Abstract
Prenatal cocaine exposure may affect developing stress response systems in youth, potentially creating risk for substance use in adolescence. Further, pathways from prenatal risk to future substance use may differ for girls versus boys. The present longitudinal study examined multiple biobehavioral measures, including heart rate, blood pressure, emotion, and salivary cortisol and salivary alpha amylase (sAA), in response to a stressor in 193 low-income 14- to 17-year-olds, half of whom were prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE). Youth’s lifetime substance use was assessed with self-report, interview, and urine toxicology/breathalyzer at Time 1 and at Time 2 (6–12 months later). PCExGender interactions were found predicting anxiety, anger, and sadness responses to the stressor, with PCE girls showing heightened responses as compared to PCE boys on these indicators. Stress Response × Gender interactions were found predicting Time 2 substance use in youth (controlling for Time 1 use) for sAA and sadness; for girls, heightened sadness responses predicted substance use, but for boys, dampened sAA responses predicted substance use. Findings suggest distinct biobehavioral stress response risk profiles for boys and girls, with heightened arousal for girls and blunted arousal for boys associated with prenatal risk and future substance use outcomes.
Substance use in adolescence is a serious public health problem. Adolescent alcohol and drug use are associated with academic problems, impaired driving, violent behaviors, and increased risk for unprotected sex and HIV transmission (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2010; Windle et al., 2008), and adolescent substance use is associated with greater cognitive and neural alterations than adult substance use in animal models (Crews, Braun, Hoplight, Switzer, & Knapp, 2000; White, Ghia, Levin, & Swartzwelder, 2000). Further, substance use during adolescence (from early through late adolescence) predicts substance use disorders and antisocial activities in adulthood (Brook, Balka, Ning, & Brook, 2007; Chassin, Pitts, & Prost, 2001).
One group of youth who are particularly vulnerable to substance use and abuse are those whowere prenatally exposed to drugs. These youth are exposed to chemical teratogens in utero, and likely other stressors both prenatally and postnatally. According to prenatal stress theory and theories of teratogen exposure, these prenatal insults impact the structure and function of developing systems in the child’s body and brain, create alterations that persist throughout the child’s lifetime, and could lead to risk for substance abuse (Mayes, 1999; Welberg & Seckl, 2001). In addition, youth whose parents use drugs may model their parent’s drug-taking behavior, consistent with social learning theories of child behavior (e.g., Bandura, 1969). Finally, youth with drug-using parents are likely exposed to suboptimal parenting, which is theorized to lead to substance use (e.g., Brook, Brook, Gordon, Whiteman, & Cohen, 1990). Consistent with these theories, research on prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) youth finds that PCE is linked to increased substance use rates in adolescence in animal model studies (Rocha, Mead, & Kosofksy, 2002) and, recently, in human studies (Bennett, Bendersky, & Lewis, 2007, for boys only; Delaney-Black et al., 2011; Frank et al., 2011).
It is important to understand pathways by which prenatal exposures lead to increased risk for substance use in adolescence. One potential pathway may be through PCE effects on stress arousal systems. We propose a conceptual model (see Figure 1) positing that prenatal exposure to cocaine, particularly in the presence of compromised postnatal caregiving environments, leads youth to develop altered emotional and physiological arousal in response to stress. Theories of PCE effects have proposed that cocaine specifically impacts developing monaminergic neurotransmitter pathways in the fetus, pathways directly involved in arousal regulation (Mayes, 2002). Our model posits that PCE youth would specifically show alterations in arousal regulation, including arousal in response to stress (see also Mayes, 2002). We propose that these altered stress responses (among other factors, such as peer environments; Hawkins, Catalano, & Miller, 1992) then lead to risk for youth substance use. We propose, consistent with diathesis-stress models of substance use (e.g., Tarter et al., 1999) that the altered stress responses in PCE youth may lead the youth to react to environmental stressors in ways that lead to substance use.
Figure 1.
Conceptual model of associations between prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) status, adolescent stress response, and adolescent substance use.
This may occur in one of two ways. First, PCE youth may be overly aroused by stress and then may seek out substances to downregulate their heightened arousal responses, consistent with self-medication and stress-reduction theories of substance use etiology (Khantzian, 1985; Sinha, 2001). Second, PCE youth may be underaroused by stress and may seek out substances to upregulate their blunted physiological arousal, consistent with sensation-seeking theories of substance use (Wills, Vaccaro, & McNamara, 1994). In our conceptual model, we propose that the pathway from PCE to stress responses and from stress responses to substance use may differ by child gender, with at-risk girls taking a heightened arousal response/self-medication pathway and at-risk boys taking an underaroused/sensation-seeking pathway, as we discuss below (see Gender Section). Finally, we propose that negative caregiving environments may exacerbate effects of PCE on stress response and substance use outcomes, as has been proposed (and found) previously in the prenatal drug exposure literature (Bada et al., 2007; Behnke et al., 2006).
PCE and Stress Responses
Some prior empirical research has demonstrated links between PCE and alterations in children’s attentional and emotional arousal and regulation, and physiological stress responses (Brooks-Gunn, McCarton, & Hawley, 1994; Frank, Augustyn, Knight, Pell, & Zuckerman, 2001; Mayes, 1999; Tronick, & Beeghley, 1999). Specifically, studies of PCE youth in early childhood have found that PCE children show greater irritability and excitability along with heightened hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis arousal in response to stressful tasks as compared to nonexposed children (Bendersky, Bennett, & Lewis, 2006; Chaplin, Fahy, Sinha, & Mayes, 2009; Dennis, Bendersky, Ramsay, & Lewis, 2006; Eiden, Veira, & Granger, 2009; Mayes, Bornstein, Chawarska, Haynes, & Granger, 1996). There are relatively few prospective studies of PCE and biobehavioral stress responses in adolescents. Two studies of 9- to 12-year-olds found blunted HPA axis responses to stress in PCE children (Fisher, Kim, Bruce, & Pears, 2012; Lester et al., 2010). Our group found PCE × Gender differences in biobehavioral stress responses in an initial report on stress responses in the first 82 participants in the current cohort (Chaplin, Freiburger, Mayes, & Sinha, 2010). In that study, PCE adolescents showed higher cortisol levels at baseline and 1 hr poststressor as compared to non–cocaine-exposed (NCE) youth, PCE girls showed heightened emotional responses to stress, and PCE boys showed blunted blood pressure responses (Chaplin et al., 2010).
Stress Responses and Youth Substance Use
As noted in our conceptual model, these PCE × Gender differences in biobehavioral stress responses may have implications for risk of adolescent substance use. One important factor in the development of substance use is emotional and physiological responses to stressful events (Anderson & Teicher, 2009; Wills, Sandy, Yaeger, Cleary, & Shinar, 2001). Research has shown that high levels of chronic life stress and particular coping responses to life stressors (e.g., less “active” coping responses) predict increases in substance use in adolescents (e.g., Dubow, Tisak, Causey, Hryshko, & Reid, 1991; Wills et al., 2001). Further, research with adults with substance use disorders finds that they show altered biobehavioral stress responses, with heightened emotional and cardiovascular (heart rate [HR] and blood pressure [BP]) responses found in cocaine- and alcohol-dependent adults and blunted HPA axis (e.g., salivary cortisol) responses in alcohol-dependent adults (Adinoff, Junghanns, Kiefer, & Krishnan-Sarin, 2005; Fox, Hong, Siedlarz, & Sinha, 2008; Sinha et al., 2009; Sinha, Garcia, Paliwal, Kreek, & Rounsaville, 2006). Some research suggests gender differences in the stress responses of addicted adults, with addicted women showing heightened subjective emotional (e.g., anxiety; Back, Brady, Jackson, Salstrom, & Zinzow, 2005) and frontolimbic brain activation (Li, Kosten, & Sinha, 2005) compared to addicted men in response to stressors.
Less is known about how biobehavioral stress responses are associated with substance use in adolescents, even though adolescence is a time of increased emotional arousal, stress, and substance use and abuse (Chambers, Taylor, & Potenza, 2003; Johnston, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2001; Steinberg, 2004). A few initial studies with adolescents have found positive correlations between higher basal cortisol levels and future substance use (Huizink, Ferdinand, Ormel, & Verhulst, 2006; Huizink, Greaves-Lord, Oldehinkel, Ormel, & Verhulst, 2009; Rao, Hammen, & Poland, 2009). Studies examining cortisol responses to stressors as these relate to adolescent substance use have been less common. In the two reports on this topic that we know of, one paper found associations between higher cortisol (and emotional and cardiovascular) responses to stress and current alcohol use (Chaplin et al., 2012) and one paper found associations between lower cortisol stress responses and current substance use (van Leeuwen et al., 2011). Thus, there are very few studies of biobehavioral responses to stress and adolescent substance use and no longitudinal studies.
Gender
As noted above in our conceptual model, gender may be important to consider when examining links between biobehavioral stress responses and risk for substance use, particularly among adolescents. Sex differences in biobehavioral stress responses have been well documented in adolescents and adults (Kudielka & Kirschbaum, 2005; Taylor et al., 2000). A number of studies have reported gender differences in stress responses in youth, with girls showing higher levels of sadness, anxiety, and HR in response to stressful tasks than boys (Brody, 1999; Chaplin & Aldao, 2013; Kudielka, Buske-Kirschbaum, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2004). We propose (see Figure 1) that there may be different pathways to substance use for girls versus boys (Amaro, Blake, Schwartz, & Flinchbaugh, 2001; Kandel, Yamaguchi, & Chen, 1992; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004). There is some empirical evidence for this. Moss, Vanyukov, Yao, and Kirillova (1999) reported that blunted salivary cortisol in prepubertal boys predicted substance use in adolescence, but no such association has been reported in girls. At-risk boys may develop a blunted physiological stress responses, which could then lead them to seek out substances to upregulate arousal, as proposed in sensation-seeking models of adolescent substance use (Wills et al., 1994). In contrast, girls may be more likely than boys to cope with stressors by experiencing heightened emotional arousal (Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994), leading girls to be at greater risk for internalizing problems, such as depressive symptoms, and for using substances to downregulate arousal, as in self-medication and stress-reduction models of substance use (Khantzian, 1985; Sinha, 2001). Thus, it is possible that different biobehavioral stress response profiles predict substance use for girls versus boys. However, to date, there has been little research on this topic.
The Present Study
The present longitudinal study examined multiple biobehavioral measures, including cardiovascular (HR and BP), HPA axis (cortisol), sympathetic nervous system (SNS; salivary alpha amylase [sAA]), and emotional (reported anxiety, anger, and sadness) measures, in response to a social stressor as predictors of future (6–12 months later) substance use in a group of low-income middle adolescent boys and girls aged 14–17 years who were followed since birth, half of whom were PCE. We examined youth in middle adolescence because this is a critical period for the development of emotion and stress regulatory systems (Casey, Jones, & Hare, 2008; Spear, 2007) and for initiation of and increases in substance use (Johnston et al., 2001; Steinberg, 2004). Although it is true that patterns of substance use change from relatively low rates of use at age 14 (e.g., 28% use alcohol at age 14; Duncan, Strycker, & Duncan, 2012) to more common use, and more binge drinking, at age 17 (e.g., 24%–41% use substances at 12th grade; Johnston et al., 2010), the present study collapsed data across youth aged 14–17 when examining prediction of substance use from stress responses, given that substance use across adolescence has been shown to have negative sequelae in adulthood and all adolescent substance use may be linked to alterations in stress responsivity. We studied substance use rather than substance abuse because substance abuse is relatively uncommon in middle adolescence, whereas substance use during adolescence is common and is associated with substance abuse in adulthood (Chassin et al., 2001). Thus, we studied substance use in adolescence, which can be considered a marker for substance abuse risk.
One strength of the present study is the inclusion of multiple measures of stress responses in these youth. Stressors acutely engage both sympathetic and HPA axis systems and lead to increased subjective feelings of emotional arousal. However, arousal in these different systems is often not correlated, indicating that each measure may provide unique information about stress responsivity (Baum et al., 1992). We hope to more fully capture stress arousal pathways to substance use by using a multimethod biopsychosocial approach, because such approaches have been recommended in the literature to understand the development of psychopathology and other risk behaviors (Susman, 1997).
Hypotheses
First, based on the emerging literature on links between prenatal cocaine exposure and adolescent substance use, we hypothesized that PCE youth would show higher rates of substance use at age 14–17 years (Time 1) and at the 6- to 12-month follow-up relative to NCE youth. Based on findings by Bennett et al. (2007) of gender differences in PCE effects on youth substance use, we tested whether the association between PCE and substance use would differ by gender.
Second, based on our conceptual model and on previous findings (e.g., Lester et al., 2010), we hypothesized that PCE youth would show altered stress responses compared to NCE youth. We also examined whether the alteration would differ by gender. For all analyses of PCE effects on substance use and stress responses, we also conducted secondary analyses to examine whether PCE group differences were moderated by caregiver–child relationship quality, because negative caregiving may exacerbate negative effects of drug exposure (see Bada et al., 2007). Third, based on our conceptual model, we hypothesized that HR, BP, cortisol, sAA, and emotional responses to stress would predict adolescents’ future use of substances. Given our conceptual model and some research suggesting different pathways to substance use for females versus males, we explored whether this prediction differed by gender.
Method
Participants
Participants were drawn from a larger longitudinal study of the emotional and cognitive development of prenatally cocaine and other drug exposed (PCE) and NCE children. Children in the larger study cohort (N = 372) were followed since birth, with biannual assessments. Youth in the larger cohort ranged in age from 11 to 17 years, and those who were aged 14 to 17 years (the age range was chosen to coincide with middle adolescence and the high school period) were invited to join the present laboratory stress study if they met criteria for the laboratory study (no acute serious psychiatric condition, no serious medical condition, not currently pregnant, and IQ > 80). Based on these criteria, 13 adolescents (6.3%) were excluded: 5 for acute psychiatric disorders requiring multiple psychotropic medications (e.g., bipolar disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder), 1 for HIV+ diagnosis, 1 for insulin-dependent diabetes, 4 for IQs < 80, 1 who was blind, and 1 for pregnancy. One-hundred-ninety-three adolescents met criteria and were invited to participate. Of these, all agreed to participate in the study. These 193 were not different from the overall sample of 372 on demographic variables (sex, age, race, and mother education level), obstetric complications at birth, parent–child relationship quality, prenatal cocaine exposure status, or amount of mother’s cocaine use in pregnancy ( ps > .18).
The 193 participants in the present study (96 boys, 97 girls) had a mean age of 14.69 years (SD = 0.93 years, range from 14 to 17 years; for demographic information, see Table 1). One-hundred-thirteen (59%) were PCE and 80 (41%) were NCE (PCE children were originally oversampled in anticipation of greater attrition in that group). Caregivers accompanying the adolescents to the present study were their current primary caregivers. These were mostly biological mothers (69.0% PCE, 95.0% NCE), with some grandmothers (9.7% PCE, 2.5% NCE), biological fathers (5.3% PCE, 1.3% NCE), aunts/uncles (4.4% PCE, 0.0% NCE), familial foster care or adoptive parents (6.2% PCE, 1.3% NCE), and nonfamilial foster care or adoptive parents (5.3% PCE, 0.0% NCE).
Table 1.
Demographic, risk factor, and youth substance use information
Non–Cocaine Exposed (n = 80) |
Cocaine Exposed (n = 113) |
Exposure Group Difference |
|
---|---|---|---|
Child race: number (%) | χ2 = 13.26**** | ||
African American | 58 (72.5) | 104 (92.0) | |
Other | 22 (27.5) | 9 (8.0) | |
Child sex: number (%) male | 36 (45.0) | 60 (53.1) | ns |
Child age: mean (SD) | 14.59 (0.84) | 14.76 (0.98) | ns |
Mother education: number (%) completed high school | 68 (85.0) | 71 (62.8) | χ2 = 11.42** |
Caregiver: number (%) had mother primary caregiver | 76 (95) | 78 (69) | χ2 = 19.60**** |
PCRa score: mean (SD) | 1.88 (0.64) | 2.08 (0.68) | t (186) = 2.02* |
Childhood trauma score (n = 190): mean (SD) | 47.17 (9.64) | 47.13 (8.90) | ns |
OCSb score: mean (SD) | 92.25 (20.96) | 79.69 (19.01) |
t (191) = −4.34**** |
Youth substance use, Time 1 (n = 193): number (%) who used |
39 (51.3) | 74 (65.5) | χ2 = 5.41* |
Alcohol | 34 (87.2) | 57 (77.0) | ns |
Tobacco | 19 (48.7) | 43 (58.1) | ns |
Marijuana | 23 (59.0) | 48 (64.9) | ns |
Cocaine | 5 (12.8) | 5 (6.8) | ns |
Inhalantsc | 10 (25.6) | 4 (5.4) | χ2 = 9.64** |
Opiates | 1 (2.6) | 3 (4.1) | ns |
Ecstasy | 4 (10.3) | 4 (5.4) | ns |
Methamphetamines | 4 (10.3) | 4 (5.4) | ns |
Steroids | 4 (10.3) | 5 (6.8) | ns |
Youth substance use, Time 2 (n = 180): number (%) who used |
49 (65.3) | 86 (76.1) | χ2 = 6.41* |
Alcohol | 44 (89.8) | 67 (77.9) | ns |
Tobacco | 27 (55.1) | 47 (54.7) | ns |
Marijuana | 33 (67.3) | 66 (76.7) | ns |
Cocaine | 2 (4.1) | 7 (8.1) | ns |
Inhalants | 3 (6.1) | 8 (9.3) | ns |
Opiates | 2 (4.1) | 7 (8.1) | ns |
Ecstasy | 2 (4.1) | 4 (4.7) | ns |
Methamphetamines | 2 (4.1) | 7 (8.1) | ns |
Steroids | 2 (4.1) | 6 (7.0) | ns |
Note: PCR, Parent–child relationship; OCS, Obstetric Complications Scale.
Higher PCR scores indicate more negative parent–child relationship.
Higher OCS scores indicate more optimal birth conditions.
Reported inhalant use included “sniffing glue, breathing the contents of aerosol spray cans, or inhaling any paints or sprays to get high.”
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .0001.
Recruitment and prenatal drug exposure categorization
Participants’ mothers were recruited over a 5-year period from women registering for prenatal care at a women’s center of a large urban hospital in the Northeast or, for those who did not receive prenatal care, upon admission to the postpartum ward. The Women’s Center provided care for inner-city women and served a low-income, primarily ethnic minority population. Trained research associates screened women for substance use. Self-report information was obtained through a detailed interview (based on the Addiction Severity Index [ASI]; McLellan, Luborsky, O’Brien, & Woody, 1980), which covered lifetime use (number of years using) and frequency and amount of use in the previous 30 days for cocaine, tobacco, alcohol, marijuana, and other drugs (e.g., sedatives and opiates). Interviews were conducted either during the first prenatal visit or (for those not receiving prenatal care) immediately following delivery. For all women, regardless of reported drug use, urine samples were obtained for toxicology either several times throughout the pregnancy (for those women attending prenatal visits) and/or at delivery. Every mother had a urine screen at delivery. Urine was screened for metabolites of cocaine (e.g., benzoylecognine), opioids, benzodiazepines, and marijuana, using the Abbott TDx system, employing the recommended cutoff levels (Poklis, 1987). In addition, meconium screening was also instituted after 2 years of recruitment into the project. Meconium screening did not identify any additional cocaine users who were both interview and urine toxicology negative.
Mothers who used opiates were excluded from the study because the study was interested in effects of cocaine exposure specifically. The rest of the mothers were invited to participate in the study. Mothers were considered to be in the cocaine-using group (PCE) if they either reported cocaine use during pregnancy or if urine toxicology results were positive for cocaine. Because cocaine use frequently co-occurs with use of tobacco, alcohol, and/or marijuana (Withers, Pulvirenti, Koob, & Gillin, 1995), mothers in the cocaine-using group were not excluded if they used these other substances. Mothers were considered to be in the NCE using group if they were negative for cocaine use during pregnancy on self-report and on urine screens. The NCE group included mothers who used some substances (typically alcohol, tobacco, and/or marijuana) during pregnancy and mothers who did not use any alcohol or drugs during pregnancy. PCE mothers’ cocaine use and PCE and NCE mothers’ use of alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana (the most frequently used other drugs) are described in Table 2.
Table 2.
Mothers’ substance use and depressive symptom history for prenatally cocaine exposed and non–cocaine exposed groups
Non–Cocaine Exposed (n = 80) |
Cocaine Exposed (n = 113) |
Exposure Group Difference |
|
---|---|---|---|
Substance use in pregnancy | |||
Cocaine | |||
Days used out of 30: mean (SD) | 0 | 4.05 (5.20) | U = 8, 927**** |
Grams/day: mean (SD) | NA | 0.68 (1.00) | NA |
Alcohol | |||
Used alcohol: number (%) | 32 (40.0) | 77 (68.1) | χ2 = 15.09**** |
Days used out of 30: mean (SD) | 1.16 (0.90) | 4.57 (6.24) | U = 1, 682**** |
Tobacco | |||
Used: number (%) | 14 (17.5) | 78 (84.8) | χ2 = 49.85**** |
Days used out of 30: mean (SD) | 3.07 (7.75) | 3.27 (7.53) | ns |
Marijuana | |||
Used: number (%) | 9 (11.3) | 58 (51.3) | χ2 = 33.20**** |
Days used out of 30: mean (SD) | 1.00 (0.00) | 1.74 (3.99) | ns |
Depressive symptoms (n = 152): mean (SD) | 3.81 (3.24) | 4.60 (4.99) | ns |
Note: Depressive symptoms were measured by average Beck Depression Inventory total scores when the child was 3 and 6 months old. NA, Not applicable.
p < .0001.
Procedure
At Time 1, adolescents attended three study sessions (each about 2–2.5 hr), spaced about 1 week apart, with caregivers also attending the first two sessions. In the first two sessions, youth and caregivers completed questionnaires, computer tasks, and interviews assessing cognitive and emotional functioning, substance use, parenting, and psychiatric disorders. In the third, the laboratory stress session, adolescents completed the Trier Social Stress Test—Child version (TSST-C; Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997). Adolescents and caregivers were compensated $50 and $25, respectively, for each session. Informed parental consent and adolescent assent was obtained. The study protocol was approved by the university’s institutional review board.
Adolescents and caregivers were invited to return to the lab at 6- and 12-month follow-ups. At these sessions, adolescents again completed questionnaires, computer tasks, and interviews assessing cognitive and emotional functioning and substance use. One hundred and eighty youth (92 boys, 88 girls) attended the 6- and/or 12-month sessions and are included in analyses predicting Time 2 substance use. These youth were not significantly different from the overall sample of 193 on most demographic variables (sex, age, and mother education level), PCE status, obstetric complications, or youth substance use at Time 1 ( ps > .09). African American youth were more likely to attend the Time 2 sessions than were non–African American youth (χ2 = 9.36, p < .01).
TSST-C laboratory stress session
On the stress session day, adolescents arrived at 4:00 p.m. This time was chosen because it coincides with the nadir in the diurnal variation of cortisol, and hence stress-induced cortisol increases are easier to detect. Adolescents were brought into the testing room with a trained research assistant and seated at a table. A pulse sensor (NELLCOR SpO2 sensor) was placed on the adolescent’s forefinger on the nonwriting hand and a BP cuff (Critikon SESNSA CUF) was placed on the adolescent’s other arm to monitor BP and to obtain a measure of HR using the Critikon Dynamap system. Adolescents were asked to have a snack 1 hr prior to the session to control for effects of food intake on SNS and HPA axis functioning, and youth were not allowed to eat during the session. Youth were asked to refrain from alcohol and drug use prior to the session to control for acute drug effects on biobehavioral responses.
Adolescents were asked about their medication use on the day of the stress session, and adolescent girls who were regularly menstruating were asked about menstrual cycle day, because medication use and menstrual cycle can affect HPA axis functioning and cortisol determination in saliva (Hibel, Granger, Kivlighan, Blair, & Family Life Project Investigators, 2006; Kirschbaum, Kudielka, Gabb, Schommer, & Hellhammer, 1999). Nine adolescents (two boys, seven girls) reported taking medications. One reported using Motrin, one used Tylenol Allergy, one used Advil, one used Clindamycin, one used Concerta and Claritin, one took a birth control pill, one used Albuteral, one used Albuterol, Singulair and Flonase, and one used Advair and Singulair. Medication use and menstrual cycle phase were considered as covariates for analyses (see Data Analysis Plan below).
Then, from 4:15 to 4:55 p.m., there was an adaptation period during which the participants were led through progressive muscle relaxation for 5 min by the research assistant and then were told to practice relaxing. At 4:55 p.m., pre-TSST (“baseline”) HR, BP, saliva, and emotion measures were taken. At 5:00 p.m., adolescents participated in the TSST-C (Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997), as described below. The TSST-C procedure lasted 20 min and was videotaped. During the TSST-C, HR was measured at the beginning of the speech task and the beginning of the math task. After the TSST-C, HR, BP, saliva, and emotion measures were taken immediately (posttask) and once every 15 min through a recovery period of 60 min (+15, +30, +45, +60). Twenty minutes after the conclusion of the TSST-C (5:40 p.m.), adolescents completed a manipulation check questionnaire (based on Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997), which asked how difficult/stressful they found the math and speech tasks to be. Adolescents were then debriefed regarding the TSST-C task.
TSST-C procedure
The TSST-C task was used as the laboratory stressor. The TSST (Kirschbaum, Wust, & Helhammer, 1992) is one of the most widely used social stress tasks with adults and children/adolescents (Dorn et al., 2003; Kudielka & Kirschbaum, 2005; Susman et al., 2010). The TSST has been found to elicit a robust activation of the HPA axis system (Kirschbaum et al., 1992), the SNS as assessed by HR, BP, and sAA (Kajantie & Phillips, 2005; Strahler, Mueller, Rosenloecher, Kirschbaum, & Rohleder, 2010), and emotional arousal as assessed by self-reported emotion (Hastings, Zahn-Waxler, & Usher, 2007) in children and adolescents. Furthermore, the TSST is a speech and math task, making it similar to events occurring in adolescents’ lives at school (Klimes-Dougan, Hastings, Granger, Usher, & Zahn-Waxler, 2001), and it is a social-evaluative stressor, which may be particularly anxiety provoking for adolescents (Elkind & Bowen, 1979).
The TSST-C was performed according to instructions provided by Buske-Kirschbaum et al. (1997), except that in this study the adolescent prepared and delivered the speech in the same room (rather than in a “preparation room”). At 5:00 p.m., two unfamiliar adults (the “judges”) entered the laboratory room and told the adolescent that he/she would have to finish writing a story. The panel told the adolescent to “make the story as exciting as possible” because he/she would be “competing against other teenagers.” The judges gave the adolescent a story stem (used by Buske-Kirschbaum et al., 1997) and then left the room. The research assistant collected measures (not used in the present report) and then told the adolescent to prepare a 5-min story (from 5:05 to 5:10 p.m.). At 5:10 p.m., the judges reentered the room. One judge placed an audiorecorder in front of the adolescent and asked him/her to stand up and recite the story for 5 min while he/she was video- and audiotaped. If the adolescent finished reading what he/she had written, the judges asked the adolescent to continue telling the story. After the 5 min, the second judge asked the adolescent to remain standing and to complete a mental arithmetic task (“subtract the number 13 from 1,023 over and over as quickly and accurately as possible”) for 5 min. Each time the subject made an error, he/she was asked to start over. The judges were trained research assistants who had not interacted with participants prior to or during the session. Judges were instructed to maintain a neutral expression and not to assist the adolescent during the tasks. Periodic fidelity checks ensured that the judges consistently followed the prescribed script.
Stress response measures
Cardiovascular response
A Critikon Dinamap 120 Patient Monitor was used to assess systolic BP (SBP) and diastolic BP (DBP). A pulse sensor was attached to the participant’s forefinger on his/her nonwriting hand and was connected to the Dinamap Monitor to provide a measure of HR (HR). For each time point, HR was recorded once every 10 s for 1 min and then averaged.
HPA axis response
Salivary cortisol levels were measured as a marker of HPA axis activation. Saliva was collected with the Salivette collection device. Participants were instructed to place a cotton swab between their tongue and cheek for approximately 2 min until the swab was completely saturated. The saliva swab was collected in a plastic tube, which was placed directly on ice and stored at −20 °C. Saliva samples were assayed in duplicate following standard radioimmunoassay kits with no modifications (Coat-A-Count Cortisol Kit, Diagnostic Products Corporation, Los Angeles) at the Yale University Core Laboratories (New Haven, CT). The intraassay coefficients of variation ranged from 3.0% to 5.1%.
SNS response
sAA levels were collected as a surrogate marker of SNS activation. sAA was assayed at the Biomarker Core Laboratory at Pennsylvania State University using a kinetic reaction assay kit (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA) that employs a chromagenic substrate, 2-chloro-p-nitrophenol, linked to maltotriose (Granger et al., 2006). The enzymatic action of sAA on this substrate yields 2-chloro-p-nitrophenol that is spectrophotometrically measured at 405 nm using a standard laboratory plate reader. The amount of sAA activity present in the sample is directly proportional to the increase (over a 2-min period) in absorbance at 405 nm. Samples were tested in singlet per assay kit instructions. The intra- and intervariations for this assay are less than 7.5% and 6%, respectively. sAA has been found to increase following stressors in adolescents (Gordis, Granger, Susman, & Trickett, 2006) and correlates with measures of SNS activity, including plasma norepinephrine, BP, and HR (e.g., Chatterton, Vogelsong, Lu, Ellman, & Hudgens, 1996; Klein, Bennett, Whetzel, Granger, & Ritter, 2010).
Emotion
Adolescents’ self-reported negative emotions (anxiety, anger, and sadness) were assessed with the Differential Emotions Scale—Revised short form (DES-R; Izard, 1972). Each subscale consists of five adjectives describing a particular emotion state. The adolescent rated on a 5-point scale the extent to which each word describes the way he or she felt in that moment. The DES shows excellent psychometric properties (Izard, 1972) and has been used with children and adolescents (Blumberg & Izard, 1985; Chaplin, 2006). To further ensure validity of the DES, at the start of the session, the research assistant read each item to the adolescent aloud and defined any words that were unfamiliar to him/her.
Missing stress response data
Data were missing at one or more time points for seven youth for BP (due to equipment malfunction), five for cortisol and sAA (insufficient saliva), and two for emotion (refused to answer items). These subjects are excluded from analyses involving those variables.
Other measures
History of birth complications
Youth’s history of birth-related complications were measured with the Obstetric Complications Scale (OCS; Littman & Parmelee, 1974). The OCS is a checklist of 41 conditions during the pregnancy and delivery that could affect the health of the newborn, including birth weight, gestational age, parity, mother age, bleeding during pregnancy, and infections or acute medical conditions during pregnancy. Higher scores on the OCS represent more optimal birth factors. The OCS was completed through mother interview and medical chart abstraction. OCS scores were calculated as the percentage of optimal scores and then changed to the “converted raw score,” following Littman and Parmelee (1974).
Mothers’ history of depressive symptoms
Youth’s mothers’ depression histories were measured with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck & Steer, 1984), a widely used self-report depression scale that was administered to mothers when the youth were infants (BDI scores were averaged for age 3 month and age 6 month sessions; BDI scores were missing for 41 families).
Mothers’ history of substance use in pregnancy
As noted above, maternal drug use in pregnancy was based on responses to an interview (based on the ASI) and urine screen taken at the time of delivery or, for those mothers coming for prenatal care, at prenatal visits.
Substance use
Youth’s lifetime substance use was assessed at both Time 1 and at Time 2 (6–12 months later) with a combination of the following: self-report on the Youth Risk Behavior Survey (Brener et al., 2002); interview using the Teen ASI (Kaminer, Bukstein, & Tarter, 1991); urine toxicology screens with the TESTCUP5 Drug Screen for opiates, cocaine, THC, PCP and barbiturates, as well as a urine test for Ethyl Glucuronide for alcohol use and a urinary cotinine test for nicotine use; and breath screens with the Alcosensor III Intoximeter for alcohol use and a CO monitor for smoked tobacco use. Youth were considered substance users if they endorsed lifetime use of any substance (including tobacco, alcohol, marijuana, cocaine, inhalants, opiates, and others) on the questionnaire or interview or if they had a positive urine or breath screen (alcohol breath screen > 0.00, CO breath screen ≥ 10 ppm) at the questionnaire/interview session (youth were asked to refrain from use on the day of the TSST lab session). Time 2 substance use scores were based on youth report or urine/breath screens at either the 6-month assessment or the 12-month assessment or at both points, depending on which assessments the particular youth attended. If youth were negative for substance use at both 6- and 12-month assessments, they were coded as negative for substance use at Time 2.
Several measures were taken to increase the likelihood of youth reporting honestly about substance use. First, at the start of the study, adolescents were told that their responses to the questions about drug use were confidential and would not be shared with their caregivers (except in cases of concerns about imminent risk of death). Second, a Certificate of Confidentiality was obtained for the study, and youth were told that the confidentiality of their reports of illegal behaviors (including substance use) was protected by the certificate. Third, the Youth Risk Behavior Survey was self-administered by computer (with each question read aloud through headphones) to increase feelings of privacy.
Missing substance use data
For the substance use data, as noted above, 13 youth did not attend either of the Time 2 sessions and thus are not included in analyses of Time 2 substance use.
Caregiver–child relationship quality
Caregiver–child relationship quality was measured by the parent–child relationship (PCR) subscale of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI; Abidin, 1990; Sheras & Abidin, 1999), a parent-report scale. The PSI is a widely used caregiver-report measure of parenting stress and parent–child relationship quality. The PCR subscale measures caregivers’ dissatisfaction with their interactions with their children and with their children generally. Higher scores on this measure indicate a more problematic caregiver–child relationship. The PCR subscale has shown good reliability and validity, correlating with self-reported and observed parenting behavior (Haskett, Ahern, Ward, & Allaire, 2006). Primary caregivers completed the PSI at a session prior to the laboratory stress session.
In this study, the PSI for parents of children aged 0–18 (Abidin, 1990) was used at first and then the study switched to using the PSI version designed specifically for parents of adolescents aged 12–17 (Sheras & Abidin, 1999) partway through the study (52 adolescents had child PSI data, 136 had adolescent PSI data, 5 were missing PSI data). The PCR subscale in the child version had 12 items, whereas the adolescent version had 16 items. Because of the different numbers of items, an average score for the subscale was used in analyses, similar to previous research (Chaplin, Freiburger, Mayes, & Sinha, 2010). The average score ranged from 1 to 5.
Data analysis plan
Covariates
We considered the following as potential covariates for analyses, because of their associations in the literature with prenatal cocaine exposure status and/or with emotional and physiological stress responses: age, race, mother education level (a variable related to socioeconomic status), childhood trauma history, number of obstetric complications at birth, caregiver status (biological mother or other caregiver), maternal history of depressive symptoms, maternal alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use during pregnancy (yes/no), and child’s medication use and menstrual cycle phase (early folicular, late folicular, ovulation, and early, mid, and late luteal) on the day of the TSST session. Caregiver–child relationship quality was included as a moderator variable in analyses, as described below. We planned to add as a control variable any of the above-listed variables that showed a statistically significant association with both the independent variable (PCE status, TSST response, or gender) and the dependent variable (TSST response or youth substance use) in the particular analysis, in order to control for any variable that could be a potential confound. We tested these associations with separate correlations or logistic regressions. We found that PCE status and youth substance use were both associated with race (with PCE youth and substance-using youth more likely to be African American), and so race was included in analyses examining PCE × Gender effects on substance use. No other covariates were found or used in analyses.
Stress response scores
Stress response variables were created for each TSST-C response index (HR, BP, cortisol, sAA, and emotions). Stress response variables were calculated as the score at the time point (or the average of time points) after the TSST when the response index was still elevated for the sample of youth (i.e., before recovery) minus the pre-TSST (baseline) score. This change from baseline approach is commonly used when interested in predicting outcomes from stress response variables in youth (e.g., Rudolph, Troop-Gordon, & Granger, 2010). We decided to examine data only to the peak time point and not through recovery because we were interested in arousal (reactivity) response to the stressor versus recovery. Each reactivity index had a different peak time point, consistent with the typical time courses of cardiovascular, HPA, SNS, and emotional responses (see Figure 2). Heart rate peak was the average of the two time points at the beginning of the TSST-C speech and math tasks. SBP and DBP peaks were collected immediately (0 min) after the TSST-C. Cortisol peak was the average of the 15- and 30-min post-TSST scores. The sAA peak was immediately (0 min) post TSST. Emotion peak was an average of the time point immediately after the TSST (0 min) and 15 min post TSST.
Figure 2.
(Color online) Average responses on all indices over time to stress, with peak response periods marked.
Analyses
Logistic regressions were conducted to test the first hypothesis predicting youth substance use (yes/no) from PCE Status × Gender, covarying for race (dummy coded with 0 = other, 1 = African American). Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to test the second hypothesis predicting youth stress responses from PCE Status × Gender. Secondary analyses included PCE Status × Parent-Reported Caregiver–Child Relationship Quality interactions to test whether parent–child relationship moderated effects of PCE status on youth substance use and stress response outcomes. For the regression analyses, a PCE Status × Parent-Reported Caregiver–Child Relationship Quality interaction term was entered into the regression. For the ANOVA analyses, parent-reported caregiver–child relationship quality was median split into high and low quality and PCE Status × Relationship Quality Median Split Variable was examined. Logistic regressions were conducted to test the third hypotheses predicting youth substance use at Time 2 (yes/no) from stress responses (and from stress responses by gender), controlling for use at Time 1. Follow-up analyses for statistically significant or trend-level ( p < .10) interactions with gender were conducted with separate logistic regressions or ANOVAs for boys and girls. For significant ANOVA findings, Cohen d effect sizes were calculated. For significant logistic regression findings, the odds ratio (Exp[B]) is reported as a metric of effect size. Analyses were also conducted for baseline (pre-TSST) levels of the stress response variables.
Results
Demographics and birth factors by PCE status
Demographics, birth status, childhood trauma history, parent–child relationship quality, and youth substance use information is shown in Table 1. As shown in Table 1, there were PCE group differences in mother education, child race, caregiver status, and parent–child relationship quality, with fewer mothers completing high school in the PCE group than in the NCE group (no mothers reported education beyond high school), a higher percentage of African American youth in the PCE group than in the NCE group, caregivers more likely to be biological mothers in the NCE than in the PCE group, and more negative parent–child relationship quality in the PCE group than in the NCE group. There were no PCE group differences in child gender or age. Birth status of the youth, as measured by scores on the OCS, is also listed in Table 1. Youth in the PCE group had lower (less optimal) OCS scores than did those in the NCE group.
Mothers’ use of cocaine and other drugs in pregnancy and mothers’ history of depressive symptoms are listed in Table 2. In addition to cocaine use, PCE mothers had greater rates of alcohol use, tobacco use, and marijuana use than did NCE mothers during pregnancy. PCE mothers were not different from NCE mothers in their depressive symptoms on the BDI when the youth were infants.
Manipulation check
Youth on average reported that they found the TSST to be moderately stressful. The mean ratings for the speech and math tasks were 5.63 (SD = 3.11) and 6.26 (SD = 3.11), respectively, on a 0 to 10 scale (10 = very stressful). The ratings of the tasks were not associated with PCE status, gender, PCE × Gender, or substance use. We also conducted separate repeated measures ANOVAs, and all of the stress response variables (HR, SBP, DBP, cortisol, sAA, and emotion) increased on average following the task, indicating that the task did produce a stress response. Figure 2 shows mean scores on all stress response variables across response and recovery time points. Note that the stress response scores used in analyses below use only the initial time points after the task (Time 0 or +15), before recovery.
Data inspection and transformations
Data were examined for normality. For cortisol and sAA, there were one and five outlier data points (>3 SD above the mean), respectively. These data points were reassigned a value equal to the next highest value that was within 3 SD from the mean. Similar procedures have been used in previous studies of cortisol (e.g., Kertes & Gunnar, 2004; Susman et al., 2007). Following this, cortisol and sAA response scores were created. Cortisol and sAA response scores were skewed and so square root transformations were performed on these scores, with a constant added to bring the values above 1 (square root [cortisol or sAA score + constant]). Transformed scores (for cortisol and sAA) were used in analyses, but untransformed scores are presented in figures for ease of interpretation.
Covariates
As noted above, we planned to add as a control variable any of the theoretically relevant variables (listed above) that showed a statistically significant association with both the independent variable (PCE status, TSST response, or gender) and the dependent variable (TSST response or youth substance use) in the particular analysis. We found that PCE status and youth substance use were both associated with race (with PCE youth and substance-using youth more likely to be African American), and so race was included in analyses examining PCE × Gender Effects on substance use. No other covariates were found or used in analyses.
Missing data analysis and multiple imputation
A small portion of data was missing on study variables due to nonresponse by participants or to equipment malfunction (missing data ranged from 2 participants for emotional stress responses to 13 participants for substance use at Time 2). A Little test was conducted to examine the nature of the missing data (58.81, p < .01), and results indicated that the data were not missing completely at random. As such, multiple imputation was deemed an appropriate strategy for handling missing data because it results in less bias than alternative procedures, such as listwise or pairwise deletion (Enders, 2010; Little & Rubin, 2002). Thus, we used multiple imputation for all study analyses presented below. Following Rubin’s guidelines (1987), three imputed data sets were found to yield appropriate efficiency for the amount of missingness in the data set and were generated using SPSS version 20 Missing Values package. All of the following analyses were conducted separately for each data set and were pooled across estimates. Pooled values are shown in the text. Results from imputed data analyses are presented in the text, in the correlation table, and in the figures. Raw (unimputed) data are presented in the tables presenting descriptive data (Tables 1 and 2) to give the most accurate picture of the descriptive data.
Baseline analyses
Analyses were first conducted for pre-TSST (baseline) scores. There were no significant PCE Status × Gender interactions. There were significant main effects of PCE status, with PCE < NCE, for pre-TSST HR, F (1, 189) = 13.45, p < .0001, and DBP, F (1, 184) = 4.01, p < .05. There was a significant main effect of gender for pre-TSST SBP, with boys showing higher SBP than girls, F (1, 184) = 8.53, p < .01. Associations between baseline variables and substance use were not significant.
Correlations
Correlations among PCE status, gender, stress response variables, and youth substance use are presented in Table 3. As shown in the table, several of the response variables were correlated, with HR responses positively correlated with DBP and cortisol responses, DBP responses correlated with SBP responses, SBP correlated with cortisol responses, and anxiety, anger, and sadness correlated with one another. However, not all response variables were correlated with one another, a common occurrence in biobehavioral research (Baum et al., 1992). Table 3 also shows that prenatal cocaine exposure was correlated with lower cortisol responses and with higher chances of substance use at Time 1 and Time 2. Finally, Table 3 shows that DBP and SBP responses were associated with lower substance use at Time 1, and that Time 1 and Time 2 substance use were correlated with one another.
Table 3.
Correlations among main variables
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Cocaine exposure | — | ||||||||||
2. Gender | −.08 | — | |||||||||
3. HR response | .09 | .17* | — | ||||||||
4. DBP response | .08 | −.02 | .32** | — | |||||||
5. SBP response | .01 | −.01 | .13 | .38** | — | ||||||
6. Cortisol response | −.17* | −.11 | .14* | .02 | .24** | — | |||||
7. sAA response | −.08 | .02 | .01 | .10 | .07 | .10 | — | ||||
8. Anxiety response | .07 | .06 | .12 | −.05 | .04 | .00 | .08 | — | |||
9. Anger response | −.03 | .12 | .11 | −.03 | .09 | .07 | .10 | .43** | — | ||
10. Sadness response | −.02 | .15* | .10 | .03 | .11 | .12 | .14 | .50** | .65** | — | |
11. Substance use, T1 | .17* | −.04 | .00 | −.20** | −.19** | −.02 | −.06 | .01 | −.05 | −.10 | — |
12. Substance use, T2 | .17* | −.13 | .04 | −.12 | −.13 | −.02 | −.10 | .04 | −.09 | −.04 | .71** |
Note: For cocaine exposure status, non–cocaine exposed = 0, cocaine exposed = 1. For gender, male = 0, female = 1. HR, Heart rate; DBP, diastolic blood pressure; SBP, systolic blood pressure; sAA, salivary alpha amylase; T1, Time 1; T2, Time 2.
p < .05.
p < .01.
*** p < .001.
Hypothesis 1: PCE × Gender predicting youth substance use
The logistic regression analyses predicting Time 1 and Time 2 substance use from PCE × Gender, controlling for race, did not show significant PCE × Gender interactions or main effects of PCE status or gender.
Hypothesis 2: PCE × Gender predicting stress responses
HR and BP
The ANOVA did not show a significant PCE × Gender interaction or PCE main effect for HR response. There was a significant gender main effect on HR response, F (1, 189) = 6.31, p < .05, d = 0.35, with girls showing higher HR response to the stressor than boys (for girls, mean HR change scores = 19.64, SD = 10.98; for boys, M = 15.98, SD = 10.06).
The PCE × Gender interaction and PCE and gender main effects for DBP and SBP were not significant.
Cortisol
There was not a significant PCE × Gender interaction for cortisol response. There was a significant main effect of PCE status, F (1, 184) = 6.32, p < .05, d = −0.40, with PCE youth showing lower cortisol response to the stressor than did NCE youth, for PCE, mean cortisol change score (in untransformed scores) = 0.08, SD = 0.05; for NCE, M = 0.10, SD = 0.06 (see Figure 3). The gender main effect for cortisol response was not significant.
Figure 3.
Average cortisol stress responses by prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) status.
sAA
The PCE × Gender interaction and the PCE and gender main effects for sAA were not significant.
Emotion
The ANOVA showed a significant PCE × Gender interaction for anxiety response, F (1, 189) = 10.68, p < .01 (see Figure 4). Follow-ups indicated that, in the PCE group, girls were significantly higher than boys, F (1, 110) = 7.75, p < .01, d = 0.50, in anxiety response. PCE girls also showed higher anxiety response than did NCE girls, F (1, 93) = 9.02, p < .01, d = 0.56, although PCE boys were not different from NCE boys. The main effects of PCE and gender for anxiety response were not significant.
Figure 4.
Average anxiety stress responses by prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) status and gender.
For anger response, a PCE × Gender interaction was found, F (1, 189) = 3.81, p < .05 (see Figure 5). In the PCE group, girls were higher than boys in anger response to stress, F (1, 110) = 6.54, p < .05, d = 0.47; in the NCE group, girls and boys were not different. Differences between PCE and NCE boys and between PCE and NCE girls were not significant. The main effects of PCE and gender for anger response were not significant.
Figure 5.
Average anger stress responses by prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) status and gender.
For sadness, a similar PCE × Gender interaction was found, F (1, 189) = 3.77, p = .05 (see Figure 6). Followups indicated that, in the PCE group, girls were higher than boys in sadness F (1, 110) = 7.85, p < .01, d = 0.51; in the NCE group, girls and boys were not different from each other. The main effects of PCE and gender for sadness response were not significant.
Figure 6.
Average sadness stress responses by prenatally cocaine exposed (PCE) status and gender.
Summary
In sum, for anxiety, anger, and sadness, the results suggest that PCE boys showed a dampened response to stress relative to PCE girls. For cortisol, PCE youth showed blunted cortisol responses to stress as compared to NCE youth regardless of gender.
Caregiving quality as a moderator of PCE effects
Interactions between PCE status and caregiver–child relationship quality were not statistically significant in predicting any of the stress response measures or in predicting substance use, and so our moderation by caregiver–child relationship quality hypothesis was not supported.
Hypothesis 3: Stress responses predicting substance use at Time 2
Time 1 results
Analyses described below examined relations between stress responses and adolescents’ future substance use at Time 2. We also examined associations between stress responses (by gender) and concurrent substance use at Time 1, but these were not significant. Although correlations between BP responses and Time 1 substance use were significant (see Table 3), these main effects were not significant in the overall logistic regressions predicting Time 1 use.
HR and BP
The Response × Gender interactions and response and gender main effects predicting substance use at Time 2 for HR and BP were not significant.
Cortisol
The Cortisol Response × Gender interaction and the cortisol response and gender main effects on substance use were not significant.
sAA
There was a significant sAA Response × Gender interaction predicting substance use, b = 0.55, Wald = 3.79, p < .05, exp(B) = 1.72 (95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.00–2.96). Follow-up analyses indicated that, for boys, lower sAA responses predicted greater chances of using substances, b = –0.53, Wald = 4.52, p < .05, exp(B) = 0.59 (95% CI = 0.38–0.99) (see Figure 7). This indicates that for every 1 point decrease in sAA response scores (which were square root transformed scores), boys were 1.69 times more likely to use substances at Time 2, controlling for Time 1 use. Follow-up analyses for girls were not statistically significant. There was also a significant main effect of sAA response predicting substance use, b = –1.07, Wald = 4.38, p < .05, exp(B) = 0.35 (95% CI = 0.13–0.94). This indicates that for every 1 point decrease in sAA response scores (which were square root transformed scores), youth were 2.86 times more likely to use substances at Time 2, controlling for Time 1 use. The gender main effect predicting substance use was not significant.
Figure 7.
Odds ratio for salivary alpha amylase (sAA) stress responses to predict substance use at Time 2 (controlling for substance use at Time 1) for boys and girls.
Emotion
The logistic regression showed a significant Sadness Response × Gender interaction predicting substance use at Time 2, b = 0.33, Wald = 4.36, p < .05, exp(B) = 1.32 (95% CI = 1.00–1.76). Follow-ups indicated that, for girls, higher sadness response predicted greater chances of using substances at Time 2, b = 0.22, Wald = 4.00, p < .05, exp(B) = 1.40 (95% CI = 1.02–1.91) (see Figure 8). This indicated that for every 1 point increase in sadness response, girls were 1.40 times more likely to use substances at Time 2, controlling for Time 1 substance use. The follow-up analysis for boys was not significant. Sadness response and gender main effects predicting substance use were not significant.
Figure 8.
Odds ratio for sadness stress responses to predict substance use at Time 2 (controlling for substance use at Time 1) for boys and girls.
Anxiety Response × Gender interactions, Anger Response × Gender interactions, and main effects of anxiety response, anger response, and gender were not significant.
Summary
In sum, there were gender differences in associations between physiological and emotional stress responses and future substance use. For girls, higher sadness responses to stress predicted future substance use, whereas for boys lower sAA responses predicted substance use.
Discussion
The present study was the first laboratory study to examine biobehavioral responses to stress and the prediction of future substance use longitudinally in PCE adolescents. We found that associations between stress responses and both prenatal/family risk status (prenatal cocaine exposure) and future substance use often depended on gender. PCE history was associated with higher stress responses for girls than for boys, and higher stress responses were associated with future substance use for girls. In contrast, PCE boys showed lower biobehavioral stress responses than PCE girls and lower stress responses predicted future substance use in boys. Overall, findings suggest different emotional and physiological risk profiles for adolescent girls versus boys.
Gender differences in PCE effects on stress responses
PCE girls showed heightened anxiety, anger, and sadness responses to the stressor as compared to PCE boys, and the pattern of means suggested that PCE girls also showed heightened emotional responses compared to NCE girls (see Figures 4–6), although this was only statistically significant in the case of anxiety. In contrast, PCE boys showed lower anxiety, anger, and sadness responses to stress as compared to PCE girls, and there was a nonsignificant pattern suggesting that PCE boys may also show blunted responses as compared to NCE boys (see Figures 4–6). Prenatal cocaine (and other drug) exposure, combined with potential postnatal exposure to compromised home and neighborhood environments, has been shown to lead to alterations in developing emotional stress response systems in children (for reviews, see Dow-Edwards, 2011; Eiden, Stevens, Scheutze, & Dombkowski, 2006; Kandel, Griesler, & Schaffran, 2009; Mayes, 1999). Our findings suggest that the alterations continue into adolescence and that the type of alteration differs by gender, with at-risk girls showing heightened emotional arousal responses to stress and at-risk boys showing dampened stress responses. It is of note that we did not find PCE × Gender interactions or PCE effects for HR, BP, or sAA, suggesting that not all biobehavioral indicators show the same pattern. For the emotional responses, though, our findings suggest heightened stress responses among PCE girls and blunted responses among PCE boys.
The gendered patterns of emotional stress responses for PCE youth suggest that the risk associated with PCE status manifests itself as higher arousal responses to stress for PCE girls than for PCE boys. These heightened arousal responses in PCE girls relative to PCE boys involved not only gender-role consistent responses of anxiety and sadness but also gender-role inconsistent responses of increased anger. This is similar to other research finding that girls from low-income urban environments are socialized to express anger to appear “tough” and protect themselves in dangerous neighborhoods (e.g., Chaplin, Casey, Sinha, & Mayes, 2010; Miller & Sperry, 1987). Several researchers suggest that gender differences in stress responses may contribute to the known gender differences in vulnerability to psychiatric disorders (Chaplin & Cole, 2005; Keenan & Hipwell, 2005; Klein & Corwin, 2002). Adolescent girls are more at risk for internalizing disorders, including depression and anxiety, than are boys, and heightened emotional responses to stress have been found to be risk factors for the development of internalizing problems (Chaplin, 2006; Klimes-Dougan et al., 2001). Conversely, boys are more at risk for externalizing psychopathologies such as oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, disorders that have been linked to blunted emotional and physiological arousal in response to stress (Gordis et al., 2006; Raine, Venables, & Mednick, 1997; Susman, 2006; Susman et al., 2010). Thus, gender differences in effects of PCE may reflect risk for different psychological vulnerabilities in at-risk (cocaine exposed) boys versus girls.
Stress responses predict future substance use
In addition to different psychological outcomes for boys versus girls, there may be different pathways to the same outcome by gender. The present study examined gender differences in longitudinal pathways from stress responses to future substance use in adolescence. We found that heightened sadness in response to stress predicted substance use for girls whereas blunted sAA responses predicted substance use for boys. Thus, in this high-risk low-income sample, there were different pathways to substance use for boys versus girls, at least for these two markers of emotional and SNS responses. Other literature has identified different pathways to alcohol and drug use in youth, with some youth taking an internalizing pathway characterized by depression and feelings of negative affect, and some taking an externalizing pathway characterized by behavior problems, disinhibition, and sensation seeking, a pathway that may be linked to blunted physiological arousal (Zucker, Ellis, & Fitzgerald, 1994). Our pattern of findings with low-income PCE and NCE youth suggests that girls may be more likely to take the internalizing pathway and boys more likely to take the externalizing pathway to substance use and potentially to later substance abuse or dependence. This is consistent with a previous finding that drinking behavior was associated with high levels of depression for girls and with high externalizing problems and low depression for boys (Chassin et al., 2001) and also consistent with theories of gender differences in biobehavioral responses to stress and their sequelae (e.g., Chaplin & Aldao, 2012; Taylor et al., 2000). Our findings suggest that stress response profiles are important to consider in understanding the development of substance use behavior in adolescence and for understanding the particular needs of boys versus girls in the prevention of adolescent substance use and risk for abuse. Stress responses could be measured and used to identify boys and girls in need of substance use prevention programs and could help to tailor these programs to be gender sensitive.
Main effects of PCE
Prenatal cocaine exposure also had main effects on stress responses that did not depend on gender. PCE youth were lower in baseline HR and DBP, and showed a lower cortisol change score in response to the stressor as compared to NCE youth. The cortisol finding suggests a blunted HPA axis response to stress in PCE youth. In our past findings with this cohort, we found PCE youth to have higher cortisol at baseline and 1 hr poststress than did NCE youth (Chaplin et al., 2010). However, when looking at change from baseline to poststress, the PCE youth were relatively flat (they started high and stayed high). Thus, taken together with the current study’s findings, this suggests that PCE youth have elevated basal cortisol levels, but a lack of cortisol increase following stress. This pattern of dampened cortisol stress responses in PCE youth is consistent with other research with early adolescent PCE youth (Fisher et al., 2012; Lester et al., 2010) and with theories that prenatal stressors and early life stressors (such as the poverty and compromised home and neighborhood environments often experienced by youth with a drug-abusing mother) lead to an attenuated HPA axis response in youth (Gunnar & Vasquez, 2001). A similar pattern of elevated basal cortisol levels and flattened cortisol response to stress has been shown in alcohol-dependent adults (e.g., Sinha et al., 2009) and in boys with substance-abusing fathers (Moss, Vanyukov, & Martin, 1995), and so this pattern of blunted cortisol response to stress may also have implications for PCE youth’s risk for addiction.
In our study, PCE status was not significantly associated with greater rates of substance use, after controlling for race, although as shown in Table 1, the simple association between PCE status and Time 1 substance use was significant. This lack of PCE effect on adolescent substance use diverges from previous findings (e.g., Delaney-Black et al., 2011; Frank et al., 2011, Rocha et al., 2002). It may be that PCE effects on substance use are not strong, but that PCE affects youth’s progression from initial use in adolescence to the development of substance use disorders in early adulthood. It will be important to follow cocaine and other drug exposed youth into young adulthood to examine this important hypothesis.
Conclusions
Overall, we found that biobehavioral responses to stress across several physiological and emotional indicators were associated with prenatal cocaine exposure and predicted future substance use in low-income urban adolescents. Prenatal/family risk and substance use outcomes were linked to different patterns of emotional and SNS stress responses for boys versus girls. For girls, a heightened emotional stress response was linked to prenatal cocaine exposure risk status and, for sadness, to future substance use. For boys, a blunted emotional and sAA stress response was associated with PCE risk status and future substance use. In addition, regardless of gender, PCE youth showed a blunted cortisol response. It is notable that cocaine exposure was associated with certain forms of dysregulation (altered emotional and HPA axis responses to stress) whereas other forms of dysregulation (altered sadness and sympathetic responses) were associated with substance use risk. There were low-level correlations among cortisol, alpha amylase, and emotional responses, and thus, perhaps altered cortisol responses may contribute to alterations in sympathetic arousal and sadness, which then lead to risk for substance use.
It is important to note that PCE “effects” on youth stress responses reflect not only the biological effects of cocaine exposure in utero but also effects of other drugs used in pregnancy by cocaine-using mothers, genetic factors, and/or compromised postnatal caregiving environments experienced by children of cocaine-using mothers. Our parent-report measure of parent–child relationship quality did not moderate PCE effects, but future studies should use more in-depth characterization of parenting, including observational measures and also measures of parental monitoring and relations with peers, to more fully determine the role of parenting and other relationships. It is also notable that the participants in the present study were only a subset of our overall cohort of PCE children. Although this subsample was not different from the overall sample on demographic or birth variables, it is possible that they may be less severe than the overall sample. However, we still find significant PCE effects on stress responses even in youth that potentially had less severe exposure. Finally, while we examined substance use as an outcome, it may be that substance use is simply a marker for general distress or levels of psychopathology. Future research should determine specific pathways to substance use versus other forms of psychopathology for girls and boys.
Despite these limitations, this study is one of the first laboratory studies to examine prospective longitudinal associations between biobehavioral responses to a laboratory stressor and substance use in a group of low-income urban adolescents with prenatal and family-based risk for substance abuse. It is important to examine adolescent substance use in this at-risk sample because substance use in adolescence is predictive of later substance use disorders and other harmful risk behaviors such as risky sex. In the PCE youth in our study, who likely encounter many chronic life stressors, heightened emotional responses to stress were linked to prenatal cocaine exposure status and to future substance use for girls, whereas blunted emotional and sAA responses were linked for boys. These findings suggest different stress-related pathways to substance use for at-risk girls versus boys. Our findings indicate that it would be useful to target girls with high stress responses and boys with low stress responses for interventions to help prevent substance use disorders. Such interventions could be gender sensitive and address different forms of responses to stress in girls versus boys.
References
- Abidin RR. Parenting Stress Index (PSI) manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Adinoff B, Junghanns K, Kiefer F, Krishnan-Sarin S. Suppression of the HPA axis stress-response: Implications for relapse. Alcoholism Clinical and Experimental Research. 2005;29:1351–1355. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000176356.97620.84. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000176356.97620.84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amaro H, Blake SM, Schwartz PM, Flinchbaugh LJ. Developing theory-based substance abuse prevention programs for young adolescent girls. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2001;21:256–293. doi:10.1177/0272431601021003002. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson SL, Teicher MH. Desperately driven and no brakes: Developmental stress exposure and subsequent risk for substance abuse. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33:516–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.009. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Back SE, Brady KT, Jackson JL, Salstrom S, Zinzow H. Gender differences in stress reactivity among cocaine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology. 2005;180:169–176. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2129-7. doi:10.1007/s00213-004-2129-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bada HS, Das A, Bauer CR, Shankaran S, Lester B, LaGasse L, et al. Impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior problems through school age. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e348–e359. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1404. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-1404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Principles of behavior modification. Holt, Rinehart & Winston; New York: 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Baum A, Grunberg NE, Singer JE. Biochemical measurement in the study of emotion. Psychological Science. 1992;3:56–60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00257.x. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA. Internal consistencies of the original and revised Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1984;40:1365–1367. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198411)40:6<1365::aid-jclp2270400615>3.0.co;2-d. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198411)40:6,1365::AID-JCLP2270400615.3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behnke M, Eyler FD, Warner TD, Garvan CW, Hou W, Wobie K. Outcome from a prospective, longitudinal study of prenatal cocaine use: Preschool development at 3 years of age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31:41–49. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj027. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsj027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bendersky M, Bennett D, Lewis M. Aggression at age 5 as a function of prenatal exposure to cocaine, gender, and environmental risk. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31:71–84. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj025. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsj025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett D, Bendersky M, Lewis M. Preadolescent health risk behavior as a function of prenatal cocaine exposure and gender. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007;28:467–472. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31811320d8. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e31811320d8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg S, Izard CE. Affective and cognitive characteristics of depression in 10- and 11-year-old children. Journal Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;49:194–202. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.49.1.194. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.49.1.194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brener ND, Kann L, McManus T, Kinchen SA, Sundberg EC, Ross JG. Reliability of the 1999 Youth Risk Behavior Survey Questionnaire. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;31:336–342. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00339-7. doi:10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00339-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brody LR. Gender, emotion, and the family. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Brook JS, Balka EB, Ning Y, Brook DW. Trajectories of cigarette smoking among African Americans and Puerto Ricans from adolescence to young adulthood: Associations with dependence on alcohol and illegal drugs. American Journal on Addictions. 2007;16:195–201. doi: 10.1080/10550490701375244. doi:10.1080/10550490701375244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brook JS, Brook DW, Gordon AS, Whiteman M, Cohen P. The psychosocial etiology of adolescent drug use: A family interactional approach. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 1990;116:111–267. PMID: 2376323. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton C, Hawley T. Effects of in utero drug exposure on children’s development: Review and recommendations. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 1994;148:33–39. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1994.02170010035007. doi:10.1001/archpedi.1994.02170010035007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buske-Kirschbaum A, Jobst S, Wustmans A, Kirschbaum C, Kirsh F, Rauh W, et al. Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1997;59:419–426. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199707000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:111–126. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.010. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers AR, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM. Anger, happiness, and sadness: Associations with depressive symptoms in late adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2006;35:977–986. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9033-x. [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Aldao A. Gender differences in emotion expression in children: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:735–765. doi: 10.1037/a0030737. doi:10.1037/a0030737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Casey J, Sinha R, Mayes LC. Gender differences in caregiver emotion socialization of low income toddlers. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. 2010;128:11–27. doi: 10.1002/cd.266. doi:10.1002/cd.266, PMID: 20552657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Cole PM. The role of emotion regulation in the development of psychopathology. In: Hankin BL, Abela JRZ, editors. Development of psychopathology: A vulnerability-stress perspective. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2005. pp. 49–74. [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Fahy T, Sinha R, Mayes LC. Emotional arousal and regulation in cocaine exposed toddlers: Implications for behavior problems across a three-year follow-up. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2009;31:275–282. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2009.05.002. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Freiburger MB, Mayes LC, Sinha R. Prenatal cocaine exposure, gender, and adolescent stress response: A prospective longitudinal study. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2010;32:595–604. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.08.007. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-1840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaplin TM, Sinha R, Simmons J, Healy S, Mayes LC, Hommer RE, et al. Parent–adolescent conflict interactions and adolescent alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37:605–612. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.01.004. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Pitts SC, Prost J. Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a high-risk sample: Predictors and substance abuse outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;70:67–78. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chatterton RT, Vogelsong KM, Lu Y, Ellman AB, Hudgens GA. Salivary a-amylase as a measure of endogenous adrenergic activity. Clinical Physiology. 1996;16:433–448. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-097x.1996.tb00731.x. doi:10.1196/annals.1384.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crews FT, Braun CJ, Hoplight B, Switzer RC, Knapp DJ. Binge ethanol consumption causes differential brain damage in young adolescent rats compared with adult rats. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:1712–1723. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb01973.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delaney-Black V, Chiodo LM, Hannigan JH, Greenwald MK, Janisse J, Patterson G, et al. Prenatal and postnatal cocaine exposure predicts teen cocaine use. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33:110–119. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.011. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis T, Bendersky M, Ramsay D, Lewis M. Reactivity and regulation in children prenatally exposed to cocaine. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:688–697. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.688. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorn LD, Campo JC, Thato S, Dahl RE, Lewin D, Chandra R, et al. Psychological comorbidity and stress reactivity in children and adolescents with recurrent abdominal pain and anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42:66–75. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200301000-00012. doi:10.1097/00004583-200301000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dow-Edwards D. Translational issues for prenatal cocaine studies and the role of environment. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.007. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubow EF, Tisak J, Causey D, Hryshko A, Reid G. A two-year longitudinal study of stressful life events, social support, and problem-solving skills: Contributions to children’s adjustment. Child Development. 1991;62:583–599. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01554.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01554.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan S, Strycker L, Duncan T. Alcohol use of African Americans and Whites from ages 9–20: Descriptive results from a longitudinal study. Journal of Ethnicity in Substance Abuse. 2012;11:214–225. doi: 10.1080/15332640.2012.701550. doi:10.1080/15332640.2012.701550. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiden RD, Stevens A, Schuetze P, Dombkowski LE. A conceptual model for maternal behavior among polydrug cocaine-using mothers: The role of postnatal cocaine use and maternal depression. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2006;20:1–10. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.20.1.1. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.20.1.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eiden RD, Veira Y, Granger DA. Prenatal cocaine exposure and infant cortisol reactivity. Child Development. 2009;80:528–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01277.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01277.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elkind D, Bowen R. Imaginary audience behavior in children and adolescents. Developmental Psychology. 1979;15:38–44. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.15.1.38. [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. Guildford Press; New York: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA, Kim HK, Bruce J, Pears KC. Cumulative effects of prenatal substance exposure and early adversity on foster children’s HPA-axis. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2012;36:29–35. doi: 10.1177/0165025411406863. doi:10.1177/0165025411406863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox HC, Hong KA, Siedlarz K, Sinha R. Enhanced sensitivity to stress and drug/alcohol craving in abstinent cocaine-dependent individuals compared to social drinkers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:796–805. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301470. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank DA, Augustyn M, Knight WG, Pell T, Zuckerman B. Growth, development, and behavior in early childhood following prenatal cocaine exposure: A systematic review. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2001;285:1613–1625. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.12.1613. doi:10.1001/jama.285.12.1613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frank DA, Rose-Jacobs R, Crooks D, Cabral HJ, Gerteis J, Hacker KA, et al. Adolescent initiation of licit and illicit substance use: Impact of intrauterine exposures and post-natal exposure to violence. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2011;33:100–109. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.002. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2010.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordis EB, Granger DA, Susman EJ, Trickett PK. Asymmetry between salivary cortisol and a-amylase reactivity to stress: Relation to aggressive behavior in adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31:976–987. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.010. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, Blair C, El-Sheikh M, Mize J, Lisonbee JA, et al. Integrating the measurement of salivary alpha-amylase into studies of child health, development, and social relationships. Journal of Personal and Social Relationships. 2006;23:267–290. doi:10.1177/0265407506062479. [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Vazquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: Potential indices of risk in human development. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:515–538. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003066. doi:10.1017/S0954579401003066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haskett ME, Ahern LS, Ward CS, Allaire JC. Factor structure and validity of the Parenting Stress Index—Short form. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:302–312. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_14. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hastings PD, Zahn-Waxler C, Usher BA. Cardiovascular and affective responses to social stress in adolescents with internalizing and externalizing problems. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2007;31:77–87. doi:10.1177/0165025407073575. [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adulthood: Implications for substance abuse prevention. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:64–105. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hibel LC, Granger DA, Kivlighan KT, Blair C, Family Life Project Investigators Individual differences in salivary cortisol: Associations with common over-the-counter and prescription medication status in infants and their mothers. Hormones and Behavior. 2006;50:293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.03.014. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizink AC, Ferdinand RF, Ormel J, Verhulst FC. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity and early onset of cannabis use. Addiction. 2006;101:1581–1588. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01570.x. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01570.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizink AC, Greaves-Lord K, Oldhinkel AJ, Ormel J, Verhulst FC. Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and smoking and drinking onset among adolescents: The longitudinal cohort Tracking Adolescents’ Lives Survey (TRAILS) Addiction. 2009;104:1927–1936. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02685.x. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02685.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izard CE. Patterns of emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression. Academic Press; New York: 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future: National results on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2002. University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research; Ann Arbor, MI: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Kajantie E, Phillips DIW. The effects of sex and hormonal status on the physiological response to acute psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;3:151–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaminer Y, Bukstein OG, Tarter R. The Teen-Addiction Severity Index: Rationale and reliability. International Journal of Addiction. 1991;26:219–226. doi: 10.3109/10826089109053184. doi:10.3109/10826089109053184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Griesler PC, Schaffran C. Educational attainment and smoking among women: Risk factors and consequences for offspring. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2009;104:S24–S33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.005. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Yamaguchi K, Chen K. Stages of progression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: Further evidence for the gateway theory. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1992;53:447–457. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1992.53.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keenan K, Hipwell AE. Preadolescent clues to understanding depression in girls. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2005;8:89–105. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-4750-3. doi:10.1007/s10567-005-4750-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kertes DA, Gunnar MR. Evening activities as a potential confound in research on the adrenocortical system in children. Child Development. 2004;75:193–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00663.x. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00663.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khantzian EJ. The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: A reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 1985;4:1067–3229. doi: 10.3109/10673229709030550. doi:10.3109/10673229709030550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM, Gaab J, Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH. Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus–pituitary–adrenal axis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1999;61:154–162. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199903000-00006. doi:0033-3174/99/6102-0154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Wust S, Hellhammer DH. Consistent sex differences in cortisol responses to psychological stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1992;54:648–657. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199211000-00004. PMID: 1454958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein LC, Bennett JM, Whetzel CA, Granger DA, Ritter FE. Effects of caffeine and stress on salivary a-amylase in young men. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2010;25:359–367. doi: 10.1002/hup.1126. doi:10.1002/hup.1126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein LC, Corwin EJ. Seeing the unexpected: How sex differences in stress responses may provide a new perspective on the manifestation of psychiatric disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2002;4:441–448. doi: 10.1007/s11920-002-0072-z. doi:10.1007/s11920-002-0072-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimes-Dougan B, Hastings PD, Granger DA, Usher BA, Zahn-Waxler C. Adrenocortical activity in at-risk and normally developing adolescents: Individual differences in salivary cortisol basal levels, diurnal variation, and responses to social challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:695–719. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003157. doi:10.1017/S0954579401003157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudielka BM, Buske-Kirschbaum A, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Differential heart rate reactivity and recovery after psychosocial stress (TSST) in healthy children, younger adults, and elderly adults: The impact of age and gender. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2004;2:116–121. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1102_8. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1102_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: A review. Biological Psychology. 2005;69:113–132. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lester BM, LaGasse LL, Shankaran S, Bada HS, Bauer CR, Lin R, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure related to cortisol stress reactivity in 11-year-old children. Journal of Pediatrics. 2010;157:288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.039. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li CR, Kosten TR, Sinha R. Sex differences in brain activation during stress imagery in abstinent cocaine users: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:487–494. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.048. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little RJA, Rubin D. Statistical analyses with missing data. 2nd Wiley; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Littman B, Parmelee AH. Manual for Obstetric Complications, Infant Studies Project. University of California–Los Angeles, School of Medicine, Department of Pediatrics; Los Angeles: 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Mayes LC. Developing brain and in utero cocaine exposure: Effects on neural ontogeny. Developmental Psychopathology. 1999;11:685–714. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes LC. A behavioral teratogenic model of the impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on arousal regulatory systems. Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 2002;24:385–395. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00200-3. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(02)00200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes LC, Bornstein MH, Chawarska K, Haynes OM, Granger RH. Impaired regulation of arousal in 3-month-old infants exposed prenatally to cocaine and other drugs. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:29–42. [Google Scholar]
- McLellan AT, Luborsky L, O’Brien CP, Woody GE. An improved evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases. 1980;168:26–33. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198001000-00006. doi:10.1097/00005053-198001000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller P, Sperry LL. The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly. 1987;33:1–31. [Google Scholar]
- Moss HB, Vanyukov MM, Martin CS. Salivary cortisol responses and the risk for substance abuse in prepubertal boys. Biological Psychiatry. 1995;38:547–555. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00382-D. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94)00382-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss HB, Vanyukov MM, Yao JK, Kirillova GP. Salivary cortisol responses in prepubertal boys: The effects of parental abuse and association with drug use behavior during adolescence. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;45:1293–1299. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00216-9. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00216-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute on Drug Abuse . Strategic plan (NIH Publication No. 10-6119) Washington, DC: 2010. Author. [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S. Gender differences in risk factors and consequences for alcohol use and problems. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:981–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2004.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:424–443. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poklis A. Evaluation of TDx cocaine metabolite assay. Journal of Analytical Toxicology. 1987;11:228–230. doi: 10.1093/jat/11.5.228. doi:10.1093/jat/11.5.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raine A, Venables PH, Mednick SA. Low resting heart rate at age 3 years predisposes to aggression at age 11 years: Evidence from the Mauritius child health project. Journal of the Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:1457–1465. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199710000-00029. doi:10.1097/00004583-19971000000029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao U, Hammen CL, Poland RE. Mechanisms underlying the comorbidity between depressive and addictive disorders in adolescents: Interactions between stress and HPA activity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:361–369. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030412. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocha BA, Mead AN, Kosofsky BE. Increased vulnerability to self-administer cocaine in mice prenatally exposed to cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 2002;163:221–229. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1140-0. doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1140-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Wiley; New York: 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Rudolph KD, Troop-Gordon W, Granger DA. Peer victimization and aggression: Moderation by individual differences in salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38:843–856. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9412-3. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9412-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheras PL, Abidin RR. Stress Index for Parents of Adolescents (SIPA) manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology. 2001;158:343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. doi:10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, Fox HC, Hong KA, Bergquist K, Bhagwagar Z, Siedlarz KM. Enhanced negative emotion and alcohol craving, and altered physiological responses following stress and cue exposure in alcohol dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1198–1208. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.78. doi:10.1038/npp.2008.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ. Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63:324–331. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spear L. The developing brain and adolescent-typical behavior patterns. In: Romer D, Walker EF, editors. Adolescent psychopathology and the developing brain: Integrating brain and prevention science. Oxford University Press; New York: 2007. pp. 9–31. [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:51–58. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.005. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strahler J, Mueller A, Rosenloecher F, Kirschbaum C, Rohleder N. Salivary alpha-amylase stress reactivity across different age groups. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:587–595. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00957.x. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00957.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susman EJ. Modeling developmental complexity in adolescence: Hormones and behavior in context. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1997;7:283–306. doi:10.1207/s15327795jra0703_3. [Google Scholar]
- Susman EJ. Psychobiology of persistent antisocial behavior: Stress, early vulnerabilities and the attenuation hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:376–389. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.002. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susman EJ, Dockray S, Granger DA, Bladesa KT, Randazzo W, Heaton JA, et al. Cortisol and alpha amylase reactivity and timing of puberty: Vulnerabilities for antisocial behaviour in young adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:557–569. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.004. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen. 2009.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susman EJ, Dockray S, Schiefelbein VL, Herwehe S, Heaton JA, Dorn LD. Morningness/eveningness, morning-to-afternoon cortisol ratio, and antisocial behavior problems during puberty. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:811–822. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.811. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tarter R, Vanyukov M, Giancola P, Dawes M, Blackson T, Mezzich A, et al. Etiology of early age onset substance use disorder: A maturational perspective. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:657–683. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002266. doi:10.1017/S0954579499002266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, Updegraff JA. Female responses to stress: Tend-and-be-friend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review. 2000;107:411–429. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.411. doi:10. 1037/0033-295X.107.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tronick E, Beeghley M. Prenatal cocaine exposure, child development, and the compromising effects of cumulative risk. Clinics of Perinatology. 1999;26:151–171. PMID: 10214547. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Leeuwen AP, Creemers HE, Greaves-Lord K, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, Huizink AC. Hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis reactivity to social stress and adolescent cannabis use: The TRAILS study. Addiction. 2011;106:1484–1492. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03448.x. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03448.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welberg LAM, Seckl JR. Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids and the programming of the brain. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2001;13:113–128. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00601.x. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2001.00601.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White AM, Ghia AJ, Levin ED, Swartzwelder HS. Binge pattern ethanol exposure in adolescent and adult rats: Differential impact on subsequent responsiveness to ethanol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2000;24:1251–1256. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000. tb02091.x. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Sandy JM, Yaeger AM, Cleary SD, Shinar O. Coping dimensions, life stress, and adolescent substance use: A latent growth analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2001;110:309–323. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.2.309. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wills TA, Vaccaro D, McNamara G. Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: An application of Cloninger’s Theory. Journal of Substance Use. 1994;6:1–20. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(94)90039-6. doi:10.1016/S0899-3289(94)90039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windle M, Spear LP, Fuligni AJ, Angold A, Brown JD, Pine D, et al. Transitions into underage drinking and problem drinking: Developmental processes and mechanisms between 10 and 15 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;121:S273–S289. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-2243C. doi:10.1542/peds.2007-2243C. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Withers NW, Pulvirenti L, Koob GF, Gillin JC. Cocaine abuse and dependence. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1995;15:63–78. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199502000-00010. doi:0.1097/00004714-199502000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zucker RA, Ellis DA, Fitzgerald HE. Developmental evidence for at least two alcoholisms: I. Biopsychosocial variation among pathways into symptomatic difficulty. In: Babor TF, Hesselbrock VM, Meyer RE, Shoemaker W, editors. Types of alcoholics: Evidence from clinical, experimental, and genetic research. New York Academy of Sciences; New York: 1994. pp. 134–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]