Skip to main content
BMC Research Notes logoLink to BMC Research Notes
. 2015 May 30;8:202. doi: 10.1186/s13104-015-1210-0

The effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment: a pilot study

Soichi Ando 1,2,, Takaaki Komiyama 3, Mizuki Sudo 4,5, Akira Kiyonaga 1,4, Hiroaki Tanaka 1,4, Yasuki Higaki 1,4
PMCID: PMC4467620  PMID: 26025443

Abstract

Background

Heat stress potentially has detrimental effects on brain function. Hence, cognitive function may be impaired during physical activity in a hot environment. Skin cooling is often applied in a hot environment to counteract heat stress. However, it is unclear to what extent neck cooling is effective for cognitive impairment during exercise in a hot environment. The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment.

Methods

Eight male young participants (mean ± SD, age = 26.1 ± 3.2 years; peak oxygen uptake = 45.6 ± 5.2 ml/kg/min) performed Spatial delayed response (DR) task (working memory) and Go/No-Go task (executive function) at rest and during exercise in the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions. After the participants completed the cognitive tasks at rest, they cycled the ergometer until their heart rate (HR) reached 160 beats/min. Then, they cycled for 10 min while keeping their HR at 160 beats/min. The cognitive tasks were performed 3 min after their HR reached 160 beats/min. The air temperature was maintained at 35°C and the relative humidity was controlled at 70%. Neck cooling was applied to the backside of the neck by a wet towel and fanning. We used accuracy of the Spatial DR and Go/No-Go tasks and reaction time in the Go/No-Go task to assess cognitive function.

Results

Neck cooling temporarily decreased the skin temperature during exercise. The accuracy of the cognitive tasks was lower during exercise than that at rest in the Hot and Hot + Cooling condition (p < 0.05). There were no differences in the accuracy between the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions (p = 0.98). Neither exercise (p = 0.40) nor cooling (p = 0.86) affected reaction time. These results indicate that temporal neck cooling did not alter cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment.

Conclusions

The present study suggests that temporal neck cooling with a wet towel and fanning is not effective for attenuating impairment of working memory and executive function during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a warm and humid environment.

Keywords: Cognition, Exercise, Brain, Hot environment, Body temperature

Background

Many sports and occupational settings require high level cognitive ability under physiological stress in a hot environment. For example, soccer players make decisions quickly and appropriately while running in a hot environment. Occupational workers are often forced to carry out complex physical works in a hot summer. Physical activity in a hot environment imposes a severe burden on the cardiovascular system because of competition between the muscle, skin, and brain for the available systemic blood flow [1, 2]. It has been suggested that exercise in a hot environment may decrease arousal level [3], and the brain seems to be particularly vulnerable to hyperthermia [4]. Although our knowledge of cognitive function in a hot environment is mainly based upon data from a resting condition [59], it is likely that exercise in a hot environment impair cognitive function.

Skin cooling is known to reduce thermal strain and discomfort at rest and during exercise [5, 913]. Hence, skin cooling is often applied in a hot environment to counteract heat stress. Recent studies indicated that head cooling has the beneficial effects on short-term memory in a hot environment [6, 14]. Furthermore, neck cooling may enhance cognitive performance in complex tasks following exercise-induced hyperthermia [15]. However, it is unclear to what extent skin cooling has beneficial effects on cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment. In many sports and occupational settings, cognitive function plays an important role under physiological stress in a hot environment. Thus, it is imperative to examine the effects of skin cooling on cognitive function during exercise under various experimental conditions. In the present study, we used temporal neck cooling because brief neck cooling is easily available and practically applicable to situations where exercise or physical activity is actually performed in a hot environment. Furthermore, this maneuver does not interfere with performing the cognitive task. In the present study, we hypothesized that if brief neck cooling has beneficial effects on cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment, the impairment in cognitive function may be attenuated by physiological and/or psychological changes induced by brief neck cooling.

When exercise duration is prolonged in a hot environment, dehydration is inevitable. Dehydration may also impair cognitive function although the results in previous studies are somewhat controversial [1624]. Thus, we used strenuous exercise with a short duration to exclude the possibility that dehydration also influences cognitive function. This allows us to focus on the effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment. This study will provide empirical evidence to demonstrate the effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a hot environment, especially its validity and limitations.

Methods

Participants

Eight male young participants (mean ± SD, age = 26.1 ± 3.2 years; height = 1.75 ± 0.06 m; weight = 69.6 ± 8.3 kg; peak oxygen uptake = 45.6 ± 5.2 ml/kg/min) were fully informed of the risks and discomforts associated with this study before giving written informed consent to participate. The participants were free of cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, or respiratory disease and were not taking any medications. They were not acclimated to exercising in a hot environment. This study was approved by the ethics committee of Fukuoka University and was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Cognitive task

A laptop computer was used to present visual stimuli. The participants performed a combination of the Spatial delayed response (Spatial DR) task and the Go/No-Go task [25, 26], where working memory and executive function were required. The participants performed the cognitive task while they faced a computer display at a viewing distance of approximately 80 cm on a cycle ergometer (75XLII, COMBI Wellness, Tokyo, Japan). Details in the present cognitive task were described elsewhere [26], and were summarized in Figure 1a. In brief, at the beginning of the cognitive task, the participants remembered the location where visual stimulus was presented (Spatial DR task). Then, one of the paired figures (Figure 1b) was presented (Go/No-Go task). In the case of a Go-trial, participants released the shift button as quickly as possible. In the case of No-Go trials, participants continued pressing the shift button. After the Go/No-Go task, visual stimuli were presented at eight locations surrounding the fixation point. The participants pressed the button of the ten-key corresponding to the remembered location (Spatial DR task). The cognitive task continued until participants completed 30 trials of both tasks. The participants pressed the ten-key with the right index finger (see right bottom of Figure 1a) and pressed the shift button on the keyboard with the left index finger. The ten-key and keyboard were horizontally situated above both sides of the handlebar of the ergometer.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

a Spatial delayed response (Spatial DR) task and Go/No-Go task. At the beginning of the Spatial DR task, a visual cue was presented at one of the eight locations. The participants remembered the location during the Go/No-Go task. After the Go/No-Go task, participants responded by pressing the button of the ten-key corresponding to the remembered location. In this case, participants had to press the number 4. b Examples of the paired figures. One of the paired figures was randomly presented in the Go/No-Go task.

We used accuracy of the cognitive task and reaction time in the Go-trial to assess cognitive function. In the Spatial DR task, error trials were defined as incorrect responses to the remembered location. In the Go/No-Go task, error trials were defined as omissions of the response in the Go-trial or incorrect response in the No-Go trial. Response accuracy was calculated as the number of correct trials divided by the total number of trials. When the participants finished five successive trials in the Go/No-Go task, the relation between correct response and the figure was reversed from the next trial [27]. After the next five successive trials were completed, new paired figures were presented. Participants were unaware when a correct response and figure was reversed or when the new paired figures were presented in advance. Thus, we excluded trials immediately after the relation between the correct response and figure was reversed or one of the new paired figures was presented in the Go/No-Go task.

Experimental procedure

The experiment was performed on two non-consecutive days. A few days before the first experiment, participants completed a series of practice blocks of the cognitive tasks at rest and during cycling until they were familiar with the task. The familiarization period continued until reaction times fall within 3 SD from the mean. All experiments were conducted inside an environmental control chamber (FHC-20S, Fuji-ika Sangyo, Chiba, Japan). In the Hot condition, the air temperature was maintained at 35°C and the relative humidity was controlled at 70%. In the Hot + Cooling condition, the air temperature and relative humidity were the same as the Hot condition, but neck cooling was applied to the backside of the neck by a towel wet in 21°C water and also by fanning from the back of the neck with a small electronic fan. Once the neck cooling was applied to the participant, the towel was put on until the experiment was completed. The towel was not changed during the experiment. The order of experimental conditions was assigned in a counterbalanced manner.

At the beginning of the experiment, the participants performed the cognitive task at rest. One minute after they completed the first cognitive task, participants started to cycle the ergometer. For the first 5 min, exercise intensity increased at 30–32 W/min in a ramp manner, and then increased at 20–21 W/min in a step manner until participants’ heart rate (HR) reached 160 beats/min. An ear sensor (COMBI Wellness, Tokyo, Japan), which was connected to the ergometer, continuously measured HR. Once the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min, exercise intensity was adjusted automatically to maintain the target HR through the ear sensor. The participants cycled for 10 min while keeping their HR at 160 beats/min. We recorded exercise intensity and HR each minute during exercise after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min. The pedalling rate during cycling was chosen freely by each participant. Participants performed the cognitive task 3 min after their HR reached 160 beats/min in the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions. In the Hot + Cooling condition, immediately after the recording of exercise intensity, HR, and skin temperature at 2 min after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min, neck cooling was started (approximately 2 min 10 s after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min). The participants did not drink beverages throughout the experiment.

Other measurements

Skin temperature (O215-WT, Dretec, Saitama, Japan) was measured from the neck to evaluate the effects of the neck cooling. Probe of the thermometer was directly contacted to the backside of the neck. Skin temperature was recorded each minute during exercise after the participants’ HR reached the target level. Ratings of perceived exertion (RPE; 6–20 Borg scale) [28] were recorded immediately after each cognitive task (within 10 s). Blood lactate concentrations were measured at rest and immediately after exercise. Capillary blood was collected from the right earlobe. Blood lactate concentration was determined with the lactate oxidase method using an automated analyzer (Lactate Pro, Arkray, Kyoto, Japan). Body weight was measured without clothes before exercise after emptying the bladder, and after exercise. Weight reduction was expressed as a percentage of body weight, and dehydration was assessed by weight reduction. Body temperature was measured from the tympanic membrane before and after exercise. We measured body temperature at the room outside the environmental chamber. In the room outside the chamber, the air temperature was maintained at 21°C and the relative humidity was controlled at 50%.

Data and statistical analysis

Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Exercise (rest and exercise) and Condition (Hot and Hot + Cooling) as the within-participants factors was performed for RPE, blood lactate concentration, body weight, body temperature, accuracy of the cognitive task, and reaction time in the Go-trial. We also performed two-way ANOVA with Time Course and Condition as the factors for exercise intensity, HR and skin temperature. Paired t tests were conducted to compare differences where appropriate. All data are expressed as mean ± SD. The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results and discussion

Exercise intensity and HR

Table 1 shows exercise intensity and HR after the participants reached a HR of 160 beats/min. We observed a significant main effect of Time Course (F [9,63] = 45.02, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.87) on exercise intensity. There were no significant main effect of Condition (F [1,7] = 1.62, p = 0.24, η2 = 0.19) and interaction (F [9,63] = 1.01, p = 0.44, η2 = 0.13). These results indicate that exercise intensity decreased as time elapsed and the degree of decrease was not different between conditions. Similarly, we found a significant main effect of Time Course (F [9,63] = 6.39, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.48) on HR. There were no significant main effect of Condition (F [1,7] = 2.91, p = 0.13, η2 = 0.29) and interaction (F [9,63] = 0.81, p = 0.61, η2 = 0.10). The significant main effect of Time Course may be ascribed to a slight higher HR around the first 3 min after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min. Overall, HR appears to be well controlled around the target level in the present study.

Table 1.

Changes in intensity and HR during exercise after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min

Time after the participants’ HR reached 160 beats/min (min)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Exercise intensity (W)
 Hot 164.5 ± 25.3 149.3 + 20.0 137.3 ± 29.9 132.5 ± 30.1 134.1 ± 27.7 130.4 ± 26.0 127.8 ± 27.4 127.9 ± 25.6 125.0 ± 29.8 123.9 ± 30.2
 Hot + Cooling 163.3 ± 22.8 149.8 ± 29.8 136.6 ± 29.9 126.0 ± 34.0 125.6 ± 33.4 123.8 ± 33.8 121.9 ± 35.1 121.0 ± 36.1 117.1 ± 35.9 114.6 ± 35.6
Heart rate (beats/min)
 Hot 162.3 ± 1.7 163.5 ± 2.2 163.6 ± 2.3 161.3 ± 2.0 159.9 ± 1.3 161.4 ± 1.7 162.1 ± 2.1 160.3 ± 3.1 160.0 ± 1.3 160.3 ± 1.9
 Hot + Cooling 162.6 ± 1.2 164.0 ± 3.0 165.1 ± 3.0 162.8 ± 1.7 160.8 ± 1.9 160.4 ± 1.6 161.1 ± 1.8 160.9 ± 1.4 161.0 ± 1.7 160.9 ± 1.3

Values are mean ± SD.

RPE, blood lactate concentration, body weight, and body temperature

Table 2 shows RPE, blood lactate concentration, body weight, and body temperature. We found significant main effects of Exercise on RPE (F [1,7] = 174.49, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.96), blood lactate concentration (F [1,7] = 43.02, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.86), body weight (F [1,7] = 55.30, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.89), and body temperature (F [1,7] = 56.82, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.89). In contrast, Condition did not affect these parameters. There were no significant interactions. Collectively, exercise increased RPE and blood lactate concentrations to the same extent in both conditions. The degree of dehydration and increase in body temperature were not different between conditions. Indeed, we observed no difference in weight reduction between conditions (0.41 ± 0.07% in the Hot condition and 0.37 ± 0.22% in the Hot + Cooling condition).

Table 2.

RPE, blood lactate concentration, body weight, and body temperature

Condition Variable Rest Exercise After
Hot RPE 7.3 ± 0.4 15.9 ± 2.0*
Blood lactate concentration, mmol/l 1.0 ± 0.2 5.3 ± 1.9*
Body weight, kg 69.1 ± 7.5 68.8 ± 7.5*
Body temperature, °C 36.1 ± 0.6 38.4 ± 0.4*
Hot + Cooling RPE 7.1 ± 0.9 15.8 ± 1.7*
Blood lactate concentration, mmol/l 1.0 ± 0.2 5.5 ± 1.7*
Body weight, kg 68.9 ± 7.7 68.6 ± 7.7*
Body temperature, °C 36.5 ± 0.6 38.3 ± 0.3*

Values are mean ± SD.

RPE ratings of perceived exertion.

* p < 0.001, vs. rest.

Effects of neck cooling on skin temperature

Figure 2 illustrates the skin temperature during exercise. The main effects of Time Course (F [1,9] = 9.60, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.58) and Condition (F [1,7] = 8.03, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.53) were significant. There was a significant interaction between Condition and Time Course (F [9,63] = 8.88, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.56). These results indicate that skin temperature changed in a different manner between conditions. We observed significant differences in skin temperature between conditions after neck cooling. However, the effects of neck cooling were temporal and did not last until the end of the exercise.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Neck skin temperature in the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions during exercise. The upper dashed arrow indicates the time when neck cooling was started. ‡‡ p < 0.01 between the conditions.

Cognitive function

Figure 3 illustrates the accuracy of the cognitive task (A) and reaction time in the Go-trial (B). We observed a significant main effect of Exercise on the accuracy of the cognitive task (F [1,7] = 8.78, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.56). We found neither a significant main effect of Condition (F [1,7] = 0.001, p = 0.98, η2 < 0.001) nor a significant interaction (F [1,7] = 0.27, p = 0.62, η2 = 0.04). These results indicate that the accuracy of the cognitive task significantly decreased during exercise, and that temporal neck cooling had no effects on the accuracy of the cognitive task. Reaction time in the Go-trial was not affected by Exercise (F [1,7] = 0.802, p = 0.40, η2 = 0.10) or Condition (F [1,7] = 0.03, p = 0.86, η2 = 0.005) in the present study.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

a Accuracy of the cognitive task at rest and during exercise. # p < 0.05 vs. rest. b Reaction time in the Go-trial at rest and during exercise.

To our knowledge, no studies have examined the effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive impairment during strenuous exercise in a hot environment. We tested if temporal neck cooling with a wet towel and fanning has beneficial effects on cognitive function during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a hot environment. The main finding of this study was that temporal neck cooling did not attenuate the detrimental effects on cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment. The present finding suggests that temporal neck cooling with a wet towel and fanning is not effective enough to attenuate cognitive impairment during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a warm and humid environment.

We assessed cognitive function using the combination of spatial DR task and Go/No-Go task. The Spatial DR task requires working memory [25, 29]. The Go/No-Go task requires executive function, including selective attention, response inhibition and interference control [30, 31]. The prefrontal and parietal cortices play a key role in spatial working memory [3235]. Activation in the prefrontal cortex is crucial for executive function [36, 37]. Furthermore, the frontopolar cortex, which is located at the most anterior part of the frontal cortex, is selectively activated in multi-tasking where execution of the primary task was maintained in a pending state while performing a secondary task [38]. Given that the present cognitive task was a modified version of multi-tasking, the present cognitive task required activation of the frontopolar cortex. Taken together, we can assume that the present cognitive task was appropriate to assess higher cognitive function in humans.

In the present study, HR was maintained at 160 beats/min during exercise. Indeed, RPE was approximately 16 immediately after the cognitive tasks during exercise. Furthermore, blood lactate concentration increased above 5 mmol/l after exercise in both conditions. Hence, in the present study, we can assume that exercise intensity was classified as strenuous.

Skin cooling may reduce thermal strain and/or discomfort at rest and during exercise [5, 913]. Thus, physiological as well as psychological changes induced by skin cooling may somehow attenuate the impairment in cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment. Previous studies indicated that head and neck cooling with frozen packs prevented the impairment of short-term memory [14, 39]. In the present study, the accuracy of the cognitive task decreased during exercise in the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions. We observed no differences in the accuracy of the cognitive task during exercise between conditions. These results suggest that temporal neck cooling for a short period did not have beneficial effects on cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment under the present condition. In the present study, while neck cooling temporarily decreased skin temperature, neck cooling did not alter body temperature. These results mean that the neck cooling applied in this study was not effective enough to decrease body temperature. The present results are in line with the findings showing that skin cooling did not reduce body temperature [911, 40]. Moreover, we found no differences in RPE between conditions. Therefore, the lack of beneficial effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function would be ascribed to that physiological and/or psychological changes induced by brief neck cooling was not sufficient enough to attenuate the detrimental effects on cognitive function.

Dehydration is one of the potential factors that may impair cognitive function in a hot environment. Hence, we used strenuous exercise with a short duration. We expected that severe dehydration is unlikely to occur even in a hot environment in the present study. Indeed, weight reduction was less than 0.5% in the Hot and Hot + Cooling conditions. Previous studies suggested that dehydration may impair complex cognitive tasks when weight reduction was greater than 2% [21, 22, 39]. Indeed, Gaoua et al. [39] suggested that, when weight reduction did not exceed 0.5%, fluid replacement is adequate and dehydration does not alter cognitive function. Hence, we can assume that the effects of dehydration on cognitive function were negligible in the present study.

In contrast to the accuracy of the cognitive task, reaction time in the Go-trial was not altered by exercise. These results suggest that the effects of strenuous exercise in a hot environment are different between accuracy and speed of response. Previous studies indicated that nerve conduction velocity increases in the hot environment [14, 39]. Thus, exercise in a hot environment may increase in nerve conduction velocity, which leads to the decrease in reaction time. However, in the present study, it is unclear whether reaction time was not affected by strenuous exercise or whether increases in conduction velocity in a hot environment was not effective, possibly due to a short duration of exercise. Furthermore, we cannot rule out the possibility that increases in nerve conduction velocity compensated for the negative effects of strenuous exercise on speed of response. Provided that multiple factors are responsible for changes in response speed during exercise, further studies will be required to elucidate these points.

It has been suggested that increase in arousal level induced by exercise affect cognitive function [41, 42]. From this viewpoint, the present results may suggest that arousal level went beyond the optimal level during strenuous exercise in a hot environment, and that temporal neck cooling was not effective to alter arousal level. However, physiological mechanisms underlying the alterations in arousal level during exercise remains to be elucidated. Thus, further studies are required to understand the association between arousal level and cognitive function.

Limitation

Whereas the present study benefited from a simple experimental design, there are several limitations in the present study. First, we matched exercise intensity during exercise based on the participants’ HR. Thus, the present results may be ascribed to the HR matched exercise. This point is a limitation to extend this finding to exercise-cognition interaction in other experimental conditions. Second, we used neck cooling by a wet towel and fanning because this method is practical and easily applicable. Once the wet towel was applied to the backside of the neck, the wet towel was not changed. Thus, the decrease in skin temperature was small and did not last more than 5 min (Figure 2). In contrast, in the previous studies that examined the effects of skin cooling on cognitive function, three cool frozen packs with a protective layer were applied to the head and neck [14, 39]. In another study, neck cooling was applied with neck cooling collar, and the cooling component was drained and replaced with a gel refrigerant [15]. Hence, cooling method in the present study was far less effective to counteract heat stress as compared with the method in the previous studies [14, 15, 39]. Furthermore, a previous study suggested that forehead cooling leads to the most beneficial effects on reducing thermoregulatory responses to a hot environment [43]. It is also suggested that the beneficial effects of head cooling may be more efficient with cognitive functions primarily involving the frontal cortex [39]. Accordingly, it is plausible that the absence of beneficial effects of neck cooling on cognitive function is attributable to the cooling method and cooled location in the present study. Future studies are needed to elucidate how skin cooling alters cognitive function during exercise in a hot environment with more effective cooling methods, including forehead cooling. Third, in the present study, exercise duration was short to avoid dehydration. Further investigation is necessary to clarify the effects of exercise with a longer duration in a hot environment on cognitive function. Finally, we measured tympanic temperature to evaluate body temperature. However, it is well known that tympanic temperature is not accurate as compared with rectal or oesophageal temperatures. Thus, accurate measurements of body temperature would be needed to reveal how and why cognitive function is altered during exercise in a hot environment.

Conclusion

We examined the effects of temporal neck cooling with a wet towel and fanning on cognitive function during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a hot environment. The present study suggests that temporal neck cooling by a wet towel and fanning may not be effective for attenuating impairment of working memory and executive function during strenuous exercise with a short duration in a hot and humid environment.

Authors’ contributions

SA, TK, MS, and YH conceived of the study, carried out the experiment, performed data analysis, and drafted the manuscript. AK and HT participated in the design of the study and helped to carry out the experiment and draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Dr. Kisou Kubota for providing software to assess cognitive function. This study was supported by a grant from the Uehara Memorial Foundation, JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 25702039, and MEXT-Supported Program for the Strategic Research Foundation at Private Universities (2008–2012).

Compliance with ethical guidelines

Competing interests The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Contributor Information

Soichi Ando, Email: soichi.ando@uec.ac.jp.

Takaaki Komiyama, Email: mt.komi51@gmail.com.

Mizuki Sudo, Email: mizuki97@hotmail.com.

Akira Kiyonaga, Email: kiyonaga@fukuoka-u.ac.jp.

Hiroaki Tanaka, Email: htanaka@fukuoka-u.ac.jp.

Yasuki Higaki, Email: higaki@fukuoka-u.ac.jp.

References

  • 1.Gonzalez-Alonso J, Crandall CG, Johnson JM. The cardiovascular challenge of exercising in the heat. J Physiol. 2008;586(1):45–53. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.142158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nybo L, Nielsen B. Middle cerebral artery blood velocity is reduced with hyperthermia during prolonged exercise in humans. J Physiol. 2001;534(Pt 1):279–286. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.t01-1-00279.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Nielsen B, Nybo L. Cerebral changes during exercise in the heat. Sports Med. 2003;33(1):1–11. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333010-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Brinnel H, Cabanac M, Hales JRS. Critical upper levels of body temperature, tissue thermosensitivity and selective brain cooling in hyperthermia. In: Hales JRS, Richards DAB, editors. Heat stress: physical exertion and environment. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1987. pp. 209–240. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Caldwell JN, Patterson MJ, Taylor NA. Exertional thermal strain, protective clothing and auxiliary cooling in dry heat: evidence for physiological but not cognitive impairment. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2012;112(10):3597–3606. doi: 10.1007/s00421-012-2340-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gaoua N. Cognitive function in hot environments: a question of methodology. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2010;20(Suppl 3):60–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01210.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hancock PA, Vasmatzidis I. Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: the current state of knowledge. Int J Hyperthermia. 2003;19(3):355–372. doi: 10.1080/0265673021000054630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.McMorris T, Swain J, Smith M, Corbett J, Delves S, Sale C, et al. Heat stress, plasma concentrations of adrenaline, noradrenaline, 5-hydroxytryptamine and cortisol, mood state and cognitive performance. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;61(2):204–215. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Simmons SE, Saxby BK, McGlone FP, Jones DA. The effect of passive heating and head cooling on perception, cardiovascular function and cognitive performance in the heat. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2008;104(2):271–280. doi: 10.1007/s00421-008-0677-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mundel T, Bunn SJ, Hooper PL, Jones DA. The effects of face cooling during hyperthermic exercise in man: evidence for an integrated thermal, neuroendocrine and behavioural response. Exp Physiol. 2007;92(1):187–195. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2006.034934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mundel T, Hooper PL, Bunn SJ, Jones DA. The effects of face cooling on the prolactin response and subjective comfort during moderate passive heating in humans. Exp Physiol. 2006;91(6):1007–1014. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2006.034629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Nunneley SA, Maldonado RJ. Head and/or torso cooling during simulated cockpit heat stress. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1983;54(6):496–499. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Simmons SE, Mundel T, Jones DA. The effects of passive heating and head-cooling on perception of exercise in the heat. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2008;104(2):281–288. doi: 10.1007/s00421-007-0652-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Racinais S, Gaoua N, Grantham J. Hyperthermia impairs short-term memory and peripheral motor drive transmission. J Physiol. 2008;586(Pt 19):4751–4762. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.157420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lee JK, Koh AC, Koh SX, Liu GJ, Nio AQ, Fan PW. Neck cooling and cognitive performance following exercise-induced hyperthermia. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2014;114(2):375–384. doi: 10.1007/s00421-013-2774-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Adam GE, Carter R, 3rd, Cheuvront SN, Merullo DJ, Castellani JW, Lieberman HR, et al. Hydration effects on cognitive performance during military tasks in temperate and cold environments. Physiol Behav. 2008;93(4–5):748–756. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Baker LB, Conroy DE, Kenney WL. Dehydration impairs vigilance-related attention in male basketball players. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39(6):976–983. doi: 10.1097/mss.0b013e3180471ff2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cian C, Barraud PA, Melin B, Raphel C. Effects of fluid ingestion on cognitive function after heat stress or exercise-induced dehydration. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001;42(3):243–251. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00142-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cian C, Koulmann N, Barraud PA, Raphel C, Jimenez C, Melin B. Influence of variations in body hydration on cognitive function: effect of hyperhydration, heat stress, and exercise-induced dehydration. J Psychophysiol. 2000;14(1):29–36. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.14.1.29. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Edwards AM, Mann ME, Marfell-Jones MJ, Rankin DM, Noakes TD, Shillington DP. Influence of moderate dehydration on soccer performance: physiological responses to 45 min of outdoor match-play and the immediate subsequent performance of sport-specific and mental concentration tests. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41(6):385–391. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2006.033860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gopinathan PM, Pichan G, Sharma VM. Role of dehydration in heat stress-induced variations in mental performance. Arch Environ Health. 1988;43(1):15–17. doi: 10.1080/00039896.1988.9934367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Neave N, Emmett J, Moss M, Ayton R, Scholey A, Wesnes K. The effects of protective helmet on physiology and cognition in young cricketers. Appl Cognit Psychol. 2004;18:1181–1193. doi: 10.1002/acp.1065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Szinnai G, Schachinger H, Arnaud MJ, Linder L, Keller U. Effect of water deprivation on cognitive-motor performance in healthy men and women. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2005;289(1):R275–R280. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00501.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tomporowski PD, Beasman K, Ganio MS, Cureton K. Effects of dehydration and fluid ingestion on cognition. Int J Sports Med. 2007;28(10):891–896. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-965004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Harada T, Okagawa S, Kubota K. Jogging improved performance of a behavioral branching task: implications for prefrontal activation. Neurosci Res. 2004;49(3):325–337. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Komiyama T, Sudo M, Higaki Y, Kiyonaga A, Tanaka H, Ando S. Does moderate hypoxia alter working memory and executive function during prolonged exercise? Physiol Behav. 2015;139:290–296. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ando S, Hatamoto Y, Sudo M, Kiyonaga A, Tanaka H, Higaki Y. The effects of exercise under hypoxia on cognitive function. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e63630. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0063630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Borg G. Simple rating for estimation of perceived exertion. In: Borg G, editor. Physical work and effort. New York: Pergamon; 1975. pp. 39–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jonides J, Smith EE, Koeppe RA, Awh E, Minoshima S, Mintun MA. Spatial working memory in humans as revealed by PET. Nature. 1993;363(6430):623–625. doi: 10.1038/363623a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Buck SM, Cohen NJ. Aerobic fitness and executive control of relational memory in preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43(2):344–349. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181e9af48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, Fernhall B, Thompson KM, Valentini TA. The effect of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on working memory. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41(4):927–934. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181907d69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Jerde TA, Curtis CE. Maps of space in human frontoparietal cortex. J Physiol Paris. 2013;107(6):510–516. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Klingberg T. Development of a superior frontal-intraparietal network for visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(11):2171–2177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Smith EE, Jonides J. Neuroimaging analyses of human working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95(20):12061–12068. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.20.12061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ungerleider LG, Courtney SM, Haxby JV. A neural system for human visual working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95(3):883–890. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(1):58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Koechlin E, Summerfield C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(6):229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature. 1999;399(6732):148–151. doi: 10.1038/20178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gaoua N, Racinais S, Grantham J, El Massioui F. Alterations in cognitive performance during passive hyperthermia are task dependent. Int J Hyperthermia. 2011;27(1):1–9. doi: 10.3109/02656736.2010.516305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Nybo L, Secher NH, Nielsen B. Inadequate heat release from the human brain during prolonged exercise with hyperthermia. J Physiol. 2002;545(Pt 2):697–704. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.030023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Brisswalter J, Collardeau M, Rene A. Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Med. 2002;32(9):555–566. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200232090-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tomporowski PD. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2003;112(3):297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Katsuura T, Tomioka K, Harada H, Iwanaga K, Kikuchi Y. Effects of cooling portions of the head on human thermoregulatory response. Appl Hum Sci. 1996;15(2):67–74. doi: 10.2114/jpa.15.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BMC Research Notes are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES