Abstract
The adolescent period is associated with high significance of interactions with peers, high frequency of stressful situations, and high rates of alcohol use. At least two desired effects of alcohol that may contribute to heavy and problematic drinking during adolescence are its abilities to both facilitate interactions with peers and to alleviate anxiety, perhaps especially anxiety seen in social contexts. Ethanol-induced social facilitation can be seen using a simple model of adolescence in the rat, with normal adolescents, but not their more mature counterparts, demonstrating this ethanol-related social facilitation. Prior repeated stress induces expression of ethanol-induced social facilitation in adults and further enhances socially facilitating effects of ethanol among adolescent rats. In contrast, under normal circumstances, adolescent rats are less sensitive than adults to the social inhibition induced by higher ethanol doses and are insensitive to the socially anxiolytic effects of ethanol. Sensitivity to the socially anxiolytic effects of ethanol can be modified by prior stress or ethanol exposure at both ages. Shortly following repeated restraint or ethanol exposure, adolescents exhibit social anxiety-like behavior, indexed by reduced social preference, and enhanced sensitivity to the socially anxiolytic effects of ethanol, indexed through ethanol-associated reinstatement of social preference in these adolescents. Repeated restraint, but not repeated ethanol, induces similar effects in adults as well, eliciting social anxiety-like behavior and increasing their sensitivity to the socially anxiolytic effects of acute ethanol; the stressor also decreases sensitivity of adults to ethanol-induced social inhibition. The persisting consequences of early adolescent ethanol exposure differ from its immediate consequences, with males exposed early in adolescence, but not females or those exposed later in adolescence, showing social anxiety-like behavior when tested in adulthood. Adult males exposed to ethanol early in adolescence also show enhanced sensitivity to the socially facilitating effects of ethanol, whereas adult males exposed to ethanol during late adolescence demonstrate insensitivity to the socially suppressing effects of ethanol. To the extent that these results are applicable to humans, stressful live events may make alcohol more attractive for stressed adolescents and adults due to its socially facilitating and socially anxiolytic properties, therefore fostering high levels of drinking. Retention of adolescent-typical responsiveness to alcohol in adult males following adolescent alcohol exposure, including enhanced sensitivity to the socially facilitating effects of ethanol following early exposure and insensitivity to the socially inhibiting effects following late adolescent exposure, may put these males at risk for the development of alcohol-related disorders later in life.
Keywords: Adolescence, Ethanol, Social Consequences, Stress, Repeated Ethanol Exposure
1. Introduction
In humans, adolescence refers to a transitional period between youth and maturity that occurs predominantly during the second decade of life, although females generally show more rapid maturation than males [1]. This transformation from immaturity to maturity and dependence to independence is a gradual developmental phase than can be seen across different mammalian species [2], with adolescents often differing markedly from those younger or older in terms of responding to a number of stimuli in their environment [3, 4]. While there is no single biological event that signals its onset or offset, adolescence in humans is often considered to subsume the second decade of life, with females tending to mature earlier than males [1]. Some adolescent-typical characteristics have been found to persist into at least the mid-twenties, a period sometimes termed “emerging adulthood” [5, 6]. Likewise, in rats, a conservative age range during which both males and females appear to exhibit adolescent-typical neurobehavioral characteristics has been defined as postnatal (P) day 28–42 [4, 7, 8], although females tend to progress into adolescence slightly earlier, and animals of both sexes, especially males, continue to show signs of adolescence for some time thereafter. Given the broad developmental periods subsumed, adolescence has been subdivided into early, mid and late stages. In humans, these stages are thought to refer to approximately 10–14 years (early), 15–17 years (mid), 18–25 years (late/emerging adulthood) [5, 6], with specific physical, hormonal, and neurobehavioral changes associated with each phase [6]. In rats as well, it has recently been suggested that the period between postnatal day (P) 28 and P42 be considered early-mid adolescence, with the interval between approximately P42 and P55 (or even P65) viewed as more analogous to the late adolescence/emerging adulthood period in humans [9–11].
2. Social interactions during adolescence
The adolescent period is associated with a high significance of interactions with peers and elevated levels of social motivation (see [2] for references). Interactions with peers become particularly important during adolescence, with these interactions not only exerting a greater influence over decision-making and behavior among adolescents than they do among children and adults [12–14], but also providing a significant source of positive experiences [15]. Adolescents spend more time interacting with peers than individuals during any other developmental period [16]. Similarly, during the early adolescent age interval (P28–P35) in the rat, animals demonstrate substantial increases in social activity relative to younger or older animals, particularly the adolescent-characteristic behavior of play fighting [17–19]. Studies using rats have also shown that interactions with peers provide a significant source of positive experiences [20] and are seemingly more rewarding for adolescents than for their more mature counterparts [3, 21]. The social interaction test has been used extensively for the assessment of anxiety-like behavior in laboratory rodents [22–24]. In the conventional social interaction test, a pair of rats is placed into a testing chamber, and overall time spent in social interactions is used as a dependent variable [22]. Yet, the discrete behavioral acts summed together for these assessments reflect behaviorally distinctive and differentially regulated forms of interactive social behaviors (e.g., social investigation and play fighting) with separable ontogenetic patterns [17, 19, 25] and differential responsiveness to seemingly anxiogenic manipulations [26]. For instance, play fighting exhibits an inverted U-shaped ontogenetic pattern that peaks around P30–35, whereas social investigation increases ontogenetically and represents a more adult-typical form of social interactions [17, 25, 27]. Play fighting, but not social investigation, is drastically increased by deprivation from social contact via isolate housing throughout the entire adolescent period [17, 19], whereas social investigation is exclusively decreased by prior history of exposure to non-social stressors [26, 28]. Taken together, these findings suggest that play fighting and social investigation may be mediated via different neural systems. Modification of the social interaction test, allowing an experimental animal to freely move toward or away from a non-manipulated social partner in a two-compartment testing apparatus, permits assessment of social motivation via a preference/avoidance coefficient in addition to measuring the frequency of play fighting and social investigation [25]. Using this modified social interaction test, we have found decreases in social preference to reflect anxiety-like alterations in social interactions [26, 28–30].
3. Ethanol-induced social facilitation
In humans, first experimentation with alcohol occurs predominantly during early adolescence [31], with underage adolescents drinking about two times more per episode than drinkers of legal age (see [32] for references and review). For instance, approximately 5.1% of 8th graders, 15.6% of 10th graders, and 23.7% of high school seniors in the United States reported a binge pattern of drinking (i.e., 5+ drinks in a row) during the previous two weeks [33], and even more elevated rates of binge drinking are reported among adolescents in many European countries [34]. The impact of social context on adolescent drinking is viewed as particularly important [35], with young individuals typically using alcohol in social situations [36]. Adolescent laboratory rodents also ingest more ethanol on a g/kg basis than adults under various testing conditions [37–45].
Given the importance of interactions with peers during adolescence, it is not surprising that ability of ethanol to facilitate interactions with peers may contribute to heavy drinking during adolescence. Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking is an important predictor of heavy drinking, with adolescent youth believing that alcohol will make them more confident and relaxed in a social setting [46, 47]. Ethanol-induced social facilitation is not restricted to human adolescents but is also evident in a simple model of adolescence in the rat [48]. Adolescent rats tested under familiar, non-anxiogenic circumstances demonstrate increases in social behavior following acute exposure to relatively low doses (0.5–0.75 g/kg) of ethanol, an ethanol-induced facilitation of social behavior that is predominantly characterized by an increase in play fighting and is not normally seen in adults [18, 48–52]. The doses producing social facilitation in adolescent rats result in blood ethanol concentrations (BECs) from approximately 40 to 80 mg/dl -- within the moderate consumption range in humans [53]. Higher doses of ethanol have different social consequences, producing social inhibition, with adolescent rats being less sensitive to these adverse social effects of ethanol than their more mature counterparts [18].
Considerable ontogenetic differences in the social consequences of acute ethanol are evident even within the adolescent period, with early adolescence being a time when adolescent-typical sensitivities to ethanol are the most pronounced. For instance, young adolescent rats tested at P28 are more sensitive to low dose ethanol-induced social facilitation and less sensitive to the social inhibition evident at higher ethanol doses than animals tested later in adolescence at P42 [50, 54].
This social facilitation is mediated, at least in part, through ethanol-induced release of endogenous ligands for the mu-opioid receptor (MOR) or an ethanol-associated enhancement of sensitivity of these receptors to their endogenous ligands, since the facilitation of play fighting by low doses of ethanol can be attenuated by the nonselective opioid antagonist naloxone, as well as by the selective MOR antagonist CTOP [55]. This finding was not surprising, given that the MOR system is implicated in modulation of play behavior, with selective MOR agonists increasing play fighting in young adolescent males and antagonists suppressing this form of social behavior (see [56] for references and review). While the endogenous MOR system plays a considerable role in facilitation of play fighting by ethanol [55], other neural systems are implicated in ethanol-associated modulation of play fighting as well. For instance, social behavior during adolescence can be facilitated by cannabinoid agonists [57, 58, 59], whereas CB1 receptor antagonists are able to diminish ethanol-induced facilitation of play behavior during early adolescence [49]. Play fighting in adolescent rats is also under inhibitory control of the NMDA system, with NMDA antagonists facilitating play fighting at low doses, but suppressing social behavior at higher doses [60] – biphasic effects on play fighting similar to those induced by ethanol [18, 50]. The NR2B subunit of the NMDA receptor may play a particularly important role, given that a selective NR2B antagonist, ifenprodil, was found to facilitate play fighting in a manner similar to that produced by low doses of less selective NMDA antagonists as well as ethanol [61].
4. Stress-related alterations
Sensitivity to the social consequences of ethanol can be modified by prior stress in adolescents and adults, although effects of stress on both social behavior and ethanol sensitivity are age-dependent. Exposure to repeated restraint (5 days, 90 min/day) during mid-late adolescence and adulthood induces anxiety-like behavioral alterations, indexed via significant decreases in social preference [26, 28, 62]. Repeated restraint stress exacerbated adolescent-typical responsiveness to the social consequences of acute ethanol in mid-adolescent animals tested after the last stressor exposure at P35, enhancing responsiveness to ethanol-induced facilitation of play fighting and further accentuating adolescent-typical insensitivities to the socially suppressing effects of ethanol [28]. Surprisingly, among late adolescents and adults (tested following the final stressor at P42 or P70, respectively), repeated exposure to the stressor reinstated a pattern of responsiveness to the social consequences of ethanol characteristic of younger animals. Specifically, these stressed late adolescents and adults demonstrated both: (a) ethanol-induced facilitation of play fighting not evident in non-stressed controls, as well as (b) an attenuated sensitivity to the social inhibition seen at higher ethanol doses relative to their non-stressed age-mates [28, 62]. These stress effects were evident in both males and females. Ethanol-induced increases in play fighting were not associated with any increases in locomotor activity when indexed via total number of crossovers in the social test context, suggesting that these activating effects of ethanol reflect ethanol-induced facilitation of social behavior per se rather than more general activation [28, 62].
Importantly, repeated restraint in mid-late adolescent and adults also induced anxiety-like behavioral alterations under social test circumstances, as indexed by decreases in social preference [28, 62]. These decreases in social preference were effectively attenuated by acute ethanol in animals of both ages. The anxiolytic effects were not evident in non-stressed controls under social test circumstances, suggesting a stress-associated enhancement of sensitivity to the socially anxiolytic effects of ethanol. This apparent stress-associated enhancement of sensitivity to ethanol anxiolysis may be related in part to stress-induced alterations in the GABAA receptor system, a neural system shown to contribute to a number of ethanol effects [63–65], including its anxiolytic properties [53]. GABAA receptors with different subunit composition appear to differentially contribute to various ethanol effects, with α1 subunits playing a role in ethanol-induced sedation and motor impairment [66], and α2/α3 subunits implicated in anxiolytic effects of ethanol [67]. Exposure to stressors has been shown to increase expression of α2 subunits in brain regions associated with anxiety [68], and these stress-associated changes in GABAA subunit expression may play a role in the enhanced sensitivity to the anxiolytic effects of ethanol observed in adolescents and adults.
Among early adolescents, effects of repeated restraint stress on social behavior and alterations in sensitivity to the social consequences of acute ethanol differed drastically relative to those discussed above in older adolescents and adults [62]. Unlike their older counterparts, young adolescents tested at P28 after receiving repeated restraint during the juvenile period (P24–P28) showed no attenuation in social preference. Instead, early adolescent males responded to the prior stressor exposure with a notable enhancement in baseline levels of play fighting – an effect of prior restraint not evident in early adolescent females. One of the possible explanations of these drastic age differences in the consequences of repeated restraint is that the early adolescent males tested at P28 do not respond to anxiety-provoking manipulations in a way their older counterparts do. It is unlikely, however, given that early adolescent rats, similar to their more mature counterparts, respond to a novel, anxiety-provoking test situation by transformation of social preference into social avoidance [19, 50].
An alternative possibility is that juvenile males perceived the 90-min periods of restraint as significant social deprivation. Indeed, repeated social deprivation (90 min/day for 5 days) was found to produce increases in play fighting in early-mid adolescent males, but not their female counterparts [26], with the most pronounced activating effects on play fighting evident following social deprivation between P23 and P28 [19]. Similarly, sex differences in play fighting have been reported for socially deprived juvenile rats, with males engaging in more play fighting than females (see [17] for references and review). Taken together, these findings suggest that during the developmental period transitioning into early adolescence, males are more sensitive than females to the activating effects of social deprivation on play fighting. Interestingly, stressful events that occur even earlier in ontogeny can also enhance play fighting in young adolescents. For instance, maternal separation (3 hr/day for 14 days) during the preweanling period was observed to increase play fighting in males tested at P35 [69]. Therefore, adverse early experiences that include social deprivation are likely to enhance the adolescent-typical social behavior of play fighting in male rats.
In the rat, the developmental period between weaning and P28 corresponds to the pre-pubertal or juvenile stage of development in both males and females [10, 11]. Some researchers suggest that pre-pubertal rats differ noticeably in their responsiveness to stress relative to post-pubertal, adult rats [70–73]. Pre-pubertal stress has been shown to have long-lasting consequences: alterations in stress responsiveness in adulthood were evident following even a brief, acute stress exposure on P28 [74]. Furthermore, pre-pubertal stress enhanced anxiety-like behavior and substantially reduced exploratory behavior in adulthood [75, 76]. However, when these young animals were tested immediately after exposure to stressors, they demonstrated increases in exploratory behavior [77] and decreases in anxiety [78], findings that are reminiscent of the immediate social consequences of repeated restraint at this age.
Repeated restraint stress not only elevated baseline levels of play fighting in P28 males, but also eliminated sensitivity to the stimulatory effects of ethanol, with no ethanol-induced facilitation of play fighting evident at any dose. The lack of ethanol-induced facilitation of play fighting in stressed P28 males is likely related, at least in part, to the dramatic stress-associated increase in baseline levels of this adolescent-characteristic form of social interactions from which it might be difficult to see further stimulatory effects [62]. Indeed, among P28 females (where repeated restraint did not influence baseline levels of play fighting), ethanol-associated facilitation of play fighting was still evident, although they required a higher ethanol dose relative to their non-stressed counterparts for this effect to emerge (1.0 g/kg versus 0.5 g/kg).
As mentioned earlier, ethanol-induced social facilitation seen normally during early-mid adolescence appears to be associated, at least in part, with ethanol-related activation of the endogenous MOR system [55], with presumably more pronounced ethanol-associated activation of the endogenous MOR system in young than older adolescents and adults. The increase in sensitivity to the socially facilitating effects of ethanol induced by repeated restraint in older adolescents and adults may also be in part due to stress-induced activation of this MOR system as well [79–82]. A number of other neural systems, including endogenous cannabinoid [49, 58] and NMDA [60, 61] receptor systems may also contribute to stress-induced alterations in play fighting observed in young adolescent males and developmental alterations in sensitivity to the effects of ethanol on social behavior, given their involvement in modulation of play fighting under normal conditions as discussed earlier.
5. Alterations associated with repeated ethanol: Immediate and delayed consequences
Baseline levels of anxiety in adolescents of both sexes were increased not only by repeated restraint stress during adolescence, but also by repeated exposure to ethanol at this time. This elevation in anxiety-like behavior (reflected by significant decreases in social preference) was seen in adolescent but not adult animals in the social interaction test following chronic ethanol exposure (1 g/kg, i.p. for 7 days; P27–P33 for adolescents and P62–P68 for adults) [51]. This suppression in social preference was reversed by acute ethanol challenge in adolescent animals. Such social anxiety seen among adolescent animals with a history of ethanol exposure may be related at least in part to ethanol-induced disruptions in neural substrates underlying social behavior. Although experimental studies that specify neural substrates critical for peer-directed social interactions and motivation for social contacts remain limited, recent findings have revealed that frontal cortical regions, the amygdala, and ventral hippocampus, are involved in the modulation of peer-directed social behavior [83, 84], as well as in generating and regulating anxiety-like responses (see [85] for review). These brain regions undergo considerable remodeling during adolescence [2, 3, 86, 87] and likely contribute to elevations in anxiety-like behavior following not only chronic adolescent alcohol exposure but repeated restraint as well.
In the Varlinskaya and Spear [51] study, however, animals were injected introperitoneally, exposed to ethanol daily and tested 48 hours following the last ethanol exposure, and hence it was not clear whether the social anxiety-like behavior and changes in ethanol sensitivity induced by repeated ethanol in adolescents are short-lasting and associated merely with ethanol withdrawal or whether they persist and can be detected later in life. In subsequent work, we examined delayed social consequences after an adolescent intermittent ethanol exposure (AIE) regimen (3.5 g/kg intragastrically, every other day for a total of 11 exposures), given that intermittent exposures have been shown to produce more pronounced consequences than continuous (daily) exposures [88, 89]. Animals were assessed in the social interaction test 25 days after early-mid adolescent exposure (early AIE): P25–P45; or late adolescent exposure (late AIE): P45–65, Significant anxiety-like social alterations were evident in adult male rats tested at P70, but not their female counterparts following early AIE, whereas neither males nor females demonstrated these alterations 25 days following late AIE, suggesting that early, but not late adolescence is the critical period for induction of long-lasting social consequences by repeated ethanol [90]. To the extent that our experimental findings are applicable to humans, the results of this study [90] suggest that young adolescent males are more vulnerable to the harmful effects of ethanol exposure than their female counterparts, in that social anxiety-like behavioral alterations were evident only in males following early AIE.
To a large extent, the studies that have assessed long-lasting anxiety-like alterations induced by AIE have included only male subjects. However, the inclusion of female subjects in such studies is important, given human data regarding gender differences in prevalence of alcohol use disorders and in negative consequences of excessive alcohol use [91, 92]. Indeed, adult women consume less alcohol and have fewer alcohol-related problems than men, with 18.6% of men and 8.4% of women demonstrating a lifetime prevalence for alcohol dependence [93]. However, the rate of alcohol use disorders is not different between boys and girls aged 12 to 17 [92]. Taken together, these observations suggest that adolescent males are at higher risk to become alcohol-dependent later in life than adolescent females.
One of the possible explanations of the sex differences in the social anxiogenic consequences of early AIE is that adolescent females may be less sensitive to ethanol-associated alterations within the brain systems implicated in modulation of anxiety-like behavior. Indeed, prior work examining the effects of intraperitoneal exposure to ethanol in adolescence on gene expression of two critical regulators of stress and anxiety in the paroventricular nucleus likewise reported increased corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) gene expression in males, but not females [94]. Neuroactive steroids may play a role in the greater protection of the female than the male brain from the detrimental effects of ethanol during adolescence. Progesterone-derived neurosteroids are present at higher levels in females than males [95, 96] and have been shown to have anxiolytic effects in a number of behavioral paradigms [97–100]. Therefore, higher levels of endogenous neurosteroid anxiolytics in adolescent females, relative to their male counterparts, may play a substantial role in protecting these females from ethanol-induced social anxiety-like behavioral alterations during AIE.
Effects of AIE on later sensitivity to the social consequences of acute ethanol challenge also were sex-specific. That is, AIE females showed little, if any, change in sensitivity to the social consequences of acute ethanol challenge, whereas both early and late AIE influenced ethanol sensitivity of males tested 25 days after AIE, although these effects differed as a function of AIE timing [90]. On the one hand, male rats exposed to ethanol during early adolescence and tested as adults showed ethanol-induced social facilitation, whereas controls demonstrated only the adult-typical inhibition of social behavior following acute ethanol challenge. This social facilitation is reminiscent of that seen normally during adolescence [18, 50], and hence is consistent with the prior suggestion that repeated exposure to ethanol during early adolescence may in some cases “preserve” an adolescent-like phenotype, including adolescent-typical ethanol sensitivities in adulthood (e.g., [101] see [102] for review). Surprisingly, anxiety-like social alterations (indexed via decreases in social preference) were not reversed by ethanol in the early AIE males. Taken together with the previous findings, these data suggest that social consequences of adolescent ethanol exposure evident in adulthood appear to differ from those observed in adolescents shortly after ethanol withdrawal. In adult males, prior early AIE resulted in the enhancement of sensitivity to the socially facilitating, rather than an enhancement of socially anxiolytic effects of ethanol that are seen during the withdrawal phase. Socially facilitating effects of ethanol have been found to be related to ethanol-induced activation of the endogenous MOR system [55] as well as NMDA receptor antagonism [61], whereas ethanol anxiolysis is generally thought to reflect interactions with the GABAA receptor system [53, 67].
In contrast to the preservation of adolescent-typical ethanol sensitivities in adult males after early adolescent ethanol exposure, adult males exposed to ethanol later in adolescence varied from their control counterparts in being notably insensitive to the socially suppressing effects of acute ethanol challenge [90]. This insensitivity to ethanol-induced social inhibition suggests that these males may have developed chronic tolerance to the social consequences of ethanol, with this tolerance still evident 25 days after repeated exposure to ethanol. This tolerance appears functional rather than metabolic in nature, given that post-test BECs were comparable among previously ethanol-exposed and non-exposed animals. These findings are reminiscent of those reported by Sherill et al. [103], who found adolescent ethanol exposure to attenuate later sensitivity to aversive effects of ethanol in males but not females tested approximately nine weeks following adolescent exposure.
Summary
Taken together, these results demonstrate that repeated restraint stress or ethanol exposure has immediate consequences during adolescence that are evident in terms of increases in social anxiety-like behavior (indexed by notably reduced baseline levels of social preference), with acute ethanol effectively restoring social preference in these adolescents. Therefore, stressful life events as well as a history of early exposure to ethanol may foster high ethanol intake levels during adolescence in part via inducing anxiety in a social context that can be efficiently counteracted by socially anxiolytic consequences of ethanol. In adults, repeated exposure to different stressors may attenuate their sensitivity to socially adverse effects of ethanol, while making alcohol more attractive for stressed adults due to its socially facilitating and socially anxiolytic properties.
The long-lasting effects of adolescent ethanol exposure, however, differ from its immediate consequences. Young adolescent males, but not their female counterparts, are particularly vulnerable to long-lasting detrimental effects of repeated ethanol. Retention of adolescent-typical sensitivity to socially facilitating properties of ethanol could make ethanol especially appealing to these males, therefore promoting drinking later in life. Late adolescent males may be at high risk for the development of alcohol-related disorders later in life as well, given their enhanced ability to develop long-lasting chronic tolerance to the socially inhibiting effects of ethanol – tolerance that could permit ingestion of relatively large amounts of ethanol with limited negative consequences.
Highlights.
Socially facilitating and anxiolytic effects of ethanol contribute to drinking.
Adolescents and adults are differentially sensitive to these ethanol properties.
Prior exposure to stress or ethanol can modify sensitivity to these social effects.
Early and late adolescents respond differently to repeated ethanol.
Persisting effects of repeated adolescent ethanol exposure are seen in males only.
Acknowledgments
The research presented in this paper was supported by NIH grant U01 AA019972.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Petersen AC, Silbereisen RK, Sorensen S. Adolescent development: a global perspective. In: Hurrelmann K, Hamilton SF, editors. Social problems and social contexts in adolescence. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1996. pp. 3–37. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Spear LP. The behavioral neuroscience of adolescence. New York: Norton; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Motivational systems in adolescence: possible implications for age differences in substance abuse and other risk-taking behaviors. Brain Cognition. 2010;72:114–23. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Spear LP. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:417–63. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood. A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. Am Psychol. 2000;55:469–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Feldman SS, Elliot GR. At the Threshold: The Developing Adolescent. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Odell WD. Sexual maturation in the rat. In: Grumbach MM, Sizonenko PC, Aubert ML, editors. Control of the Onset of Puberty. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins; 1990. pp. 182–210. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Spear LP, Brake SC. Periadolescence: age-dependent behavior and psychopharmacological responsivity in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1983;16:83–109. doi: 10.1002/dev.420160203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Schneider M. Puberty as a highly vulnerable developmental period for the consequences of cannabis exposure. Addict Biol. 2008;13:253–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00110.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Schneider M. Adolescence as a vulnerable period to alter rodent behavior. Cell Tissue Res. 2013;354:99–106. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1581-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Vetter-O'Hagen CS, Spear LP. Hormonal and physical markers of puberty and their relationship to adolescent-typical novelty-directed behavior. Dev Psychobiol. 2012;54:523–35. doi: 10.1002/dev.20610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev Psychol. 2005;41:625–35. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Grosbras MH, Jansen M, Leonard G, McIntosh A, Osswald K, Poulsen C, et al. Neural mechanisms of resistance to peer influence in early adolescence. J Neurosci. 2007;27:8040–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1360-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Steinberg L. Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2005;9:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescent development. Ann Rev Psychol. 2001;52:83–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hartup WW, Stevens N. Friendships and adaptation in the life course. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:355–70. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vanderschuren LJ, Niesink RJ, Van Ree JM. The neurobiology of social play behavior in rats. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:309–26. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00020-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Acute effects of ethanol on social behavior of adolescent and adult rats: role of familiarity of the test situation. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2002;26:1502–11. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000034033.95701.E3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Social interactions in adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats: impact of social deprivation and test context familiarity. Behavioural Brain Res. 2008;188:398–405. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Trezza V, Campolongo P, Vanderschuren LJ. Evaluating the rewarding nature of social interactions in laboratory animals. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2011;1:444–58. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Douglas LA, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Rewarding properties of social interactions in adolescent and adult male and female rats: impact of social versus isolate housing of subjects and partners. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;45:153–62. doi: 10.1002/dev.20025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.File SE. The use of social interaction as a method for detecting anxiolytic activity of chlordiazepoxide-like drugs. J Neurosci Methods. 1980;2:219–38. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(80)90012-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.File SE, Hyde JR. Can social interaction be used to measure anxiety? Br J Pharmacol. 1978;62:19–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1978.tb07001.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.File SE, Seth P. A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:35–53. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01273-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP, Spear NE. Social behavior and social motivation in adolescent rats: role of housing conditions and partner's activity. Physiol Behav. 1999;67(4):475–82. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00285-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Social and non-social anxiety in adolescent and adult rats after repeated restraint. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:484–94. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Panksepp J. The ontogeny of play in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1981;14:327–32. doi: 10.1002/dev.420140405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Varlinskaya EI, Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Spear LP. Repeated restraint stress alters sensitivity to the social consequences of ethanol in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2010;96:228–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Morales M, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Anxiolytic effects of the GABA(A) receptor partial agonist, L-838,417: impact of age, test context familiarity, and stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2013;109:31–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Increases in anxiety-like behavior induced by acute stress are reversed by ethanol in adolescent but not adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2012;100:440–50. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Faden VB. Trends in initiation of alcohol use in the United States 1975 to 2003. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:1011–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00115.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Spear LP, Varlinskaya EI. Sensitivity to ethanol and other hedonic stimuli in an animal model of adolescence: implications for prevention science? Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:236–43. doi: 10.1002/dev.20457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2012. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ahlström SK, Österberg EL. International Perspectives on Adolescent and Young Adult Drinking. Alcohol Research Health. 2004/2005;28:228–68. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Read JP, Wood MD, Kahler CW, Maddock JE, Palfai TP. Examining the role of drinking motives in college student alcohol use and problems. Psychol Addict Behav. 2003;17:13–23. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.17.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25:841–61. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Broadwater M, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure in early adolescent and adult male rats: effects on tolerance, social behavior, and ethanol intake. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1392–403. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01474.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brunell SC, Spear LP. Effect of stress on the voluntary intake of a sweetened ethanol solution in pair-housed adolescent and adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1641–53. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000179382.64752.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Doremus TL, Brunell SC, Rajendran P, Spear LP. Factors influencing elevated ethanol consumption in adolescent relative to adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:1796–808. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000183007.65998.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hargreaves GA, Wang EY, Lawrence AJ, McGregor IS. Beer promotes high levels of alcohol intake in adolescent and adult alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol. 2011;45:485–98. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lancaster FE, Brown TD, Coker KL, Elliott JA, Wren SB. Sex differences in alcohol preference and drinking patterns emerge during the early postpubertal period. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1996;20:1043–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01945.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Moore EM, Mariani JN, Linsenbardt DN, Melon LC, Boehm SL., 2nd Adolescent C57BL/6J (but not DBA/2J) mice consume greater amounts of limited-access ethanol compared to adults and display continued elevated ethanol intake into adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:734–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01143.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Schramm-Sapyta NL, Francis R, MacDonald A, Keistler C, O'Neill L, Kuhn CM. Effect of sex on ethanol consumption and conditioned taste aversion in adolescent and adult rats. Psychopharmacology. 2014;231:1831–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3319-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Vetter CS, Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Spear LP. Time course of elevated ethanol intake in adolescent relative to adult rats under continuous, voluntary-access conditions. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1159–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00417.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vetter-O'Hagen C, Varlinskaya E, Spear L. Sex differences in ethanol intake and sensitivity to aversive effects during adolescence and adulthood. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44:547–54. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mackintosh MA, Earleywine M, Dunn ME. Alcohol expectancies for social facilitation: A short form with decreased bias. Addict Behav. 2006;31:1536–46. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Smith GT, Goldman MS, Greenbaum PE, Christiansen BA. Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: the divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104:32–40. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.1.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Spear LP, Varlinskaya EI. Adolescence. Alcohol sensitivity, tolerance, and intake. Recent developments in alcoholism : an official publication of the American Medical Society on Alcoholism, the Research Society on Alcoholism, and the National Council on Alcoholism. 2005;17:143–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Trezza V, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJ. Prosocial effects of nicotine and ethanol in adolescent rats through partially dissociable neurobehavioral mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2560–73. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Differences in the social consequences of ethanol emerge during the course of adolescence in rats: social facilitation, social inhibition, and anxiolysis. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48:146–61. doi: 10.1002/dev.20124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Chronic tolerance to the social consequences of ethanol in adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2007;29:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Willey AR, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Social interactions and 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in adolescent and adult rats. Behav Brain Res. 2009;202:122–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Eckardt MJ, File SE, Gessa GL, Grant KA, Guerri C, Hoffman PL, et al. Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on the central nervous system. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:998–1040. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb03695.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Changes in sensitivity to ethanol-induced social facilitation and social inhibition from early to late adolescence. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021:459–61. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Ethanol-induced social facilitation in adolescent rats: role of endogenous activity at mu opioid receptors. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:991–1000. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00920.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Trezza V, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJ. The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2010;31:463–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2010.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Trezza V, Vanderschuren LJ. Bidirectional cannabinoid modulation of social behavior in adolescent rats. Psychopharmacology. 2008;197:217–27. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-1025-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Trezza V, Vanderschuren LJ. Cannabinoid and opioid modulation of social play behavior in adolescent rats: differential behavioral mechanisms. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18:519–30. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Trezza V, Vanderschuren LJ. Divergent effects of anandamide transporter inhibitors with different target selectivity on social play behavior in adolescent rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2009;328:343–50. doi: 10.1124/jpet.108.141069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Siviy SM, Line BS, Darcy EA. Effects of MK-801 on rough-and-tumble play in juvenile rats. Physiol Behav. 1995;57:843–7. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)00361-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Morales M, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Low doses of the NMDA receptor antagonists, MK-801, PEAQX, and ifenprodil, induces social facilitation in adolescent male rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;250:18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Varlinskaya EI, Truxell EM, Spear LP. Repeated restraint stress alters sensitivity to the social consequences of ethanol differentially in early and late adolescent rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2013;113:38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Enoch MA. The role of GABA(A) receptors in the development of alcoholism. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kumar S, Porcu P, Werner DF, Matthews DB, Diaz-Granados JL, Helfand RS, et al. The role of GABA(A) receptors in the acute and chronic effects of ethanol: a decade of progress. Psychopharmacology. 2009;205:529–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1562-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Lobo IA, Harris RA. GABA(A) receptors and alcohol. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90:90–4. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Werner DF, Blednov YA, Ariwodola OJ, Silberman Y, Logan E, Berry RB, et al. Knockin mice with ethanol-insensitive alpha1-containing gamma-aminobutyric acid type A receptors display selective alterations in behavioral responses to ethanol. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2006;319:219–27. doi: 10.1124/jpet.106.106161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Morris HV, Dawson GR, Reynolds DS, Atack JR, Stephens DN. Both alpha2 and alpha3 GABAA receptor subtypes mediate the anxiolytic properties of benzodiazepine site ligands in the conditioned emotional response paradigm. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:2495–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04775.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G. Short- and long-term effects of juvenile stressor exposure on the expression of GABAA receptor subunits in rats. Stress. 2012;15:416–24. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.634036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Veenema AH, Neumann ID. Maternal separation enhances offensive play-fighting, basal corticosterone and hypothalamic vasopressin mRNA expression in juvenile male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:463–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Koenig JI, Walker CD, Romeo RD, Lupien SJ. Effects of stress across the lifespan. Stress. 2011;14:475–80. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.604879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.McCormick CM, Mathews IZ. HPA function in adolescence: role of sex hormones in its regulation and the enduring consequences of exposure to stressors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:220–33. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.McCormick CM, Mathews IZ. Adolescent development, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function, and programming of adult learning and memory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010;34:756–65. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Romeo RD. Pubertal maturation and programming of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal reactivity. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2010;31:232–40. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Avital A, Richter-Levin G. Exposure to juvenile stress exacerbates the behavioural consequences of exposure to stress in the adult rat. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:163–73. doi: 10.1017/S1461145704004808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G. Differential impact of juvenile stress and corticosterone in juvenility and in adulthood, in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2010;214:268–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Tsoory M, Cohen H, Richter-Levin G. Juvenile stress induces a predisposition to either anxiety or depressive-like symptoms following stress in adulthood. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17:245–56. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Horovitz O, Tsoory MM, Hall J, Jacobson-Pick S, Richter-Levin G. Post-weaning to pre-pubertal ('juvenile') stress: a model of induced predisposition to stress-related disorders. Neuroendocrinology. 2012;95:56–64. doi: 10.1159/000331393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Jacobson-Pick S, Audet MC, McQuaid RJ, Kalvapalle R, Anisman H. Stressor exposure of male and female juvenile mice influences later responses to stressors: modulation of GABAA receptor subunit mRNA expression. Neuroscience. 2012;215:114–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Coventry TL, Jessop DS, Finn DP, Crabb MD, Kinoshita H, Harbuz MS. Endomorphins and activation of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. J Endocrinol. 2001;169:185–93. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1690185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.degli Uberti EC, Petraglia F, Bondanelli M, Guo AL, Valentini A, Salvadori S, et al. Involvement of mu-opioid receptors in the modulation of pituitary-adrenal axis in normal and stressed rats. J Endocrinol Invest. 1995;18:1–7. doi: 10.1007/BF03349688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Drolet G, Dumont EC, Gosselin I, Kinkead R, Laforest S, Trottier JF. Role of endogenous opioid system in the regulation of the stress response. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2001;25:729–41. doi: 10.1016/s0278-5846(01)00161-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Marinelli PW, Quirion R, Gianoulakis C. An in vivo profile of beta-endorphin release in the arcuate nucleus and nucleus accumbens following exposure to stress or alcohol. Neuroscience. 2004;127:777–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Daenen EW, Wolterink G, Gerrits MA, Van Ree JM. The effects of neonatal lesions in the amygdala or ventral hippocampus on social behaviour later in life. Behav Brain Res. 2002;136:571–82. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00223-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Shah AA, Treit D. Excitotoxic lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex attenuate fear responses in the elevated-plus maze, social interaction and shock probe burying tests. Brain Res. 2003;969:183–94. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)02299-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Canteras NS, Resstel LB, Bertoglio LJ, de Carobrez AP, Guimaraes FS. Neuroanatomy of anxiety. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010;2:77–96. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Crews F, He J, Hodge C. Adolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86:189–99. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Spear LP. Assessment of adolescent neurotoxicity: rationale and methodological considerations. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2007;29:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Diaz-Granados JL, Graham DL. The effects of continuous and intermittent ethanol exposure in adolesence on the aversive properties of ethanol during adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:2020–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00534.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.O'Dell LE, Roberts AJ, Smith RT, Koob GF. Enhanced alcohol self-administration after intermittent versus continuous alcohol vapor exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2004;28:1676–82. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000145781.11923.4e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Varlinskaya EI, Truxell E, Spear LP. Chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence: effects on social behavior and ethanol sensitivity in adulthood. Alcohol. 2014;48:433–44. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.01.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Nolen-Hoeksema S. Gender differences in risk factors and consequences for alcohol use and problems. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;24:981–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Schulte MT, Ramo D, Brown SA. Gender differences in factors influencing alcohol use and drinking progression among adolescents. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29:535–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Grant BF. Prevalence and correlates of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the United States: results of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58:464–73. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Przybycien-Szymanska MM, Rao YS, Pak TR. Binge-pattern alcohol exposure during puberty induces sexually dimorphic changes in genes regulating the HPA axis. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2010;298:E320–8. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00615.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Corpechot C, Young J, Calvel M, Wehrey C, Veltz JN, Touyer G, et al. Neurosteroids: 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha-pregnan-20-one and its precursors in the brain, plasma, and steroidogenic glands of male and female rats. Endocrinology. 1993;133:1003–9. doi: 10.1210/endo.133.3.8365352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Torres JM, Ruiz E, Ortega E. Effects of CRH and ACTH administration on plasma and brain neurosteroid levels. Neurochem Res. 2001;26:555–8. doi: 10.1023/a:1010925331768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Bitran D, Hilvers RJ, Kellogg CK. Anxiolytic effects of 3 alpha-hydroxy-5 alpha[beta]-pregnan-20-one: endogenous metabolites of progesterone that are active at the GABAA receptor. Brain Res. 1991;561:157–61. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90761-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Brot MD, Koob GF, Britton KT. Anxiolytic effects of steroid hormones during the estrous cycle. Interactions with ethanol. Recent developments in alcoholism : an official publication of the American Medical Society on Alcoholism, the Research Society on Alcoholism, and the National Council on Alcoholism. 1995;12:243–59. doi: 10.1007/0-306-47138-8_16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Carboni E, Wieland S, Lan NC, Gee KW. Anxiolytic properties of endogenously occurring pregnanediols in two rodent models of anxiety. Psychopharmacology. 1996;126:173–8. doi: 10.1007/BF02246353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Eser D, Baghai TC, Schule C, Nothdurfter C, Rupprecht R. Neuroactive steroids as endogenous modulators of anxiety. Curr Pharm Des. 2008;14:3525–33. doi: 10.2174/138161208786848838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Fleming RL, Acheson SK, Moore SD, Wilson WA, Swartzwelder HS. In the rat, chronic intermittent ethanol exposure during adolescence alters the ethanol sensitivity of tonic inhibition in adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36:279–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01615.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Spear LP, Swartzwelder HS. Adolescent alcohol exposure and persistence of adolescent-typical phenotypes into adulthood: A mini-review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;45C:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Sherrill LK, Berthold C, Koss WA, Juraska JM, Gulley JM. Sex differences in the effects of ethanol pre-exposure during adolescence on ethanol-induced conditioned taste aversion in adult rats. Behav Brain Res. 2011;225:104–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]