Abstract
The present investigation examined the incremental validity of anxiety sensitivity in the context of distress intolerance in terms of predicting smoking motives and outcome expectancies. Participants were a community sample of 144 daily smokers (85 women, Mage=29.72 years, SD=11.96). Results indicated that above and beyond discomfort intolerance, Axis I diagnoses, gender, volume of alcohol consumption, and average number of cigarettes smoked per day, anxiety sensitivity significantly incrementally predicted habitual, addictive, and negative affect reduction motives to smoke, as well as negative reinforcement outcome expectancies. No such incremental effects were similarly evident for distress intolerance. Findings are discussed in relation to the role of anxiety sensitivity in smoking motives and outcome expectancies.
Introduction
An increased amount of scientific attention has focused on the role of affective vulnerability in terms of smoking behavior (Anda et al., 1999; Brown, Lejuez, Kahler, Strong, & Zvolensky, 2005; Piasecki, Kenford, Smith, Fiore, & Baker, 1997; Quinn, Brandon, & Copeland, 1996; Zvolensky & Bernstein, 2005). In general, work has identified that both emotional vulnerability factors and psychiatric conditions are related to decreased success in quitting smoking, heavier rates of smoking, and greater perceived withdrawal symptoms during quit attempts (Brown & Wolfe, 1994; Feldner, Babson, & Zvolensky, 2007; Kalman, Morissette, & George, 2005; Koenen et al., 2005; Morissette, Tull, Gulliver, Kamholz, & Zimering, 2007; Morrell & Cohen, 2006).
To delineate underlying mechanisms explaining relationships between emotion vulnerability and smoking, one promising line of research has focused on anxiety sensitivity (Zvolensky & Bernstein, 2005). Anxiety sensitivity is operationalized as the fear of anxiety and arousal-related sensations (McNally, 2002) that reflects a trait-like, cognitive predisposition for anxiety psychopathology (Bernstein & Zvolensky, 2007; Taylor, 1999). Specifically, when high anxiety-sensitive individuals experience anxiety and related perturbation, they become “alarmed” by their own anxiety-related sensations, thereby intensifying their anxiety. Anxiety sensitivity is theoretically and empirically distinguishable from anxiety symptoms and other negative affect states (Rapee & Medoro, 1994). Over the past two decades, research has found that anxiety sensitivity is concurrently, incrementally, and prospectively associated with anxiety disorders, particularly panic and posttraumatic stress psychopathology (Hayward, Killen, Kraemer, & Taylor, 2000; Li & Zinbarg, 2007; Maller & Reiss, 1992; Schmidt, Lerew, & Jackson, 1997, 1999; Schmidt, Zvolensky, & Maner, 2006).
Empirical work also suggests anxiety sensitivity is related to clinically relevant smoking processes. Indeed, anxiety sensitivity has been significantly associated with early lapse and relapse during quit attempts among treatment-seeking and non-treatment-seeking adult smokers from the United States and Mexico (Brown, Kahler, Zvolensky, Lejuez, & Ramsey, 2001; Zvolensky, Bernstein et al., 2007). Zvolensky, Bonn-Miller, Bernstein, and Marshall (2006) found, for example, that anxiety sensitivity was significantly associated with increased risk of early smoking relapse among adult daily smokers. As another example, higher levels of anxiety sensitivity were related to greater intensity of nicotine withdrawal symptoms during past quit attempts (Zvolensky, Baker, et al., 2004) and increased levels of perceived barriers in quitting smoking (Zvolensky, Vujanovic, et al., 2007). Thus high, compared to low, anxiety-sensitive smokers evidence greater difficulties quitting, more intense withdrawal symptoms, and greater perceived barriers to cessation.
Efforts have also been increasingly directed toward the study of mechanisms linking the anxiety sensitivity construct to the motivational and cognitive-based beliefs about smoking behavior. Here, integrative models have suggested that clinically relevant linkages exist between anxiety sensitivity and affect regulatory motives and outcome expectancies (Zvolensky & Bernstein, 2005; Zvolensky, Schmidt, & Stewart, 2003). High, relative to low, anxiety-sensitive daily smokers, specifically, may be more apt to expect smoking to help alleviate aversive anxiety states and be motivated to smoke for affect regulation purposes despite also believing such behavior may yield negative personal consequences (e.g., respiratory irritation, physical illness). In the absence of other more adaptive (nonsmoking) affect-regulation skills, high anxiety-sensitive daily smokers may tend to rely on smoking to manage anxiety states and, paradoxically, fears about health status. Smoking among high anxiety-sensitive daily smokers also may be more likely to become an automatized drug response and predominant type of self-control strategy, because it is likely to become a frequent and reflexive means by which to manage psychological distress and well-being. Consistent with such theorizing, work has thus far indicated that anxiety sensitivity is associated with negative affect reduction smoking motives among adolescents (Comeau, Stewart, & Loba, 2001) and college student daily smokers (Novak, Burgess, Clark, Zvolensky, & Brown, 2003; Stewart, Karp, Pihl, & Peterson, 1997; Zvolensky et al., 2006). Also, anxiety sensitivity has been found to be related to smoking outcome expectancies for negative personal consequences (e.g., beliefs smoking will cause physical impairment such as cancer) as well as negative reinforcement (e.g., beliefs smoking will reduce negative affect) among adult treatment-seeking heavy smokers (>20 cigarettes/day; Brown, Kahler, et al., 2001) and college student daily smokers (Zvolensky, Feldner, et al., 2004). These findings suggest that fears of negative consequences may be related to smoking behavior to dampen (perceived or real) negative affect, while ultimately and perhaps paradoxically increasing the risk for anxiety problems (Zvolensky & Bernstein, 2005).
Although past work on anxiety sensitivity and smoking motives and outcome expectancies has been promising, three central limitations to such work are in need of further empirical scrutiny. First, it remains unclear whether other theoretically-related individual difference factors exhibit similar types of relationships with motivational processes and outcome expectancies, and more specifically, whether such relationships are independent of anxiety sensitivity. Of the theoretically-relevant factors that could explain anxiety sensitivity effects, distress intolerance is a viable candidate (Bernstein & Zvolensky, 2007; Brown et al., 2005). A number of terms have been used to describe “affect tolerance processes,” including distress intolerance, distress tolerance, discomfort intolerance, and task persistence (Zvolensky & Otto, 2007). Although nuances between these labels exist, their conceptual underpinnings are generally the same (Brown et al., 2005). We use the term distress intolerance here because we believe it best captures the operative processes at play for this construct. Distress intolerance reflects a trait-like individual difference variable referring to the subjective interpretation of sensations as either tolerable or threatening (Schmidt, Richey, Cromer, & Buckner, in press; Schmidt, Richey, & Fitzpatrick, 2006). Distress intolerance is significantly related to both anxiety symptoms (Schmidt et al., in press; Schmidt, Richey, et al., 2006) and early lapse to smoking (Brown, Lejuez, Kahler, & Strong, 2002). Thus previously observed effects between anxiety sensitivity and affect-relevant motivational and outcome expectancy factors may simply be due to shared variance with distress intolerance rather than fear of anxious arousal and its consequences more specifically. Second, given that anxiety sensitivity is related to mood-related and substance use disorder psychopathology (Schmidt, Zvolensky, et al., 2006), and such psychological conditions are related to the maintenance of smoking (Brown & Wolfe, 1994; Feldner et al., 2007; Kalman et al., 2005), the previously observed effects for anxiety sensitivity may be attributable to preexisting clinical conditions. However, none of the previous research on smoking motives or outcome expectancies has examined whether anxiety sensitivity effects are apparent above and beyond the variance attributable to concurrent psychopathology. Finally, previous work on anxiety sensitivity and smoking motives and outcome expectancies has focused largely on college student samples (Novak et al., 2003; Stewart et al., 1997; Zvolensky et al., 2006). The ability to generalize past work needs to be evaluated by extending sampling strategies to focus on daily smokers from the general community. This segment of society represents a large percentage of those persons who ultimately seek treatment for quitting smoking, and about whom it is particularly important to understand mechanisms underlying tobacco use and dependence (Orleans, Woolf, Rothemich, Marks, & Isham, 2006).
Overall, the present investigation sought to replicate and extend past research by evaluating the incremental validity of anxiety sensitivity in regard to smoking motivational and outcome expectancy factors among a community-recruited sample of daily smokers. It was hypothesized that higher levels of anxiety sensitivity would be significantly related to smoking for habitual, addictive, and negative affect reduction motives, for use above and beyond the shared variance with distress intolerance, Axis I diagnoses, gender, concurrent weekly alcohol use, and daily smoking rate. Additionally, it was hypothesized that higher levels of anxiety sensitivity would similarly be incrementally related to smoking outcome expectancies for negative personal consequences and negative reinforcement beyond the covariates.
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 144 daily smokers (85 women, Mage=29.72 years, SD=11.96). The racial distribution of the sample generally reflected that of the Vermont population (State of Vermont Department of Health, 2007): 92.4% of the total sample identified as White, 2.8% identified as Black, 3.4% identified as Hispanic, 0.7% identified as Black/Hispanic, and 0.7% identified as “other.” In terms of educational status, 2.9% of participants did not complete high school/GED, 16.4% completed high school/GED, 57.9% completed some college, 15.7% graduated from college, 2.1% completed some graduate work, 4.3% attained a graduate degree, and 0.7% did not report educational status. Participants were daily smokers, averaging 16.12 cigarettes/day (SD=7.54), and had a mean age at onset of daily cigarette use of 16.62 years (SD=5.03). The average score on the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND; Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerström, 1991) was 3.15 (SD=1.82), indicating a relatively low level of dependence. Approximately 65.5% of the sample reported using alcohol at least two to four times per month, averaging three to four alcoholic drinks per occasion.
Of the sample, 57.1% met criteria for at least one current Axis I disorder, while 41.9% of the participants did not meet criteria for any current Axis I disorders. Among all participants, 33.6% met criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder, 22.8% for generalized anxiety disorder, 19.2% for panic disorder (14.2% met criteria for panic disorder without agoraphobia, 5.0% with agoraphobia), 20.7% for major depression, 17.8% for social phobia, 6.4% for specific phobia, 2.8% for dysthymia, and 0.7% for bipolar disorder. Of the total sample, 15.3% met criteria for at least two Axis I disorders, 29.2% for three or more Axis I disorders, and 6.4% for four or more Axis I disorders.
Exclusionary criteria for the investigation included (a) limited mental competency or the inability to provide informed, written consent, (b) current suicidal or homicidal ideation, (c) current or past history of psychotic-spectrum symptoms, (d) current major medical problems (e.g., heart disease, cancer), (e) current use of nicotine replacement therapy, (f) current use of tobacco products other than cigarettes (e.g., cigars, chewing tobacco), and (g) pregnancy.
Measures
Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Client Interview Schedule (ADIS-IV; Brown et al., 1994)
The ADIS-IV is a semistructured diagnostic interview used to assess DSM-IV anxiety, mood, somatoform, and substance use disorders as well as to screen for the presence of psychotic disorders. Reliability of this measure has shown good to excellent interrater agreement for the majority of anxiety and mood disorders among participants who were given two independent administrations of the ADIS-IV (Brown, Di Nardo, Lehman, & Campbell, 2001). The presence of current Axis I psychopathology was assessed using the ADIS-IV in the present study by trained doctoral-level administrators. For the present investigation, diagnostic reliability ratings by an independent rater (MJZ) were completed on a random selection of 20% of the protocols, with no cases of disagreement being noted.
Smoking History Questionnaire (SHQ; Brown et al., 2002)
The SHQ is a continuous self-report questionnaire used to assess smoking history and pattern. The SHQ includes 20 items pertaining to smoking rate, age at onset of smoking initiation, and years of being a daily smoker. The response format varies by individual question. The SHQ has been used successfully in previous studies as a measure of smoking history (Zvolensky, Lejuez, Kahler, & Brown, 2004).
Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire (FTQ; Fagerström, 1978; Heatherton et al., 1991)
The FTQ was used as a continuous self-report measure of nicotine dependence and cigarettes smoked per day. The response format varies by individual question. Specifically, the FTQ was administered, and it was scored as the FTND. The FTND has shown good internal consistency, positive relationships with smoking variables (e.g., salivary cotinine; Heatherton et al., 1991; Payne, Smith, McCracken, McSherry, & Antony, 1994), and high degrees of test-retest reliability (Pomerleau, Carton, Lutzke, Flessland, & Pomerleau, 1994). For the current investigation, item 2 on the FTND was used to determine the average number of cigarettes smoked per day.
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; Babor, de la Fuente, Saunders, & Grant, 1992)
The AUDIT is a 10-item self-report screening measure developed by the World Health Organization to assess frequency of alcohol consumption and to identify individuals with alcohol problems (Babor et al., 1992). A large body of literature attests to the psychometric properties of the AUDIT (Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, & Grant, 1993). As in past work (Stewart, Zvolensky, & Eifert, 2001), the frequency and quantity items of the AUDIT were used to index weekly alcohol consumption (an average weekly-based frequency by quantity per occasion composite score).
Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI; Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, & McNally, 1986)
The ASI is a 16-item self-report measure rated on a 4-point Likert-type scale (0=very little to 4=very much) administered to assess the degree to which participants fear negative consequences stemming from anxiety symptoms. The ASI shows adequate test-retest reliability (r=.75 for 2 weeks) and criterion validity (e.g., individuals with agoraphobia score higher than those with other anxiety disorders and those with no disorder), and is distinct from trait anxiety (Peterson & Reiss, 1992). In the present investigation, the total ASI score was used because it represents the global-order anxiety sensitivity factor and therefore takes into account different types of fears, including fears of panic-related somatic, cognitive, and social cues.
Discomfort Intolerance Scale (DIS; Schmidt, Richey, et al., 2006)
The DIS is a five-item self-report measure rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale (0=“not at all like me” to 6=“extremely like me”) that was used to assess discomfort intolerance (the ability to tolerate physically aversive stimuli), which has been hypothesized as a risk factor for anxiety. Factor analysis has shown that the DIS contains an intolerance factor (α=.91), comprising two items, and an avoidance factor (α=.72), comprising three items. The intolerance factor measures intolerance of discomfort and pain, and the avoidance factor measures avoidance of physical discomfort. The DIS has shown strong criterion validation, when tested against established measures of psychopathology such as the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988), and also has shown construct validation (Schmidt, Richey, et al., 2006). In the present investigation, the DIS total score was used as a primary predictor variable, as it reflects the global distress intolerance construct.
Reasons for Smoking (RFS; Ikard, Green, & Horn, 1969)
The RFS is a self-report measure consisting of 23 items, rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1=never to 5=always), used to assess smoking motives. The psychometric properties of this scale, including measures of factor structure, internal consistency, and test-retest reliability, have been well established (Shiffman, 1993). For the present investigation, the following RFS scales were used owing to their theoretical relevance to the study objectives: habitual (e.g., “I’ve found a cigarette in my mouth and didn’t remember putting it there”), addictive (e.g., “Between cigarettes, I get a craving only a cigarette can satisfy”), and negative affect reduction (e.g., “When I feel uncomfortable or upset about something, I light up a cigarette”).
Smoking Consequences Questionnaire (SCQ; Brandon & Baker, 1991)
The SCQ is a 50-item self-report measure that assesses smoking expectancies on a 10-point scale for likelihood of occurrence (0=“completely unlikely” to 9=“completely likely”). The entire measure and its constituent factors have demonstrated sound psychometric properties (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Buckley et al., 2005; Downey & Kilbey, 1995). In the present investigation, the negative personal consequences subscales (e.g., “The more I smoke, the more I risk my health”) and negative reinforcement (e.g., “Smoking helps me calm down when I feel nervous”) of the SCQ were used to measure theoretically relevant outcome expectancies for smoking.
Procedure
Participants were recruited from the greater Burlington, Vermont, community, for participation in a smoking cessation study via placement of flyers throughout marketplaces and well-traveled locations, and posting of printed advertisements in local newspapers. Data for the present investigation were gathered at the baseline appointments, before participants set a smoking cessation date. During this baseline appointment, participants (a) provided verbal and written informed consent, (b) underwent a diagnostic evaluation (ADIS-IV) conducted by trained doctoral-level students to determine if any exclusion criteria were met, (c) provided carbon monoxide analysis of breath samples to biochemically verify their smoking status, and (d) completed an initial battery of self-report assessments, including those used in the present investigation. For all participants, the baseline evaluation occurred 2–3 weeks prior to their scheduled quit day. All participants received US$25 for completion of the baseline assessment session; participation in all eight appointments of the cessation study yielded a total compensation amount of $225. Only the baseline component of the data was used in the present report.
Data analyses
Five hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted using SPSS version 13.0 to examine the incremental validity of anxiety sensitivity (ASI–total score) and discomfort intolerance (DIS–total score), entered concurrently, in relation to the following criterion variables: (a) RFS–habitual, (b) RFS–addictive, (c) RFS–negative affect reduction, (d) SCQ–negative personal consequences, and (e) SCQ–negative reinforcement. These dependent variables were identified on an a priori basis for their theoretical relevance in terms of the present study objectives. At step 1 of each of the models, total number of Axis I diagnoses (up to three), gender, alcohol consumption (weekly alcohol consumption volume), and average number of cigarettes smoked per day were entered as covariates. At step 2 of each of the models, the main effects of anxiety sensitivity and discomfort intolerance were entered simultaneously. Overall, this analytic model ensured that any variance accounted for by the main effects at step 2 was not attributable to covariates at step 1 of the models (Sechrest, 1963). Furthermore, simultaneous entry of anxiety sensitivity and discomfort intolerance at step 2 ensured that any variance accounted for by the main effect of either putative predictor was not attributable to shared variance with the other.
Results
Zero-order association among theoretically relevant variables
See Table 1 for a summary of zero-order relations and descriptive data among all studied variables. The ASI–total score was significantly associated with the DIS–total score (r=.38, p<.01, 14% shared variance). Both the ASI–total score (r=.74, p<.01) and the DIS–total score (r=.37, p<.01) were significantly correlated with number of Axis I diagnoses, but not with any other covariates (i.e., gender, alcohol consumption, or average number of cigarettes smoked per day). Furthermore, the ASI–total score was significantly associated with each of the five criterion variables, including RFS–habitual (r=.34, p<.01), RFS–addictive (r=.44, p<.01), RFS–negative affect reduction (r=.47, p<.01), SCQ–negative personal consequences (r=.35, p<.01), and SCQ–negative reinforcement (r=.49, p<.01). Additionally, the DIS–total score was significantly correlated with four of the criterion variables, including RFS–habitual (r=.24, p<.01), RFS–addictive (r=.26, p<.01), RFS–negative affect reduction (r=.33, p<.01), and SCQ–negative reinforcement (r=.28, p<.01). The DIS–total score was not significantly related to SCQ–negative personal consequences.
Table 1.
Descriptive data and zero-order correlations among theoretically relevant variables (N=144).
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | Mean | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Anxiety sensitivity (total)a | – | .38** | .74** | .14 | −.04 | .06 | .34** | .44** | .47** | .35** | .49** | 21.6 | 13.1 |
2 | Discomfort intolerance (total)b | – | – | .37** | −.09 | −.11 | .17 | .24** | .26** | .33** | .17 | .28** | 12.3 | 5.2 |
3 | Axis I diagnoses (up to 3)c | – | – | – | .20* | −.13 | .14 | .30** | .43** | .54** | .39** | .53** | 1.3 | 1.3 |
4 | Genderd | – | – | – | – | −.12 | −.08 | .13 | .12 | .29** | .20* | .24** | – | – |
5 | Alcohol consumption (AUDIT)e | – | – | – | – | – | −.17* | −.11 | −.22** | −.26** | −.14 | −.12 | 5.1 | 5.1 |
6 | Cigarettes smoked/day (FTQ)f | – | – | – | – | – | – | .34** | .32** | .30** | .13 | .24** | 16.1 | 7.6 |
7 | RFS–habitualg | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | .58** | .44** | .32** | .46** | 2.1 | 0.68 |
8 | RFS–addictiveh | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | .59** | .34** | .53** | 3.1 | 0.82 |
9 | RFS–negative affect reductioni | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | .31** | .75** | 3.3 | 0.87 |
10 | SCQ–negative personal consequencesj | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | - | - | - | .35** | 5.4 | 1.9 |
11 | SCQ–negative reinforcementk | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | 6.5 | 1.4 |
Note.
Anxiety Sensitivity Index–total score.
Discomfort Intolerance Scale–total score.
number of current Axis I diagnoses (up to 3)–ADIS-IV.
Gender (1=male, 2=female).
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (frequency × quantity).
cigarettes smoked/day, Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–habitual subscale.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–addictive subscale.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–negative affect reduction subscale.
Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–negative personal consequences subscale.
Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–negative reinforcement subscale.
p<.05;
p<.01.
Hierarchical regression analyses
Table 2 summarizes the hierarchical regression analyses. In terms of the RFS–habitual subscale, step 1 of the model accounted for 17% of the variance, with number of Axis I diagnoses (p<.05) and average number of cigarettes smoked per day (p<.01) as significant univariate predictors. The second step of the model accounted for an additional and significant 5% of unique variance in predicting the RFS–habitual subscale. Anxiety sensitivity (ASI–total score) (p<.05), as expected, was the only unique significant predictor at step 2 of the model.
Table 2.
Summary of hierarchical regression analyses.
ΔR2 | t | β2 | sr2 | p value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(each predictor) | |||||
Criterion variable: RFS–habituala | |||||
Step 1 | .17 | <.01 | |||
Axis I diagnosesf | 2.63 | .22 | .05 | <.05 | |
Genderg | .99 | .08 | .01 | ns | |
Alcohol consumptionh | .10 | .01 | .00 | ns | |
Cigarettes smoked/dayi | 3.58 | .30 | .09 | <.01 | |
Step 2 | .05 | <.05 | |||
ASI–totalj | 2.35 | .28 | .04 | <.05 | |
DIS–totalk | .90 | .08 | .01 | ns | |
Criterion variable: RFS–addictiveb | |||||
Step 1f–i | .28 | <.01 | |||
Axis I diagnoses | 4.81 | .38 | .16 | <.01 | |
Gender | .42 | .03 | .00 | ns | |
Alcohol consumption | 1.70 | .13 | .02 | ns | |
Cigarettes smoked/day | 3.14 | .25 | .07 | <.01 | |
Step 2j,k | .06 | <.01 | |||
ASI–total | 3.19 | .35 | .08 | <.01 | |
DIS–total | .12 | .01 | .00 | ns | |
Criterion variable: RFS–negative affect reductionc | |||||
Step 1f–i | .38 | <.01 | |||
Axis I diagnoses | 5.95 | .44 | .22 | <.01 | |
Gender | 2.66 | .20 | .05 | <.01 | |
Alcohol consumption | 1.82 | .13 | .03 | ns | |
Cigarettes smoked/day | 3.02 | .22 | .07 | <.01 | |
Step 2j,k | .04 | <.05 | |||
ASI–total | 2.10 | .21 | .03 | <.05 | |
DIS–total | 1.56 | .12 | .02 | ns | |
Criterion variable: SCQ–negative personal consequencesd | |||||
Step 1f–i | .17 | <.01 | |||
Axis I diagnoses | 3.99 | .34 | .11 | <.01 | |
Gender | 1.41 | .12 | .02 | ns | |
Alcohol consumption | 1.02 | .09 | .01 | ns | |
Cigarettes smoked/day | .79 | .07 | .01 | ns | |
Step 2j,k | .01 | ns | |||
ASI–total | .85 | .10 | .01 | ns | |
DIS–total | .30 | .03 | .00 | ns | |
Criterion variable: SCQ–negative reinforcemente | |||||
Step 1f–i | .32 | <.01 | |||
Axis I diagnoses | 6.12 | .47 | .23 | <.01 | |
Gender | 1.85 | .14 | .03 | ns | |
Alcohol consumption | .02 | .00 | .00 | ns | |
Cigarettes smoked/day | 2.37 | .18 | .04 | <.05 | |
Step 2j,k | .04 | <.05 | |||
ASI–total | 2.50 | .27 | .05 | <.05 | |
DIS–total | .77 | .06 | .00 | ns |
Note. β=standardized beta weight.sr2=squared semi-partial correlation.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–habitual subscale.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–addictive subscale.
Reasons for Smoking Questionnaire–negative affect reduction subscale.
Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–negative personal consequences subscale.
Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–negative reinforcement subscale.
Number of current Axis I diagnoses (up to 3)–ADIS-IV.
Gender (1=male, 2=female).
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test.
Cigarettes smoked/day (Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire).
Anxiety Sensitivity Index–total score.
Discomfort Intolerance Scale–total score.
For the RFS–addictive subscale, step 1 of the model accounted for 28% of the variance, and both number of Axis I diagnoses (p<.01) and average number of cigarettes smoked per day (p<.01) were significant predictors. The second step of the model accounted for an additional and significant 6% of unique variance; anxiety sensitivity (ASI–total score) was the only significant incremental predictor (p<.01).
In regard to the RFS–negative affect reduction, step 1 of the model predicted 38% of the variance; gender (p<.01), number of Axis I diagnoses (p<.01), and number of cigarettes smoked per day (p<.01) were significant univariate predictors. The second step of the model accounted for an additional and significant 4% of variance. Anxiety sensitivity (ASI–total score) (p<.05) was, again, the only unique predictor at step 2 of the model.
For the SCQ–negative personal consequences subscale, step 1 of the model predicted 17% of the variance; number of Axis I diagnoses (p<.01) was the only significant univariate predictor. The second step of the model did not significantly predict any additional variance in terms of this criterion variable.
Finally, in terms of the SCQ–negative reinforcement subscale, step 1 of the model predicted 32% of the variance; number of Axis I diagnoses (p<.01) and average number of cigarettes smoked per day (p<.05) were significant univariate predictors. The second step of the model accounted for an additional and significant 4% of unique variance. As hypothesized, anxiety sensitivity (ASI–total score) was the only unique predictor at step 2 of the model (p<.05).
Discussion
Consistent with prediction, in terms of smoking-motivational processes, anxiety sensitivity was significantly and incrementally related to habitual, addictive, and negative affect reduction motives for smoking among the present sample of adult daily smokers. The observed effects were evident above and beyond the statistically significant amount of variance accounted for by Axis I diagnoses, gender, weekly alcohol consumption, and daily cigarette usage. Moreover, the incremental effect of anxiety sensitivity was not due to shared variance with distress intolerance as indexed by discomfort intolerance. The present results provide novel evidence that anxiety sensitivity is associated with habitual and addictive motivational smoking processes. These findings also extend past work on anxiety sensitivity and its association with negative affect reduction motives (Comeau et al., 2001; Novak et al., 2003; Stewart et al., 1997; Zvolensky et al., 2006). These findings are collectively consistent with the conjecture that high, relative to low, anxiety-sensitive daily smoking adults have a multifaceted motivational profile for their smoking behavior, including reasons related to habitual patterns of use, self-control-oriented addictive reasons, and coping with negative affect stressors. These linkages between anxiety sensitivity and smoking motives may help explain the lower rates of success in quitting among high anxiety-sensitive smokers (Brown, Kahler, et al., 2001; Zvolensky, Bernstein, et al., 2007; Zvolensky et al., 2006). Specifically, in the absence of other more adaptive (nonsmoking) coping strategies, high anxiety-sensitive daily smokers may rely on smoking to manage aversive mood states in a largely reflexive and self-control-oriented fashion. When attempting to quit, for example, such physical and emotional distress is highly probable, and lapse and relapse to smoking may become increasingly likely among high anxiety-sensitive smokers.
A second series of analyses focused on clarifying the relationships between anxiety sensitivity and smoking outcome expectancies for negative personal consequences and negative reinforcement. Here, the hypotheses were supported only partially. Although past work documented an association between anxiety sensitivity and negative personal consequences (Zvolensky, Feldner, et al., 2004), the present study suggests that such an effect may be attributable to co-occurring psychopathology rather than anxiety sensitivity per se, indicating that daily smokers with greater levels of psychopathology may be more likely to believe that smoking is a personally dangerous activity. Given that daily smokers with psychopathology often have difficulty quitting (Brown & Wolfe, 1994; Kalman et al., 2005; Koenen et al., 2006), it may be useful for future research to explore relationships between psychopathology and motivation to quit smoking. To the extent that psychopathology is related to smoking outcome expectancies for negative personal consequences, it presumably would be related to enhanced motivation to quit (Rundmo, Smedslund, & Gö testam, 1997). Yet, as expected, anxiety sensitivity was significantly related to negative reinforcement outcome expectancies; the incremental effect of anxiety sensitivity was not due to shared variance with distress intolerance or Axis I diagnoses, gender, weekly alcohol consumption, or daily cigarette usage. These findings offer novel empirical evidence for an association between anxiety sensitivity and negative reinforcement outcome expectancies. This observation helps clarify that past findings (Zvolensky, Feldner, et al., 2004) were not likely attributable to these other theoretically relevant factors such as level of Axis I psychopathology or distress intolerance. Moreover, such results suggest that daily smokers with high, compared to low, anxiety sensitivity tend to expect that smoking will relieve negative affect states. Thus, for these high-risk smokers to quit smoking successfully, interventions may need to target such beliefs through specialized psychoeducational activities.
Distress intolerance was not significantly incrementally predictive of any of the dependent variables after accounting for shared variance with anxiety sensitivity. Such findings are intriguing given that distress intolerance has been related to shorter duration of smoking abstinence among daily smokers (Brandon et al., 2003; Brown et al., 2002; Hajek, 1991; Hajek, Belcher, & Stapleton, 1987; West, Hajek, & Beltcher, 1989). The present results suggest that distress intolerance, as measured by the DIS, does not maintain robust relationships with smoking-relevant habitual, addictive, or negative affect reduction smoking motives, or negative personal consequences and negative reinforcement smoking outcome expectancies. Based on the present findings, it may be fruitful for future work to evaluate the incremental validity of distress intolerance in regard to smoking outcomes of interest, after accounting for variance explained by anxiety sensitivity.
Overall, the present study supports the hypothesized association between anxiety sensitivity and specific smoking motives and outcome expectancies. The main implication of the present work is that it may be clinically useful to integrate anxiety sensitivity into smoking cessation programs as a way to tailor interventions to yield more success for certain difficult-to-treat groups of daily smokers (e.g., daily smokers with anxiety disorders or high levels of anxiety sensitivity). For example, by providing psychoeducation about the motives and expectancies related to smoking behavior among high anxiety-sensitive smokers, it may be possible to facilitate a better understanding of their reasons for smoking and beliefs about smoking. Such knowledge could prompt more sophisticated efforts to understand and presumably alter smoking behavior among individuals who want to quit smoking. As a second example, by using cognitive restructuring and interoceptive exposure exercises to aversive internal cues (e.g., chair spinning, voluntary hyperventilation) specifically targeting anxiety sensitivity, it may be possible to reduce levels of anxiety sensitivity prior to a cessation attempt and, by extension, alter the tendency to use smoking for habitual, addictive, or negative affect reduction reasons (Zvolensky, Bernstein, Yartz, McLeish, & Feldner, et al., in press). Finally, acceptance-based therapies (e.g., acceptance and commitment therapy) could perhaps be usefully applied to target high anxiety-sensitive smokers to facilitate smoking cessation (Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999). Again, such efforts could theoretically offer greater probabilities of success during quitting by disrupting the interconnection between anxiety sensitivity and specific motives, and expectancies about smoking behavior.
The present study has a number of limitations. First, although the present study’s cross-sectional design provided an initial test of the associations between anxiety sensitivity and smoking motives and outcome expectancies, such an approach cannot shed light on processes over time or isolate causal or directional relationships between these variables. Future work could extend the present findings by using prospective methodologies to model anxiety sensitivity and smoking motivational and expectancy relationships over time.
Second, self-report measures were used as the primary assessment methodology. Self-report methods do not protect against reporting errors and may inflate observed relationships via shared method variance. Future studies could build on the present work by using alternative assessment instruments such as cognitive science tasks that tap implicit and automatic smoking motives and expectancies. Third, the present sample comprised a relatively homogenous group of daily smokers who were regular but not “heavy” smokers (i.e., low levels of nicotine dependence; Pomerleau, Majchrzak, & Pomerleau, 1989). Thus, the results may be related to a self-selection bias and the corresponding smoking history characteristics of the present sample. It will be useful for researchers to draw from a more diverse group of heavy smokers to determine whether the observed findings may be generalized to other populations.
Fourth, we used the DIS as a measure of distress intolerance. Although the DIS is a psychometrically sound self-report index of a theoretically important dimension of distress intolerance (for physical-based stress), other smoking work has used other indices of the construct (e.g., the behavioral latency to terminate an aversive event; Brown et al., 2002). Future work could therefore usefully replicate and extend the present findings through the use of tasks such as behavioral indices of intolerance for psychological and physical stress (Brown et al., 2002). For example, work could ascertain whether alternative measures of distress intolerance for psychological stressors (e.g., negative affect) may play a unique explanatory role in terms of smoking expectancies and motivational processes. Overall, the present results may best capture the explanatory relevance of distress intolerance for physical stress, but may not be fully applicable to psychological stressors.
Finally, although the present study covaried for psychopathology in tests of anxiety sensitivity and discomfort intolerance, the rates of specific disorders was not equally represented. Specifically, there were more persons with posttraumatic stress disorder than other disorders. Although all participants were recruited from the community for the same study, differential base rates of specific disorders were apparent. Future work could benefit by replicating and extending the present study among smokers with a more diverse and balanced psychopathology profile.
Together, the present investigation provides novel empirical data pertaining to the association between anxiety sensitivity and specific smoking motives and outcome expectancies. Findings suggest that anxiety sensitivity is uniquely related to negative affect reduction motives and expectancies, as well as habitual and addictive motives for use among daily adult smokers. Such effects were not accounted for by shared variance with level of distress intolerance or psychopathology, concurrent weekly alcohol, daily tobacco use, or gender.
Acknowledgments
Declaration of interest: This paper was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse research grants (MH076629-01, DA018734-01A1, and DA16307-01) awarded to Dr. Zvolensky. This work also was supported by a National Research Service award (DA021006-01) to Anka A. Vujanovic.
References
- Anda R, Croft JB, Felitti VJ, Nordenberg D, Giles WH, Williamson DF, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and smoking during adolescence and adulthood. The Journal of the American Medical Association. 1999;282:1652–1658. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.17.1652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Saunders J, Grant M. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for use in primary care (WHO Publication No. 92.4) Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Epstein M, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56:893–897. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Garbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review. 1988;8:77–100. [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein A, Zvolensky MJ. Anxiety sensitivity: Selective review of promising research and future directions. Expert Review in Neurotherapeutics. 2007;2:97–101. doi: 10.1586/14737175.7.2.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandon TH, Baker TB. The Smoking Consequences Questionnaire: The subjective expected utility of smoking in college students. Psychological Assessment. 1991;3:484–491. [Google Scholar]
- Brandon TH, Herzog TA, Juliano LM, Irvin JE, Lazev AB, Nath V. Pretreatment task persistence predicts smoking cessation outcome. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:448–456. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA, Di Nardo P, Barlow DH. Anxiety disorders interview schedule adult version (ADIS-IV): Client interview schedule. Albany, NY: Graywind Publications; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Brown TA, Di Nardo PA, Lehman CL, Campbell LA. Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: Implications for the classification of emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:49–58. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RA, Kahler CW, Zvolensky MJ, Lejuez CW, Ramsey SE. Anxiety sensitivity: Relationship to negative affect smoking and smoking cessation in smokers with past major depressive disorder. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26:887–899. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00241-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR. Distress tolerance and duration of past smoking cessation attempts. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:180–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RA, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Strong DR, Zvolensky MJ. Early lapse following smoking cessation: Relationship to negative affect and distress tolerance. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:713–733. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown PJ, Wolfe J. Substance-use and posttraumatic stress disorder comorbidity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1994;35:51–59. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(94)90110-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckley TC, Kamholz BW, Mozley SL, Gulliver SB, Holohan DR, Helstrom AW, et al. A psychometric evaluation of the Smoking Consequences Questionnaire–Adult in smokers with psychiatric conditions. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2005;7:739–745. doi: 10.1080/14622200500259788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comeau N, Stewart SH, Loba P. The relations of trait anxiety, anxiety sensitivity, and sensation seeking to adolescents’ motivations for alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use. Addictive Behaviors. 2001;26:803–825. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00238-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Downey KK, Kilbey MM. Relationship between nicotine dependence and alcohol expectancies and substance dependence. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1995;3:174–182. [Google Scholar]
- Fagerström K. Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addictive Behaviors. 1978;3:235–241. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(78)90024-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldner MT, Babson KA, Zvolensky MJ. Smoking, traumatic event exposure, and post-traumatic stress: A critical review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:14–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajek P. Individual differences in difficulty quitting smoking. British Journal of Addiction. 1991;86:555–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01807.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hajek P, Belcher M, Stapleton J. Breath-holding endurance as a predictor of success in smoking cessation. Addictive Behaviors. 1987;12:285–288. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(87)90041-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Hayward C, Killen J, Kraemer H, Taylor C. Predictors of panic attacks in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:207–214. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200002000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO. The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: A revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction. 1991;86:1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ikard FF, Green DE, Horn D. A scale to differentiate between types of smoking as related to the management of affect. The International Journal of Addictions. 1969;4:649–659. [Google Scholar]
- Kalman D, Morissette SB, George TP. Co-morbidity of smoking and psychiatric and substance use disorders. American Journal on Addictions. 2005;14:106–123. doi: 10.1080/10550490590924728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenen KC, Hitsman B, Lyons MJ, Niaura R, McCaffery J, Goldberg J, et al. A twin registry study of the relationship between posttraumatic stress disorder and nicotine dependence in men. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:1258–1265. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W, Zinbarg RE. Anxiety sensitivity and panic attacks: A one-year longitudinal study. Behavior Modification. 2007;31:145–161. doi: 10.1177/0145445506296969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maller RG, Reiss S. Anxiety sensitivity in 1984 and panic attacks in 1987. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 1992;6:241–147. [Google Scholar]
- McNally RJ. Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:938–946. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01475-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morissette SB, Tull MT, Gulliver SB, Kamholz BW, Zimering RT. Anxiety, anxiety disorders, tobacco use, nicotine: A critical review of interrelationships. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:245–272. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrell HER, Cohen LM. Cigarette smoking, anxiety, and depression. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2006;28:281–295. [Google Scholar]
- Novak A, Burgess ES, Clark M, Zvolensky MJ, Brown RA. Anxiety sensitivity, self-reported motives for alcohol and nicotine use and level of consumption. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2003;17:165–180. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00175-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orleans C, Woolf S, Rothemich S, Marks J, Isham G. The top priority: Building a better system for tobacco-cessation counseling. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2006;31:103–106. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne TJ, Smith PO, McCracken LM, McSherry WC, Antony MM. Assessing nicotine dependence: A comparison of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire (FTQ) with the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) in a clinical sample. Addictive Behaviors. 1994;19:307–317. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90032-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RA, Reiss S. Anxiety sensitivity index manual. Worthington, OH: International Diagnostic Systems; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Piasecki TM, Kenford SL, Smith SS, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Listening to nicotine: Negative affect and the smoking withdrawal conundrum. Psychological Science. 1997;8:184–189. [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau CS, Carton SM, Lutzke ML, Flessland KA, Pomerleau OF. Reliability of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire and the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence. Addictive Behaviors. 1994;19:33–39. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90049-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau CS, Majchrzak MI, Pomerleau OF. Nicotine dependence and the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire: A brief review. Journal of Substance Abuse. 1989;1:471–477. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn E, Brandon T, Copeland A. Is task persistence related to smoking and substance abuse? The application of learned industriousness theory to addictive behaviors. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;4:186–190. [Google Scholar]
- Rapee RM, Medoro L. Fear of physical sensations and trait anxiety as mediators of the response to hyperventilation in nonclinical subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103:696–699. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.4.693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiss S, Peterson RA, Gursky M, McNally RJ. Anxiety, sensitivity, anxiety frequency, and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1986;24:1–8. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(86)90143-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rundmo T, Smedslund G, Gö testam KG. Motivation for smoking cessation among the Norwegian public. Addictive Behaviors. 1997;22:377–386. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(96)00056-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the AUDIT: WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption–II. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Lerew DR, Jackson RJ. The role of anxiety sensitivity in the pathogenesis of panic: Prospective evaluation of spontaneous panic attacks during acute stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:355–364. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Lerew DR, Jackson RJ. Prospective evaluation of anxiety sensitivity in the pathogenesis of panic: Replication and extension. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:532–537. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Cromer KR, Buckner JD. Discomfort intolerance: Evaluation of a potential risk factor for anxiety pathology. Behavior Modification. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.08.004. (in press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Fitzpatrick KK. Discomfort intolerance: Development of a construct and measure relevant to panic disorder. Anxiety Disorders. 2006;20:263–280. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt NB, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK. Anxiety sensitivity: Prospective prediction of panic attacks and Axis I psychopathology. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2006;40:691–699. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sechrest L. Incremental validity: A recommendation. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1963;23:153–158. [Google Scholar]
- Shiffman S. Assessing smoking patterns and motives. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:732–742. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.5.732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- State of Vermont Department of Health. 2007 Retrieved June 30, 2007, from www.healthvermont.gov.
- Stewart SH, Karp J, Pihl RO, Peterson RA. Anxiety sensitivity and self-reported reasons for drug use. Journal of Substance Abuse. 1997;9:223–240. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(97)90018-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stewart S, Zvolensky M, Eifert G. Negative-reinforcement drinking motives mediate the relation between anxiety sensitivity and increased drinking behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;31:157–171. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor S. Anxiety sensitivity: Theory, research, and treatment of the fear of anxiety. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- West R, Hajek P, Belcher M. Severity of withdrawal symptoms as a predictor of outcome of an attempt to quit smoking. Psychological Medicine. 1989;19:981–985. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700005705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Baker KM, Leen-Feldner EW, Bonn-Miller MO, Feldner MT, Brown RA. Anxiety sensitivity: Association with intensity of retrospectively-rated smoking-related withdrawal symptoms and motivation to quit. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2004;33:114–125. doi: 10.1080/16506070310016969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A. Cigarette smoking and panic psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14:301–305. [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Cardenas SJ, Colotla VA, Marshall EC, Feldner MT. Anxiety sensitivity and early relapse to smoking: A test among Mexican daily, low-level smokers. Nicotine and Tobacco and Research. 2007;9:483–491. doi: 10.1080/14622200701239621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Bernstein A, Yartz AR, McLeish A, Feldner MT. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of comorbid panic psychopathology and tobacco use and dependence. In: Stewart SH, Conrad P, editors. Comorbidity of anxiety and substance use disorders. New York: Springer; (in press). [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Bonn-Miller MO, Bernstein A, Marshall EC. Anxiety sensitivity and abstinence duration to smoking. Journal of Mental Health. 2006;15:659–670. [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Feldner MT, Leen-Feldner E, Bonn-Miller MO, McLeish AC, Gregor K. Evaluating the role of anxiety sensitivity in smoking outcome expectancies among regular smokers. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2004;28:473–486. [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Lejuez CW, Kahler CW, Brown RA. Nonclinical panic attack history and smoking cessation: An initial examination. Addictive Behaviors. 2004;29:825–830. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Otto MW. Affective intolerance, sensitivity, and processing: Advances in clinical science. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38:228–233. [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB, Stewart SH. Panic disorder and smoking. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2003;10:29–51. [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Vujanovic AA, Bonn-Miller MO, Bernstein A, Yartz AR, Gregor KL, et al. Incremental validity of anxiety sensitivity in terms of motivation to quit, reasons for quitting, and barriers to quitting among community-recruited daily smokers. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2007;9:965–975. doi: 10.1080/14622200701540812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]