Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jun 5.
Published in final edited form as: J Health Soc Behav. 2015 Apr 24;56(2):225–245. doi: 10.1177/0022146515582099

Longitudinal Associations among Discordant Sexual Orientation Dimensions and Hazardous Drinking in a Cohort of Sexual Minority Women

Amelia E Talley 1, Frances Aranda 2, Tonda L Hughes 2, Bethany Everett 3, Timothy P Johnson 2
PMCID: PMC4456672  NIHMSID: NIHMS690033  PMID: 25911224

Abstract

We examined differences between sexual minority women’s (SMW’s) sexual identity and sexual behavior or sexual attraction as potential contributors to hazardous drinking across a 10-year period. Data are from a longitudinal study examining drinking and drinking-related problems in a diverse, community-based sample of self-identified SMW (Wave 1: n = 447; Wave 2: n = 384; Wave 3: n = 354). Longitudinal cross-lagged models showed that SMW who report higher levels of identity-behavior or identity-attraction discordance may be at greater risk of concurrent and subsequent hazardous drinking. Results of multigroup models suggest that sexual orientation discordance is a more potent risk factor for risky drinking outcomes among SMW in older adulthood than in younger adulthood. Findings support that discordance between sexual orientation dimensions may contribute to hazardous drinking among SMW and provide evidence that cognitive-behavioral consistency is important for individuals expressing diverse and fluid sexual identities, attraction, and behavior.

Keywords: alcohol use, hazardous drinking, longitudinal, sexual minorities, women’s sexuality

BACKGROUND

Sexual minority–identified women (e.g., bisexual, lesbian) report higher rates of hazardous drinking than heterosexual-identified women (Dermody et al. 2013; Hughes et al. 2010; King et al. 2008; Marshal et al. 2008, 2009; McCabe et al. 2009). Previous research has investigated risk factors, such as victimization and discrimination, associated with alcohol-related disparities between sexual minority and heterosexual women (Hughes et al. 2010, 2014; McCabe et al. 2010). Although important, such risk factors do not fully account for variance in hazardous drinking across sexual minority subgroups (Hughes et al. 2014; McCabe et al. 2010). In the current work, we examine differences (i.e., discordance) between sexual minority women’s (SMW’s) sexual identity and two other major dimensions of sexual orientation (sexual attraction and behavior) as potential contributors to hazardous drinking. An individual’s sexual orientation is informed, in part, by his or her self-identification (i.e., self-labeling of sexual identity), sexual attraction (i.e., degree of opposite- vs. same-sex sexual desire), and sexual behavior (i.e., degree of opposite- vs. same-sex sexual activity) (Laumann et al., 1994). Thus, we use the term SMW to refer to a diverse group of women who indicate any nonexclusive heterosexual identity, same-sex sexual behavior, or same-sex sexual attraction.

Although few researchers assess multiple dimensions of sexual orientation, those who do have found that sexual identity, sexual attraction, and sexual behavior are often not concordant within individuals (Burgard, Cochran, and Mays 2005; Smith et al. 2003), even within the same assessment (Brooks and Quina 2009; Chandra et al. 2011; Igartua et al. 2009; Narring, Huwiler, and Michaud 2003; Pathela et al. 2006; Ross et al. 2003). Women tend to endorse more differences among and changes in their sexual orientation dimensions than men (Diamond 2000; Ott et al. 2013; Peplau and Garnets 2000). Diamond (2000:242) posits that transitions, or “fluidity,” among sexual identity labels may be “an inevitable consequence of the fact that most sexual minority women experience attractions for both sexes, although in widely varying degrees.” Diamond (2008, 2012) argues that transitions in sexual identity may reflect attempts to accurately represent and resolve common fluctuations in sexual experiences and desires over time. Thus, discordance among sexual orientation dimensions at any given time may merely reflect greater sexual fluidity or, alternatively, difficulty integrating dynamic aspects of sexual orientation into one’s sense of self.

Data from interviews with sexual minority individuals (Galupo et al. 2014) suggest that a self-ascribed sexual identity is essential for contemplating and defining sexual orientation. In this study, participants viewed their current sexual identity as “primary over current and past experience that might otherwise be interpreted as ‘contradictory’” (Galupo et al. 2014:16) and indicated that they used multiple dimensions of their sexual self-concept when labeling their sexual identity. These qualitative findings support the notion that sexual minority persons value the individualistic meanings that can be ascribed to sexual identity labels.

Recent quantitative studies (Diamond 2000, 2008, 2012; Galupo et al. 2014; Katz-Wise and Hyde 2014; Ott et al. 2013; Savin-Williams, Joyner, and Rieger 2012) also support that SMW report high levels of fluidity over the life course in regard to both their sexual identity and the sex of their sexual and romantic partners. Additionally, some women maintain a heterosexual or lesbian identity, even as they engage in sexual and romantic relationships or acknowledge attractions that appear to contradict their self-ascribed identity label (Diamond 2000, 2005, 2008, 2012). Although not all SMW view such contradictions as a “threat” to their identity or an indication that they are unable to endorse prescribed values and behaviors associated with that identity (Diamond 2005), some may retain a lesbian or bisexual identity due to fear of fulfilling negative stereotypes that their identity was “just a phase” or anticipated rejection from heterosexual or sexual minority peers (Diamond 2003; Tabatabai and Linders 2011).

Despite evidence that apparent contradictions among sexual orientation dimensions are normative, little is known about the potential consequences of incongruence. Social identity theory posits that maintaining alignment between an identity standard (the normative values and behaviors associated with a specific identity) and one’s self-evaluation of his or her performance of that standard is critical to mental health (Burke 1991, 2006; Stets and Burke 2000; Stone and Cooper 2001). Disjuncture between an identity standard and evaluation of associated behaviors may be associated with high levels of cognitive dissonance (Festinger 1962; Stone and Cooper 2001) and evoke strategies intended to alleviate accompanying psychological discomfort (Elliot and Devine 1994).

Given the well-established need for individuals’ cognitions to align with their normative self-standards (e.g., Stone and Cooper 2001)—a major tenet of cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger 1962)—discordance among sexual orientation dimensions may contribute to greater risk of hazardous drinking. Steele, Southwick, and Critchlow (1981) showed that alcohol may be misused by some individuals to lessen psychological discomfort that results from feelings of dissonance. The authors emphasize that use of alcohol may reduce psychological discomfort by, ostensibly, providing a means of distraction without necessarily compelling dissonance-reducing cognitions or attitude change.

Few studies have examined the association between alcohol use and discordant sexual orientation dimensions, and those that do have relied on samples of predominately heterosexual-identified women. Using data from the 2000 National Alcohol Survey, Drabble, Midanik, and Trocki (2005) found that self-identified heterosexual women who reported a history of same-sex sexual behavior consumed a higher mean number of drinks per year than did heterosexual-identified women who reported only opposite-sex behavior. Similarly, Bauer, Jairam, and Baidoobonso (2010) found that self-identified heterosexual women who reported recent same-sex sexual behavior were more likely to drink weekly and engage in heavy episodic drinking than heterosexual-identified women who had not reported recent same-sex behavior. Using data from the National Epidemiologic Survey of Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC), Gattis, Sacco, and Cunningham-Williams (2012) found that self-identified heterosexual women who reported any history of same-sex sexual behavior (i.e., discordant sexual behavior) were at heightened risk for a lifetime alcohol use disorder (AUD) compared to both self-identified heterosexual women who reported a lifetime history of sexual behavior with only men and lesbian/bisexual women who reported a lifetime history of sexual behavior with any same-sex partners. Thus, identity-behavior discordance appears to relate to elevated alcohol involvement, particularly among heterosexual-identified women.

Whereas all of the aforementioned studies examined sexual identity–behavior discordance and alcohol involvement, only Gattis and colleagues (2012) also presented associations between sexual identity–attraction discordance and alcohol involvement. Their results showed that self-identified heterosexual women who reported “mostly” or “only” same-sex attractions appeared to be at lower risk of a lifetime AUD diagnosis than both self-identified heterosexual women who reported being “mostly” or “only” attracted to men (i.e., attraction-concordant) and lesbian/bisexual women who reported being “equally,” “mostly,” or “only” attracted to women. Thus, although previous work has examined associations between discordance among sexual orientation dimensions and alcohol use in samples of predominately heterosexual women, virtually none have examined these relations in samples of SMW.

Acknowledging a lack of available studies, it is reasonable to consider that associations between identity-behavior or identity-attraction discordance and hazardous drinking will differ based on respondents’ life stage or racial-ethnic identity. Because sexual fluidity is considered more normative in young adulthood (Dube and Savin-Williams 1999; Savin-Williams and Diamond 2000), we expect discordance among sexual orientation dimensions will be less problematic for women in younger adulthood than older adulthood. Importantly, sexual minority women of color, especially black SMW, are less likely than their white counterparts to disclose being lesbian or bisexual within their racial-ethnic community (Bradford, Ryan, and Rothblum 1994; Morris and Rothblum 1999), which may be due, in part, to greater perceptions of stigma associated with homosexuality in communities of color (Chae et al. 2010; Doll and Beeker 1996; Scheer et al. 2003). Nevertheless, research findings suggest that there are few differences in the timing of various sexual identity development milestones (e.g., age of first awareness of same-sex attraction) and current sexual identity status among black, Latino, and white young adults (Rosario, Schrimshaw, and Hunter 2004). Moreover, although Parks, Hughes, and Matthews (2004) found that black and Latina SMW were significantly younger than white SMW in reaching all sexual identity milestones, the elapsed time between milestones was similar across racial-ethnic groups. Given previous findings of few racial-ethnic differences in the timing and ascription of a minority sexual identity but possible discrepancies in levels of identity integration and disclosure across public and private domains (Bradford et al. 1994; Morris and Rothblum 1999; Parks et al. 2004), we expect more reliable relations between identity-behavior and identity-attraction discordance and hazardous drinking for racial-ethnic minority SMW.

Current Study

We used data from the Chicago Health and Life Experiences of Women (CHLEW) study, a three-wave longitudinal study of adult SMW, to investigate whether discordance among sexual orientation dimensions influences risk for hazardous drinking over a 10-year period. Given the central role of sexual identity among sexual minority persons (Galupo et al. 2014), we chose to examine discordance between SMW’s sexual identity and their concurrently reported sexual behavior or attraction. We hypothesize that SMW who report higher levels of identity-behavior discordance will report higher levels of hazardous drinking at the same assessment as well as at subsequent assessments. Although individuals are more likely held accountable for identity standards related to enacted behavior (e.g., sexual behavior), as opposed to internal feelings (e.g., sexual attraction) that are less observable, we expect that higher levels of identity-attraction discordance will also arouse psychological discomfort and result in elevated hazardous drinking both concurrently and prospectively but note that that these effects may be less pronounced than those for identity-behavior discordance. Capitalizing on the diversity of this sample, we also examine whether results differ based on respondents’ stage of life course and racial-ethnic identity.

DATA AND METHODS

Participants

As described previously, data are from the CHLEW study, a prospective study of risk and protective factors related to hazardous drinking among SMW. The first wave of data was collected in 2000–2001 (n = 447); the second wave, collected in 2004, retained 86% of the original sample (n = 384). At Wave 3 (10-year follow-up), 84% (n = 354) of women in the original sample who were alive and healthy enough (respondent-determined) to be interviewed participated in follow-up interviews. Advertisements were placed in local gay and mainstream newspapers in Chicago and flyers posted on relevant Internet list-servs and locations frequented by SMW. Outreach efforts targeted hard-to-reach women who have been underrepresented in previous studies of SMW, including women of color, older women, and women with lower levels of education and income (see Table 1 for a sample description).

Table 1.

Sample Characteristics for Total Sample and Split by Age and Race-ethnic Group, Chicago Health and Life Experiences of Women 2000–2010 (N = 447).

Total Sample
≤40 Years
≥41 Years
Racial-ethnic Minority
White
N = 447
n = 269
n = 178
n = 234
n = 213
n % n % n % n % n %
Age
 ≤30 141 32 82 35 59 28
 31–40 years 128 29 83 35 45 21
 41–50 years 104 23 56 24 48 23
 ≥ 51 years 74 17 13 6 61 29
Race
 African American 123 28 84 31 39 22
 Hispanic/Latina 88 20 63 23 25 14
 White 213 48 104 39 109 61
 Other 23 5 18 7 5 3
Education
 ≤High school 62 14 40 15 22 12 47 20 15 7
 Some college 135 30 96 36 39 22 91 39 44 21
 Bachelor’s 118 26 76 28 42 24 54 23 64 30
 Graduate/professional 132 29 57 21 75 42 42 18 90 42
Employment
 Full-time 305 68 179 67 126 71 151 65 154 72
 Part-time 51 11 30 11 21 12 25 11 26 12
 Unemployed, looking 45 10 34 13 11 6 29 12 16 8
 Unemployed, not looking 46 10 26 10 20 11 29 12 17 8
Income
 < $20,000 111 25 84 31 27 15 70 30 41 20
 $20,000–$39,999 113 26 71 26 42 24 68 29 45 21
 $40,000–$74,999 121 27 69 26 52 29 60 26 61 29
 ≤$75,000 96 22 41 15 55 31 33 14 63 30
Relationship
 Living with partner 196 46 117 43 79 44 88 38 108 52
 Not living with partner 97 23 63 23 34 19 55 24 42 20
 Not in committed relationship 136 32 81 30 55 31 80 34 56 27
Parental status
 Ever children 140 31 70 26 70 39 92 39 48 23
 Children at home 90 21 61 23 29 16 60 26 30 15

Note. Ever children and children at home were measured with a binary response options. Racial-ethnic minority participants self-identified as black/African American, Latina/Hispanic, Native American, Asian/Pacific Islander, or “other” race-ethnicity.

Women were eligible to participate in the study if they were 18 years old or older, self-identified as lesbian, spoke English, and lived in Chicago or surrounding suburbs. Although women who identified as heterosexual, bisexual, or another identity in the telephone screening were excluded, in the baseline interview 11 women self-identified as bisexual. Face-to-face interviews lasting 60 to 90 minutes were conducted by female interviewers. All baseline (Wave 1) interviews took place in respondents’ homes, study offices, or another private setting. In subsequent waves, participants who had moved from the area were interviewed by phone (Wave 2, n = 49; Wave 3, n = 97).

Participants were 37.5 (SD = 11.7) years old, on average, at baseline. Forty-eight % identified as non-Hispanic white, 48% identified as black/African American or Latina, and about 5% identified as multiethnic or “other” race-ethnicity. Ninety% of the baseline sample participated in at least one follow-up assessment, and 74.5% completed all assessments. To assess possible bias due to attrition, nonresponse rates were examined in relation to major drinking outcomes and demographic variables (age, race-ethnicity, education, income, employment, relationship status, and having children living at home). In logistic regression models controlling for demographic and drinking variables, having a high school education or less significantly predicted attrition at Wave 2 (odd ratio [OR] = 3.39), whereas reporting a history of childhood sexual abuse (OR = 1.46) or one or more children under age 18 living in the home (OR = 1.48) were each predictors of attrition at Wave 3.

Measures

Sexual Orientation

Three major sexual orientation dimensions were assessed at each wave. Current sexual identity was assessed at each wave with a single item: “How do you define your sexual identity?” Response options were: 1 = only homosexual/lesbian/gay, 2 = mostly homosexual/lesbian/gay, 3 = bisexual, 4 = mostly heterosexual, 5 = only heterosexual. Similarly, sexual attraction and sexual behavior were assessed using 5-point items asking participants, “Which of the following best describes who you could be sexually attracted to?” and “In the past five years, has your partner(s) in your sexual relationship(s) been … ” (1 = only women, 2 = mostly women, 3 = equally men and women, 4 = mostly men, 5 = only men). Thus, participants’ sexual identity, behavior, and attraction were measured at each wave on a 1 to 5 response scale. At Wave 1, participants were asked about the sex of sexual partners since age 18, whereas at subsequent waves, they were asked about the sex of partners “in the past five years.” Within each wave, sexual orientation discordance was operationalized with a difference score, specified as a latent variable, that explicitly represented the mathematical difference between observable responses on items assessing sexual identity and behavior as well as sexual identity and attraction, respectively (see Analytic Procedure in the following) (Selig and Preacher 2009).

Hazardous Drinking

Consistent with previous work using these data (Johnson et al., 2013), at each wave, a latent variable was specified with four dichotomous indicators of hazardous drinking in the previous 12 months: (1) heavy episodic drinking, (2) intoxication, (3) alcohol-related negative consequences, and (4) symptoms of alcohol dependence. Specifically, heavy episodic drinking was assessed at each wave by asking, “During the last 12 months, how often did you have six or more drinks of wine, beer, or liquor in a single day?” (0 = never, 1 = at least once). Frequency of intoxication was assessed by asking: “About how often in the last 12 months did you drink enough to feel drunk—that is, where drinking noticeably affected your thinking, talking, and behavior?” (0 = never, 1 = at least once). Responses to questions about experiences of eight negative consequences associated with drinking (e.g., driving while drunk or high from alcohol) in the previous 12 months were summed. Participants who endorsed one or more negative consequences in the previous 12 months were coded as ‘1’ those who did not endorse any were coded as ‘0’. Experiences of five symptoms of potential alcohol dependence (e.g., memory lapses, morning drinking) in the previous 12 months were also assessed. Participants who endorsed one or more symptoms of potential alcohol dependence were coded as ‘1’ those who did not endorse any were coded as ‘0’. These four dichotomous indicator variables were used to create a latent variable representing hazardous or risky drinking.

Analytic Procedure

We used cross-lagged latent factor models (e.g., Burkholder and Harlow 2003) to examine concurrent and prospective associations between hazardous drinking and discordance of sexual orientation dimensions across the three waves. Models were estimated in Mplus 5.1 (Muthén and Muthén 2012) using full-information maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors to allow for analysis of data containing missing values. Cross-lagged models allow for simultaneous examination of within-wave (i.e., concurrent) associations between constructs, as well as prospective relations over subsequent assessments, using random effects conceptualized as latent variables. In all models, analyses excluded women who reported alcohol abstention (n = 6) throughout the study. Additionally, women with missing data on either of the two sexual orientation indicators across all waves (e.g., women who reported that they were not sexually active) were excluded from respective models (n = 36 for identity-behavior models; n = 10 for identity-attraction models).

Difference scores, modeled as continuous latent variables (α = 0, ψ = 1), represent mathematical differences between two observed (i.e., indicator) scores at each assessment by way of model specifications (Selig and Preacher 2009). Discordance is represented as a latent construct at each wave, capturing the mathematical difference between sexual behavior (or attraction) scores subtracted from sexual identity scores, adjusted for measurement error. We used a modified specification of latent difference score models (McArdle 2009) to capture differences in sexual identity and sexual attraction or behavior dimensions, respectively, at each wave. For each respective time point (e.g., Wave 1 identity-behavior discordance; Wave 1 identity-attraction discordance), a latent factor was specified as the difference between two observed, single-item dimension scores. The factor loading (λ) of the latent factor regressed onto the sexual identity indicator was fixed at 1 for every wave. The coefficient of the sexual behavior (or sexual attraction) manifest variable, in respective models, was regressed onto the sexual identity indicator and also fixed at 1 for every wave (McArdle 2009). Only the coefficient associated with either the sexual behavior or attraction manifest variable regressed onto the corresponding latent difference factor was freely estimated. We constrained this estimated factor loading to be equal across all measurement occasions (i.e., difference factors across time are tau equivalent). Finally, the variance of the sexual identity variable (ζ) was set to zero across all waves. This permits a fairly straightforward interpretation of estimated latent difference scores at each wave such that scores above zero reflect behavior (or attraction) that is more opposite sex–oriented relative to participants’ sexual identity at that same assessment, whereas scores below zero reflect behavior (or attraction) that is more same sex–oriented relative to participants’ sexual identity at that same assessment. Thus, a latent difference score of zero is interpreted to mean that participants’ self-reported behavior is concordant with their identity on that assessment occasion. A superordinate, hierarchical latent factor was then specified to reflect “true score” intra-individual differences in the corresponding latent difference factors over time. This superordinate latent factor reflects levels of discordance indicated by SMW across all three waves of data collection, whereas the subordinate, within-wave latent difference factors reflect levels of relative discordance among two sexual orientation dimensions within a specific wave of data collection.

Corresponding subordinate hazardous drinking latent factors (i.e., weighted sum of hazardous drinking indicators) reflect levels of hazardous drinking within specific waves of data collection. The factor loading of the first indicator variable at each wave was set at 1 for model identification purposes. As before, we constrained factor loadings (λ) for each respective hazardous drinking indicator to be equivalent across measurement occasions. Thresholds were also constrained to be equal across time for each dichotomous indicator variable. A superordinate, hierarchical latent factor was then specified to represent “true score” levels of hazardous drinking. This superordinate latent factor reflects the level of hazardous drinking indicated by SMW across a 10-year period, whereas the subordinate latent factors reflect levels of hazardous drinking within specific waves. Because the latent factor model incorporated dichotomous criterion variables, no standard model fit indices are provided.

Multigroup Model Subgroups

We created a dichotomous variable, using age at baseline, to delineate SMW in younger adulthood (< 40 years old) and older adulthood (≥ 40 years old). We coded participants’ race as white (0) or black/African American, Latina/Hispanic, Native American, Asian/Pacific Islander, or other race-ethnicity (1) to examine whether effects differed across racial-ethnic majority and minority groups.

RESULTS

Estimated hazardous drinking and identity-behavior discordance factor-mean scores and standard deviations are reported in Table 2. Corresponding factor-mean scores and standard deviations estimates for identity-attraction discordance models are presented in Table 3. Tables 2 and 3 also provide total model sample sizes, observed frequencies of sexual orientation dimensions at each wave, and specific information about how these observed values relate to estimated superordinate latent factor scores for hazardous drinking and sexual orientation discordance, as well as subordinate latent factor scores, for specific waves. In general, these tables are consistent with the larger literature (e.g., Marshal et al. 2008), suggesting that self-ascribing a bisexual identity (i.e., mostly heterosexual, bisexual, mostly homosexual) confers risk for elevations in hazardous drinking (i.e., factors scores elevated above zero and higher relative to other sexual identities). For example, women whose sexual identity was bisexual at Wave 1 (n = 11) are estimated to report elevations in Wave 1 levels of hazardous drinking (M = .74, SD = 1.98). Tables 2 and 3 also show relative elevations in factor scores that reflect sexual orientation discordance on assessment occasions in which SMW report engaging in more opposite sex–oriented sexual behaviors, relative to same-sex behaviors, or being attracted to more opposite-sex rather than same-sex persons. For example, women who reported that their sexual partners were “equally men and women” at Wave 1 (n = 21) also showed elevated levels of identity-behavior discordance (M = 1.37, SD = .68) at Wave 1 compared to SMW who reported than their sexual partners were “mostly” or “only” women.

Table 2.

Estimated Factor Scores over Time for Hazardous Drinking and Identity-behavior Discordance Based on Observed Sexual Orientation Dimensions, Chicago Health and Life Experiences of Women 2000–2010 (n = 411).

n Hazardous Drinking
Identity-behavior Discordance
Wave 1 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 2 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 3 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 1 Identity-behavior Discordance
Wave 2 Identity-behavior Discordance
Wave 3 Identity-behavior Discordance
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Overall 411 .01 1.93 .00 .07 .01 2.08 −.36 2.57 −.64 2.03 −2.98 .85 −3.10 .65 −2.96 .77
Wave 1 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 0
 Mostly heterosexual 0
 Bisexual 11 .52 1.77 −.12 .08 .74 1.98 .49 2.47 .18 2.03 .52 1.21 −.17 1.10 .22 1.25
 Mostly homosexual 111 .15 1.88 −.04 .09 .18 2.01 .10 2.52 .12 2.05 .36 1.01 −.08 .80 −.04 1.11
 Only homosexual 287 −.07 1.96 .02 .06 .12 2.11 −.07 2.60 −.06 2.03 .14 .68 .04 .56 .01 .55
Wave 1 sexual behavior
 Only men 3 .20 2.17 −.12 .18 .15 2.28 .50 3.27 −.67 1.87 2.85 1.00 .10 1.73 .45 .52
 Mostly men 18 .53 1.66 −.02 .09 .39 1.82 .97 2.22 .41 1.87 2.07 .65 .10 .69 .43 1.09
 Equally men and women 21 .91 2.25 −.01 .09 1.01 2.43 1.22 3.02 .74 2.54 1.37 .68 −.13 .75 .32 1.42
 Mostly women 90 .43 1.72 −.02 .09 .49 1.88 .52 2.34 .38 1.85 .31 .64 .06 .94 −.06 .79
 Only women 270 −.20 1.95 .01 .06 .24 2.09 −.29 2.57 −.18 2.04 .35 .41 −.02 .47 −.03 .66
Wave 2 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 2 −.98 1.91 −.32 .12 −.95 1.90 2.11 2.56 −1.84 .39 −.16 .00 1.4 2.12 −.47 .71
 Mostly heterosexual 7 .25 1.72 −.19 .05 .32 1.99 .21 2.13 .15 1.91 .70 1.21 .18 1.38 .43 .97
 Bisexual 20 .72 1.74 −.11 .08 .88 1.90 .61 2.45 .68 2.01 .54 1.17 .15 1.21 .78 1.38
 Mostly homosexual 75 .55 1.72 −.06 .06 .62 1.86 .56 2.31 .35 1.94 .05 1.08 .56 .66 −.21 1.00
 Only homosexual 239 −.39 1.93 .03 .04 −.47 2.03 .44 2.61 −.32 2.02 −.06 .72 .15 .23 −.05 .47
Wave 2 sexual behavior
 Only men 5 −.03 1.75 −.12 .14 −.1 1.66 .04 3.12 −.29 1.72 .64 .84 1.90 1.92 −.07 .74
 Mostly men 9 .62 1.61 −.08 .08 .99 1.94 .48 1.99 .52 1.71 .73 1.45 .88 .67 .86 1.03
 Equally men and women 14 .19 1.62 −.05 .11 .07 1.74 .24 2.25 .42 1.81 .56 1.54 .75 1.01 .65 1.27
 Mostly women 28 1.25 1.96 −.02 .13 1.34 2.14 1.69 2.65 1.07 2.21 .38 .96 .32 1.03 .15 .92
 Only women 289 −.27 1.88 .01 .06 −.33 1.99 .35 2.52 −.27 1.99 −.09 .76 .12 .49 −.10 .64
Wave 3 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 6 −.04 1.56 −.18 .16 −.05 1.73 .09 2.44 1.59 1.20 .68 .75 .34 2.68 .30 .52
 Mostly heterosexual 7 .53 1.64 −.12 .06 .42 1.5 .90 2.47 .53 2.03 .56 1.25 .17 .93 .54 1.81
 Bisexual 37 .37 1.73 −.10 .07 .43 1.9 .40 2.32 .15 1.80 .39 1.14 −.08 .93 .05 1.38
 Mostly homosexual 65 .28 2.08 −.05 .06 .26 2.19 .31 2.80 .05 2.28 −.17 .99 −.26 .66 .55 .66
 Only homosexual 197 −.26 1.89 .04 .03 −.31 2.00 −.33 2.53 .10 1.99 −.08 .68 .06 .37 .08 .21
Wave 3 sexual behavior
 Only men 14 .09 1.53 −.11 .12 .10 1.64 −.01 2.17 .16 1.99 .70 1.10 .32 .83 1.53 1.56
 Mostly men 11 .35 2.05 −.10 .13 .53 2.31 .29 2.78 .26 1.92 .03 1.17 .15 2.26 .67 .92
 Equally men and women 17 .89 1.61 −.04 .08 .93 1.73 1.11 2.11 .77 2.15 .61 .97 −.19 .75 .74 1.36
 Mostly women 34 .75 1.93 −.02 .09 .75 2.09 .95 2.70 .78 2.02 .34 1.13 −.07 .59 .21 .83
 Only women 241 −.20 1.91 .01 .06 −.25 2.02 −.25 2.55 .17 2.06 −.12 .76 −.02 .50 .21 .60

Note: Sexual minority women (SMW) who were missing either of the two sexual orientation indicator variables at any given wave (e.g., women who reported that they were not sexually active, Wave 1, n = 6; Wave 2, n = 20; Wave 3, n = 21) were excluded from structural equation modeling analyses (n = 36). SMW who reported a sexual identity as “other” (Wave 1, n = 2; Wave 2, n = 6; Wave 3, n = 6) or “transgender” (n = 4 at Wave 3 only) are not included in this table but retained in analyses. Bolded values highlight factor scores corresponding to sexual orientation dimensions observed at that same wave of data collection. Factor scores are centered at each respective wave. Higher (more positive) estimated identity-behavior discordance scores reflect sexual behavior that is more opposite sex–oriented than sexual identity, whereas lower (more negative) scores reflect sexual behavior that is more same sex–oriented than contemporaneous sexual identity.

Table 3.

Estimated Factor Scores over Time for Hazardous Drinking and Identity-attraction Discordance Based on Observed Sexual Orientation Dimensions, Chicago Health and Life Experiences of Women 2000–2010 (n = 437).

n Hazardous Drinking
Identity-attraction Discordance
Wave 1 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 2 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 3 Hazardous Drinking
Wave 1 Identity-attraction Discordance
Wave 2 Identity-attraction Discordance
Wave 3 Identity-attraction Discordance
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Overall 437 −.02 1.68 .00 .05 −.02 1.81 −.26 2.13 −.49 1.63 −2.11 .58 −2.11 .57 −2.06 .71
Wave 1 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 0
 Mostly heterosexual 0
 Bisexual 11 .54 1.51 −.07 .05 .71 1.68 .50 1.99 .26 1.51 .45 .60 −.02 .60 −.29 .54
 Mostly homosexual 111 .17 1.61 −.02 .06 .22 1.71 .21 2.05 .22 1.62 .20 .59 .09 .75 .06 .93
 Only homosexual 287 −.13 1.70 .01 .04 .13 1.84 −.11 2.16 −.09 1.64 .07 .53 −.04 .48 −.01 .61
Wave 1 sexual attraction
 Only men 0
 Mostly men 2 1.65 .30 −.13 .03 1.61 .50 1.96 1.04 .73 .80 1.23 .71 −.20 1.41 −.70 .71
 Equally men and women 38 −.04 1.55 −.02 .06 .05 1.66 −.08 1.89 .08 1.63 .81 .71 .03 .62 .18 .86
 Mostly women 172 .00 1.60 .00 .06 .01 1.70 .07 2.04 .04 1.55 .24 .55 .11 .67 .11 .83
 Only women 225 −.05 1.76 .00 .04 .02 1.92 −.05 2.24 −.04 1.70 .34 .26 −.09 .45 −.10 .55
Wave 2 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 2 −.64 1.44 −.13 .05 −.84 1.48 1.73 1.95 −1.28 .80 −.27 .00 1.20 .00 −.70 .71
 Mostly heterosexual 6 .30 1.55 −.11 .02 .21 1.79 .40 2.10 −.05 1.60 .40 .82 1.37 .98 −.36 .69
 Bisexual 21 .79 1.55 −.07 .05 .84 1.66 .66 2.02 .69 1.59 .40 .86 .30 .62 −.01 .98
 Mostly homosexual 81 .42 1.49 −.02 .05 .46 1.62 .43 1.91 .34 1.50 .03 .64 .16 .47 .06 .86
 Only homosexual 257 −.38 1.67 .02 .04 −.39 1.77 .36 2.15 −.28 1.63 −.04 .53 .06 .49 −.01 .52
Wave 2 sexual attraction
 Only men 0
 Mostly men 9 .58 1.45 −.07 .09 .59 1.56 .61 2.18 .52 1.72 .07 .50 .69 1.69 −.33 .79
 Equally men and women 28 .45 1.56 −.03 .08 .53 1.64 .51 1.98 .46 1.46 .48 .97 .37 .74 .07 .96
 Mostly women 133 .14 1.72 .00 .05 .15 1.81 .24 2.18 .20 1.70 .06 .60 .22 .72 .14 .77
 Only women 201 −.43 1.60 .01 .04 −.44 1.72 .50 2.05 −.39 1.54 −.10 .48 .23 .21 −.09 .51
Wave 3 sexual identity
 Only heterosexual 6 −.04 1.37 −.13 .04 −.04 1.54 −.12 2.02 .96 .88 .23 .55 −.05 .76 .86 .82
 Mostly heterosexual 7 .50 1.40 −.11 .03 .44 1.31 .58 2.11 .16 1.60 −.27 .58 −.36 .90 .77 1.13
 Bisexual 43 .26 1.60 −.06 .04 .29 1.72 .28 1.96 .01 1.47 .17 .83 .03 .72 .35 .71
 Mostly homosexual 66 .22 1.80 −.03 .04 .23 1.92 .27 2.32 .13 1.83 .08 .57 −.05 .53 .32 .51
 Only homosexual 213 −.28 1.64 .02 .03 −.27 1.74 −.28 2.10 .15 1.62 −.03 .55 .00 .49 .09 .51
Wave 3 sexual attraction
 Only men 4 −.57 .97 −.02 .18 −.59 1.00 −.91 1.05 .75 1.74 −.02 .50 −.22 .82 1.30 3.00
 Mostly men 14 .12 1.55 −.05 .08 .05 1.63 .11 2.08 .26 1.39 .23 .52 −.07 1.10 .30 1.10
 Equally men and women 40 .47 1.27 −.03 .07 .58 1.39 .50 1.63 .43 1.31 .28 .93 .02 .62 .35 1.24
 Mostly women 110 .03 1.78 .01 .05 .04 1.87 .12 2.25 .05 1.74 .11 .57 .12 .59 .25 .67
 Only women 172 −.26 1.69 .01 .04 −.27 1.80 −.28 2.16 .16 1.70 −.11 .50 −.08 .44 .31 .44

Note: Sexual minority women (SMW) who were missing either of the two sexual orientation indicator variables at any given wave were excluded from structural equation modeling analyses (n = 10). SMW who reported a sexual identity as “other” (Wave 1, n = 2; Wave 2, n = 7; Wave 3, n = 6) or “transgender” (n = 4 at Wave 3 only) are not included in this table but retained in analyses. Bolded values highlight factor scores corresponding to sexual orientation dimensions observed at that same wave of data collection. Factor scores are centered at each respective wave. Higher (more positive) estimated identity-attraction discordance scores reflect sexual attraction that is more opposite sex–oriented than sexual identity, whereas lower (more negative) scores reflect sexual attraction that is more same sex–oriented than contemporaneous sexual identity.

Overall Sample Cross-lagged Models1

In unconditional models, which included no covariates, patterns of estimated latent factor means suggested that hazardous drinking tended to decrease over time (Wave 1 α = .03; Wave 2 α = −.05; Wave 3 α = −.09). As specified in the model, the estimated superordinate latent factor mean for hazardous drinking was close to zero for the overall sample (α = −.03). Estimated variances of subordinate hazardous drinking latent factors suggested that the greatest variation in hazardous drinking occurred at Wave 2 (Wave 1 ψ = 7.78; Wave 2 ψ = 9.91; Wave 3 ψ = 3.92).

Identity-behavior Discordance

We used conditional models (Figure 1) to examine associations between trait levels of hazardous drinking and identity-behavior discordance. The correlation between the superordinate latent factors showed no overall relation (b = .15, p = .55), suggesting that SMW who generally report higher levels of hazardous drinking over time are no more or less likely to report identity-behavior discordance over time than those who generally engage in less hazardous drinking. We then examined contemporaneous associations, at each respective wave, between levels of hazardous drinking and identity-behavior discordance. At Wave 3, a significant association between levels of identity-behavior discordance and hazardous drinking (b = .46, p < .001) suggested that SMW who reported more opposite-sex sexual behavior, relative to their sexual identity, also reported higher levels of hazardous drinking at that same wave. Prospective, cross-lagged effects were examined to determine whether levels of identity-behavior (I-B) discordance at prior waves (W) of data collection predicted subsequent levels of hazardous drinking (HD). Results indicated that SMW who reported more opposite-sex sexual behavior, relative to their sexual identity, at a given assessment were likely to report higher levels of hazardous drinking at subsequent waves: W1 I-B Discordance → W2 HD: b = .03, p = .01 and W2 I-B Discordance → W3 HD: b = .07, p < .001. Alternative prospective cross-lagged effects were then examined to determine if higher levels of hazardous drinking predicted greater identity-behavior discordance at subsequent time points. Findings showed that hazardous drinking at prior waves was unrelated to subsequent levels of identity-behavior discordance.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Overall Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Behavior Discordance (n = 411).

Note: Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Behv = sexual behavior.

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Identity-attraction Discordance

In a separate conditional model (Figure 2), consistent with results for identity-behavior discordance, we found no overall association between superordinate levels of hazardous drinking and identity-attraction discordance (b = −.36; SE = .28, p = .20). We proceeded to examine contemporaneous associations between these constructs at each wave, and findings showed a significant within-time association between identity-attraction (I-A) discordance and hazardous drinking at Wave 3 (b = .33, p = .03), such that SMW who reported more opposite sex–oriented sexual attraction, relative to their sexual identity at that time, also reported higher concurrent levels of hazardous drinking. Prospective cross-lagged effects suggested that SMW who reported more opposite sex–oriented sexual attraction, relative to their sexual identity, reported higher levels of hazardous drinking at subsequent waves: W1 I-A Discordance → W2 HD: b = .03, p = .049 and W2 I-A Discordance → W3 HD: b = .08, p = .001. Alternative, prospective cross-lagged effects suggested no association between levels of hazardous drinking and subsequent identity-attraction discordance. These cross-lagged associations are consistent with those found in identity-behavior discordance models and suggest that SMW who endorse more opposite sex–oriented sexual behavior or attraction, relative to their self-ascribed sexual identity, are at risk for higher levels of hazardous drinking.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Overall Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Attraction Discordance (n = 437).

Note: Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Attr = sexual attraction.

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Developmental Period Multigroup Models

Identity-behavior Discordance

Latent mixture models with known classes (SMW in younger vs. older adulthood) were specified, constraining the measurement model to be equivalent across age groups (Figure 3). Structural paths were examined to determine if participants’ life stage influenced previously described associations. Structural paths suggested concurrent positive associations between levels of hazardous drinking and identity-behavior discordance at Wave 3 among SMW in both younger (b = .26; SE = .11, p = .02) and older adulthood (b = .44; SE = .22, p = .05). Nested model comparisons (http://www.statmodel.com/chidiff.shtml) indicated this effect was marginally stronger among SMW in older adulthood (TRd: χ2[1] = 2.80, p = .06). Prospective cross-lagged associations were confirmed in both age groups, supporting that regardless of participants’ life stage at baseline, higher levels of identity-behavior discordance were predictive of increases in hazardous drinking at subsequent waves (Younger adults: W1 I-B Discordance → W2 HD: b = .16, p = .001 and W2 I-B Discordance → W3 HD: b = .16, p = .003; Older adults: W1 I-B Discordance → W2 HD: b = .16, p = .001 and W2 I-B Discordance → W3 HD: b = .21, p = .002). Nested model comparisons indicated these effects were statistically equivalent for SMW in younger and older adulthood. Interestingly, an alternative cross-lagged association was found among older SMW (TRd: χ2[1] = 3.04, p = .05) and suggested that older SMW who reported higher levels of hazardous drinking at baseline subsequently reported more opposite-sex sexual behavior, relative to their sexual identity, at Wave 2 (b = .30, p = .004). In sum, cross-lagged findings suggest that regardless of age, SWM who engage in more opposite sex–oriented behavior, relative to their sexual identity, are at elevated risk for hazardous drinking. By contrast, older SMW who engage in hazardous drinking at baseline are subsequently more likely to report more opposite sex–oriented sexual behavior, relative to their sexual identity.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Multigroup Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Behavior Discordance for Sexual Minority Women in Younger and Older Adulthood.

Note: Estimates preceding slash are for women in younger adulthood (< 40 years of age; n = 251). Estimates following slash are for women in older (≥ 40 years of age, n = 160) adulthood. Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Behv = sexual behavior.

p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Identity-attraction Discordance

Structural paths in the identity-attraction discordance models (Figure 4) suggested that among younger adult SMW, there was an association between superordinate levels of identity-attraction discordance and hazardous drinking (b = .01, p = .03). This overall association was negative and nonsignificant among older adult SMW, and the difference between these two effects was significant (TRd: χ2[1] = 37.83, p < .001). Notably, at Wave 2, older SMW who reported more opposite sex–oriented sexual attraction, relative to their sexual identity, also indicated higher concurrent levels of hazardous drinking. Nested model comparisons (TRd: χ2[1] = 11.61, p < .001) showed this within-time association was significantly different between older (b = .31, p = .006) and younger SMW (b = .02, p = .86). Multigroup models replicated the within-time association between levels of hazardous drinking and identity-attraction discordance at Wave 3 among younger SMW (b = .23, p = .04) but not older SMW (b = .11, p = .52); notably, nested model comparisons indicated these effects were statistically equivalent across age groups. Findings suggest that identity-attraction discordance in late young adulthood and throughout older adulthood may relate to higher concurrent levels of hazardous drinking. The pattern of prospective cross-lagged associations between levels of identity-attraction discordance and subsequent hazardous drinking was replicated among SMW in both age groups (Younger adults: W1 I-A Discordance → W2 HD: b = .14, p = .09 and W2 I-A Discordance → W3 HD: b = .21, p = .02; Older adults: W1 I-A Discordance → W2 HD: b = .27, p = .003 and W2 I-A Discordance → W3 HD: b = .27, p = .006). Nested model comparisons confirmed the parallel cross-lagged effects were statistically equivalent regardless of respondents’ age, suggesting that higher levels of identity-attraction discordance prospectively predicted greater engagement in hazardous drinking among both younger and older SMW.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Multigroup Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Attraction Discordance for Sexual Minority Women in Younger and Older Adulthood.

Note: Estimates preceding slash are for women in younger adulthood (< 40 years of age; n = 253). Estimates following slash are for women in older (≥ 40 years of age, n = 184) adulthood. Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Attr = sexual attraction.

p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Race-ethnicity Multigroup Models

Identity-behavior Discordance

Latent mixture models with known classes (white SMW vs. racial-ethnic-minority SMW) were specified constraining measurement models to be equivalent across groups (Figure 5). Structural paths were examined to determine if effects differed based on respondents’ race-ethnicity. There were no overall associations between superordinate levels of identity-behavior discordance and hazardous drinking among either white (b = .11, p = .11) or racial-ethnic minority SMW (b = −.02, p = .68). Within-time positive associations between hazardous drinking and identity-behavior discordance at Wave 3 were found among both white (b = .30, p = .02) and racial-ethnic minority SMW (b = .25, p = .02). Nested model comparisons confirmed these effects were statistically equivalent. The pattern of prospective cross-lagged associations between identity-behavior discordance and subsequent hazardous drinking were similar for both subgroups (white SMW: W1 I-B Discordance → W2 HD: b = .10, p = .05 and W2 I-B Discordance → W3 HD: b = .11, p = .06; Racial-ethnic minority SMW: W1 I-B Discordance → W2 HD: b = .09, p = .14 and W2 I-B Discordance → W3 HD: b = .18, p = .002). Again, nested model comparisons indicated the magnitude of these parallel effects were statistically equivalent across subgroups. Interestingly, there were alternative cross-lagged associations shown for white SMW such that higher levels of hazardous drinking were predictive of significantly more same sex–oriented behavior, relative to their sexual identity, at subsequent waves: W1 HD → W2 I-B Discordance: b = −.29, p = .001 and W2 HD → W3 I-B Discordance: b = −.14, p = .04. Parallel associations were essentially zero, however, among racial-ethnic minority SMW: W1 HD → W2 I-B Discordance: b = .01, p = .94 and W2 HD → W3 I-B Discordance: b = .01, p = .86. Nested model comparisons confirmed that only the effect from Wave 2 to Wave 3 was statistically different between white and racial-ethnic minority SMW (TRd: χ2[1] = 4.46, p = .02). Among white SMW, in particular, higher levels of hazardous drinking may subsequently increase levels of identity-behavior discordance such that they subsequently report more same sex–oriented sexual behavior relative to their sexual identity.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Multigroup Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Behavior Discordance for White and Ethnic-racial Minority Sexual Minority Women.

Note: Estimates preceding slash are for white sexual minority women (n = 187). Estimates following slash are for ethnic-racial minority sexual minority women (n = 224). Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Behv = sexual behavior.

p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Identity-attraction Discordance

As shown in Figure 6, racial-ethnic minority SMW reported higher levels of hazardous drinking at Wave 2 when their sexual attraction was more opposite sex–oriented than their sexual identity (b = .19, p = .02). Nevertheless, nested model comparisons indicated this effect was statistically equivalent to the association found among white SMW (b = .08, p = .54). A cross-lagged association showed that racial-ethnic minority SMW who reported more opposite sex–oriented sexual attraction at Wave 2, relative to their sexual identity, were at increased risk of hazardous drinking at Wave 3 (b = .27, p = .001). Despite that the parallel cross-lagged association was only marginally significant among white SMW (b = .16, p = .07), nested model comparisons confirmed the effects were equivalent.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Multigroup Longitudinal Cross-lagged Model Depicting Associations between Hazardous Drinking and Sexual Identity-Attraction Discordance for White and Ethnic-racial Minority Sexual Minority Women.

Note: Estimates preceding slash are for white sexual minority women (n = 207). Estimates following slash are for ethnic-racial minority sexual minority women (n = 230). Unstandardized estimates are shown. Standard errors are presented in parentheses. Bisecting lines depict equality constraints. W = wave; Idnt = sexual identity; Attr = sexual attraction.

p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

DISCUSSION

Given well-documented alcohol-related health disparities based on sexual orientation, we examined whether discordance between concurrently reported sexual identity and sexual behavior or attraction might help explain such disparities within a diverse sample of SMW. The current study contributes to a growing literature focusing on how identity-related characteristics influence patterns of hazardous drinking with in sexual minority populations (Hatzenbuehler 2009; Ott et al. 2013; Rosario et al. 2011). We posited that although discordance among sexual orientation dimensions is relatively common among SMW, it may arouse psychological discomfort and compel distraction or dissonance-reduction tactics, such as hazardous drinking. Findings generally showed no superordinate relations between SMW’s likelihood of engaging in hazardous drinking and their overall levels of discordance with regard to dimensions of sexual orientation across a 10-year period. As predicted by social identity and cognitive dissonance theory, cross-lagged associations suggested a time-delimited phenomenon whereby identity-behavior or identity-attraction discordance conferred risk of subsequent elevations in hazardous drinking.

This study is among the first to examine longitudinal associations and show that normative declines in drinking over a 10-year period may be less likely for SMW whose sexual identity is incongruent with their sexual behavior or attraction. Results are consistent with findings using samples of primarily heterosexual-identified individuals (Bauer et al. 2010; Drabble et al. 2005; Gattis et al. 2012; Jeffries 2009) and imply that some level of cognitive-behavioral consistency is important for contemplating and defining sexual orientation

Findings also showed that among older SMW, more opposite sex–oriented sexual behavior, relative to their sexual identity, may be a potent predictor of hazardous drinking. There are at least two potential explanations for this age-related finding. First, sexual exploration is more common in young adulthood (e.g., Arnett and Tanner 2006; Erikson 1968), and so young lesbian/bisexual women may feel less distress than their older counterparts to endorse sexual behavior or attraction that appears to contradict their self-ascribed sexual identity. Second, some SMW may continue to identify as bisexual or lesbian even as they experience fluctuations in sexual behavior or attraction. Potential reasons for this include fear of fulfilling negative stereotypes that their lesbian or bisexual identity was “just a phase” or fear of rejection from their sexual minority peers (Diamond 2003; Tabatabai and Linders 2011). It is possible that responses to such fears are exacerbated among older SMW who are presumed to have had more life experience to inform their sexual orientation, and as such, divergence from potentially long-held and prescribed expectations may be less acceptable to the woman herself or close others.

Finally, we were afforded a unique opportunity to examine whether the primary findings were replicated across majority and minority racial-ethnic identity groups. Contemporaneous and prospective associations seen in the overall sample were largely replicated, regardless of SMW’s racial-ethnic identity. The current findings are generally consistent with those from national school-based studies that show similar patterns of alcohol use across various subgroups of racial-ethnic minority sexual minority youth (Talley et al. 2014; cf. Button, O’Connell, and Gealt 2012). Previous work (McCabe et al. 2010; McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, and Keyes 2010; Rosario et al. 2014) has also found generally equivalent associations between sexual minority–specific stressors and recent alcohol use across racial-ethnic groups. Unfortunately, due to small subsample sizes, we were unable to explore disparities among racial-ethnic minority subgroups (cf. Hughes et al., 2006). Despite results that were largely consistent, relations between sexual orientation discordance and hazardous drinking may be more nuanced among sexual minority people of color. For example, our findings for racial-ethnic minority SMW show a marginal, positive superordinate relation between hazardous drinking and the tendency to report more same-sex attraction relative to their sexual identity, suggesting person-specific moderating factors warrant further investigation.

Limitations and Future Directions

Enrollment criteria required women to self-identify as lesbian, which, at various waves of assessment, resulted in smaller subsample sizes based on self-reported sexual orientation dimensions (e.g., all but 11 participants identified as “mostly” or “only” lesbian at baseline). Notably, SMW who indicated discordant sexual orientation dimensions at baseline were necessarily reporting more opposite-sex sexual behavior and/or attraction. As a consequence, variance in latent difference scores in early study waves was restricted and may have blunted expected associations. This proposition is reflected in results suggesting more reliable associations between sexual orientation discordance and hazardous drinking as the study progressed and greater variation in sexual identity, behavior, and attraction was reported.

As mentioned previously, discordance between sexual identity and sexual behavior or attraction may be indicative of active transitions related to sexual identity or sexual orientation among SMW. Although a determination of how shifts in sexual identity contribute to hazardous drinking was beyond the scope of the current work, findings are important to understanding how relative discrepancies between SMW’s sexual identity, sexual attraction, and sexual behavior may influence drinking. Moreover, these results cannot explain why sexual orientation discordance predicts higher subsequent and, at times, higher concurrent levels of hazardous drinking. Although our hypotheses, derived from social identity and cognitive dissonance theory, were generally supported, we were unable to directly test potential mechanisms. For example, the study lacked assessments of respondents’ psychological discomfort or identity integration. Identity integration is associated with acceptance and comfort with one’s sexual identity (Rosario et al. 2011) and may be a robust predictor of alcohol-related disparities among SMW. Future researchers may consider including measures that assess perceived discordance among sexual orientation dimensions as well as specific sexual minority identity–related characteristics to replicate the current findings and determine whether associations are accounted for by additional aspects of identity.

Although our longitudinal modeling approach intended to disentangle directional effects, data are correlational and findings should be interpreted with this in mind. Despite evidence consistent with our primary hypotheses, it is possible that other variables not examined may account for the findings. For example, SMW who report discordance among sexual orientation dimensions and who also engage in hazardous drinking may be more likely to have experienced victimization/maltreatment (e.g., Beitchman et al. 1992; Miller, Downs, and Testa 1993) or may be higher on sensation-seeking or related traits (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema 2004; Trocki, Drabble, and Midanik 2009) that are known correlates of both same-sex sexuality and hazardous drinking.

Future work should continue to investigate psychosocial factors that can potentially account for associations between identity-related characteristics and hazardous drinking outcomes (see e.g., Hatzenbuehler 2009; Talley and Littlefield 2014) in sexual minority populations. Increased awareness of the ways in which women’s sexual orientation uniquely relates to patterns of risky drinking may improve providers’ ability to appropriately recognize and characterize alcohol-related pathology.

Acknowledgments

FUNDING

The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Data collection and manuscript preparation efforts were supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants AA0013328 to Tonda L. Hughes and AA019974 to Amelia E. Talley. In addition, manuscript preparation was supported by a supplement to AA0013328, awarded to Frances Aranda, and an award from the Office of Research on Women’s Health (ORWH) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (K12HD055892) to Bethany Everett.

Biographies

Amelia E. Talley received her BA degree in psychology in 2001 from Texas A&M University and obtained her doctoral degree in 2009 from the University of Missouri. Currently, Dr. Talley is an assistant professor in the Department of Psychological Sciences at Texas Tech University. Her research interests span the addiction, stigma, and identity literatures.

Frances Aranda, PhD, MPH, MS, is a senior research specialist in the College of Nursing at the University of Illinois at Chicago. She co-manages a longitudinal project funded by National Institute on Drug Abuse examining a culturally tailored smoking cessation treatment program for LGBTQ smokers. Her research interests include racial-ethnic differences in depression, sexual orientation, alcohol use among lesbians, and smoking among LGBTs.

Tonda L. Hughes, RN, PhD, FAAN, is professor and associate dean for Global Health in the College of Nursing at the University of Illinois at Chicago (UIC). She is also Co-Director of the UIC Building Interdisciplinary Research Careers in Women’s Health (BIRCWH) program and Visiting Senior Scientist at Fenway Institute in Boston, Massachusetts. Dr. Hughes is PI of the Chicago Health and Life Experiences of Women (CHLEW) study, the world’s longest running longitudinal study of alcohol use and health among sexual minority women.

Bethany Everett is an assistant professor in the Department of Sociology at the University of Utah. She was an NIH Building Interdisciplinary Research Careers in Women’s Health (BIRCWH) Scholar at the University of Illinois at Chicago from 2012–2014. Her research focuses on the social determinants of health disparities, in particular those related to gender and sexual orientation.

Timothy P. Johnson is director of the Survey Research Laboratory and Professor of Public Administration at the University of Illinois at Chicago. His main areas of research include survey measurement error and the social epidemiology of health behaviors in vulnerable populations.

Footnotes

1

We ran supplemental models to examine the associations between attraction-behavior discordance and hazardous drinking in the overall sample. Similar to the primary conditional models for identity-behavior and identity-attraction discordance, there was no overall relation between “trait levels” of attraction-behavior discordance and hazardous drinking, suggesting that sexual minority women (SMW) who generally report higher levels of hazardous drinking are no more or less likely to report attraction-behavior discordance compared to SMW who generally report lower levels of hazardous drinking. We then examined contemporaneous associations and replicated the within-time association between levels of attraction-behavior discordance and hazardous drinking at Wave 3 seen in the primary models, such that SMW who reported more opposite-sex sexual behavior, relative to their sexual attraction at that same wave, also reported higher contemporaneous levels of hazardous drinking. We then examined prospective cross-lagged effects; consistent with primary models, findings indicated that SMW who reported more opposite sex–oriented sexual behavior, relative to their sexual attraction at the time, endorsed higher levels of hazardous drinking at the next subsequent wave. Finally, we also examined alternative prospective cross-lagged effects and found that SMW women who reported higher levels of hazardous drinking at baseline prospectively reported more same-sex behavior, relative to their sexual attraction, at Wave 2.

References

  1. Arnett Jeffrey J, Tanner Jennifer L. Emerging Adults in America: Coming of Age in the 21st Century. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bauer Greta R, Jairam Jennifer A, Baidoobonso Shamara M. Sexual Health, Risk Behaviors, and Substance use in Heterosexual-identified Women with Female Sex Partners: 2002 US National Survey of Family Growth. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2010;37(9):531–37. doi: 10.1097/OLQ.0b013e3181d785f4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beitchman Joseph H, Zucker Kenneth J, Hood Jane E, DaCosta Granville A, Akman Donna, Cassavia Erika. A Review of the Long-term Effects of Child Sexual Abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1992;16(1):101–18. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(92)90011-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bradford Judith, Ryan Caitlin, Rothblum Esther D. National Lesbian Health Care Survey: Implications for Mental Health Care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62(2):228–42. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.62.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brooks Kelly D, Quina Kathryn. Women’s Sexual Identity Patterns: Differences among Lesbians, Bisexuals, and Unlabeled Women. Journal of Homosexuality. 2009;56(8):1030–45. doi: 10.1080/00918360903275443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burgard Sarah A, Cochran Susan D, Mays Vickie M. Alcohol and Tobacco Use Patterns among Heterosexually and Homosexually Experienced California Women. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2005;77(1):61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Burke Peter J. Identity Processes and Social Stress. American Sociological Review. 1991;56(6):836–49. [Google Scholar]
  8. Burke Peter J. Identity Change. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2006;69(1):81–96. [Google Scholar]
  9. Burkholder Gary J, Harlow Lisa L. An Illustration of a Longitudinal Cross-lagged Design for Larger Structural Equation Models. Structural Equation Modeling. 2003;10(3):465–86. [Google Scholar]
  10. Button Deeanna M, O’Connell Daniel J, Gealt Roberta. Sexual Minority Youth Victimization and Social Support: The Intersection of Sexuality, Gender, Race, and Victimization. Journal of Homosexuality. 2012;59(1):18–43. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2011.614903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chae David H, Krieger Nancy, Bennett Gary G, Lindsey Jane C, Stoddard Anne M, Barbeau Elizabeth M. Implications of Discrimination Based on Sexuality, Gender, and Race/Ethnicity for Psychological Distress among Working-class Sexual Minorities: The United for Health Study, 2003–2004. International Journal of Health Services. 2010;40(4):589–608. doi: 10.2190/HS.40.4.b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chandra Anjani, Mosher William D, Copen Casey, Sionean Catlainn. Sexual Behavior, Sexual Attraction, and Sexual Identity in the United States: Data from the 2006–2008 National Survey of Family Growth. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dermody Sarah S, Marshal Michael P, Cheong JeeWon, Burton Chad, Hughes Tonda L, Aranda Frances, Friedman Mark S. Longitudinal Disparities of Hazardous Drinking between Sexual Minority and Heterosexual Individuals from Adolescence to Young Adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013;43(1):30–39. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9905-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Diamond Lisa M. Sexual Identity, Attractions, and Behavior among Young Sexual-minority Women over a 2-Year Period. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36(2):241–50. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diamond Lisa M. Was It a Phase? Young Women’s Relinquishment of Lesbian/Bisexual Identities over a 5-Year Period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84(2):352–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diamond Lisa M. A New View of Lesbian Subtypes: Stable versus Fluid Identity Trajectories over an 8-Year Period. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2005;29(2):119–28. [Google Scholar]
  17. Diamond Lisa M. Female Bisexuality from Adolescence to Adulthood: Results from a 10-Year Longitudinal Study. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(1):5–14. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Diamond Lisa M. The Desire Disorder in Research on Sexual Orientation in Women: Contributions of Dynamical Systems Theory. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012;41(1):73–83. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9909-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Doll Lynda S, Beeker Carolyn. Male Bisexual Behavior and HIV Risk in the United States: Synthesis of Research with Implications for Behavioral Interventions. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1996;8(3):205–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Drabble Laurie, Midanik Lorraine T, Trocki Karen. Reports of Alcohol Consumption and Alcohol-related Problems among Homosexual, Bisexual and Heterosexual Respondents: Results from the 2000 National Alcohol Survey. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2005;66(1):111–20. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dubé Eric M, Savin-Williams Ritch C. Sexual Identity Development among Ethnic Sexual-minority Male Youths. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35(6):1389–98. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.6.1389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Elliot Andrew J, Devine Patricia G. On the Motivational Nature of Cognitive Dissonance: Dissonance as Psychological Discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67(3):382–94. [Google Scholar]
  23. Erikson EH. Identity: Youth and Crisis. Oxford, UK: Norton & Co; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  24. Festinger Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Vol. 2. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press; 1962. [Google Scholar]
  25. Galupo M Paz, Davis Kyle S, Grynkiewiez Ashley L, Mitchell Renae C. Conceptualization of Sexual Orientation Identity among Sexual Minorities: Patterns across Sexual and Gender Identity. Journal of Bisexuality. 2014;14(3–4):433–56. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gattis Maurice N, Sacco Paul, Cunningham-Williams Renee M. Substance Use and Mental Health Disorders among Heterosexual Identified Men and Women Who Have Same-sex Partners or Same-sex Attraction: Results from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012;41(5):1185–97. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9910-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hatzenbuehler Mark L. How Does Sexual Minority Stigma ‘Get Under the Skin’? A Psychological Mediation Framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(5):707–30. doi: 10.1037/a0016441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hughes Tonda, Johnson Timothy P, Steffen Alanda D, Wilsnack Sharon C, Everett Bethany. Lifetime Victimization, Hazardous Drinking and Depression among Heterosexual and Sexual Minority Women. LGBT Health. 2014;1(3):192–203. doi: 10.1089/lgbt.2014.0014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hughes Tonda L, Szalacha Laura A, Johnson Timothy P, Kinnison Kelly E, Wilsnack Sharon C, Cho Young. Sexual Victimization and Hazardous Drinking among Heterosexual and Sexual Minority Women. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(12):1152–56. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hughes Tonda L, Wilsnack Sharon C, Szalacha Laura A, Johnson Timothy, Bostwick Wendy B, Seymour Rachel, Aranda Frances, Benson Perry, Kinnison Kelly E. Age and Racial/Ethnic Differences in Drinking and Drinking-related Problems in a Community Sample of Lesbians. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67(4):579–90. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Igartua Karine, Thombs Brett D, Burgos Giovani, Montoro Richard. Concordance and Discrepancy in Sexual Identity, Attraction, and Behavior among Adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45(6):602–08. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jeffries William L. Sociodemographic, Sexual, and HIV and Other Sexually Transmitted Disease Risk Profiles of Nonhomosexual-identified Men Who Have Sex with Men. American Journal of Public Health. 2009;99(6):1042–45. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.125674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Johnson Timothy P, Hughes Tonda L, Cho Young Ik, Wilsnack Sharon C, Aranda Frances, Szalacha Laura A. Hazardous Drinking, Depression, and Anxiety among Sexual-minority Women: Self-medication or Impaired Functioning? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2013;74(4):565–75. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Katz-Wise Sabra L, Hyde Janet S. APA Handbook of Sexuality and Psychology, Vol. 1: Person-based Approaches. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2014. Sexuality and Gender: The Interplay; pp. 29–62. [Google Scholar]
  35. King Michael, Semlyen Joanna, Tai Sharon See, Killaspy Helen, Osborn David, Popelyuk Dmitri, Nazareth Irwin. A Systematic Review of Mental Disorder, Suicide, and Deliberate Self-harm in Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual People. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8:70. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Laumann EO, Gagnon JH, Michael RT, Michaels S. The Social Organization of Sexuality: Sexual Practices in the United States. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  37. Marshal Michael P, Friedman Mark S, Stall Ron, King Kevin M, Miles Jonathan, Gold Melanie A, Bukstein Oscar G, Morse Jennifer Q. Sexual Orientation and Adolescent Substance Use: A Meta-analysis and Methodological Review. Addiction. 2008;103(4):546–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02149.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Marshal Michael P, Friedman Mark S, Stall Ron, Thompson Amanda L. Individual Trajectories of Substance Use in Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual Youth and Heterosexual Youth. Addiction. 2009;104(6):974–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02531.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McArdle John J. Latent Variable Modeling of Differences and Changes with Longitudinal Data. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:577–605. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McCabe Sean E, Bostwick Wendy B, Hughes Tonda L, West Brady T, Boyd Carol J. The Relationship between Discrimination and Substance Use Disorders among Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Adults in the United States. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(10):1946–52. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.163147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McCabe Sean E, Hughes Tonda L, Bostwick Wendy B, West Brady T, Boyd Carol J. Sexual Orientation, Substance Use Behaviors and Substance Dependence in the United States. Addiction. 2009;104(8):1333–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02596.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. McLaughlin Katie A, Hatzenbuehler Mark L, Keyes Katherine M. Responses to Discrimination and Psychiatric Disorders among Black, Hispanic, Female, and Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Individuals. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100:1477–84. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.181586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Miller BA, Downs WR, Testa M. Interrelationships between Victimization Experiences and Women’s Alcohol Use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 1993;11:109–17. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1993.s11.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Morris Jessica F, Rothblum Esther D. Who Fills Out a ‘Lesbian’ Questionnaire? The Interrelationship of Sexual Orientation, Years ‘Out,’ Disclosure of Sexual Orientation, Sexual Experience with Women, and Participation in the Lesbian Community. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1999;23(1999):537–57. [Google Scholar]
  45. Muthén Linda, Muthén Bengt. “Mplus” [Computer software] Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  46. Narring F, Huwiler SM, Michaud P-A. Prevalence and Dimensions of Sexual Orientation in Swiss Adolescents: A Cross-sectional Survey of 16- to 20-Year-old Students. Acta Paediatrica. 2003;92(2):233–39. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2003.tb00532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Gender Differences in Risk Factors and Consequences for Alcohol Use and Problems. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24(8):981–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ott Miles Q, Wypij David, Corliss Heather L, Rosario Margaret, Reisner Sari L, Gordon Allegra R, Austin SB. Repeated Changes in Reported Sexual Orientation Identity Linked to Substance Use Behaviors in Youth. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;52(4):465–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Parks Cheryl A, Hughes Tonda L, Matthews Alicia K. Race/Ethnicity and Sexual Orientation: Intersecting Identities. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2004;10(3):241–54. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.3.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Pathela Preeti, Blank Susan, Sell Randall L, Schillinger Julia A. The Importance of Both Sexual Behavior and Identity. American Journal of Public Health. 2006;96(5):765. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.079186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Peplau Letitia A, Garnets Linda D. A New Paradigm for Understanding Women’s Sexuality and Sexual Orientation. Journal of Social Issues. 2000;56(2):330–35. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rosario Margaret, Corliss Heather L, Everett Bethany G, Russell Stephen T, Buchting Francisco O, Birkett Michelle A. Mediation by Peer Violence Victimization of Sexual Orientation Disparities in Cancer-related Tobacco, Alcohol, and Sexual Risk Behaviors: Pooled Youth Risk Behavior Surveys. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(6):1113–23. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rosario Margaret, Schrimshaw Eric W, Hunter Joyce. Ethnic/Racial Differences in the Coming-out Process of Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Youths: A Comparison of Sexual Identity Development over Time. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2004;10(3):215–28. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.3.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Rosario Margaret, Schrimshaw Eric W, Hunter Joyce. Different Patterns of Sexual Identity Development over Time: Implications for the Psychological Adjustment of Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Youths. Journal of Sex Research. 2011;48(1):3–15. doi: 10.1080/00224490903331067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ross Michael W, Essien EJ, Williams Mark L, Fernandez-Esquer Maria E. Concordance between Sexual Behavior and Sexual Identity in Street Outreach Samples of Four Racial/Ethnic Groups. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2003;30(2):110–13. doi: 10.1097/00007435-200302000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Savin-Williams Ritch C, Diamond Lisa M. Sexual Identity Trajectories among Sexual-Minority Youths: Gender Comparisons. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2000;29(6):607–27. doi: 10.1023/a:1002058505138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Savin-Williams Ritch C, Joyner Kara, Rieger Gerulf. Prevalence and Stability of Self-reported Sexual Orientation Identity during Young Adulthood. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012;41(1):103–10. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9913-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Scheer Susan, Parks Cheryl A, McFarland Willi, Page-Shafer Kimberly, Delgado Viva, Ruiz Juan D, Molitor Fred, Klausner Jeffrey D. Self-reported Sexual Identity, Sexual Behaviors and Health Risks: Examples from a Population-based Survey of Young Women. Journal of Lesbian Studies. 2003;7(1):69–83. doi: 10.1300/J155v07n01_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Selig James P, Preacher Kristopher J. Mediation Models for Longitudinal Data in Developmental Research. Research in Human Development. 2009;6(2–3):144–64. [Google Scholar]
  60. Smith Anthony, Rissel Chris E, Richters Juliet, Grulich Andrew E, Visser Richard O. Sex in Australia: Sexual Identity, Sexual Attraction and Sexual Experience among a Representative Sample of Adults. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2003;27(2):138–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2003.tb00801.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Steele Claude M, Southwick Lillian L, Critchlow Barbara. Dissonance and Alcohol: Drinking Your Troubles Away. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1981;41(5):831–46. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.41.5.831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Stets Jan E, Burke Peter J. Identity Theory and Social Identity Theory. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2000;63(3):224–37. [Google Scholar]
  63. Stone Jeff, Cooper Joel. A Self-standards Model of Cognitive Dissonance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2001;37(3):228–43. [Google Scholar]
  64. Tabatabai Ahoo, Linders Annulla. Vanishing Act: Non-straight Identity Narratives of Women in Relationships with Women and Men. Qualitative Sociology. 2011;34(4):583–99. [Google Scholar]
  65. Talley Amelia E, Hughes Tonda L, Aranda Frances, Birkett Michelle, Marshal Michael P. Exploring alcohol-use behaviors among heterosexual and sexual minority adolescents: intersections with sex, age, and race/ethnicity. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(2):295–303. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Talley Amelia E, Littlefield Andrew K. Pathways between Concealable Stigmatized Identities and Substance Misuse. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2014;8(10):569–82. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Trocki Karen F, Drabble Laurie A, Midanik Lorraine T. Tobacco, Marijuana, and Sensation Seeking: Comparisons across Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual, and Heterosexual Groups. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2009;23:620–31. doi: 10.1037/a0017334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES