Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2015 Jun 8;5(6):e007184. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007184

Are family, neighbourhood and school social capital associated with higher self-rated health among Croatian high school students? A population-based study

Dario Novak 1,2, Etsuji Suzuki 3, Ichiro Kawachi 4
PMCID: PMC4466611  PMID: 26056122

Abstract

Objectives

We investigated the associations between self-rated health and social capital among Croatian high school students.

Design

A cross-sectional survey among high school students was carried out in the 2013–2014 school year.

Setting

High schools in Croatia.

Participants

Subjects were 3427 high school students (1688 males and 1739 females), aged 17–18 years.

Main outcome measure

Self-rated health was assessed by the single item: “How do you perceive your health?”. Possible responses were arranged along a five-item Likert-type scale: 1 very poor, 2 poor, 3 fair, 4 good, 5 excellent. The outcome was binarised as ‘good health’ (excellent, good or fair) versus ‘poor health’ (poor or very poor).

Methods

We calculated ORs and 95% CIs for good self-rated health associated with family, neighbourhood and school social capital, while adjusting for gender, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress, physical activity and body mass index. We used generalised estimating equations using an exchangeable correlation matrix with robust SEs.

Results

Good self-rated health was significantly associated with higher family social capital (OR 2.43; 95% CI 1.55 to 3.80), higher neighbourhood trust (OR 2.02; 95% CI 1.48 to 2.76) and higher norms of reciprocity at school (OR 1.79; 95% CI 1.13 to 2.84). When all of the social capital variables were entered simultaneously, good self-rated health remained significantly associated with higher family social capital (OR 1.98; 95% CI 1.19 to 3.30), neighbourhood trust (OR 1.77; 95% CI 1.25 to 2.51) and reciprocity at school (OR 1.71; 95% CI 1.08 to 2.73).

Conclusions

Higher levels of social capital were independently associated with higher self-rated health among youth. Intervention and policies that leverage community social capital might serve as an avenue for health promotion in youth.

Keywords: EPIDEMIOLOGY, PUBLIC HEALTH


Strengths and limitations of this study.

  • This study is one of the fewer studies to date that have focused on social capital and health among children and youth.

  • We used a random sampling approach to select 20 high schools in Zagreb, all of which agreed to take part in the survey. A total of 3427 students (93.8%) responded to the survey which was given during class.

  • To clarify which source of social capital is likely to affect adolescents’ health, we assessed three different sources of social capital—in the family, in the neighbourhood, and at school—among high school students.

  • We used the generalised estimating equations using an exchangeable correlation matrix with robust SEs in order to correct SEs for clustering.

  • Owing to the cross-sectional design, we cannot exclude the possibility of reverse causation. Since we used a subjective measure of health and social capital, there is a possibility of common method bias.

Introduction

Social capital has been defined as the “resources embedded in a social structure which are accessed and/or mobilised in purposive actions.”1 2 Some scholars have conceptualised social capital as the social networks themselves, or as both the network structures and the resources channelled through the networks.3 4 Social capital has garnered increasing attention as a potential influence on the development of youth. In the field of education, research has primarily focused on the role of social capital in children's academic performance;5 6 however, subsequent research has expanded the range of outcomes to include health behaviours and population health outcomes.4 7 Social capital theory posits that interpersonal trust, norms of reciprocity and exchange of social support between members of a networks each constitutes a type of resource, and that access to these resources may facilitate the actions of group members.4 7 8 Investigation of possible influences of social capital on health may be particularly salient in adolescents because previous work has suggested that contextual influences may have effects on the somatic and psychological development of young people throughout the life course.9 10

Associations between social capital and health have been extensively investigated in adult samples.11–13 Social capital has been found in previous studies to be related to physical activity and body mass index (BMI).14–16 At the same time, physical activity and BMI are predictors of self-rated health. Therefore, both are not considered to be confounders of the relation between social capital and self-rated health. Rather, our underlying hypothesised model is: social capital→physical activity/BMI→self-rated health. However, fewer studies to date have focused on social capital and health among children and youth. According to Urie Bronfenbrenner's ecological systems theory of child development, human development is conceptualised as being shaped by the interaction between an individual and his or her environment; furthermore, there are many different levels of environmental influences that can affect a child’s development, starting with people and institutions immediately surrounding the child (ie, parents and families), to school environments, to residential neighbourhoods and eventually the societal culture.17 In-line with this, we hypothesised that family, neighbourhood and school social capital may be associated with an adolescent's good health and that students who report higher levels of social capital in all three domains will have higher self-rated health. However, few studies have simultaneously examined the contribution of different sources of social capital to youth health.

Accordingly, in the present study, we investigated the influences of different sources of social capital—in the family, in the neighbourhood, and at school—on levels of self-rated health among a sample of high school students in Croatia.

Methods

Participants

We administered a survey among high school students in Zagreb, a mid-sized urban city in central Croatia with a population of about 1 000 000 people. A random sampling approach was used to select high schools. All of the 20 schools that we approached agreed to take part in the survey, representing 3650 students enrolled in the 2013–2014 school year. Of these, 3427 students (1688 males and 1739 females, aged 17–18 years) responded to the survey (93.8%) which was given during class. Finally, the data of 3427 students aged 17–18 years were analysed.

Self-rated health

Self-rated health was assessed in these young adolescents using the standard single-item measure: “How do you perceive your health?”. Possible responses were arranged along a five-item Likert-type scale: 1 very poor, 2 poor, 3 fair, 4 good, 5 excellent. We binarised the outcome, that is, fair, good and excellent were collapsed into one category (good health), while poor and very poor were designated as poor health. Perceived health is an easily administered and widely used outcome measure in social epidemiology studies, and it has been shown to be a reliable predictor of mortality and healthcare use in adults.18–20 The measure has also been used in adolescents.21–24

Social capital indicators

On the survey, we inquired about individual perceptions of social capital in the family, neighbourhood and high school settings.3 7 25 Family social capital was assessed by the question: ‘‘Do you feel your family understands and gives attention to you?’’.7 26 Neighbourhood social capital was assessed by using two items; ‘‘Do you feel people trust each other in your neighbourhood (neighbourhood trust)?’’ ‘‘Do you feel that your neighbours step in to criticise someone's deviant behaviour during high school (informal social control)?’’.7 School social capital was assessed by three items; ‘‘Do you feel teachers and students trust each other in your high school (vertical school trust)?’’ ‘‘Do you feel students trust each other in your high school (horizontal school trust)?’’ ‘‘Do you feel students collaborate with each other in your high school (reciprocity at school)?’’ The response options were: ‘strongly agree’; ‘agree’; ‘neither agree or disagree’; ‘disagree’; ‘strongly disagree’. Then, for each response, we created a dichotomous variable (high: ‘strongly agree’, ‘agree’ and ‘neither agree or disagree’; low: ‘disagree’ and ‘strongly disagree’).7 The Cronbach α of the school social capital scale was 0.71, and since other domains have fewer than three questions, we considered it not appropriate to check Cronbach αs for these scales.

Covariates

As a measure of physical activity, we considered students’ total physical activity in the past 7 days. Physical activity was assessed using the validated short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) and was expressed as metabolic equivalent-hours per week).27 As additional potential mediators, we considered BMI based on the calculation from self-reported height and weight (scoring of responses in the range ≥25 kg/m2 vs <25 kg/m2 discriminates between respondents with and without high BMI). Socioeconomic status (SES) was entered in our regression models as a potential confounder, that is, theoretically associated with self-rated health and social capital.28 The classification of SES was based on both parents’ occupation at the time when the research was conducted. Self-perceived SES was categorised into three levels as high SES (ie, managers and professionals), middle SES (white collar) and low SES (blue collar),29 and it was dichotomised as high/middle (responses in the range 2–4) and low (responses in the range 5–6). Psychological distress was also assessed as a potential confounder using the six-item Kessler scale by the questions: ‘‘About how often during the past 30 days did you feel nervous?’’, ‘‘During the past 30 days, about how often did you feel hopeless?’’, ‘‘During the past 30 days, about how often did you feel restless or fidgety?’’, ‘‘How often did you feel so depressed that nothing could cheer you up?’’, ‘‘During the past 30 days, about how often did you feel that everything was an effort?’’ and ‘‘During the past 30 days, about how often did you feel worthless?’’.30 Each question is scored from 0 (none of the time) to 4 (all of the time). Scores of the six questions were then summed (0–24), with a lower score indicating low levels of psychological distress. Previous research has shown that dichotomous scoring of responses in the range 13+ versus 0–12 discriminates between respondents with and without significant psychological distress.30 31 A test of interaction between psychological distress and gender is performed and there is statistical gender difference in psychological distress (p<0.001).

Data analysis

We used the generalised estimating equations using an exchangeable correlation matrix with robust SEs in order to correct SEs for clustering. We calculated ORs and 95% CIs for good self-rated health according to levels of perceived social capital. The association of self-rated health with social capital indicators was examined by conducting a multiple logistic regression analysis. Furthermore, the study included students from 20 schools; thus, one of the assumptions was that the measurements within school might not be independent. Therefore, the cluster effect was considered in the analysis. For this purpose, we adjusted the SEs by computing clustered robust SEs for the coefficients. We also performed a χ2 test to check the statistical significance of the gender difference in proportion of boys versus girls reporting good self-rated health. As potential confounders, we entered gender, self-perceived SES and psychological distress. We also included physical activity and BMI as potential mediators of the association between social capital and self-rated health. We investigated the association between self-rated health and family social capital (model 1), neighbourhood social capital (model 2) and school social capital (model 3). Finally, we entered all of these social capital variables simultaneously (model 4) to assess their independent contributions to self-rated health. The interaction term between social capital and gender was not statistically significant so we dropped the sex-stratified analyses. A p value of <0.05 (two sided) was considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed with Stata, V.12 (StataCorp, Texas, USA).

Results

Boys reported a higher percentage of good self-rated health (85.8%) compared with girls (75.6%). Roughly 20% of the participants reported poor health. It is worth noting that the prevalence of psychological distress in girls was twice as high as that in boys. Males do generally have a higher BMI than females (22.95±2.85 kg/m2 vs 20.81±2.54 kg/m2). Most adolescents evaluated their family SES as high/middle (60.2%) with no statistically significant differences between boys and girls. Boys are significantly more physically active than girls. It is worthwhile to note that a χ2 test shows the statistical significance of the gender difference in proportion of boys versus girls reporting good self-rated health (table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study subjects, Zagreb, Croatia, 2014

Total
(N=3427)
Males
(N=1688)
Females
(N=1739)
p Value*
N (%) N (%) N (%)
Self-rated health
 Poor 664 (19.4) 239 (14.2) 425 (24.4)
 Good 2763 (80.6) 1449 (85.8) 1314 (75.6) <0.001
Family social capital
 Low 185 (5.4) 90 (5.4) 95 (5.5)
 High 3242 (94.6) 1598 (94.6) 1644 (94.5) 0.808
Neighbourhood trust
 Low 1104 (32.2) 466 (27.6) 638 (36.6)
 High 2323 (67.8) 1222 (72.4) 1101 (63.4) <0.001
Informal social control
 Low 828 (24.2) 419 (24.8) 409 (23.5)
 High 2599 (75.8) 1269 (75.2) 1330 (76.5) 0.342
Vertical school trust
 Low 1050 (30.6) 485 (28.7) 565 (32.5)
 High 2377 (69.4) 1203 (71.2) 1174 (67.5) 0.014
Horizontal school trust
 Low 840 (24.5) 339 (20.1) 501 (28.8)
 High 2587 (75.5) 1349 (79.9) 1238 (71.2) <0.001
Reciprocity at school
 Low 459 (13.4) 186 (11.1) 273 (15.7)
 High 2968 (86.6) 1502 (88.9) 1466 (84.3) <0.001
Body mass index
 Normal 3001 (87.6) 1367 (80.9) 1634 (93.9)
 Overweight/obese 426 (12.4) 321 (19.1) 105 (6.1) <0.001
Self-perceived socioeconomic status
 High/middle 2064 (60.2) 1008 (59.7) 1056 (60.7)
 Low 1363 (39.8) 680 (40.3) 683 (39.3) 0.706
Psychological distress
 High 848 (24.7) 274 (16.3) 574 (33.0)
 Low 2579 (75.3) 1414 (83.7) 1165 (67.0) <0.001
Physical activity
 High/moderate 2943 (85.9) 1499 (88.8) 1444 (83.1)
 Low 484 (14.1) 189 (11.2) 295 (16.9) <0.001

*Univariable, χ2 test.

The association between social capital and self-rated health is shown in table 2. Overall, self-rated health was significantly associated with each domain of social capital. Good self-rated health was significantly associated with higher family social capital (OR 2.43; 95% CI 1.55 to 3.80) and higher neighbourhood trust (OR 2.02; 95% CI 1.48 to 2.76). Regarding school social capital, good self-rated health was significantly associated only with perceptions of reciprocity at school (OR 1.79; 95% CI 1.13 to 2.84). When all social capital variables were entered into the model (model 4), good self-rated health was significantly associated with higher family social capital (OR 1.98; 95% CI 1.19 to 3.30), neighbourhood trust (OR 1.77; 95% CI 1.25 to 2.51) and reciprocity at school (OR 1.71; 95% CI 1.08 to 2.73).

Table 2.

ORs for good self-rated health among high school students, Zagreb, Croatia, 2014

Model 1
OR (95% CI)
Model 2
OR (95% CI)
Model 3
OR (95% CI)
Model 4
OR (95% CI)
Family social capital
 Low
 High 2.43 (1.55 to 3.80)*** 1.98 (1.19 to 3.30)**
Neighbourhood trust
 Low
 High 2.02 (1.48 to 2.76)*** 1.77 (1.25 to 2.51)***
Informal social control
 Low
 High 1.42 (0.99 to 2.04) 1.37 (0.95 to 1.98)
Vertical school trust
 Low
 High 1.34 (0.99 to 1.81) 1.19 (0.87 to 1.62)
Horizontal school trust
 Low
 High 1.24 (0.83 to 1.84) 1.19 (0.79 to 1.79)
Reciprocity at school
 Low
 High 1.79 (1.13 to 2.84)** 1.71 (1.08 to 2.73)*
Gender
 Male
 Female 0.46 (0.29 to 0.73)*** 0.49 (0.31 to 0.78)** 0.48 (0.30 to 0.78)** 0.48 (0.30 to 0.79)**
Body mass index
 Normal
 Overweight/obese 0.44 (0.25 to 0.77)** 0.43 (0.23 to 0.78)** 0.43 (0.23 to 0.79)** 0.42 (0.23 to 0.77)**
Self-perceived socioeconomic status
 High/middle
 Low 1.01 (0.64 to 1.60) 1.05 (0.66 to 1.68) 1.03 (0.65 to 1.64) 1.05 (0.66 to 1.65)
Psychological distress
 High
 Low 0.37 (0.24 to 0.57)*** 0.38 (0.24 to 0.59)*** 0.38 (0.24 to 0.59)*** 0.41 (0.26 to 0.64)***
Physical activity
 High/moderate
 Low 0.64 (0.42 to 1.00)* 0.65 (0.42 to 1.01)* 0.65 (0.41 to 1.01)* 0.66 (0.42 to 1.04)*

These four models were examined in a sequence of four logistic regression models considering clustering for schools.

Model 1: examine the association between family social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for gender, body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 2: examine the association between neighbourhood social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for gender, body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 3: examine the association between school social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for gender, body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 4: examine the association between all social capital variables and youth self-rated health adjusting for gender, body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.

Table 3 presents the association between family, neighbourhood and school social capital with good self-rated health among high school students. Good self-rated health was significantly associated with higher family social capital (coefficient 0.88; 95% CI 0.35 to 1.42), higher neighbourhood trust (coefficient 0.70; 95% CI 0.34 to 1.06) and higher norms of reciprocity at school (coefficient 0.58; 95% CI 0.13 to 1.04). When all types of social capital were entered simultaneously, good self-rated health remained significantly associated with higher family social capital (coefficient 0.68; 95% CI 0.13 to 1.23), neighbourhood trust (coefficient 0.57; 95% CI 0.20 to 0.94) and reciprocity at school (coefficient 0.54; 95% CI 0.08 to 0.99).

Table 3.

Coefficients for good self-rated health associated with family, neighbourhood and school social capital among high school students, Zagreb, Croatia, 2014

Model 1
Coefficient (95% CI)
Model 2
Coefficient (95% CI)
Model 3
Coefficient (95% CI)
Model 4
Coefficient (95% CI)
Family social capital
 Low
 High 0.88 (0.35 to 1.42)*** 0.68 (0.13 to 1.23)**
Neighbourhood trust
 Low
 High 0.70 (0.34 to 1.06)*** 0.57 (0.20 to 0.94)**
Informal social control
 Low
 High 0.35 (−0.03 to 0.74) 0.31 (−0.07 to 0.71)
Vertical school trust
 Low
 High 0.29 (−0.09 to 0.69) 0.17 (−0.22 to 0.57)
Horizontal school trust
 Low
 High 0.21 (−0.21 to 0.65) 0.17 (−0.25 to 0.61)
Reciprocity at school
 Low
 High 0.58 (0.13 to 1.04)** 0.54 (0.08 to 0.99)*
Gender
 Male
 Female −0.77 (−1.18 to −0.36)*** −0.70 (−1.11 to −0.29)*** −0.71 (−1.12 to −0.30)*** −0.71 (−1.12 to −0.30)***
Body mass index
 Normal
 Overweight/obese −0.81 (−1.28 to −0.34)*** −0.83 (−1.30 to −0.36)*** −0.83 (−1.31 to −0.36)*** −0.84 (−1.32 to −0.36)***
Self-perceived socioeconomic status
 High/middle
 Low 0.01 (−0.35 to 0.38) 0.05 (−0.31 to 0.42) 0.03 (−0.33 to 0.40) 0.04 (−0.32 to 0.42)
Psychological distress
 High
 Low −0.97 (−1.33 to −0.60)*** −0.96 (−0.32 to −0.59)*** −0.95 (−1.32 to −0.59)*** −0.87 (−1.24 to −0.50)***
Physical activity
 High/moderate
 Low −0.43 (−0.87 to 0.00)* −0.42 (−0.86 to 0.01)* −0.42 (−0.87 to 0.01)* −0.40 (−0.85 to 0.35)

These four models were estimated by generalised estimating equations using an exchangeable correlation matrix with robust SEs.

Model 1: examine the association between family social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 2: examine the association between neighbourhood social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 3: examine the association between school social capital and youth self-rated health adjusting for body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

Model 4: examine the association between all social capital variables and youth self-rated health adjusting for body mass index, self-perceived socioeconomic status, psychological distress and physical activity.

*p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001.

Discussion

Previous studies in the USA and Europe have suggested that higher levels of informal social control were associated with higher levels of perceived health.10 Drukker et al32 found that higher levels of community informal social control in the Netherlands may directly prevent young people from engaging in deleterious health behaviours as well as indirectly provide them with self-confidence and a sense of protection. Furuta et al7 have shown that the association of social capital with self-rated oral health is not uniform; higher trust is associated with better oral health, whereas higher informal control in the community is associated with worse oral health.

To better understand the findings of this research, it is very important to briefly explain the Croatian social context and the theoretical approach to young people. The mid-1980s in Croatia was a period of socialism before the collapse of the Soviet bloc. The first decade of the 1990s was more turbulent in Croatia compared with other post socialist countries. Croatia experienced armed conflicts that lasted for several years. Finally, at the beginning of the 1990s with the state's declaration of independence, and the abolition of the totalitarian regime, the nation became one of many transition countries. Young people in Croatia are one of the population segments most rapidly affected by these processes and changes. The reasons for this are multiple and related to the timing of political transition, with the transition from childhood to adulthood.33

In this study, we have found a statistically significant association between higher levels of family social capital and higher self-rated health. For young people, family should be important for ‘being there’ in times of need and family members are often regarded as a crucial source of support.34 Morgan and Haglund35 reported that a sense of belonging in family was related to self-rated health and health behaviours in adolescents. In transitional societies, the changes in hierarchical order and value structures accompanying the shift from socialism to free markets meant that families became especially important as a source of social support.36

We also found that those living in high-trust communities reported better health compared with youth living in low-trust communities. According to surveys, Croatian youth frequently spend their time with friends in the neighbourhood engaged in sport or other activities (ie, watching TV and videos, listening to the radio).33

The indicators based in the school social environment suggested that higher reciprocity at school (collaboration between pupils) was associated with higher self-rated health, whereas vertical and horizontal social capital were not significantly associated with self-rated health. Spending time with peers at school may engender a sense of belonging,34 and it may promote better health. The data show that 78% of Croatian youth frequently talk to their school peers about going out and leisure, music, movies and books.33 A previous study in Denmark found that school connectedness and a sense of belonging may have a strong impact on adolescent psychological health.37

Our study has some limitations. First, owing to the cross-sectional design, we cannot exclude the possibility of reverse causation, that is, poor health led to a low level of trust and other indicators of social capital. To mitigate this, we adjusted for psychological distress. In other words, the students with psychological distress would report lower levels of social capital in all three domains simultaneously and psychological distress could potentially affect their self-ratings of health. Therefore, we adjusted for individual differences in psychological distress in order to rule out this possible bias. When we included an interaction term between psychological distress and gender, its coefficient was statistically significant. However, we observed no substantial change in the association between social capital indicators and good health. Moreover, the differential effect estimates of each type of social capital on health cannot be fully explained by reverse causation. Second, we used a subjective measure of health and social capital, and therefore there is a possibility of common method bias which may have resulted in bias away from the null. Again, the differential findings for each type of social capital suggest that this is less likely. Third, since the students responded to the questionnaires during the class, there is a possibility of measurement error of school social capital, in particular vertical social capital. Additionally, the possibility of type 1 error is high because of the clustering. We used the generalised estimating equations using an exchangeable correlation matrix with robust SEs in order to correct SEs for clustering. Fourth, the social capital variables in our study are analysed at the individual level. Therefore, we are referring to the students’ individual perceptions of social capital. Unfortunately, we did not have information about which neighbourhoods the respondents lived in nor about which classrooms the students attended within each school. We cannot fully disentangle the effects of school social capital and neighbourhood social capital in this study, partly because of the lack of information about class and neighbourhood. And fifth, all types of social capital were assessed in the primary sample. Future studies are warranted to assess all three domains (family, neighbourhood and school social capital) by approaching different sample subjects who are not participating in the primary sample.

This study shows that higher levels of family social capital, neighbourhood trust and reciprocity school (ie, collaboration relationships between pupils) were associated with better health among youth. Interestingly, the interaction term between social capital and gender was not statistically significant in this study, although there are some researches showing that, among adolescents, girls tend to report higher levels of social capital, especially school and family belonging than do boys.38 We can speculate that this was found since adolescent girls have a greater number of friends than do boys; they expect and desire more nurturing behaviour from their friends and family members, and experience more empathy, more self-disclosure and less overt hostility in their friendships than do boys.39 Additional studies are needed to identify interventions that can increase social capital to engender healthy habits with the ultimate goal of achieving healthier students. More studies exploring social capital and health in different countries should be conducted since social capital in general and in particular levels of informal social control may depend on different cultural norms and values.40 41

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the students and teachers for their enthusiastic participation in this study.

Footnotes

Twitter: Follow Dario Novak at @DNovakPhD

Contributors: DN conceptualised and designed the study, conducted the statistical analyses and interpreted the data, and wrote the article. ES participated in the conceptualisation of the study, contributed to the drafting of the article and reviewed the paper. IK reviewed the results and contributed to the drafting of the article. All authors approved the final manuscript.

Funding: This research was self-funded.

Competing interests: None declared.

Patient consent: Obtained.

Ethics approval: Ethics Committee/Institutional Review Board of the Faculty of Kinesiology University of Zagreb, Croatia.

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data sharing statement: The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are freely available.

References

  • 1.Lin N. Social capital: a theory of social structure and action. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2001:95–139. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kim D, Subramanian SV, Kawachi I. Bonding versus bridging social capital and their associations with self rated health: a multilevel analysis of 40 US communities. J Epidemiol Community Health 2006;60:116–22. 10.1136/jech.2005.038281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Putnam RD. Bowling alone. New York: Simon & Schuster, 2000:296. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D. Social capital and health. New York: Springer Science, 2010:215–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Coleman JS. Social capital and the development of youth. Momentum 1987;18:6–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Coleman JS. Foundations of social theory. London: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, 1990:300–2. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Furuta M, Ekuni D, Takao S et al. . Social capital and self-rated oral health among young people. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 2012;40:97–104. 10.1111/j.1600-0528.2011.00642.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Putnam RD. Making democracy work: civic traditions in modern Italy. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1993:167. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kalff AC, Kroes M, Vles JS et al. . Neighbourhood level and individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health 2001;55:246–50. 10.1136/jech.55.4.246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Drukker M, Buka SL, Kaplan C. Social capital and young adolescents’ perceived health in different sociocultural settings. Soc Sci Med 2005;61:185–98. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kim D, Subramanian SV, Kawachi I. Social capital and physical health: a systematic review of the literature. In: Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D, eds. Social capital and health. New York: Springer, 2008:139–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Murayama H, Fujiwara Y, Kawachi I. Social capital and health: a review of prospective multilevel studies. J Epidemiol 2012;22:179–87. 10.2188/jea.JE20110128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Virtanen M, Ervasti M, Oksanen T et al. . Social capital in schools. In: Kawachi I, Takao S, Subramanian SV, eds. Global perspectives on social capital and health. New York: Springer, 2013:65–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lindstrom M, Hanson BS, Ostergren PO. Socioeconomic differences in leisure-time physical activity: the role of social participation and social capital in sharping health related behavior. Soc Sci Med 2001;52:441–51. 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00153-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lindstrom M, Moghaddassi M, Merlo J. Social capital and leisure-time physical activity: a population-based multilevel analysis of individual and neighbourhood level data in Malmo. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:23–8. 10.1136/jech.57.1.23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ali SM, Lindstrom M. Psychosocial work conditions and leisure time physical activity: a population base study. Scand J Public Health 2006;34:209–16. 10.1080/14034940500307515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological models of human development. Oxford: Elsevier, 1994:37–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fylkesnes K. Determinants of health care utilization—visits and referrals. Scand J Sociol Med 1993;21:40–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Sociol Behav 1997;38:21–37. 10.2307/2955359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lundberg O, Manderbacka K. Assessing reliability of a measure of self-rated health. Scand J Soc Med 1996;24:218–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Johnson PB, Richter L. The relationship between smoking, drinking, and adolescents’ self-perceived health and frequency of hospitalization: analyses from the 1997 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse. J Adolesc Health 2002;30:175–83. 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00317-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Koivusilta L, Arja R, Andres V. Health behaviours and health in adolescence as predictors of educational level in adulthood: a follow-up study from Finland. Soc Sci Med 2003;57:577–93. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00405-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Pastor Y, Balaguer I, Pons D et al. . Testing direct and indirect effects of sports participation on perceived health in Spanish adolescents between 15 and 18 years of age. J Adolesc 2003;26:717–30. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tobi H, Meijer WM, Tuinstra J et al. . Socio-economic differences in prescription and OTC drug use in Dutch adolescents. Pharm World Sci 2003;25:203–6. 10.1023/A:1025836704150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Morrow V. Conceptualising social capital in relation to the well-being of children and young people: a critical review. Sociol Rev 1999;44:744–65. 10.1111/1467-954X.00194 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Duke NN, Skay CL, Pettingell SL et al. . From adolescent connections to social capital: predictors of civic engagement in young adulthood. J Adolesc Health 2009;44:161–8. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjöström M et al. . International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2003;35:1381–95. 10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Subramanian SV, Kim DJ, Kawachi I. Social trust and self-rated health in us communities: a multilevel analysis. J Urban Health 2002;79(Suppl 1):S21–34. 10.1093/jurban/79.suppl_1.S21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wang Z, Byrne NM, Kenardy JA et al. . Influences of ethnicity and socioeconomic status on the body dissatisfaction and eating behaviour of Australian children and adolescents. Eat Behav 2005;6:23–33. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kessler RC, Barker PR, Colpe LJ et al. . Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:184–9. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA et al. . Improving the K6 short scale to predict serious emotional disturbance in adolescents in the USA. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2010;19:23–35. 10.1002/mpr.314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Drukker M, Kaplan C, Feron F. Children's health related quality of life, neighborhood socio-economic deprivation and social capital. A contextual analyses. Soc Sci Med 2003;57:825–41. 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00453-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ilisin V, Potocnik D. A sociological portrait of contemporary Croatian youth. Ser Hist Sociol 2010;20:41–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Morrow V. Young people's explanations and experiences of social exclusion: retrieving bourdieu's concept of social capital. Int J Sociol Soc Policy 2001;21:37–63. 10.1108/01443330110789439 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morgan A, Haglund BJ. Social capital does matter for adolescent health: evidence from the English HBSC study. Health Promot Int 2009;24:363–72. 10.1093/heapro/dap028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kennedy BP, Kawachi I, Brainerd E. The role of social capital in the Russian mortality crisis. World Dev 1998;26:2029–43. 10.1016/S0305-750X(98)00094-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Due P, Lynch J, Holstein B et al. . Socioeconomic health inequalities among a nationally representative sample of Danish adolescents: the role of different types of social relations. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:692–8. 10.1136/jech.57.9.692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Newman BM, Lohman BJ, Newman PR. Peer group membership and a sense of belonging: their relationship to adolescent behavior problems. Adolesc 2007;42:241–63. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Galambos N. Gender and gender-role development in adolescence. In: Lemer R, Steinberg L eds. The handbook of adolescent psychology. New York: Wiley, 2004:233–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Binbay T, Drukker K, Alptekin H et al. . Evidence that the wider social environment moderates the association between familial liability and psychosis spectrum outcome. Psychol Med 2012;42:2499–510. 10.1017/S0033291712000700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Onyx L, Bullen P. Measuring social capital in five communities. J Appl Behav Sci 2000;36:23–42. 10.1177/0021886300361002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES