Abstract
Objective. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effect of gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus in its temperament 3 months after birth. Method. A total of 302 mother-3-month-infant dyads enrolled the retrospective cohort study. 76 mothers had regular gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus in their pregnancy; 62 mothers had irregular tactile stimulation on the fetus, and the rest of 164 mothers who had no tactile stimulation served as nonexposure group. Temperament was assessed using the EITS (a nine-dimensional scale of temperament). Results. Significant difference in temperament type was found among infants in 3 groups at 3 months of age. In the regular practice group, the babies with easy type temperament accounted for 73.7%, which was higher than that in irregular practice group (53.2%, P = 0.012) and that in the control group (42.1%, P < 0.001). Compared to infants in no practice group, the infants who had received regular gentle tactile stimulation before birth were lower in negative mood (P = 0.047) while higher in adaptability (P < 0.001), approach (P = 0.001), and persistence (P = 0.001), respectively. Conclusion. Regular gentle tactile stimulation on fetus may promote the formation of easy type infant temperament.
1. Introduction
Temperament refers to individual differences in an infant's expressions of arousal and emotion and describes infant self-regulation, reactivity, and modulation [1]. It was initially defined as stable, biologically based individual differences in reactivity and regulation that form the core of personality [2], but recently it has been viewed as a predisposing set of individual characteristics with the potential to systematically change over time as a child develops [3, 4]. Infant temperament has been classified into 4 types: easy, intermediate, difficult, and slow to warm up. According to the measure standard, the infant with easy type temperament is more amiable, regular in living, and open than the infant with the other temperaments. The difficult type infant is more active, irritable, and irregular in living. The slow type baby is slower in response to environment, is more socially withdrawn, and has less interest in the people around.
Fetus has sense and feeling in uterus. Neuronal multiplication occurs mainly from the 10th to the 20th gestational weeks. Somatosensory functions are developed at an early stage [5]. While the basic physical structure of the sensory receptors develops early in gestation, most of the neurosensory development occurs in the last sixteen to twenty weeks [6]. The human fetus is able to feel pain after 24-week gestation [7]. During the seventh month, the fetus begins to show signs of personality and intentional behavior [8]. Some findings provide evidence of fetal attention and memory after 32 gestational weeks [9]. The fetus has its psychology: feeling, dreaming, and even enjoying [10]. Babies are particularly sensitive to touch, which is the most highly developed sense at birth [11].
Since the 1960s, there have been numerous studies that have shown the benefits of massage on the development of newborns and children referring to dimension of physics and psychology [12–25]. In addition, preterm neonates who received massage showed more mature habituation and orientation [26], better results on developmental tests [27], and fewer stress behaviors [28] than control infants. All these affected psychological elements are all related to temperament. Massage therapy by mothers combined with skin-to-skin care during neonatal hospital stay could improve child's neurodevelopment outcome at 2-year corrected age [29]. Infant's temperament is influenced by the mother's personality, family economic status, pregnancy-specific anxiety, depression and stress in pregnancy and after delivery, parenting, and mode of feeding [4, 30–34]. Prenatal attachment has been discussed in the literature for decades and has evolved in recent years into the construct of maternal-fetal attachment (MFA). Cranley developed the Maternal-Fetal Attachment Scale (MFAS) to measure the construct [35]. Music fetal education means playing music to fetal, which is also one kind of maternal fetal attachment as talking to the fetus and tactile stimulation on the fetus. Nowadays, gentle abdominal tactile stimulation through abdomen in pregnancy is one way of maternal-fetal attachment. Some study has testified that massage can relieve the pregnant anxiety [36]. Although there is considerable volume of international research on infant massage from the scope of physics and psychology, surprisingly little scientific research has been conducted on the topic of gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus. We speculated that when the pregnant women give gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus, she may feel fetus' response more and her abdomen also receives more massage than often. These may relieve her anxiety and depression and reduce the level of cortisol in blood and amniotic fluid, which is testified to have indirect effects on infant temperament [37]. Meanwhile we hypothesized that tactile stimulation through abdomen promotes the flow of amniotic fluid and enhances the gentle stimulation on the fetal skin and may has not only physical effect, but also psychological effect similar to that of massage on preterm babies.
The maternal-fetal attachment education center in our hospital has been opened for about 20 years and there are about 150 pregnant women who receive the education through the lecture in every month. So we could have enough samples for study on maternal-fetal attachment. In the present study, we have tested the hypothesis that regular gentle tactile stimulation contributes to the formation of easy type infant temperament 3 months after birth. The result is hoped to find the availably effective method for the easy type infant temperament in the future.
2. Subjects and Methods
2.1. Subjects
A total of 302 mother-3-month-infant dyads enrolled the retrospective cohort study by sequential sampling, using the registration number in August 2014. The inclusion criteria were mothers willing to participate. The exclusion criteria were smoking or drinking during pregnancy, having serious mental illness, having obstetric complications such as maternal organ diseases, obstetric-special diseases, or fetal abnormality, premature delivery, prenatal mortality, or newborn abnormalities (Apgar score < 8, birth weight < 2500 g, malformation, or other problems).
2.2. Study Design
2.2.1. Exposure Factor and Groups
The retrospective study was designed among the 3-month postpartum women who attended the public health lecture in our hospital. Exposure factor is gentle abdominal tactile stimulation on the fetus during pregnancy. The method was based on “Modern maternal-fetal attachment” which was edited by Jiangdixian and published in January 2014 by Second Military Medical University press. The method was described on pages 48–63. The theory and method of musical fetal education, gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus, and talking to fetus were transferred by lessons. The key features of this method were (1) duration: performing from the 24th gestational week until delivery; (2) interval: performing for 5 minutes, once or twice a day, on a fixed time schedule; (3) maneuver: the activities including moderate shaking, striking on the abdomen, and mild pressing of fingers into the abdomen where fetal body can be felt; (4) Stopping immediately: when feeling any abdomen pain, bleeding, or fetal movement becoming fierce. Mothers did not continue with massage postnatally. Engaging in these practices, in accordance with all 4 points, was defined as regular practice; doing otherwise was considered to be irregular practice. According to this, the study participants were divided into three groups: no practice group (n = 164), the irregular practice group (n = 62), and the regular practice group (n = 76).
2.2.2. Retrospective Data Collection
The postpartum women completed a questionnaire containing data about demographic characteristic, the practice of abdominal tactile stimulation in pregnancy, method of delivery, the newborn's health, and method of feeding. Data were also collected on the regularity of other maternal-fetal attachment ways such as musical fetal education (playing music to fetus) and language fetal education (talking to fetus) as defined in “Modern maternal-fetal attachment.”
2.2.3. Measures
We used 4 psychological scales: Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS, a self-rating scale to measure the depression in the past week whose correlation coefficient with HAMA scale is 0.365), Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS, a self-rating scale to measure the depression in the past week that has high and moderate correlation with other depression scales such as BDI and HRSD and was translated to Chinese version in 1985), the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire for adults (EPQA, a scale developed by H. J. Eyscenck and B. G. Eysenck in 1952 for personality measurement that has been found to have high reliability and validity, China has revised the version and formulated T score tables on the norms of samples in 6 areas and the Chinese research in 30 provinces showed that the reliability and validity of all subscales all meet the requirement of psychological measurement) for mothers, and the Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire (EITQ, norm scale in Shanghai, China) [38] for 3 month-old infants. The scale is normal in Shanghai, China. Education was final education; income, maternal age, mode of delivery, and feeding method consulted the patients' dictate; maternal temperament consulted EPQA; maternal depression and anxiety after delivery consulted SAS and SDS.
Both SDS and SAS contain 20 items, which are widely used to evaluate the feelings of depression and anxiety. EPQA includes 4 subscales: Extraversion (E), Neuroticism (N), Psychoticism (P), and Lie (L) scale. The Chinese revised version contains 88 items. EITQ, designed by Carey and revised by Chinese experts, assesses the type of temperament of 1–4-month-old infants, according to their primary caregiver [38]. Its internal consistency is between 0.43 and 0.76. The reliability for 3-4-month-old baby is 0.74. The scale consists of 9 dimensions: activity (the frequency of activity and the pace of action in the whole day), rhythmicity (the regularity of Physiological activity such as sleeping and eating), approach/withdrawal (the attitude of accepting or rejecting to the new stimulation), adaptability (the easy degree to adapt himself to the new environment), intensity (the reaction intensity to stimulation), mood (the proportion of pleasant or unpleasant, kind or unkind, happy or unhappy emotion in the whole day), persistence (the tendency to continue to uphold the original activity when encountering setback or difficulties), distractibility (easy degree to shift its attention), and reaction threshold (the stimulation amount needed to cause irritation reaction). The items are rated on a 6-point scale. The higher dimension score indicates higher activity, weaker rhythmicity, lower approach, weaker adaptability, stronger intensity, negative mood, shorter persistence, higher distractibility, and lower threshold.
2.2.4. Target Variable/Outcome
The target variable was the temperament type of the infant, which was measured using EITS, and the difference in infant temperament among the three groups. Multivariate logistic regression was conducted with infant temperament as the dependent variable (1, easy temperament; 0, other infant temperament), with gentle tactile stimulation (0, no practice; 1, irregular practice; 2, regular practice) as the chief independent variable, and using characteristics which were distributed unevenly among the groups as the independent variables: musical fetal education (the method was also based on “Modern maternal-fetal attachment”): 0, no; 1, irregular practice; 2, regular practice; temperament tendency of woman: 1, typical extrovert; 2, ambivert; 3, typical introvert; pregnant woman being only child: 0, no; 1, yes; and annual family income: 1, ≤ 5 000$; 2, 5 000–8 000$; 3, 8 000–16 000$; 4, 16 000–32 000$; 5, ≥32 000$.
2.3. Data Analysis
The software named “Cary's children temperament questionnaire testing system” developed by the Xin hua Hospital, Jiaotong University, was used to assess the temperament of the infants. Data analysis was conducted with SPSS 17.0 to compare differences among groups, using chi-square test, Fisher's exact probability test, the Mann-Whitney U test, ANOVA test, and multivariate analyses. For all the tests, the 95% confidence was used and a P value of 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.
3. Results
3.1. Characteristics of the Groups
As shown in Tables 1 and 2, no significant difference was found among groups in terms of age (F = 1.353, P = 0.260), maternal nerve stability, mother's education, working status, mode of feeding, delivery method, talking to the fetus, and depression and anxiety after delivery. However, statistically significant differences were found in pregnant women's temperament tendency, being only child, musical fetal education, and family income, respectively (P < 0.05). And these factors were included in the multivariable analysis as confounding factors.
Table 1.
Variables | Options | No practice group N (%) | Irregular practice group N (%) | Regular practice group N (%) | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Maternal temperament tendency | Typical extrovert | 68 (41.5) | 28 (45.2) | 16 (21.1) | 0.002a |
Ambivert | 86 (52.4) | 34 (54.8) | 58 (76.3) | ||
Typical introvert | 10 (6.1) | 0 | 2 (2.6) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Maternal temperament stability | Typically stable | 83 (50.6) | 26 (41.9) | 28 (36.8) | 0.172a |
Ambivert | 77 (47.0) | 34 (54.8) | 48 (63.2) | ||
Typically unstable | 4 (2.4) | 2 (3.2) | 0 | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Education | High school or below | 12 (7.3) | 2 (3.2) | 0 | 0.529a |
College | 106 (64.6) | 48 (77.4) | 64 (84.2) | ||
Master | 44 (26.8) | 12 (19.4) | 12 (15.8) | ||
Doctor | 2 (1.2) | 0 | 0 | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Pregnant women as only child | No | 50 (30.5) | 9 (14.5%) | 24 (31.6) | 0.037b |
Yes | 114 (69.5) | 53 (85.5%) | 52 (68.4) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Family annual income | ≤5 000$ | 14 (8.6) | 0 | 2 (2.6) | 0.003a |
5 000–8 000$ | 18 (11.1) | 6 (9.7) | 8 (10.5) | ||
8 000–16 000$ | 58 (35.8) | 16 (25.8) | 18 (23.7) | ||
16 000–32 000$ | 40 (24.7) | 20 (32.3) | 26 (34.2) | ||
≥32 000$ | 32 (19.8) | 20 (32.3) | 22 (28.9) | ||
Total | 162 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) |
aRank chi-square, bPearson chi-square.
The criteria of EPQA: ambivert, T score in E or N subscale is between 38.5 and 61.5; typical introvert: T score in E subscale <38.5; typical extrovert: T score in E subscale >61.5; typically nerve stable: T score in N subscale <38.5; typically nerve unstable: T score in N subscale >61.5.
Table 2.
Variables | Options | No practice group N (%) | Irregular practice group N (%) | Regular practice group N (%) | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Working status | Full time job | 43 (26.2) | 14 (22.6) | 18 (23.7) | 0.790a |
Part time job | 75 (45.7) | 35 (56.5) | 35 (46.1) | ||
Unemployed | 46 (28.0) | 13 (21.0) | 23 (30.3) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Musical fetal education | No | 80 (48.8) | 0 | 2 (2.6) | <0.001d |
Irregular practice | 54 (32.9) | 40 (64.5) | 26 (34.2) | ||
Regular practice | 30 (18.3) | 22 (35.5) | 48 (63.2) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Talking with fetus | No | 78 (47.6) | 26 (41.9) | 33 (44.6) | 0.734b |
Yes | 86 (52.4) | 36 (58.1) | 41 (55.4) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 74 (100) | ||
| |||||
Delivery mode | Vaginal delivery | 66 (40.2) | 21 (33.9) | 38 (50.0) | 0.145b |
C-S | 98 (59.8) | 41 (66.1) | 38 (50.0) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Chief caregiver | Mother | 78 (47.6) | 26 (41.9) | 30 (39.5) | 0.071c |
Father | 80 (48.8) | 32 (51.6) | 46 (60.5) | ||
Grand parents | 2 (1.2) | 4 (6.5) | 0 | ||
Others | 4 (2.4) | 0 | 0 | ||
Total | 164 | 62 | 76 | ||
| |||||
Mode of feeding | Pure breast feeding | 40 (24.4) | 14 (22.6) | 15 (19.7) | 0.820b |
Mixed feeding | 100 (70.0) | 41 (66.1) | 52 (68.4) | ||
Artificial feeding | 24 (14.6) | 7 (11.3) | 9 (11.8) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Depression or anxiety | No | 134 (81.7) | 49 (79.0) | 64 (84.2) | 0.735b |
Yes | 30 (18.3) | 13 (21) | 12 (15.7) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) |
aRank chi-square, bPearson chi-square, cFisher's exact, and drank correlation test.
Norm of SDS in China: score ≥ 53 as “depression.”
Norm of SAS in China: score ≥ 50 as “anxiety.”
3.2. The Effect of Gentle Tactile Stimulation on the Fetus on Its Temperament 3 Months after Birth
There were notable differences in the types of infant temperament among the three groups (Fisher's exact < 0.001, Table 3). Segmentation of chi-square test indicated that a large majority (73.7%) of the infants in the regular practice group had an easy temperament, more than that (53.2%) in irregular groups (χ 2 = 6.241, P = 0.012) and that (42.1%) in no practice group (χ 2 = 20.794, P < 0.001). However, there was no difference in the ratio of easy type between no practice group and irregular practice group (χ 2 = 2.260, P = 0.133). Although the ratio of difficult type in irregular practice group was higher than the other two groups, there was no significant difference among 3 groups (Fisher's exact = 0.289). Table 4 showed that gentle regular tactile stimulation was still positively associated with an easy temperament (P < 0.001) including other relative factors. As shown in Table 5, ANOVA test showed differences among the three groups on three of the temperament dimensions. Infants who were given regularly gentle fetal tactile stimulation before birth were higher in adaptability (F = 12.408, P < 0.001), approachability (F = 6.064, P = 0.003), and persistence (F = 5.984, P = 0.003). Comparison in pair of groups showed that there was no notable difference in 9 dimensions scores between no practice group and irregular practice group (all P > 0.05). Infants in regular practice group were higher in adaptability (T = 2.969, P = 0.004) and persistence (T = 2.493, P = 0.014), compared by infants in irregular practice group. Meanwhile, infants in regular practice group were lower in negative mood (T = 2.001, P = 0.047), while higher in adaptability (T = 5.009, P < 0.001), approach (T = 3.457, P = 0.001), and persistence (T = 3.259, P = 0.001), compared with infants in no practice group. As shown in Table 6, linear-regression analysis showed that higher adaptability, approachability, and persistence were still positively associated with the gentle regular tactile stimulation including other relative factors.
Table 3.
Temperament type | Options | Groups | P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
No practice group | Irregular practice group | Regular practice group | |||
Different types | Easy | 69 (42.1) | 33 (53.2) | 56 (73.7) | <0.001a |
Intermediate | 85 (51.8) | 23 (37.1) | 17 (22.4) | ||
Difficult | 6 (3.7) | 4 (6.5) | 1 (1.3) | ||
Slow | 4 (2.4) | 2 (3.2) | 2 (2.6) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Easy type | Easy | 69 (42.1) | 33 (53.2) | 56 (73.7) | <0.001b |
Others | 95 (57.9) | 29 (46.8) | 20 (26.3) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) | ||
| |||||
Difficult type | Difficult | 6 (3.7) | 4 (6.5) | 1 (1.3) | 0.289a |
Others | 158 (96.3) | 58 (93.5) | 75 (98.7) | ||
Total | 164 (100) | 62 (100) | 76 (100) |
aFisher's exact; bPearson chi-square.
The temperament type is defined by rhythmicity, approach/withdraw, adaptability, intensity, and mood dimensions. Easy type: among 5-dimension scores, not more than 2 is above the average and none is more than 1 standard deviation. Difficult type: 4- or 5-dimension scores are above the average, including intensity dimension, and 2-dimension scores are more than 1 standard deviation. Slow type: it is similar to difficult type, but approach/withdraw or adaptation dimension score is more than 1 standard deviation. Meanwhile, activity dimension score is not more than 1/2 standard deviation and mood dimension score is not less than 1/2 standard deviation. Intermediate type: all others.
Table 4.
ORc | 95.0% C.I. | P | ORa | 95.0% CI | P | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | |||||
Gentle tactile stimulation (none) | Ref (dnm b variable) | |||||||
| ||||||||
Gentle tactile stimulation (irregular) | 1.567 | .871 | 2.819 | 0.134 | 1.598 | .785 | 3.251 | 0.196 |
| ||||||||
Gentle tactile stimulation (regular) | 3.855 | 2.121 | 7.006 | <0.001 | 5.098 | 2.457 | 10.579 | <0.001 |
| ||||||||
Pregnant woman being only child | 1.283 | 1.019 | 1.615 | 0.034 | ||||
| ||||||||
Annual family income | .652 | .365 | 1.165 | 0.149 | ||||
| ||||||||
Temperament tendency of woman | .331 | .205 | .532 | 0.000 | ||||
| ||||||||
Musical fetal education (none) | Ref (dnm b variable) | |||||||
| ||||||||
Musical fetal education (irregular) | .805 | .438 | 1.479 | 0.484 | ||||
| ||||||||
Musical fetal education (regular) | 1.170 | .541 | 2.533 | 0.689 |
aAdjusted OR (adjusted for confounding factors: musical fetal education, temperament tendency of woman, pregnant woman being only child, and annual family income).
cCrude OR.
b2 cases which did not answer the question about family income were excluded.
Table 5.
Groups | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dimensions | No practice group | Irregular practice group | Regular practice group | F | P a | |||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |||
Activity | 3.342 | 0.449 | 3.234 | 0.385 | 3.229 | 0.33 | 2.736 | 0.066 |
Rhythmicity | 3.311 | 0.706 | 3.234 | 0.666 | 3.273 | 0.72 | 0.288 | 0.750 |
Adaptability | 2.995 | 0.627 | 2.882 | 0.618 | 2.56 | 0.646 | 12.408 | <0.001 |
Approach/withdrawal | 2.377 | 0.507 | 2.243 | 0.48 | 2.14 | 0.516 | 6.064 | 0.003 |
Intensity | 3.334 | 0.547 | 3.38 | 0.428 | 3.297 | 0.519 | 0.251 | 0.778 |
Mood | 2.725 | 0.481 | 2.716 | 0.52 | 2.588 | 0.56 | 1.989 | 0.139 |
Persistence | 2.742 | 0.594 | 2.706 | 0.487 | 2.463 | 0.657 | 5.984 | 0.003 |
Distractibility | 2.352 | 0.663 | 2.428 | 0.521 | 2.295 | 0.629 | 0.735 | 0.481 |
Threshold | 4.447 | 0.67 | 4.412 | 0.719 | 4.31 | 0.707 | 1.04 | 0.355 |
aANOVA test (the higher dimension score indicates higher activity, weaker rhythmicity, lower approach, weaker adaptability, stronger intensity, negative mood, shorter persistence, higher distractibility, and lower threshold.).
Table 6.
Dependent variable | Self-variable | Crude estimated B | 95.0% CI | P | Adjusted estimated B a | 95.0% CI | P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower Bound | Upper Bound | Lower Bound | Upper Bound | ||||||
Adaptability | Gentle tactile stimulation (irregular) | −.127 | −.314 | .061 | 0.185 | −0.084 | −.294 | .126 | 0.432 |
Gentle tactile stimulation (regular) | −.449 | −.623 | −.274 | 0.000 | −0.427 | −.628 | −.226 | 0.000 | |
| |||||||||
Approach/withdrawal | Gentle tactile stimulation (irregular) | −.142 | −.291 | .006 | 0.059 | −.154 | −.323 | 0.014 | 0.072 |
Gentle tactile stimulation (regular) | −.245 | −.383 | −.107 | 0.001 | −.259 | −.420 | −.097 | 0.014 | |
| |||||||||
Persistence | Gentle tactile stimulation (irregular) | −.035 | −.208 | .138 | 0.693 | .026 | −.174 | 0.225 | 0.801 |
Gentle tactile stimulation (regular) | −.278 | −.439 | −.117 | 0.001 | −.226 | −.417 | −.035 | 0.020 |
aAdjusted for confounding factors: musical fetal education, temperament tendency of woman, pregnant woman being only child, and annual family income.
b2 cases which did not answer the question about family income were excluded.
4. Discussion
The results of the current study demonstrate the effect of gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus in its temperament 3 months after birth. Significant difference in temperament type was found among infants in 3 groups at 3 months of age. In the regular practice group, the babies with easy type temperament accounted for 73.7%, which was higher than that in other two groups (P < 0.05). In addition, the infants who had received regular gentle tactile stimulation before birth had notably higher adaptability (P < 0.001), persistence (P = 0.003), and approach (P = 0.003), respectively. Infants in regularly practice group were lower in negative mood (T = 2.001, P = 0.047) while higher in adaptability (T = 5.009, P < 0.001), approach (T = 3.457, P = 0.001), and persistence (T = 3.259, P = 0.001), compared with infants in no practice group. The present data suggests that regular gentle tactile stimulation on fetus may promote the formation of easy type infant temperament.
Temperament type influences the relationship between the infant and the people around it, as well as the infant's physical and behavioral development [39]. A more easy-going temperament was associated with more well-being [40]. Aggressive children are likely to have more challenging temperament characteristics [41–44] that have the potential to influence the responses of others, setting up mutually reinforcing interpersonal interaction patterns that may maintain problem behavior over the long term [45]. Previous studies have found that infants with a difficult temperament remain irritable and are less likely to fit in with their environment for several years and may exhibit abnormal behavior in childhood [43, 46–48], while an easy temperament functions as a protective mechanism for these outcomes in social-emotional development [40, 46]. The results showed that the proportion of babies with easy temperament was notably higher in the regular practice group than in the irregular practice groups and no practice groups. The multivariable models showed that gentle tactile stimulation on the fetus was associated with an easy temperament although other variables which could affect temperament were included in the models.
Researches in disparate populations of children have consistently demonstrated an association of temperament characteristics such as approach/withdrawal, adaptability, persistence, and activity with externalizing behavior [43, 46–48]. The present study on infants showed that increased nocturnal sleep was correlated with increased approachability and increased diurnal sleep duration was also correlated with increased adaptability in 11-month-old baby [49]. Infants' early persistence and mothers' teaching are direct pathways to cognitive status at the start of the second year [50]. The study of Sakimura et al. indicates that 38.7% of 3- to 5-year-old children with aggressive behavior demonstrated low adaptability and low persistence [45]. Language development is also associated with adaptability and persistence [51]. The results showed that infants in regularly practice group were lower in negative mood and higher in adaptability, approach, and persistence, compared with infants in no practice group. Regular tactile stimulation can reduce mother's negative emotions and may calm the fetus directly. It can also attract fetus to cater to the stimulation of mothers and achieve higher approach and adaptability. Besides, if mothers insist to do regular tactile stimulation during pregnancy, fetus can also cultivate their perseverance to this interaction, and this kind of disposition may continue after birth. For these reasons, we concluded that the influence of gentle abdominal tactile stimulation on these dimensions may be good for the development of infant.
Nerve endings in skin are rich. Alberto Gallace's review showed that tactile sensations in interaction can have surprisingly powerful effects on people's behaviors and emotions. Specifically, touch appears to be capable of modulating people's compliance [52]. So we speculated one possible mechanism: the fetus lives in the warm amniotic fluid and tactile stimulation through the abdomen may enhance the flow of amniotic fluid and produces gentle stimulation on the fetal skin. This may satisfy the feeling of the fetus, which may be conducive to the formation of the easy type of infant temperament. On the other hand, when the pregnant woman gives tactile stimulation on the fetus, she may feel fetus' response more and her abdomen also receives more massage than often. These may relieve her anxiety and depression and reduce the level of cortisol in blood and amniotic fluid, which is believed to be good for the fetal psychological development and another possible mechanism [37]. However, considering that this is a retrospective research, further long-term studies are necessary to testify them.
Meanwhile there was no notable difference in the ratio of easy type between irregular practice group and no practice group, which indicated that irregular tactile stimulation has no significant effect on the formation of the easy type of infant temperament. Although the ratio of difficult type in irregular practice group was higher than other 2 groups, there was no significant difference among 3 groups. We speculated that there were two underlying reasons for this result: the sample size was not big enough; there was no relative effect indeed.
Although the present study illuminates that regular gentle tactile stimulation on fetus may promote the formation of easy type infant temperament, it is necessary to indicate some limitations of this study. Since this is retrospective design with potential for confounding of maternal behavior pre- and postnatally and maternal reporting of what happened in pregnancy may be influenced by what she is doing postnatally, then it is possible that mothers who massage their infants prenatally become attuned to their infant's cues and may be more sensitive. Besides, the roles of genetics, maternal personality, temperament, and mother's anxiety trait were not assessed at baseline when the intervention took place. However, it has been observed that maternal trait anxiety in the third trimester of pregnancy predicts infant difficult temperament when babies are four and six month old, independently of mother's antenatal and postnatal depression levels [30]. Therefore, the fact that in this study infants who received regular practice of tactile stimulation during pregnancy revealed more often easy temperament might be a result of mother's lower anxiety levels [53] already present during pregnancy, rather than the tactile stimulation intervention. Further studies will include this point to validate the findings of the study.
In summary, our study shows that regular gentle tactile stimulation on fetus may promote the formation of easy type infant temperament. Fetus undergoing regular gentle tactile stimulation show lower negative mood, improved notably higher approach/withdrawal, adaptability, and persistence. Further study and assessments should be done to deeply evaluate the regular gentle tactile stimulation on fetus in the formation of easy type infant temperament.
Acknowledgment
At the point of finishing this paper, the authors would like to express their gratitude to their colleague in Xin Hua Hospital who offered them the software to assess the temperament of the infants.
Disclosure
Zhe-Wei Wang and Jing Hua are regarded as co-first author.
Conflict of Interests
The authors have no conflict of interests to declare and no competing interests with other organizations.
References
- 1.Goldsmith H. H., Buss A. H., Plomin R., et al. Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development. 1987;58(2):505–529. doi: 10.2307/1130527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Rothbart M. K., Derryberry D. Advances in Developmental Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Erlbaum; 1981. Development of individual differences in temperament. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wachs T. D. Child Psychology: A Handbook of Contemporary Issues. New York, NY, USA: Psychology Press; 2006. The nature, etiology, and consequences of individual differences in temperament; pp. 27–52. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Britton J. R. Infant temperament and maternal anxiety and depressed mood in the early postpartum period. Women & Health. 2011;51(1):55–71. doi: 10.1080/03630242.2011.540741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lagercrantz H., Forssberg H. Functional development of the brain in the fetus and the newborn infant. Nordisk Medicin. 1991;106(10):264–268. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Graven S. N., Browne J. V. Sensory development in the fetus, neonate, and infant: introduction and overview. Newborn and Infant Nursing Reviews. 2008;8(4):169–172. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2008.10.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kosińska-Kaczyńska K., Wielgoś M. Is fetus able to feel pain? Ginekologia Polska. 2011;82(2):133–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kenner C., Lubbe W. Fetal stimulation—a preventative therapy. Newborn and Infant Nursing Reviews. 2007;7(4):227–230. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2007.06.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kisilevsky B. S., Hains S. M. J., Brown C. A., et al. Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behavior and Development. 2009;32(1):59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Meyer G. J., Finn S. E., Eyde L. D., et al. Psychological testing and psychological assessment: a review of evidence and issues. American Psychologist. 2001;56(2):128–165. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.56.2.128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Montagu A. Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. New York, NY, USA: Columbia University Press; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Von Knorring A.-L., Söderberg A., Austin L., Uvnäs-Moberg K. Massage decreases aggression in preschool children: a long-term study. Acta Paediatrica. 2008;97(9):1265–1269. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00919.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lau C., Fucile S., Gisel E. G. Impact of nonnutritive oral motor stimulation and infant massage therapy on oral feeding skills of preterm infants. Journal of Neonatal-Perinatal Medicine. 2012;5(4):311–317. doi: 10.3233/npm-1262612. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Badiee Z., Samsamshariat S., Pourmorshed P. Massage therapy by mother or nurse: effect on weight gain in premature infants. Journal of Isfahan Medical School. 2011;29(144):804–810. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Underdown A., Barlow J., Stewart-Brown S. Tactile stimulation in physically healthy infants: results of a systematic review. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 2010;28(1):11–29. doi: 10.1080/02646830903247209. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kulkarni A., Kaushik J. S., Gupta P., Sharma H., Agrawal R. K. Massage and touch therapy in neonates: the current evidence. Indian Pediatrics. 2010;47(9):771–776. doi: 10.1007/s13312-010-0114-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Field T., Hernandez-Reif M., Diego M., Feijo L., Vera Y., Gil K. Massage therapy by parents improves early growth and development. Infant Behavior and Development. 2004;27(4):435–442. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Agarwal K. N., Gupta A., Pushkarna R., Bhargava S. K., Faridi M. M. A., Prabhu M. K. Effects of massage & use of oil on growth, blood flow & sleep pattern in infants. The Indian Journal of Medical Research. 2000;112:212–217. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Field T. Massage therapy for infants and children. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 1995;16(2):105–111. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Serrano M. S. C., Doren F. M., Wilson L. Teaching chilean mothers to massage their full-term infants: effects on maternal breast-feeding and infant weight gain at age 2 and 4 months. The Journal of Perinatal & Neonatal Nursing. 2010;24(2):172–181. doi: 10.1097/jpn.0b013e3181db5377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gitau R., Modi N., Gianakoulopoulos X., Bond C., Glover V., Stevenson J. Acute effects of maternal skin-to-skin contact and massage on saliva cortisol in preterm babies. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology. 2002;20(2):83–88. doi: 10.1080/02646830220134595. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kelmanson I. A., Adulas E. I. Massage therapy and sleep behaviour in infants born with low birth weight. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice. 2006;12(3):200–205. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2005.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lorenz L., Moyse K., Surguy H. The benefits of baby massage: a scheme to improve young children's physical health and psychological development by introducing baby massage is described by members of a Sure Start team. Paediatric Care. 2005;17:15–18. doi: 10.7748/paed2005.03.17.2.15.c967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bennett C., Underdown A., Barlow J. Massage for promoting mental and physical health in typically developing infants under the age of six months. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2013;(4) doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD005038.pub3.CD005038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Field T., Diego M., Hernandez-Reif M. Massage therapy research. Developmental Review. 2007;27(1):75–89. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.12.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Field T. M., Schanberg S. M., Scafidi F., et al. Tactile/kinesthetic stimulation effects on preterm neonates. Pediatrics. 1986;77(5):654–658. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Abdallah B., Badr L. K., Hawwari M. The efficacy of massage on short and long term outcomes in preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013;36(4):662–669. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hernandez-Reif M., Diego M., Field T. Preterm infants show reduced stress behaviors and activity after 5 days of massage therapy. Infant Behavior and Development. 2007;30(4):557–561. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Procianoy R. S., Mendes E. W., Silveira R. C. Massage therapy improves neurodevelopment outcome at two years corrected age for very low birth weight infants. Early Human Development. 2010;86(1):7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Austin M.-P., Hadzi-Pavlovic D., Leader L., Saint K., Parker G. Maternal trait anxiety, depression and life event stress in pregnancy: relationships with infant temperament. Early Human Development. 2005;81(2):183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Macedo A., Marques M., Bos S., et al. Mother's personality and infant temperament. Infant Behavior and Development. 2011;34(4):552–568. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jaddoe V. W. V., Van Duijn C. M., Van Der Heijden A. J., et al. The generation R study: design and cohort update 2010. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2010;25(11):823–841. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9516-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.de Lauzon-Guillain B., Wijndaele K., Clark M., et al. Breastfeeding and infant temperament at age three months. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029326.e29326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Blair M. M., Glynn L. M., Sandman C. A., Davis E. P. Prenatal maternal anxiety and early childhood temperament. Stress. 2011;14(6):644–651. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.594121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cranley M. S. Development of a tool for the measurement of maternal attachment during pregnancy. Nursing Research. 1981;30(5):281–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Field T., Diego M., Hernandez-Reif M., Medina L., Delgado J., Hernandez A. Yoga and massage therapy reduce prenatal depression and prematurity. Journal of Bodywork and Movement Therapies. 2012;16(2):204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2011.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Baibazarova E., van de Beek C., Cohen-Kettenis P. T., Buitelaar J., Shelton K. H., van Goozen S. H. M. Influence of prenatal maternal stress, maternal plasma cortisol and cortisol in the amniotic fluid on birth outcomes and child temperament at 3 months. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(6):907–915. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Medoff-Cooper B., Carey W. B., McDevitt S. C. The early infancy temperament questionnaire. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 1993;14(4):230–235. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gong Y.-H., Ji C.-Y., Shan J.-P. Longitudinal study on infants' temperament and physical development in Beijing, China. International Journal of Nursing Practice. 2013;19(5):487–497. doi: 10.1111/ijn.12104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.De Schipper J. C., Tavecchio L. W. C., Van IJzendoorn M. H., Van Zeijl J. Goodness-of-fit in center day care: relations of temperament, stability, and quality care with the child's adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2004;19(2):257–272. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kingston L., Prior M. The development of patterns of stable, transient, and school-age onset aggressive behavior in young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1995;34(3):348–358. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199503000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Andersson H. W., Sommerfelt K. Infant temperamental factors as predictors of problem behavior and IQ at age 5 years: interactional effects of biological and social risk factors. Child Study Journal. 1999;29:207–226. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Tschann J. M., Kaiser P., Chesney M. A., Alkon A., Boyce W. T. Resilience and vulnerability among preschool children: family functioning, temperament, and behavior problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(2):184–192. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199602000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Esparó G., Canals J., Torrente M., Fernández-Ballart J. D. Psychological problems and associated factors at 6 years of age: differences between sexes. The Spanish Journal of Psychology. 2004;7(1):53–62. doi: 10.1017/s1138741600004741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sakimura J. N., Dang M. T., Ballard K. B., Hansen R. L. Cognitive and temperament clusters in 3- to 5-year-old children with aggressive behavior: research article. Journal of School Health. 2008;78(1):38–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00264.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Veenstra R., Lindenberg S., Oldehinkel A. J., de Winter A. F., Ormel J. Temperament, environment, and antisocial behavior in a population sample of preadolescent boys and girls. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2006;30(5):422–432. doi: 10.1177/0165025406071490. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bussing R., Gary F. A., Mason D. M., Leon C. E., Sinha K., Garvan C. W. Child temperament, ADHD, and caregiver strain: exploring relationships in an epidemiological sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2003;42(2):184–192. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200302000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Derauf C., Lagasse L., Smith L., et al. Infant temperament and high-risk environment relate to behavior problems and language in toddlers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2011;32(2):125–135. doi: 10.1097/dbp.0b013e31820839d7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Spruyt K., Aitken R. J., So K., Charlton M., Adamson T. M., Horne R. S. C. Relationship between sleep/wake patterns, temperament and overall development in term infants over the first year of life. Early Human Development. 2008;84(5):289–296. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Banerjee P. N., Tamis-LeMonda C. S. Infants' persistence and mothers' teaching as predictors of toddlers' cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development. 2007;30(3):479–491. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dixon W. E., Jr., Smith P. H. Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2000;46(3):417–440. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Gallace A., Spence C. The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(2):246–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Buitelaar J. K., Huizink A. C., Mulder E. J., de Medina P. G. R., Visser G. H. A. Prenatal stress and cognitive development and temperament in infants. Neurobiology of Aging. 2003;24(supplement 1):S53–S60. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(03)00050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]