered carefully, the reasons that intervention studies can produce null results often provide support for the concept of sensitive/critical periods for nutrient-brain interactions (Cusick & Georgieff, 2012; Wachs et al, 2014).

Assessment of outcomes in interventional nutritional trials must follow certain developmental biological principles to be valid (Wachs et al, 2014). First, the nutritional intervention must occur at a time when the brain or selected brain region has a high demand for the nutrient being supplemented. Mistiming of the intervention, eg, provision of the nutrient during a low demand state, will not result in a detectable effect. Second, the target population must have a reasonable risk of being deficient of the supplemented nutrient. There is little evidence that supplementation of an already sufficient individual achieves superior brain development (an exception may be choline-see below). Conversely, a population that is extremely nutrient deficient may not benefit from a small increase in nutrient delivery. The dose must match the proposed deficit, a fact rarely factored into supplementation trials. Third, the outcome behaviors to be tested must map onto the proposed neural structures that are influenced by the nutrient. Since many nutrient effects are highly specific in their effects, general neurodevelopmental assessments typically used in large-scale studies because they are the lowest common denominator that can be performed across all the sites lack the sensitivity to identify those effects. The behavioral effect size should be of the scale seen behaviorally and anatomically in developmentally appropriate preclinical models. Fourth, the assessments should be administered within a reasonable time frame of the intervention in order to reduce the possibility of post-intervention confounding variables. However, herein lies the conundrum with respect to plasticity. Many early developing systems, while highly vulnerable to nutritional insults, demonstrate a remarkable degree of plasticity and functional recoverability (Riggins et al, 2009; Townsend et al, 2005). While this is good news for the developing child, the problem becomes one of a moving target for assessment and intervention. In the case of fetal iron deficiency, hippocampally based abnormalities of declarative memory are prominent at birth (Siddappa et al, 2004) and at 3.5 years of age (Riggins et al, 2009), but disappear by 5 years (Townsend et al, 2005). In early adolescence, learning and memory problems are not seen, but prefrontally-based planning and inhibition abnormalities appear. The etiology of this shift in domain over time is unclear, but the phenomenon certainly influences how investigators consider their choices of long-term assessments. Failure to choose the correct domain may lead to erroneous conclusions about critical periods, nutritional effects and developmental plasticity.

In spite of these caveats, the importance of nutritional timing and its relationship to critical or sensitive periods for growth and nutrition has been supported by multiple positive prevention/supplementation studies in humans (Cusick & Georgieff, 2013; Walker et al, 2007). From a macronutrient perspective, growth velocity prior to 1 year of age is a strong predictor of IQ at 9 years of age (Pongcharoen et al, 2012). Consistent with other literature on the relationship between linear growth (or its inverse condition, stunting) and outcome (Ramel et al, 2012), linear growth at birth and in the first year had a stronger association with 9 year IQ than weight gain. In contrast, growth between 1 and 9 years had no relationship to IQ. Similarly, a trial of fetal supplementation of iron improved working memory, inhibitory control and fine motor abilities at 7-9 years (Christian et al, 2010), but supplementation between 12 and 36 months had no effect when given to the group that received placebo in the fetal supplementation trial and had no additional positive effect in the group that received fetal iron (Murray-Kolb et al, 2012).

Given the high prevalence of nutrient deficiencies in children world-wide, it is not surprising a very large number of observational/interventional trials document their negative effects on brain development and their response to therapy (for summary, see Walker, et al, 2007). This literature as a whole has been criticized for the inability of investigators to control known and unknown potential confounding variables (eg, poverty, toxic stress). Nevertheless, the sheer number of studies on each nutrient that documents consistent changes in populations from around the world makes it difficult to ignore their conclusions. Table 3 provides examples of long-term outcomes drawn from these trials for a number of salient early life nutrients. For each nutrient and time, developmentally sensitive pre-clinical models provide a biological plausibility for the underlying biology of the long-term effects on adult synaptic plasticity (for review, see Fuglestad et al, 2010).

Table 3.

Examples of the importance of timing of nutrient deficits on long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes

Nutrient/Child Age High risk
for deficiency?
Period of high brain
demand
Long-term
Neurodevelopmental
Impact after Resolution
of Nutrient Deficit?
Protein/ Fetus Yes due to maternal
malnutrition;
maternal
hypertension
3rd trimester Lower IQ at age 7
years (Pylipow, et al, 2009)
Protein/ Child Variable based on
growth rates
6 months-10 years Yes, cognition (Pollitt, et al, 1993)
Long Chain Poly-
unsaturated Fatty Acids/
Fetus & Neonate
Yes since fetus and
neonate cannot
synthesize de novo
3rd trimester- 2
months postnatal
Lower Bayley Scales at
18 months; Slower
neural processing
Iron/ Fetus Yes due to maternal
anemia, diabetes in
pregnancy, maternal
smoking
3rd trimester Impaired recognition
and working memory
(Riggins et al, 2009)
Iron/ Infant & Toddler Yes due to rapid
growth, poor dietary
sources, intestinal
blood loss
6 months-24 months Increased hesitancy,
wariness, motor
abnormalities, slower
electrical conduction;
increased depression
(Shafir et al, 2008;
Alagarin et al, 2003;
Lukowski et al, 2010)
Iron/ Teenager Yes in females due to
onset of menses
No No
Zinc/ Fetus Yes due to maternal
zinc deficiency
Yes Decreased novelty
preference as toddler
Iodine/1st trimester fetus Yes based on
maternal thyroid/I
status
Yes Profound mental
deficits
Iodine/3rd trimester-early
childhood
Yes due to endemic I
deficiency in food
source
Yes Reduced verbal IQ;
Decreased Reaction
Time

Iron and Choline as Paradigms of Nutritional Modulation of Neural Plasticity

Iron as a paradigm of the effects of nutrient deficiency during development

There are two fundamental mechanisms by which a nutrient’s status early in life can affect neural plasticity during development and have an impact on long-term brain function. Early life iron deficiency serves as a good paradigm to explore both (Georgieff, 2008).

The first mechanism is that structural deficits induced during development may persist into adulthood (Brunette et al. 2010). The synaptic efficacy and plasticity of hippocampal neurons is a function of the complexity of its dendritic arbors, particularly in area CA1. Fetal/neonatal iron deficiency significantly compromises the integrity of the arbor accompanied by reduced expression of synaptic plasticity genes that regulate actin polymerization such as Profilin 1 and 2; Cofilin-1, BDNF and SDF-1 (Brunette et al, 2010; Carlson et al, 2007). Subsequent to treatment, the formerly iron deficient hippocampus in the adult rodent has a 14% loss in volume, a truncated dendritic arbor, persistent changes in synaptic plasticity and structural genes, and astrocytic gliosis (Brunette et al, 2010; Carlson et al, 2007; Fretham et al, 2012; Rao et al, 2011). In short, the lack of iron during a period of rapid dendritogenesis early in life results in a poorly constructed hippocampus that underperforms because of its abnormal structure in spite of treatment (Fretham et al, 2012). The formerly iron deficient hippocampus generates less long-term potentiation in slice culture preparations (Jorgenson et al, 2005; Pisansky et al, 2013) and has reduced expression of multiple synaptic genes/proteins (Brunette et al, 2010).

If gene expression important for structural growth and plasticity is altered short-term, this disrupted regulation would inhibit learning, but not prevent it entirely. The decreased transcript levels for actin regulatory genes suggest a decreased plasticity in the actin cytoskeleton. These molecules are needed in early development to grow and remodel the apical arbor as well as for spine development and maintenance. Their acute reduction changes inherent structural capabilities in addition to possibly altering their regulation long term (see below). Proteins affecting actin polymerization and depolymerization include the RhoGTPases -RhoA, Rac1, and Cdc42 and their downstream effectors - Cypin, Proflin-1, Profilin-2, and Cofilin-1. Collapsin response mediator protein1 (Crmp1) expression is also decreased by early iron deficiency anemia. It is an upstream effector of neurite guidance localized to dendrites acting in the semaphorin pathway (Bretin et al, 2005; Tang, 2003) (Brunette et al, 2010). It is small wonder that the formerly iron deficient animal performs poorly on hippocampally dependent spatial memory mazes (Felt and Lozoff, 1996; Kennedy et al, 2014; Schmidt et al, 2007).

The second more intriguing hypothesis is an induction of permanent dysregulation of synaptic plasticity genes by early life iron deficiency through epigenetic chromatin modifications. Evidence for this possibility comes from the documented suppression on adult rat BDNF gene and protein expression and their downstream effectors by early life iron deficiency (Blegen et al, 2014; Brunette et al, 2010; Tran et al, 2009). The evidence supporting the epigenetically-driven dysregulation hypothesis includes the finding that BDNF is epigenetically modifiable by early life events (Lubin et al, 2010; Roth, et al, 2009), that a set of iron containing proteins (JARIDs) that regulate histone methylation status of BDNF are altered in adulthood by early life deficiency (Blegen et al, 2013) and that provision of the methyl donor choline in the maternal diet reverses these abnormalities and rescues behavior (Kennedy et al, 2014).

Both potential mechanisms strongly support a developmental origins of adult mental health pathogenesis. Both mechanisms speak to a sensitive period where proper nutrient supply is critical to hippocampal development. The two mechanisms, of course, are not mutually exclusive.

As noted above, gestational and early postnatal iron deficiency cause multiple long term effects in spite of prompt iron repletion in infancy, implying a critical or sensitive period for iron dependent neuronal processes. Plasticity is necessary for rescue treatments (e.g. iron) to work in order to develop a fully intact organism at maturity. However, it should also be noted that adverse events (e.g. iron deficiency) generally have a greater impact when they occur early in development compared with later in life because of the rapidity and scale of change occurring during neurodevelopment. The impact of an early insult might also trigger adaptive responses important for short-term survival or for reaching reproductive maturity even though the insult will also result in long-term deficits. The specific deficits that occur (especially in response to limited resource availability) will be dependent on which developmental processes were occurring at the time of iron deficiency and how each process was prioritized to receive the limited amount of iron available to the brain.

Altered adult plasticity due to early life iron deficiency may be influenced by a developmental sensitive period. A recent study shows that fetal/neonatal iron deficiency alters the cellular and molecular dynamics of critical period opening and closure for dendrite growth and differentiation in the hippocampus, thus potentially explaining the altered dynamics of neural plasticity observed in electrophysiology and behavioral experiments (Callahan et al, 2013; Gewirtz et al, 2008; Jorgenson et al, 2005; Tran et al, 2008). These experiments demonstrate that iron deficiency that occurs at the time of rapid hippocampal dendritic arborization in the rodent (postnatal days 7-30) delays the appearance of parvalbumin positive GABAergic cells, effectively causing a developmental delay in hippocampal maturation and specification. This “shift to the right” of the time line, however, can also be viewed as a potential opportunity for maintaining the plasticity of the system by extending the time window. Theoretically, if iron were to become available during this time extension, long-term effects may be attenuated or abrogated. The electrophysiologic evidence that supports this concept with iron deficiency is that the iron deficient postnatal day 30 hippocampus has the long-term potentiation maturity pattern of the postnatal day 15 iron sufficient hippocampus (Jorgenson et al, 2005). A similar retention of a more immature pattern is seen in fear-potentiated trace conditioning, a hippocampally mediated behavior (Gewirtz et al, 2008).

The unfortunate aspect of early life iron deficiency with respect to critical period dynamics and neural plasticity is that, although the opening is delayed, the closure occurs more abruptly and more completely (Callahan et al, 2013). Perineuronal nets, which herald the closure of the critical period, are more abundant in the hippocampus of the formerly iron deficient young adult, a finding that implies a less plastic adult system (Callahan et al, 2013). Electrophysiology experiments demonstrate less long-term potentiation in the adult (Jorgenson et al, 2005; Pisansky et al, 2013) accompanied by poorer performance on hippocampus-mediated behavioral tasks such as the Morris water maze (Felt & Lozoff, 1996) and the win-shift radial arm maze (Schmidt et al, 2007). Nevertheless, restoration of neuronal iron in a prompt manner during the proposed critical period in a genetic mouse model with isolated, reversible hippocampal iron deficiency results in preservation of parvalbumin expression, BDNF expression, and adult neuronal plasticity (Fretham et al, 2012). This finding demonstrates that the system remains malleable during its critical period, preserving the potential of normal adult synaptic plasticity.

Understanding the timing and mechanisms involved in the onset and offset of critical periods would inform the type and timing of clinical treatments for nutrient deficient infants. Altered regulation of the excitatory and inhibitory tone throughout development suggests a potential target for treatment or prevention of deficits due to iron deficiency. Administration of fluoxetine, which increases BDNF expression, and infusion of BDNF itself have been demonstrated to increase excitation and cause a corresponding shift in the critical period in the visual cortex (Huang et al, 1999; Sugiyama et al, 2008). Therefore, the decrease in BDNF at postnatal day 15 in the iron deficient rat indicates a loss of excitatory tone during differentiation, which remains depressed long after iron repletion. This decrease during a period of high growth may disrupt the development of the circuitry used to create plasticity for learning and memory, causing the deficits observed after iron repletion. It is plausible that alteration of the excitatory and inhibitory balance due to early IDA may significantly disrupt plasticity and prevent full maturation of the learning circuitry, resulting in long-term cognitive and behavioral deficits.

In summary, functional deficits that have been previously described in humans and preclinical models of ID may be due to loss of neural plasticity through regulation of actin filament remodeling factors. An example of this deficit using Hebb’s model of synaptic potentiation would show a less responsive system overall (Hebb, 1949). Specifically, if synapses that were supposed to be pruned were not completely eliminated, only reduced in strength, and those that were supposed to be strengthened were not as responsive to electrical activity, there would be less efficient synapses overall. This reduced synaptic efficacy demonstrated in electrophysiology experiments by Jorgenson et al, (2005) inspires future experiments examining maintenance and molding of the synapses during iron deficiency. Overall, we postulate that there are less efficient synapses with less mature spines in ID rats during development, as determined by decreased spine head diameter during early differentiation (Brunette et al, 2010). The altered dendrite branches may be less able to respond to growth and guidance factors and maintain their appropriate structure into adulthood. With decreased availability of actin and tubulin binding proteins important for cytoskeletal regulation during adulthood, dendrites and spines may be less capable of dynamic responses to activity further reducing plasticity dependent learning as adults in spite of complete iron repletion (Jorgenson et al, 2005; McEchron et al, 2005; Schmidt et al, 2007).

Ultimately, structural changes seen in CA1 apical dendrites are likely to result in altered connectivity within the hippocampus and among circuits that depend on hippocampal integrity (eg, the VTA loop). The latter circuit is particularly interesting given the epidemiologic data demonstrating a relationship between the degree of maternal-fetal iron deficiency during pregnancy and the later risk of schizophrenia in the offspring (Insel et al, 2008). Paired-pulse inhibition (PPI) is heavily dependent on hippocampal activity modulating parts of the VTA loop and is considered a surrogate for neural gating in pre-clinical models of schizophrenia. PPI is markedly abnormal in the isolated hippocampal iron deficiency mouse (Pisansky et al, 2013).

Reduced transcript and protein expression of genes involved in actin and MT dynamics are associated with altered branching distribution and spine morphology. It remains unclear if there is an effect of iron deficiency on promoter regions for these genes, or if the effect is mediated through modification of growth factor expression, such as decreased BDNF. In humans and pre-clinical models, iron treatment to the iron deficient anemic individual at the equivalent time of human term birth is not sufficient to rescue the structural, genomic or behavioral phenotype, suggesting a very early sensitive period. Future work should determine if stimulating growth factors, specifically BDNF, during gestation and early postnatal life can positively affect dendritogenesis. Increased BDNF may contribute to an increased ability to maintain the systems during early IDA and allow the system to remain more plastic after repletion. Future work could also focus on critical period modulation, the role of GABAergic maturation, and the excitatory and inhibitory balance of electrical activity in maintaining optimal neural plasticity during development. The findings suggest that the consequence of early life alteration of transcripts for structural growth reduce plasticity in adulthood and result in reduced synaptic efficacy and poorer learning in adulthood. If restoration of the proper excitatory and inhibitory balance can be achieved during development or the critical period reopened as Sugiyama and colleagues (2008) have done with the visual system, long term plasticity deficits may be rescued.

The intriguing case of choline: can neural plasticity be enhanced by a nutrient?

Most studies in humans and pre-clinical models determine the role of a nutrient in long-term brain health by assessing deficit states at specific times of development. Studies are designed in this way because for most of human existence and for many human populations currently, food deficits have been the dominant issue (Walker, et al, 2007). Recently, there have been trials to determine whether any nutrients provided early in life truly enhance neurodevelopment beyond the expected normative outcome. This is an enormously difficult task since traditionally any improvement with a nutrient supplement has been used as prima face evidence of a previously unrecognized underlying deficiency. Nevertheless, studies on the role of early life choline have provided some interesting insights and food for thought regarding early life nutrient supplementation and adult brain health, particularly since this compound is essential for early brain development (Fisher, et al, 2002) demonstrates distinct critical period effects (Meck, et al, 2008).

In the late 1980’s, Meck and Williams began a series of studies investigating whether early life choline supplementation affects hippocampal development in rats (Meck, et al, 1988, Meck, et al, 1989). Choline is an interesting nutrient that could potentially affect neurodevelopment through at least three mechanisms. It is a substrate for the neurotransmitter acetylcholine. More acetylcholine (Ach) may be available in the synaptic compartment, increased acetylcholine (ACh) responses to electrical stimulation are present and less acetylcholinesterase is produced, suggesting that neurotransmission is affected by prenatal choline supplementation (Meck, et al, 1989). It is also a component of the myelin compounds phosphotidylcholine and phosphotidylethanolamine. However, the most intriguing role from a neural plasticity/critical period perspective is that it can act as a methyl donor for epigenetic modification of chromatin (Zeisel, 2010). As such, it has the potential to modify the methyl status of CpG islands as well as histones. The three mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and can be difficult to disentangle when positive effects are found after supplementation.

Through a carefully timed supplementation regimen where the pregnant and lactating rat dam was exposed to short periods of choline, Meck and Williams determined that embryonic days 13-18 and postnatal days 15-25 were particularly effective in improving the electrophysiology, biochemistry and brain morphology in the hippocampus of the offspring (Meck, et al, 1988). The exposure during gestation was more effective than during lactation, suggesting that earlier exposure was better. The specific timing was of interest because embryonic days 13-18 coincides with a period of rapid neurogenesis (proliferation) in the hippocampus (Glenn, et al, 2007). Choline provided in this time frame to rodents increases hippocampal cell proliferation and reduces apoptosis. Postnatal days 15-25 are the middle of the period of peak dendritogenesis (differentiation). Choline provided in this time frame increases dendritic arbor complexity in the hippocampus. Either or both may account for the improved learning and memory performance in the early-life supplemented rats when they became adults.

While the data might have been interpreted as having discovered that pregnant rats were not normally receiving enough choline during pregnancy, the studies that followed suggest otherwise. Data from our laboratory demonstrated that continuous choline supplementation throughout pregnancy and lactation had worse outcomes with respect to BDNF gene expression than control and far worse than targeted supplementation at embryonic days 13-18 (Figure 1). These data strongly suggest a critical period of efficacy.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The effect of targeted (left panel) versus universal (right panel) maternal choline supplementation during pregnancy and lactation on hippocampal BDNF gene expression in the iron sufficient adult offspring. Note that choline supplementation from gestational day 12 to 18 (FID-Ch) resulted in preserved BDNF levels in the offspring in adulthood compared to animals whose mothers were not treated with choline (FID). Levels of BDNF gene expression were similar to always iron sufficient (IS) animals. In contrast, non-targeted supplementation of the mother resulted in suppression of adult BDNF expression in the offspring.

* indicates a p-value<0.05.

Moreover, studies across multiple species (eg, rats, mice) and multiple lesions (eg, environmental, genetic) showed efficacy using the targeted timing discovered by Meck and Williams (Meck, et al, 1988). Rats with fetal alcohol exposure performed better on hippocampally dependent spatial memory tasks with either prenatal or postnatal targeted choline administration compared to animals not receiving choline (but not as well as non-alcohol exposed controls) (Ryan, et al, 2008). The MeCP2 knock-out mouse, that phenotypically resembles Rett’s syndrome, has improved memory capability when treated with prenatal or postnatal choline (Ricceri, et al, 2013). Similarly, the genetic mouse model of Down Syndrome with early cognitive decline performs better on learning and memory tasks if treated with prenatal choline (Moon, et al, 2010). Recently, we found a similar behavioral effect in adult rats that had been iron deficient as fetuses and neonates (Kennedy, et al, 2014).

What could account for such robust effects across multiple species and conditions? The hypothesis is that choline modifies something fundamental in the biology of these animals. A good candidate is epigenetic modification of synaptic plasticity genes whose chromatin can be modified by environmental factors (eg, brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). BDNF gene expression can be modified by rearing practices through methylation of CpG islands (Roth et al, 2009). This effect has been shown to cross generations (Roth, et al, 2009), lending further support to the idea. In our studies, specifically timed prenatal choline supplementation not only partially rescued adult learning and memory behavior in fetal/neonatal iron deficient rats, it also significantly elevated adult hippocampal BDNF gene expression (Kennedy, et al, 2014).

Why are these findings important with respect to neural plasticity? The previous dogma based on 40 years of human and pre-clinical model studies was that early life iron deficiency always led to long-term behavioral dysfunction in spite of diagnosis and treatment (Lozoff & Georgieff, 2006). Similarly, the neurodevelopmental effects of fetal alcohol syndrome, Down Syndrome and Rett’s Syndrome appeared to be severe and relatively non-mutable. These three syndromes have the following in common: removal of the primary condition (alcohol, gene mutation) is not possible and thus direct treatment of the condition is not in the therapeutic arsenal (unlike iron deficiency where iron could be provided as a treatment that directly assess the problem).

Yet, individual variations in the behavioral phenotypes of these syndromes exist. While one could argue that this may be a function of the degree of alcohol exposure in fetal alcohol syndrome (ie, a dose response to an environmental stressor), this argument is more difficult in the genetic syndromes. One then has to ask the question whether host factors or host-environment interactions (eg, epigenetic modifications of synaptic plasticity genes) may be in play in determining the amount and quality of neural plasticity in any given individual with the syndrome.

The new thinking is that neural plasticity may be restorable through understanding the basic biology underlying the loss of plasticity (eg, chromatin modification; closure of critical periods) and providing therapies (eg, targeted methyl diets, pharmacologic or genetic dissolution of perineuronal nets) that resolve that biology and function as “work-arounds” of the primary condition. Specific nutrients play an important role in mediating optimal neural plasticity in developing brains, and, it is a therapy that is readily at hand.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from NIH (R01 HD29421-18) to MKG.

Contributor Information

Michael K. Georgieff, Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, Institute of Child Development, College of Education and Human Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.

Katya E Brunette, Boys’ Town USA, Omaha, NE.

Phu V Tran, Department of Pediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.

References

  1. Algarín C, Peirano P, Garrido M, Pizarro F, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy: long-lasting effects on auditory and visual system functioning. Pediatric Research. 2003;53:217–23. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000047657.23156.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Armstrong VL, Brunet PM, He C, et al. What is so critical?: A commentary on the reexamination of critical periods. Developmental Psychobiology. 2006;48:326–331. doi: 10.1002/dev.20135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Artavanis-Tsakonas S, Rand MD, Lake RJ. Notch signaling: cell fate control and signal integration in development. Science. 1999;284:770–776. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5415.770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bagot RC, van Hasselt FN, Champagne DL, Meaney MJ, Krugers HJ, Joels M. Maternal care determines rapid effects of stress mediators on synaptic plasticity in adult rat hippocampal dentate gyrus. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2009;92:292–300. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balmer TS, Carels VM, Frisch JL, Nick TA. Modulation of perineuronal nets and parvalbumin with developmental soung learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(41):12878–12885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2974-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bekenstein JW, Lothman EW. An in vivo study of the ontogeny of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the CA1 region and in the dentate gyrus of the rat hippocampal formation. Developmental Brain Research. 1991;63:245–51. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(91)90084-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Blegen MB, Kennedy BC, Thibert KA, Tran PV, Georgieff MK. Multigenerational effects of fetal-neonatal iron deficiency on hippocampal BDNF signaling. Physiological Reports. 2013;1(5):e00096. doi: 10.1002/phy2.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bornstein MH. Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretations. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;105:179–197. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bretin S, Reibel S, Charrier E, Maus-Moatti M, Auvergnon N, Thevenoux A, Glowinski J, Rogemond V, Premont J, Honnorat J, Gauchy C. Differential expression of CRMP1,CRMP2A,CRMP2B, and CRMP5 in axons or dendrites of distinct neurons in the mouse brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;486:1–17. doi: 10.1002/cne.20465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris P. The bioecological model of Human Development. In: Lerner R, editor. Theoretical Models of Human Development: Vol 1 of the Handbook of Child Psychologogy. 6th ed. Wiley; Hoboken NJ: 2006. pp. 793–828. 793-828. [Google Scholar]
  11. Brunette KE, Tran PV, Wobken JD, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Gestational and Neonatal Iron Deficiency Alters Apical Dendrite Structure of CA1 Pyramidal Neurons in Adult Rat Hippocampus. Developmental Neuroscience. 2010;32:238–48. doi: 10.1159/000314341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Callahan LSN, Thibert KA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Early life iron deficiency anemia alters the development and long-term expression of parvalbumin and perineuronal nets in the rat hippocampus. Developmental Neurscience. 2013;35:427–436. doi: 10.1159/000354178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carlson ES, Stead JD, Neal CR, Petryk A, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency results in altered developmental expression of genes mediating energy metabolism and neuronal morphogenesis in hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2007;17:679–91. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carvin CD, Parr RD, Kladde MP. Site-selective in vivo targeting of cytosine-5 DNA methylation by zinc-finger proteins. Nucleic Acids Research. 2003;31:6493–6501. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkg853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Celio MR, Spreafico R, De Biasi S, Vitellaro-Zuccarello L. Perineuronal nets: past and present. Trends in Neurosciences. 1998;21:510–515. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01298-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Champagne DL, Bagot RC, van Hasselt F, Ramakers G, Meaney MJ, deKloet ER, Joels M, Krugers H. Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: evidence for experience-dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:6037–6045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0526-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Christian P, Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK, et al. Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2010;304:2716–23. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.1861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Christian P, Morgan ME, Murray-Kolb L, et al. Preschool iron-folic acid and zinc supplementation in children exposed to iron-folic acid in utero confers no added cognitive benefit in early school-age. Journal of Nutrition. 2011;141:2042–8. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.146480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cicchetti D, Tucker D, editors. Neural plasticity, sensitive periods, and psychopathology [Special Issue] Development and Psychopathology. 1994;6(4):531–814. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Colombo J. The critical period concept: Research, Methodology and theoretical issues. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;91:260–275. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cusick SE, Georgieff MK. Nutrient supplementation and neurodevelopment: Timing is the key. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2012;155:481–2. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dallman PR, Schwartz HC. Cytochrome C Concentrations during Rat and Guinea Pig Development. Pediatrics. 1964;33:106–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. deKloet ER, Vreugdenhil E, Oitzl MS, Joels M. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocrine Reviews. 1998;19:269–301. doi: 10.1210/edrv.19.3.0331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Donato F, Rompani SR, Caroni P. Parvalbumin-expressing basket-cell network plasticity induced by experience regulates adult learning. Nature. 2013;504:272–277. doi: 10.1038/nature12866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Encío IJ, Detera-Wadleigh SD. The genomic structure of the human glucocorticoid receptor. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1991;266:7182–7188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Felt BT, Lozoff B. Brain iron and behavior of rats are not normalized by treatment of iron deficiency anemia during early development. Journal of Nutrition. 1996;126:693–701. doi: 10.1093/jn/126.3.693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fisher MC, Zeisel SH, Mar M-H, Sadler TW. Perturbations in choline metabolism cause neural tube defects in mouse embryos in vitro. FASEB Journal. 2002;16:619–21. doi: 10.1096/fj.01-0564fje. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fretham SJB, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. The role of iron in learning and memory. Advances in Nutrition. 2011;2:1–10. doi: 10.3945/an.110.000190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fretham SJB, Carlson ES, Wobken J, Tran PV, Petryk A, Georgieff MK. Temporal manipulation of transferrin receptor-1 dependent iron uptake identifies a sensitive period in mouse hippocampal neurodevelopment. Hippocampus. 2012;22:1691–1702. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fuglestad AJ, Ramel SE, Georgieff MK. Miconutrient needs of the developing brain: priorities and assessment. In: Packer L, editor. Micronutrients and brain health, Oxidative stress and disease. CRC Press; Boca Raton: 2010. pp. 93–115.pp. Xxi [Google Scholar]
  31. Georgieff MK. The role of iron in neurodevelopment: fetal iron deficiency and the developing hippocampus. Biochemical Society Transactions. 2008;36:1267–1271. doi: 10.1042/BST0361267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gewirtz JC, Hamilton KL, Babuh MA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Effects of gestational iron deficiency on fear conditioning in juvenile and adult rats. Brain Research. 2008;1237:195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Glenn MJ, Gibson EM, Kirby ED, Mellott TJ, Blusztajn JK, Williams CL. Prenatal choline availability modulates hippocampal neurogenesis and neurogenic responses to enriching experiences in adult female rats. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25:2473–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05505.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gluckman PD, Hanson MA. Living with the past: evolution, development and patterns of disease. Science. 2004;305:1733–1736. doi: 10.1126/science.1095292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hebb DO. The organization of behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. Wiley; New York: 1949. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hensch T. Critical period regulation. Annual Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;27:549–79. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Huang ZJ, Kirkwood A, Pizzorusso T, Porciatti V, Morales B, Bear MF, Maffei L, Tonegawa S. BDNF regulates the maturation of inhibition and the critical period of plasticity in mouse visual cortex. Cell. 1999;98:739–755. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)81509-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Insel BJ, Schaefer CA, McKeague IW, Susser ES, Brown AS. Maternal iron deficiency and the risk of schizophrenia in offspring. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:1136–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.10.1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Isaacsom JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron. 2011;72:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jaworski J, Spangler S, Seeburg DP, Hoogenraad CC, Sheng M. Control of dendritic arborization by the phosphoinositide-3′-kinase-Akt-mammalian target of rapamycin pathway. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:11300–12. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2270-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Johnson M. Sensitive periods in functional brain development: Problems and Prospects. Developmental. Psychobiology. 2005;46:287–292. doi: 10.1002/dev.20057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jorgenson LA, Sun M, O’Connor M, Georgieff MK. Fetal iron deficiency disrupts the maturation of synaptic function and efficacy in area CA1 of the developing rat hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2005;15:1094–1102. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kennedy BC, Dimova JG, Tran PV, Gewirtz JC, Siddappa AJM, Georgieff MK. Protective Effects of Prenatal Choline Supplementation on Long-Term Alterations in Behavior and Hippocampal Gene Expression Resulting from Fetal-Neonatal Iron Deficiency. Journal of Nutrition. 2014 [Google Scholar]
  44. Knox S, Ge H, Dimitroff BD, Ren Y, Howe KA, Arsham AM, et al. Mechanisms of TSC-mediated control of synapse assembly and axon guidance. PLoS One. 2007;2:e375. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kretchmer N, Beard JL, Carlson S. The role of nutrition in the development of normal cognition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1996;63:997S–1001S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/63.6.997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kuzawa CW. Adipose tissue in human infancy and childhood: An evolutionary perspective. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology. 1998;41:177–209. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8644(1998)107:27+<177::aid-ajpa7>3.0.co;2-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lerner R. Structure and process in relational, developmental systems theories: A commentary on contemporary changes in the understanding of developmental changes across the life span. Human Development. 2011;54:34–43. [Google Scholar]
  48. Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science. 1997;277:1659–1662. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5332.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 2006;13:158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lubin FD, Roth TL, Sweatt JD. Epigenetic regulation of bdnf gene during memory consolidation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:10576–10586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lucassen PJ, Naninck EF, van Goudoever JB, Fitzsimons C, Joels M, Korosi A. Perinatal programming of adult hippocampal structure and function: emerging roles of stress, nutrition and epigenetics. Trends in Neuroscience. 2013;36:621–31. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2010;13:54–70. doi: 10.1179/147683010X12611460763689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Markram H, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wang Y, Gupta A, Silberberg G, Wu C. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:793–807. doi: 10.1038/nrn1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. McEchron MD, Cheng AY, Liu H, Connor JR, Gilmartin MR. Nutritional iron deficiency permanently impairs hippocampus-dependent trace fear conditioning in rats. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2005;8:195–206. doi: 10.1080/10284150500162952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Meaney MJ, Diorio J, Francis D, Weaver S, Yau J, Chapman K, Seckl JR. Postnatal handling increases the expression of cAMP-inducible transcription factors in the rat hippocampus: the effects of thyroid hormones and serotonin. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:3926–3935. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-10-03926.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Meck WH, Smith RA, Williams CL. Pre- and postnatal choline supplementation produces long-term facilitation of special memory. Developmental Psychobiology. 1988;21:339–53. doi: 10.1002/dev.420210405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Meck WH, Smith RA, Williams CL. Organizational changes in cholinergic activity and enhanced visuospatial memory as a function of choline administered prenatally or postnatally or both. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1989;103:1234–1241. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.6.1234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Meck WH, Williams CL, Cermak JM, Blusztajn JK. Developmental periods of choline sensitivity provide an ontogenetic mechanism for regulating memory capacity and age-related dementia. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 2008;1:7. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.007.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Michel G, Tyler A. Critical period: A history of the transition from questions of when, to what, to how. Developmental Psychobiology. 2005;46:163–183. doi: 10.1002/dev.20058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Moon J, Chen M, Gandhy SU, Strawderman M, Levitsky DA, Maclean KN, et al. Perinatal choline supplementation improves cognitive functioning and emotion regulation in the Ts65Dn mouse model of Down syndrome. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2010;124:346–61. doi: 10.1037/a0019590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Morishita H, Hensch TK. Critical Period revisited: impact on vision. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2008;18:101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK, Katz J, Schaefer BA, Cole PM, LeClerq SC, Morgan ME, Tielsch JM, Christian P. Preschool micronutrient supplementation effects on intellectual and motor function in school-aged Nepalese children. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2012;166:404–10. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Pisansky MT, Wickham RJ, Su J, Fretham S, Yuan L-L, Sun M, Gewirtz JC, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency with or without anemia impairs prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex. Hippocampus. 2013;23:952–62. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Plato . Republic. Oxford University Press; New York: 1993. 377 a-b. R. Waterfield Translator. [Google Scholar]
  65. Pokorny J, Yamamoto T. Postnatal ontogenesis of hippocampal CA1 area in rats. I. Development of dendritic arborization in pyramidal neurons. Brain Research Bulletin. 1981;7:113–120. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(81)90075-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle PL, Martorell R, Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding and cognition: effects over two decades. Monogram of the Society of Research in Child Development. 1993;58:1–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pongcharoen T, Ramakrishnan U, DiGirolamo AM, Winichagoon P, Flores R, Singkhornard J, Martorell R. Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2012;166:411–6. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pylipow M, Spector LG, Puumala SE, Boys C, Cohen J, Georgieff MK. Early postnatal weight gain, intellectual performance, and body mass index (BMI) at seven years of age in term infants with intrauterine growth-restriction (IUGR) Journal of Pediatrics. 2009;154:201–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ramel SE, Demerath EW, Gray HL, Younge N, Boys C, Georgieff MK. The relationship of poor linear growth velocity with neonatal illness and two-year neurodevelopment in preterm infants. Neonatology. 2012;102:19–24. doi: 10.1159/000336127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rao R, Tkac I, Schmidt A, Georgieff M. Fetal and neonatal iron deficiency causes volume loss and alters the neurochemical profile of the adult rat hippocampus. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2011;14:59–65. doi: 10.1179/1476830511Y.0000000001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rice D, Barone S., Jr. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: evidence from humans and animal models. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2000;108(Supplement 3):511–33. doi: 10.1289/ehp.00108s3511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ricceri L, De Filippis B, Laviola G. Rett syndrome treatment in mouse models: Searching for effective targets and strategies. Neuropharmacology. 2013;68:106–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Riggins T, Miller NC, Bauer PB, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Consequences of low neonatal iron status due to maternal diabetes mellitus on explicit memory performance in childhood. Developmental Neuropsychlogy. 2009;34:762–79. doi: 10.1080/87565640903265145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Roth TL, Lubin FD, Funk AJ, Sweatt JD. Lasting epigenetic influence of early-life adversity on the BDNF gene. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;65:760–76. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ryan SH, Williams JK, Thomas JD. Choline supplementation attenuates learning deficits associated with neonatal alcohol exposure in the rat: Effects of varying the timing of choline administration. Brain Research. 2008;1237:91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Schmidt AT, Waldow KJ, Grove WM, Salinas JA, Georgieff MK. Dissociating the long-term effects of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency on three types of learning in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;121:475–482. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Siddappa AJ, Rao RB, Wobken JD, Leibold EA, Connor JR, Georgieff MK. Developmental changes in the expression of iron regulatory proteins and iron transport proteins in the perinatal rat brain. Journal of Neuroscience Research. 2002;68:761–75. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Siddappa AM, Georgieff MK, Wewerka S, Worwa C, Nelson CA, deRegnier R-A. Auditory recognition memory in iron-deficient infants of diabetic mothers. Pediatric Research. 2004;55:1034–1041. doi: 10.1203/01.pdr.0000127021.38207.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Silveira PP, Portella AK, Goldani MZ, Barbieri MA. Developmental origins of health and disease (DOHaD) Jornal De Pediatria. 2007;83:494–504. doi: 10.2223/JPED.1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Jing Y, Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency and infant motor development. Early Human Development. 2008;84:479–485. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sugiyama S, Di Nardo AA, Aizawa S, Matsuo I, Volovitch M, Prochiantz A, Hensch TK. Experience-dependent transfer of Otx2 homeoprotein into the visual cortex activates postnatal plasticity. Cell. 2008;134:508–520. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.05.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tang BL. Inhibitors of neuronal regeneration: mediators and signaling mechanisms. Neurochemistry International. 2003;42:189–203. doi: 10.1016/s0197-0186(02)00094-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Thompson RA, Nelson CA. Developmental science and the media. Early brain development. American Psychologist. 2001;56(1):5–15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Townsend EL, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Neurobehavioral functioning in five-year-old children born to diabetic mothers. Cognite, Creier, Comportament. 2005:363–381. [Google Scholar]
  85. Tran PV, Carlson ES, Fretham SJ, Georgieff MK. Early-life iron deficiency anemia alters neurotrophic factor expression and hippocampal neuron differentiation in male rats. Journal of Nutrition. 2008;138:2495–501. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.091553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Tran PV, Fretham SJB, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Long-term reduction of hippocampal BDNF activity following fetal-neonatal iron deficiency in adult rats. Pediatric Research. 2009;65:493–498. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819d90a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wachs TD, Georgieff M, Cusick S, McEwan B. Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: contributions from nutrition, neuroscience and psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2014;1308:89–106. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollit EA, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Effects of visual deprivation on morphology and physiology of cells in the cat’s lateral geniculate body. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1963;26:978–993. doi: 10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Wong RO. Retinal waves and visual system development. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1999;22:29–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wong OL, Ghosh A. Activity-dependent regulation of dendritic growth and patterning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3:803–812. doi: 10.1038/nrn941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wullschleger S, Loewith R, Hall MN. TOR signaling in growth and metabolism. Cell. 2006;124:471–84. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Yiu G, He Z. Glial inhibition of CNS axon regeneration. Nature Reviews. 2006;7:7617–627. doi: 10.1038/nrn1956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Zeisel SH. Choline: clinical nutrigenetic/nutrigenomic approaches for identification of functions and dietary requirements. Journal of Nutrigenetics and Nutrigenomics. 2010;3:209–19. doi: 10.1159/000324357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES