Skip to main content
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience logoLink to Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
. 2014 Feb 12;10(1):106–113. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu022

Effects of reward sensitivity and regional brain volumes on substance use initiation in adolescence

Snežana Urošević 1,, Paul Collins 1, Ryan Muetzel 2, Ann Schissel 1, Kelvin O Lim 3, Monica Luciana 1
PMCID: PMC4471534  PMID: 24526186

Abstract

This longitudinal study examines associations between baseline individual differences and developmental changes in reward [i.e. behavioral approach system (BAS)] sensitivity and relevant brain structures’ volumes to prospective substance use initiation during adolescence. A community sample of adolescents ages 15–18 with no prior substance use was assessed for substance use initiation (i.e. initiation of regular alcohol use and/or any use of other substances) during a 2-year follow-up period and for alcohol use frequency in the last year of the follow-up. Longitudinal ‘increases’ in BAS sensitivity were associated with substance use initiation and increased alcohol use frequency during the follow-up. Moreover, adolescents with smaller left nucleus accumbens at baseline were more likely to initiate substance use during the follow-up period. This study provides support for the link between developmental increases in reward sensitivity and substance use initiation in adolescence. The study also emphasizes the potential importance of individual differences in volumes of subcortical regions and their structural development for substance use initiation during adolescence.

Keywords: adolescence, behavioral approach system (BAS), reward sensitivity, substance use initiation

INTRODUCTION

Adolescence is characterized by increased substance use (Eaton et al., 2006) and risk for related disorders (Kessler et al., 2005). Approximately 18% of eighth graders and 55% of 12th graders report ever being drunk; use of illicit drugs is also common (Johnston et al., 2009). Adolescent substance use is linked to suicidal ideation (Windle et al., 1992), sexual risk-taking (Hingson et al., 2003) and later substance dependence (King and Chassin, 2007). We have suggested that adolescents experience an increased sensitivity of the behavioral-activation/approach system (BAS: Fowles, 1987; Gray, 1991; Depue and Collins, 1999) that mediates approach to rewards (Luciana et al., 2012; Urošević et al., 2012), including substances of abuse. Longitudinal studies are needed to clarify whether developmental increases, vs stable individual difference factors, in reward sensitivity and its neural substrates, predict adolescents’ substance use initiation. This study addresses this question.

Studies support adolescents’ reward hypersensitivity. Adolescents show stronger effects of monetary incentives on cognitive task performance (Jazbec et al., 2006; Hardin et al., 2007) and increased positive affect (Ernst et al., 2005) relative to adults. They are relatively more sensitive to positive feedback (Cauffman et al., 2010). In functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigms, adolescents exhibit greater ventral striatal/nucleus accumbens (Nacc) activity in response to rewards compared with other ages (Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan et al., 2006; Cohen et al., 2010; Somerville et al., 2011 but see Bjork et al., 2004, 2010). During risk-taking, adolescents’ ventral striatal activity is greater when peers are present, suggesting that social context serves as a potent source of reward (Chein et al., 2011). The BAS system that underlies reward sensitivity is facilitated by dopamine (DA) projections from the ventral tegmental area to Nacc and dorsal striatum, as well as the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (Depue and Collins, 1999; Wahlstrom et al., 2010). This DA-modulated network is implicated in adolescent brain development (Ernst et al., 2006b; Wahlstrom et al., 2010) and it contributes to rewarding aspects of abused substances (Koob and Volkow, 2010). Thus, increased reward sensitivity and associated Nacc activity may promote adolescent substance use (Galvan et al., 2006).

In a recent longitudinal study, self-reported reward/BAS sensitivity peaked from early to late adolescence and declined thereafter, a pattern also observed for Nacc volumes (Urošević et al., 2012). Individuals with greater Nacc and medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) volumes at baseline exhibited greater longitudinal increases in self-reported BAS. This pattern was not found for threat [behavioral inhibition system (BIS)] sensitivity (Urošević et al., 2012).

Cross-sectional studies link BAS hypersensitivity to substance misuse (e.g. Franken and Muris, 2006). In adults, BAS hypersensitivity predicts substance use disorders (Johnson et al., 2003), heightened craving responses to conditioned substance-related cues (Kambouropoulos and Staiger, 2001, 2004) and cravings for alcohol among residential patients (Franken, 2002). In a large adolescent sample, increased BAS sensitivity predicted a composite measure that combined frequency and onset age of substance use (Knyazev et al., 2004). Similarly, BAS hypersensitivity is linked to alcohol abuse in adolescent females (Loxton and Dawe, 2001). The relationship between BAS hypersensitivity and substance use remains significant when controlling for extraversion, neuroticism and psychoticism (Knyazev, 2004; Knyazev et al., 2004).

Links between BAS hypersensitivity and substance use are also evident in individuals transitioning from adolescence into young adulthood. In undergraduates, BAS hypersensitivity predicts alcohol and tobacco use (O’Connor et al., 2009), infrequent, impairing heavy alcohol use (O’Connor and Colder, 2005), illicit drug use (Simons et al., 2008) and lifetime number of substances used (Franken and Muris, 2006). These associations may be moderated by working memory and inhibitory control (Patrick et al., 2008).

Most of the above cross-sectional studies of links between BAS hypersensitivity and substance use rely on the BIS/BAS scales’ (Carver and White, 1994) assessment of BAS/reward sensitivity. The BIS/BAS scores correlate with electroencephalography (EEG) indices of approach and withdrawal (Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1997; Sutton and Davidson, 1997), responses to reward and punishment (Carver and White, 1994) and clinical symptomatology (Meyer et al., 2001; Alloy et al., 2006). Twin data suggest a moderate genetic effect on variance in BIS/BAS scores (Takahashi et al., 2007).

There are no longitudinal studies examining effects of reward sensitivity on substance use and no studies of associations with substance use ‘initiation’. Cross-sectional approaches cannot determine whether it is the normative ‘increase’ in reward sensitivity during adolescence that predicts substance use, or whether individuals with reward hypersensitivity exhibit greater substance use regardless of developmental stage. The few existing longitudinal studies on prospective risk factors of substance use initiation have examined other predictors, such as aggression and impulsivity (Ernst et al., 2006a). Another longitudinal study failed to find a significant relationship between preferences for high risk/high reward task choices and substance use initiation in a mixed sample of adolescents with and without psychopathology (Ernst et al., 2010). Longitudinal studies focusing on reward sensitivity as a predictor of substance use initiation in otherwise healthy adolescents are needed.

Similarly, little research has examined individual differences in brain structures involved in reward processing and their associations with adolescent substance use. Cheetham et al. (2012) found individual differences in OFC, but not amygdala, hippocampus, or anterior cingulate volumes to predict prospective cannabis use initiation. Whether these associations extend to other substances or other brain regions is unknown.

Based on the prior studies described earlier, we hypothesize that both baseline individual differences and ‘longitudinal increases’ in reward/BAS sensitivity will predict prospective substance use initiation and alcohol use frequency in adolescents. Furthermore, we predict that baseline individual differences in volumes of brain structures relevant for reward processing (i.e. Nacc and medial OFC) will predict prospective substance use initiation and alcohol use frequency. Longitudinal changes in these structures’ volumes have been observed in the course of typical adolescent development, coincident with similar changes in BAS sensitivity (Urošević et al., 2012). Thus, we hypothesize that longitudinal increases in Nacc volumes will predict substance use initiation and frequency of use. Finally, we will determine the specificity of reward sensitivity and relevant brain structures volumes’ effects by also examining threat (BIS) sensitivity as well as amygdala and lateral OFC volumes. The BIS responds to threat/punishment and inhibits approach in situations of risk/reward conflict (Gray and McNaughton, 2000).

METHOD

Participants

The sample is part of a larger longitudinal study of normative adolescent brain development (see below). This report focuses on adolescents [15- to 18-year-old at Time 1 (T1)] who completed two data collection waves with a mean follow-up interval of 2.25 ± 0.34 years. This baseline age range was targeted, because substance use initiation is common between ages 15 and 20 (Eaton et al., 2006). At T1, minor participants were recruited through a University-sponsored database of metro community families that agreed, at their child’s birth, to be contacted about research participation or by postcards mailed to the University’s non-academic employees. Adult participants were recruited through flyers and mass mailings.

At T1, eligibility was determined with a phone screening and in-person clinical interview (Kaufman et al., 1996). Exclusions included histories of neurological or Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition Text Revision, (DSM-IV-TR) Axis I disorders (including substance use disorders), birth complications, loss of consciousness, learning disabilities, psychoactive medication use, non-native English speaking, uncorrected vision/hearing, non-right-handedness (Oldfield, 1971) and MRI contraindications. More stringent exclusion criteria for the present analyses included endorsement of any substance use screens in the clinical interview at T1. That is, participants were substance naïve at T1.

This procedure yielded 34 participants (15 female, 19 male) aged 15.33–18.94 years at T1 (M = 16.88, s.d. = 1.19) and from 17.22 to 21.95 years at Time 2 (T2) (M = 19.13, s.d. = 1.30). Participants self-identified predominantly as Caucasian (97.1%) and Asian/Pacific Islander (2.9%), overall consistent with Minnesota’s demographics (2010 US Census, published by the U.S. Census Bureau: http://factfinder.census.gov). Socio-economic status (SES) was determined by participants’ parental education and occupation (A.B.Hollingshead, unpublished data), yielding a mean SES of 52.29 (s.d. = 10.07) and a range from 17 to 66, largely representing middle to upper-middle SES.

Procedure

At T1 and T2, participants completed diagnostic and demographics interviews, questionnaires, behavioral tasks and structural brain imaging (see Olson et al., 2007, 2009; Muetzel et al., 2008; Luciana et al., 2009; Porter et al., 2011; Schissel et al., 2011; Urošević et al., 2012). The present analyses focus on measures of age, sex, IQ as measured by Matrix Reasoning and Vocabulary subtests (Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence: Wechsler, 1999), reward and threat sensitivities as measured by the BIS/BAS scales, interview and questionnaire assessments of substance use, and structural MRI data. Participants received monetary compensation. Prior to data collection, consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki, minors provided written assent; parents/legal guardians and adult participants provided written consent. The local Institutional Review Board approved the protocol.

Measures

Magnetic resonance imaging

Volumetric measures were derived for the left and right hemisphere in the following regions of interest (ROIs): lateral OFC, medial OFC, amygdala, and Nacc at T1 and T2. MRI images were acquired on a 3 Tesla Siemens Trio (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) at the University of Minnesota’s Center for Magnetic Resonance Research. Three-dimensional images were obtained with a coronal T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo sequence (repetition time (TR) = 2530 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.65 ms, inversion time (TI) = 1100 ms, 240 slices, voxel size = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm, flip angle = 7°, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm).

Cortical and subcortical volume estimates were obtained by processing the high-resolution anatomical images in the FreeSurfer v.4.5.0 image analysis suite (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). During T2 data collection, the 3 T system was upgraded to a Siemens TIM Trio. To minimize upgrade-related variability, T1-weighted images were corrected for distortions resulting from gradient nonlinearity (Jovicich et al., 2006) before processing. After this correction, the standard FreeSurfer’s longitudinal data processing pipeline was followed (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/LongitudinalProcessing; Fischl et al., 2002; Reuter et al., 2010, 2012; see Supplementary Data).

Reward and threat sensitivities

Reward/BAS and threat/BIS sensitivities were assessed by the BIS/BAS scales (Carver and White, 1994), with BAS Reward Responsiveness, BAS Drive, BAS Fun Seeking, and BAS Total assessing reward sensitivity and the BIS scale assessing threat sensitivity. The same four-factor structure characterizes adolescent and adult samples. Thus, the measure yields comparable indices across development (Cooper et al., 2007) (see Supplementary Data for additional information).

Substance use measures

At T1 and T2, participants completed the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) semi-structured diagnostic interview (Kaufman et al., 1996), including all substance use screen questions. For minors, a parent simultaneously completed the interview (reporting on the participant) with a different interviewer. Consensus ratings for each interview item were derived.

At T1, due to exclusion criteria, no participants endorsed any substance use screens. This lack of endorsement at T1 indicated lifetime absence of tobacco and illicit substances use. The K-SADS-PL alcohol screen requires two alcoholic beverages per week on four occasions for endorsement. No participant endorsed this level of alcohol use at T1. Participants endorsed either never having a whole alcoholic beverage (84.9%) or consuming minimal amounts of alcohol at family functions, such as a holiday celebration or wedding (15.1%).

At T2, participants completed age-appropriate versions of the Personal Experiences Inventory (PEI; Winters, 1999; Winters et al., 2004), which assess substance use severity and use-related motivations. ‘Alcohol use frequency’ at T2 was assessed by one item [How many times have you had alcoholic beverages (including beer, wine and liquor) to drink during the last 12 months?]. Ratings across versions were coded into a common scale of alcohol use frequency for the last 12 months: 0 (never), 1 (1–5 times), 2 (6–20 times), 3 (21–49 times) and 4 (50+ times). This variable was square-root transformed for subsequent analyses.

At T2, K-SADS-PL screen questions assessed whether participants used tobacco, alcohol or illicit substances (i.e. cannabis, stimulants, cocaine, barbiturates/anxiolytics/narcotics, opiods, phencyclidine (PCP), hallucinogens, inhalants or other substances) during the follow-up. Participants were rated using a binary variable of ‘clinical-level substance use initiation’ with scores of ‘1’ indicating positive responses to any K-SADS-PL substance use screen item and scores of ‘0’ indicating no substance use initiation. The K-SADS-PL’s alcohol screen required two alcoholic beverages per week on four occasions for endorsement, whereas the screens for other substances are less stringent and required any use (i.e. once or more) during the follow-up for endorsement. Lack of K-SADS-PL-determined clinical-level alcohol use initiation at T2 did not preclude subclinical levels of alcohol use during the follow-up as captured by the PEI (e.g. a participant could have one drink per week on multiple occasions and still be considered subthreshold by the K-SADS-PL assessment).

Statistical analyses

Effects of age, sex, IQ, SES and follow-up interval length on clinical-level substance use initiation and alcohol use frequency at T2 were examined first by calculating bivariate correlations and binary logistic regressions to determine which variables were significantly associated with outcomes. There were no significant effects of sex or IQ. Older age, Wald’s χ2(1) = 4.53, P = 0.033, OR = 2.04, 95% CI = 1.06–3.92 significantly predicted clinical-level substance use initiation. Lower SES marginally predicted clinical-level substance use initiation, Wald’s χ2(1) = 2.81, P = 0.094, OR = 0.93, 95% CI = 0.86–1.01. Older age, r = 0.43, P = 0.012, and longer follow-up length, r = 0.39, P = 0.025, significantly predicted greater alcohol use frequency at T2. There were no significant correlations between age, SES and follow-up interval length. All subsequent analyses controlled for age, SES and follow-up interval length, and when appropriate, total brain volumes and scanner upgrade. Covariates were kept constant across all analyses to enhance interpretability and comparability of findings in relation to the two outcome variables of interest. Table 1 provides a summary of all covariates and predictors, as well as outcome variables, for binary logistic and hierarchical regression analyses.

Table 1.

Statistical analytic approach for predicting prospective substance use

Regression analyses’ predictors of substance usea
Regression step Regression model/analyses
Individual difference in the BIS/BAS Individual differences in ROI volumes Developmental change in the BIS/BAS Developmental change in the ROI volumes
1 Age, SES, follow-up length Age, SES, follow-up length, total brain volumeb Age, SES, follow-up length Age, SES, follow-up length
2 BIS/BAS subscale score at T1 L and R ROI volumes at T1 Unstandardized Residual for BIS/BAS T2 scorec Unstandardized Residuals for L and R ROI volumes at T2d

aThere were two types of outcome variables for two different types of regressions: a binary clinical-level substance use initiation (yes/no) in binary logistic regression analyses and continuous alcohol use frequency in hierarchical regression analyses; however, the set and order of predictors were the same in both regressions types. bAs recommended by the FreeSurfer group, the ‘brainsegnonvent’ variable yielded by standard processing was used as a measure of total brain volume excluding the ventricles. cRegressions with the relevant BAS or BIS subscale at T1 predicting the same subscale at T2 yielded these unstandardized residual scores. dRegressions with relevant ROI volume at T1 predicting the same ROI volume at T2, controlling for total brain volume and scanner upgrade, yielded these unstandardized residuals.

Binary logistic regressions examined effects of individual differences (i.e. baseline measures) and developmental changes (i.e. residual change) in the BIS/BAS scales and ROI brain volumes on prospective clinical-level substance use initiation. Age, follow-up interval and SES were covariates with a dichotomous outcome variable (1/0) of substance use initiation. Similar hierarchical regressions were conducted with age, follow-up interval and SES as covariates and a continuous outcome variable of alcohol use frequency during the last 12 months at T2.

The developmental/longitudinal changes in predictors were indexed by unstandardized residuals from each T2 predictor regression on its T1 value. In regression analyses involving brain ROI variables, left and right hemisphere estimates for each ROI were entered together, yielding four regression models (i.e. one per ROI), in analyses of baseline ROI individual differences’ effects and four regressions (i.e. one per ROI) in analyses of developmental changes in ROI volumes’ effects. Table 1 and the Supplementary Data summarize relevant regression models.

RESULTS

Descriptive analyses

Table 2 includes means and standard deviations of the BIS/BAS scales and brain ROI volumes at T1 and T2. The BIS/BAS scales at T1 and T2 were examined for associations with ROI brain volumes at each relevant time point. There were no significant associations at T1, except for a trend between BAS Drive and right Nacc volume, r = 0.29, P = 0.096. At T2, BAS Reward Responsiveness was significantly related to left medial OFC volume, r = 0.37, P = 0.03, and at a trend-level to right lateral OFC volume, r = 0.34, P = 0.051. T2 BIS was also related to left medial OFC volume, r = 0.44, P = 0.009 and inversely related to left Nacc volume, r = −0.37, P = 0.032. No other associations between the BIS/BAS scales and ROI volumes at T2 were significant.

Table 2.

Means and standard deviations for BIS/BAS scales and ROI brain volumes

Measures T1 T2
BAS Total 38.76 (4.83) 39.21 (4.46)
BAS Drive 10.12 (2.10) 10.35 (2.21)
BAS Fun Seeking 11.35 (2.17) 11.62 (2.03)
BAS Reward Responsiveness 17.29 (1.78) 17.24 (2.06)
BIS scale 20.32 (3.29) 20.03 (3.55)
R amygdala 1733.50 (188.98) 1724.71 (193.03)
L amygdala 1567.68 (143.47) 1575.15 (144.98)
R Nacc 720.79 (104.60) 715.85 (104.27)
L Nacc 716.50 (111.90) 686.44 (110.14)
R medial OFC 5733.44 (737.34) 5501.32 (652.63)
L medial OFC 5448.50 (888.36) 5235.47 (829.33)
R lateral OFC 8879.74 (1111.70) 8510.26 (867.63)
L lateral OFC 9012.74 (836.73) 8678.35 (805.09)

L, left hemisphere; R, right hemisphere. All brain volumes are presented in mm3 and not corrected for total brain volume, but in all reported statistical analyses, total brain volume is controlled.

Based on the T2 K-SADS, Table 3 describes substance use for 14 participants (41.2%) who initiated clinical-level use of 1–3 substances (M = 1.50, s.d. = 0.65; median = 1.00) during the follow-up interval. Two of these met alcohol abuse criteria, in partial remission, at T2. Based on the T2 PEI-item assessment of alcohol use frequency, an additional 11 (32.4%) participants reported drinking 1–5 times and one participant (2.9%) 6–20 times in the last year of the follow-up, but did not meet the K-SADS criteria for clinically significant use initiation. Finally, eight (23.5%) participants reported no substance use during the follow-up.

Table 3.

Adolescents with substance use initiation during the 2-year follow-up

K-SADS-PL assessment
PEI assessmenta
Sex Ages T1, T2 Alcohol use frequency Maximum alcohol quantity in 24 h Tobacco use frequency Cannabis use frequency Other drug use frequency Alcohol use in last 12 months
1 M 17.63, 19.96 2–3×/month 12–13 beers 6–20×
2 M 17.71, 19.92 1×/semester 3 beers 1–5×
3 M 17.51, 19.67 1×/week 0.5 l of 80-proof vodka <10 cigarettes ever 6×/summer 21–49×
4 M 15.70, 17.97 1×/3 months 6–8 drinks 1–5×
5 M 18.59, 21.95 1×/week 6 beers >10× ever 21–49×
6 M 18.94, 20.88 1×/week 5 drinks ≥50×
7 F 18.55, 20.96 1×/week 4 drinks 6–20×
8 F 15.63, 17.59 4–5× ever 3–4 drinks 1× ever, inhaler 1–5×
9 F 16.49, 19.07 <1×/month 3 drinks 6–20×
10 M 17.76, 19.79 1×/2–3 weeks 8 shots 1× ever 6–20×
11 M 18.20, 20.77 1×/week 2/3 of rum bottle 2× ever ≥50×
12 F 17.70, 20.10 1×/month 5–6 drinks 5× ever 6–20×
13 M 17.69, 20.02 1×/2 weeks 75-proof whiskeyb ≥50×
14 F 15.76, 17.93 (2× ever)c 4 drinks 1× ever 6–20×

M, male; F, female. aK-SADS-PL assessed for frequency of use for any period of time that a participant drank alcohol, whereas the PEI item assessed frequency of alcohol use for the last 12 months; given this and other differences in assessment, it is important to report both frequencies. bThis participant was not certain of the exact amount of whiskey consumed, but medical attention was required. cThis participant did not meet the K-SADS-PL alcohol screen criteria due to drinking two drinks in a week on only two occasions.

Baseline predictors of T2 alcohol use frequency

There were no significant effects of BIS/BAS scores at T1 on T2 alcohol use frequency. In hierarchical regression analyses of the baseline ROI volumes’ effects on prospective alcohol use frequency, there were no significant effects.

Developmental predictors of T2 alcohol use frequency

In five hierarchical regressions examining effects of developmental changes in the BIS/BAS scales’ scores from T1 to T2 on prospective alcohol use frequency, there were significant effects for increases in BAS Total, partial r = 0.40, t = 2.32, P = 0.028, and BAS Drive, partial r = 0.39, t = 2.25, P = 0.032, after controlling for age, SES and follow-up interval significant effects (covariates’ Step 1 R2 = 0.31, P = 0.010). There were no other significant BIS/BAS effects.

When developmental changes in structural brain ROIs’ volumes from T1 to T2 on alcohol use frequency during the follow-up were similarly examined, there were no significant effects.

Baseline predictors of clinical-level substance use initiation during the follow-up

When individual differences in T1 (baseline) BIS/BAS were examined, there were no significant effects of the BIS/BAS scales on clinical-level substance use initiation.

In binary logistic regressions examining effects of ROIs’ volumes at T1 on prospective substance use initiation, there was a significant effect for the left Nacc, Wald’s χ2(1) = 4.13, P = 0.042, OR = 0.985, 95% CI = 0.971–0.999, after controlling for age, SES and total brain volume. This effect remained after potential influential cases and an outlier were excluded from the analyses (see Supplementary Data).

To address the specificity of this effect within the striatum, post hoc analyses were conducted to examine whether baseline volumes of the caudate, putamen and pallidum were similarly associated with prospective substance use initiation. There were no significant effects.

Developmental predictors of T2 clinical-level substance use initiation

In five logistic regressions examining effects of developmental changes in the BIS/BAS scale scores from T1 to T2 on prospective substance use initiation, there was a significant effect for increases in BAS Total, Wald’s χ2(1) = 3.88, P = 0.049, OR = 1.34, 95% CI = 1.002–1.789 and increases in BAS Reward Responsiveness, Wald’s χ2(1) = 4.20, P = 0.040, OR = 2.59, 95% CI = 1.043–6.435, controlling for the covariates. Again, these significant effects remained after potential influential cases and outliers were excluded from analyses (see Supplementary Data). Other effects were not significant.

When effects of the changes in ROI volumes from T1 to T2 on prospective substance use initiation were similarly examined, there were no significant effects.

DISCUSSION

Substance misuse is a significant public health problem in the United States and other regions. When substances (e.g. alcohol) are introduced during periods of rapid neural development (e.g. the prenatal period), negative consequences are evident (Streissguth et al., 1980). Whether similar consequences are evident during periods of more subtle brain development, such as adolescence, is an active area of investigation (for reviews, see Elofson et al., 2013; Jacobus and Tapert, 2013). An interpretive dilemma in discerning the behavioral and neural impacts of adolescent substance use is that premorbid vulnerabilities cannot be reliably distinguished from the direct effects of substances. This is largely due to most existing studies either not involving adolescents who were free of substance use at baseline or not following adolescents from periods of no use into active substance use initiation. This study is unique in that the prospective predictors and subsequent impacts of substance use initiation can both be examined.

Using a theoretically guided analytic strategy, we found evidence of baseline vulnerabilities for substance use initiation during adolescence. Individuals in the mid-adolescent period who were most likely to initiate substance use were identified prior to the use initiation on the basis of their premorbid Nacc volumes. Adolescents who initiated clinical-level of substance use exhibited significantly smaller Nacc volumes at baseline, after controlling for potential confounds, compared with their peers. At baseline, participants were ages 15–18, a period associated with peak levels of Nacc volume, followed by a decrease from age 18 into early 20s in our larger longitudinal study (Urošević et al., 2012). Based on the present findings, adolescents aged 15–18 with non-optimal timing of these normative Nacc volume changes may be at increased risk for substance use. In other words, adolescent who are either lagging (i.e. not reaching peak) or accelerating (i.e. already peaked and moving into pruning usually seen into early 20s), compared with their peers, may be more likely to initiate clinical-levels of substance use.

In another longitudinal study of adolescents ages 11–13, left Nacc volumes exhibited similar increases during a 3- to 4-year follow-up (Dennison et al., 2013). Other recent studies have reported different developmental trajectories for Nacc volumes, such as decreases with greater pubertal development compared with peers (Goddings et al., 2014) and mean annual decreases in Nacc volumes from ages 8 to 22 (Tamnes et al., 2013). These longitudinal studies differ on a number of methodological issues, such as use of 3 T MRI scanners and modeling of hemisphere-specific trajectories (Urošević et al., 2012; Dennison et al., 2013) vs reliance on 1.5 T MRI scanners and averages of volumes between hemispheres (Tamnes et al., 2013; Goddings et al., 2014) and statistical approaches to modeling age effects. Differences in samples’ age ranges may also influence results when modeling developmental age effects (Fjell et al., 2010). Furthermore, Goddings et al.’ (2014) findings of pubertal effects on Nacc volumes suggest a complex set of influences on developmental changes. Findings might be clarified in future longitudinal studies by modeling the full range of adolescence, hemisphere-specific trajectories, and the effects of both pubertal development and chronological age. These discrepancies regarding the nature of typical developmental changes in Nacc volumes during adolescence do not detract from the potential significance of the present finding that adolescents with smaller Nacc volumes at ages 15–18 are at greater risk for substance use initiation.

Possible explanations for the association of smaller baseline Nacc volumes and prospective substance use initiation in adolescence require consideration of the larger neural circuitry involving the Nacc. The Nacc is part of a broad network that promotes approach toward potential sources of rewards and engagement with positively reinforcing stimuli once they are encountered (Koob and Volkow, 2010). Through its efferent connections with the ventral pallidum, which further relays information through the thalamus to the prefrontal cortex and back to the striatum, the Nacc is positioned to facilitate the translation of approach motivation to behavioral action (Depue and Collins, 1999). Afferents from the medial prefrontal cortex, insula, extended amygdala, hippocampus and ventral tegmental area modulate neuronal responses to rewarding stimuli within the Nacc. Together, these afferent and efferent pathways serve to guide reward-related motivations and behaviors. In particular, interconnections between the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex enable a cost–benefit analysis that weighs a potential reward’s value against positive and negative potential consequences of pursuing the reward.

The interconnections between prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum are complex and feature topographically segregated projections, e.g. ventromedial prefrontal cortex projections terminate primarily in patches within the shell region of the Nacc whereas dorsolateral prefrontal cortex projections terminate primarily in the head of the caudate nucleus. In addition, the ventral striatum contains zones where the prefrontal projections overlap, forming the basis for integration of information from different prefrontal areas (e.g. involved in reward pursuit vs cognitive control) prior to sending efferent signals to the ventral pallidum as information is processed throughout the striatal–thalamocortical circuit (Ferry et al., 2000; Haber et al., 2006). The integration of competing forms of prefrontal input within the ventral striatum may play a unique role in selecting potential rewards to pursue (or not) under conditions of strong incentive-reward motivation. In this context, smaller Nacc volumes may set a structural limit on the capacity for weighing potential reward values vs potential consequences, both pleasant and aversive, during risk-reward decision-making. Additional empirical support for this hypothesis includes a finding from a large multi-site study that smaller left Nacc volumes predicted a greater risk-taking bias in adolescents (Schneider et al., 2012). Moreover, smaller Nacc volumes mediated the association between risk-taking and Nacc’s functional activity as measured using fMRI (Schneider et al., 2012). Together, these findings suggest that smaller Nacc volumes during adolescence render individuals vulnerable to risk-taking behaviors, including substance use and potential misuse.

Like Cheetham et al. (2012), the present findings indicate that a priori differences in regional brain volumes predict substance use patterns in adolescence. However, Cheetham et al. (2012) found that smaller baseline OFC volumes (not Nacc volumes) predicted cannabis use initiation. Differences across studies may reflect unique neurobiological markers for cannabis initiation, or differences in the two studies’ sample characteristics. Notably, both studies implicate structures that are important nodes within the reward processing network and point to structural limits on integration of information related to reward pursuit.

In addition to premorbid vulnerabilities, this study supports certain longitudinal predictors of substance use in adolescence. We have reported that self-reported engagement with rewards normatively increases over time in the mid-adolescent period (Urošević et al., 2012). Within the present subsample, relative increases in reward sensitivity from baseline to follow-up were associated with a greater likelihood of substance use initiation and a relatively greater self-reported frequency of alcohol use. This patterning provides longitudinal support for the hypothesized associations between normative increases in reward sensitivity and substance use risk (Bjork et al., 2004, 2010; Ernst et al., 2005; Galvan et al., 2006; Geier et al., 2010; Urošević et al., 2012). There were no such effects for threat sensitivity. This pattern coheres with cross-sectional studies showing that BAS hypersensitivity is associated with heightened craving responses to substance cues (Kambouropoulos and Staiger, 2001, 2004) and greater substance use (e.g. Knyazev, 2004) in adolescents and young adults.

Moreover, this study adds novel nuances to the nature of the reward/BAS hypersensitivity and adolescent substance use relationship. Relative increases in different aspects of reward sensitivity were associated with different substance use outcomes. Increases in emotional responses to rewards, indexed by BAS Reward Responsiveness, were linked to greater substance use initiation risk, whereas increases in persistence of reward pursuits, indexed by BAS Drive, were linked to greater alcohol use frequency. This patterning is intriguing in light of findings suggesting that the three components of the BAS total score are empirically dissociable (Ross et al., 2002), with specific neural, behavioral and clinical correlates that extend to social decision-making (Scheres and Sanfrey, 2006). BAS Reward Responsiveness reflects positive affect and excitability in the presence of rewards, whereas BAS Drive is associated with behavioral action, approach motivation and the tendency to prioritize one’s individual goals over those of others (Smits and De Boeck, 2006). Our observation that adolescents who increase in their relative levels of approach motivation (BAS Drive) use alcohol with relatively greater frequency most likely reflects their tendency to seek situations where rewards, such as alcohol, are likely to be present. Approach motivation is distinct from one’s experience when a reward is actually encountered. Those who endorse high levels of positive affect or excitability when encountering rewards, otherwise construed as sensitivity to the ‘magnitude of reward’ and as indexed by BAS Reward Responsiveness, are likely to initiate clinical-level use of substances in general (e.g. alcohol, cannabis).

Despite potential for overlap, it is notable that BAS Fun Seeking did not emerge as an independent predictor of either outcome. BAS Fun Seeking has been more specifically associated with reward-related impulsivity (Smillie et al., 2006) and is elevated in individuals likely to advance to addiction (O’Connor et al., 2009; Park et al., 2013). Its lack of association with outcomes in this sample suggests that approach motivation and enjoyment of rewarding pursuits, rather than impulsivity, may represent the most salient vulnerability factors for adolescent substance use. Future studies of adolescent substance use will need to further examine whether the link between prospective increases in reward pursuit and greater frequency of use holds for substances other than alcohol. Future studies will also need to examine whether increases in positive affect in presence of rewards predict risk not just for initiation of clinical-level of substance use, but risk for development of full syndromes of substance use disorders.

Limitations

This study recruited healthy adolescents, and this analysis is based on a small sample. Our sample was predominantly Caucasian, with middle-class socio-economic backgrounds, and not of sufficient size to fully examine contributions from sex and ethnicity. In addition, the clinical implications of these findings are not yet clear. Further longitudinal study is needed to confirm whether increases in behavioral aspects of reward sensitivity, as well as Nacc structural differences, prior to use initiation predict substance use disorders and how this cascade, if present, might be modulated by other sources of neural influence, such as prefrontal integrity. Our approach within this study was theoretically guided and did not involve a whole-brain assessment. Given the a priori ROI-based hypotheses and the preliminary nature of the work, no corrections for multiple comparisons were applied in the brain ROI analyses. Accordingly, replications of the present findings in larger samples with diverse backgrounds and with correction for multiple-comparisons are needed.

Summary

This study is unique in its ability to longitudinally identify prospective risk factors, at both the neural and behavioral levels, associated with substance use vulnerability. Findings imply that adolescents with the greatest developmental increases in aspects of reward sensitivity that are tightly linked with the BAS and with potential neurobiological predispositions, i.e. structural differences in regions involved in reward pursuit (Nacc) are at greater risk for substance use initiation and greater alcohol consumption. Additional studies are needed to fully examine functional implications of individual differences in regional brain volumes of interest and their behavioral correlates. Overall, this study suggests novel neurobehavioral methods for prospectively identifying individuals who may be at risk for later substance-related difficulties.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES

The authors have no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest to report.

SUPPLEMENTARY DATA

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.

Supplementary Data

Acknowledgments

Data collection and analysis were supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant R01 DA 017843 and National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grant AA020033 to M.L. S.U.’s work on the manuscript was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants T32 MH 017069 and K01 MH 093621. This work was also supported by BTRC grants awarded to the CMRR, P41 RR008079, P41 EB015894 and 1P30 NS076408, and the University of Minnesota’s Supercomputing Institute and Center for Neurobehavioral Development.

REFERENCES

  1. Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Walshaw PD, et al. Behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity and bipolar spectrum disorders: a retrospective and concurrent behavioral high-risk design. Motivation and Emotion. 2006;30:143–55. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW. Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:1793–802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4862-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, Hommer DW. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS One. 2010;5:e11440. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67:319–33. [Google Scholar]
  5. Cauffman E, Shulman EP, Steinberg L, et al. Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:193–207. doi: 10.1037/a0016128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cheetham A, Allen NB, Whittle S, Simmons JG, Yucel M, Lubman DI. Orbitofrontal volumes in early adolescence predict initiation of cannabis use: a 4-year longitudinal and prospective study. Biological Psychiatry. 2012;71:684–92. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science. 2011;14:ppF1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cohen JR, Asarnow RF, Sabb FW, et al. A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:669–71. doi: 10.1038/nn.2558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cooper A, Gomez R, Aucote H. The behavioral inhibition system and behavioural approach system (BIS/BAS) scales: measurement and structural invariance across adults and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43:295–305. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dennison M, Whittle S, Yucel M, et al. Mapping subcortical brain maturation during adolescence: evidence of hemisphere- and sex-specific longitudinal changes. Developmental Science. 2013;16:772–91. doi: 10.1111/desc.12057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Depue RA, Collins PF. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22:491–569. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eaton DK, Kann L, Kinchen S, et al. Youth risk behavioral surveillance—United States, 2005. Surveillance Summaries. 2006;55/SS-5:1–108. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Elofson J, Gongvatana W, Carey KB. Alcohol use and cerebral white matter compromise in adolescence. Addictive Behaviors. 2013;38:2295–305. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ernst M, Luckenbaugh DA, Moolchan ET, et al. Behavioral predictors of substance-use initiation in adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006a;117:2030–9. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ernst M, Luckenbaugh DA, Moolchan ET, et al. Decision-making and facial emotion recognition as predictors of substance-use initiation among adolescents. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35:286–9. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ernst M, Nelson EE, Jazbec S, et al. Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage. 2005;25:1279–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine. 2006b;36:299–312. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ferry AT, Ongur D, An X, Price JL. Prefrontal cortical projections to the striatum in macaque monkeys: evidence for an organization related to prefrontal networks. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2000;425:447–70. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000925)425:3<447::aid-cne9>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–55. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Westlye LT, et al. When does brain aging accelerate? Dangers of quadratic fits in cross-sectional studies. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1376–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fowles DC. Application of a behavioral theory of motivation to the concepts of anxiety and impulsivity. Journal of Research in Personality. 1987;21:417–35. [Google Scholar]
  22. Franken IHA. Behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity predicts alcohol craving. Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;32:349–55. [Google Scholar]
  23. Franken IHA, Muris P. BIS/BAS personality characteristics and college students’ substance use. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40:1497–503. [Google Scholar]
  24. Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, et al. Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behaviors in adolescents. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6885–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Geier CF, Terwilliger R, Teslovich T, Velanova K, Luna B. Immaturities in reward processing and its influence on inhibitory control in adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1613–29. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goddings A-L, Mills KL, Clasen LS, Giedd JN, Viner RM, Blakemore S-J. The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. Neuroimage. 2014;88:242–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gray JA. Neural systems, emotion and personality. In: Madden J IV, editor. Neurobiology of Learning, Emotion and Affect. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1991. pp. 273–306. [Google Scholar]
  28. Gray JA, McNaughton N. The Neuropsychology of Anxiety. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  29. Haber SN, Kim KS, Mailly P, Calzavara R. Reward-related cortical inputs define a large striatal region in primates that interface with associative cortical inputs, providing a substrate for incentive-based learning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:8368–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0271-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hardin MG, Schroth E, Pine DS, Ernst M. Incentive-related modulation of cognitive control in healthy, anxious, and depressed adolescents: development and psychopathology related differences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:446–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01722.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Harmon-Jones E, Allen JJB. Behavioral activation sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: covariation of putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:159–63. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter MR, Wechsler H. Early age of first drunkenness as a factor in college students’ unplanned and unprotected sex attributable to drinking. Pediatrics. 2003;111:34–41. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jacobus J, Tapert SF. Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9:703–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jazbec S, Hardin MG, Schroth E, McClure E, Pine DS, Ernst M. Age-related influence of contingencies on a saccade task. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;174:754–62. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0520-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Johnson SL, Turner RJ, Iwata N. BIS/BAS levels and psychiatric disorder: an epidemiological study. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2003;25:25–36. [Google Scholar]
  36. Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2008. Volume I: Secondary School Students. 2009. (NIH Publication No. 09-7402). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. [Google Scholar]
  37. Jovicich J, Czanner S, Greve D, et al. Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. NeuroImage. 2006;30:436–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kambouropoulos N, Staiger PK. The influence of sensitivity to reward on reactivity to alcohol-related cues. Addiction. 2001;96:1175–85. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.968117510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kambouropoulos N, Staiger PK. Reactivity to alcohol-related cues: relationship among cue type, motivational processes, and personality. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2004;18:275–83. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.3.275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Ryan N. Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime (K-SADS-PL) Version 1.0. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinics; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  41. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. King KM, Chassin L. A prospective study of the effects of age of initiation of alcohol and drug use on young adult substance dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:256–65. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Knyazev GG. Behavioural activation as predictor of substance use: mediating and moderating role of attitudes and social relationships. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2004;75:309–21. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Knyazev GG, Slobodskaya HR, Kharchenko II, Wilson GD. Personality and substance use in Russian youths: the predictive and moderating role of behavioural activation and gender. Personality and Individual Differences. 2004;37:827–43. [Google Scholar]
  45. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:217–38. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Loxton NJ, Dawe S. Alcohol abuse and dysfunctional eating in adolescent girls: the influence of individual differences in sensitivity to reward and punishment. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2001;29:455–62. doi: 10.1002/eat.1042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Luciana M, Collins PF, Olson EA, Schissel AM. Tower of London performance in healthy adolescents: the development of planning skills and associations with self-reported inattention and impulsivity. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;34:461–75. doi: 10.1080/87565640902964540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Luciana M, Wahlstrom D, Porter JN, Collins PF. Dopaminergic modulation of incentive motivation in adolescence: age-related changes in signaling, individual differences, and implications for the development of self-regulation. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:844–61. doi: 10.1037/a0027432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Meyer B, Johnson SL, Winters R. Responsiveness to threat and incentive in bipolar disorder: relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2001;23:133–43. doi: 10.1023/A:1010929402770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Muetzel RL, Collins PF, Mueller BA, Schissel AM, Lim KO, Luciana M. The development of corpus callosum microstructure and associations with bimanual task performance in healthy adolescence. NeuroImage. 2008;39:1918–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. O’Connor RM, Colder CR. Predicting alcohol patterns in first-year college students through motivational systems and reasons for drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19:10–20. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. O’Connor RM, Stewart SH, Watt MC. Distinguishing BAS risk for university students’ drinking, smoking, and gambling behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences. 2009;46:514–9. [Google Scholar]
  53. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Olson EA, Collins PF, Hooper CJ, Muetzel R, Lim KO, Luciana M. White matter integrity predicts delay discounting behavior in 9- to 23-year-olds: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:1406–21. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Olson EA, Hooper CJ, Collins P, Luciana M. Adolescents’ performance on delay and probability discounting tasks: contributions of age, intelligence, executive functioning, and self-reported externalizing behavior. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43:1886–97. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Park SM, Park YA, Lee HW, Yung HY, Lee JY, Choi JS. The effects of behavioral inhibition/approach system as predictors of internet addiction in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 2013;54:7–11. [Google Scholar]
  57. Patrick ME, Blair C, Maggs JL. Executive function, approach sensitivity, and emotional decision making as influences on risk behaviors in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2008;30:449–62. doi: 10.1080/13803390701523109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Porter JN, Collins PF, Muetzel RL, Lim KO, Luciana M. Associations between cortical thickness and verbal fluency in childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1865–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Reuter M, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Highly accurate inverse consistent registration: a robust approach. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1181–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1402–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ross SR, Mills SR, Bonebright TL, Bailley SE. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavioral Inhibition and Activation Scales. Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;33:861–5. [Google Scholar]
  62. Scheres A, Sanfrey AG. Individual differences in decision-making: drive and reward responsiveness affect strategic bargaining in economic games. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2006;2:35. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schissel AM, Olson EA, Collins PF, Luciana M. Age-independent effects of pubertal status on behavioral constraint in healthy adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 2011;51:975–80. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Schneider S, Peters J, Bromberg U, et al. IMAGEN Consortium Risk taking and the adolescent reward system: a potential common link to substance abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169:39–46. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Batien BD. Methamphetamine use in a rural college population: associations with marijuana use, sensitivity to punishment, and sensitivity to reward. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2008;22:444–9. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Smillie LD, Jackson CJ, Dalgleish LI. Conceptual distinctions between Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: a reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective. Personality and Individual Differences. 2006;40:1039–50. [Google Scholar]
  67. Smits DJM, De Boeck P. From BIS/BAS to the big five. European Journal of Personality. 2006;20:255–70. [Google Scholar]
  68. Somerville LH, Hare T, Casey BJ. Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:2123–34. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Streissguth AP, Landesman-Dwyer S, Martin JC, Smith DW. Teratogenic effects of alcohol in humans and laboratory animals. Science. 1980;209:353–61. doi: 10.1126/science.6992275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sutton SK, Davidson RJ. Prefrontal brain asymmetry: a biological substrate of the behavioral approach and behavioral inhibition systems. Psychological Science. 1997;8:204–10. [Google Scholar]
  71. Takahashi Y, Yamagata S, Kijima N, Shigemasu K, Ono Y, Ando J. Continuity and change in behavioral inhibition and activation systems: a longitudinal behavioral genetic study. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;43:1616–25. [Google Scholar]
  72. Tamnes CK, Walhovd KB, Dale AM, et al. The Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Brain development and aging: overlapping and unique patterns of change. Neuroimage. 2013;68:63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Urošević S, Collins P, Muetzel R, Lim K, Luciana M. Longitudinal changes in behavioral approach system sensitivity and brain structures involved in reward processing during adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:1488–500. doi: 10.1037/a0027502. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wahlstrom D, Collins P, White T, Luciana M. Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain and Cognition. 2010;72:146–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  76. Windle M, Miller-Tutzauer C, Domenico D. Alcohol use, suicidal behavior, and risky activities among adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1992;2:317–30. [Google Scholar]
  77. Winters KC. A new multiscale measure of adult substance abuse. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 1999;16:237–46. doi: 10.1016/s0740-5472(98)00035-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Winters KC, Latimer WW, Stinchfield RD, Egan E. Measuring adolescent drug abuse and psychosocial factors in four ethnic groups of drug-abusing boys. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2004;12:227–36. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.4.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Data

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES