Abstract
The ability to imitate complex sounds is rare, and among birds has been found only in parrots, songbirds, and hummingbirds. Parrots exhibit the most advanced vocal mimicry among non-human animals. A few studies have noted differences in connectivity, brain position and shape in the vocal learning systems of parrots relative to songbirds and hummingbirds. However, only one parrot species, the budgerigar, has been examined and no differences in the presence of song system structures were found with other avian vocal learners. Motivated by questions of whether there are important differences in the vocal systems of parrots relative to other vocal learners, we used specialized constitutive gene expression, singing-driven gene expression, and neural connectivity tracing experiments to further characterize the song system of budgerigars and/or other parrots. We found that the parrot brain uniquely contains a song system within a song system. The parrot “core” song system is similar to the song systems of songbirds and hummingbirds, whereas the “shell” song system is unique to parrots. The core with only rudimentary shell regions were found in the New Zealand kea, representing one of the only living species at a basal divergence with all other parrots, implying that parrots evolved vocal learning systems at least 29 million years ago. Relative size differences in the core and shell regions occur among species, which we suggest could be related to species differences in vocal and cognitive abilities.
Introduction
Vocal learning, a critical behavior for spoken language, is the ability to modify acoustic and/or syntactic features of sounds produced, including improvisation and imitation. Complex vocal learning has so far been found in five distantly related groups of mammals (humans, bats, elephants, cetaceans [dolphins and whales], and pinnipeds [seals and sea lions]) and three distantly related groups of birds (parrots, songbirds, and hummingbirds) [1–5]. Vocal learning is thought to have evolved to allow for more complex communication and cultural transmission of learned conspecific vocal repertoires that are important for social cohesion [2,6]. Because complex vocal learning is not found in their close relatives, it is generally thought that each vocal learning lineage evolved this trait independently [1,2]. Recent studies have placed parrots as a sister group to passeriform songbirds [7,8], leading to an alternative interpretation of a common origin of complex vocal learning in parrots and oscine songbirds, followed by two subsequent losses in suboscines and New Zealand Wrens.
Interestingly, not all vocal learners display vocal mimicry, defined as the ability to imitate heterospecific vocalizations [9–13]. Species that readily imitate other species include parrots, such as the African Grey (Psittacus erithacus), and corvid songbirds, such as crows and the Greater Indian Hill Mynahs (Gracula religiosa intermedia), and humans [14–16]. Some vocal learning bird species have a remarkable ability to imitate rudimentary human speech [10,15,17,18]. Vocal mimicry has also been observed in some mammalian vocal learners, including speech or whistle imitation in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), killer whales (Orcinus orca), harbor seals (Phoca vitulina), and elephants [13,19–23], but none have been reported to have the ability to mimic human speech sounds as well as parrots.
The neural pathways for vocal learning have been characterized in multiple songbird and hummingbird species, but in only one parrot species, the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) [1,24–29]. In these species, immediate early genes (IEGs), tracers, brain region inactivation and electrophysiology recordings (with the exception in hummingbirds) have been used to identify up to seven cerebral (telencephalic) vocal nuclei each, which are considered analogous in all three avian lineages (Fig 1, yellow and red [24–27,30–34]). The seven cerebral song nuclei are distributed within two pathways [24,27,30] best characterized in songbirds as: (1) an anterior song pathway that forms a pallial-basal-ganglia-thalamic loop and influences vocal learning, syntax, and social context functions (Fig 1, red); and (2) a posterior song pathway that influences the production of learned song (Fig 1, yellow). In all three groups, the song nuclei of both pathways are embedded within or adjacent to non-vocal movement activated brain regions (Fig 1, green), a finding that led to the motor theory of vocal learning origin, where vocal learning pathways are proposed to have evolved from a pre-existing ancestral motor learning pathway shared by vertebrates [35].
Thus far, differences found between vocal learning bird species include that the posterior vocal motor pathway is also anatomically adjacent to the auditory pathway in songbirds, but more distant to it in hummingbirds and parrots (Fig 1, blue) [24,27]. The connectivity between the posterior and anterior pathways is more different between songbirds and parrots than within each subpathway (tracing experiments in hummingbirds are not extensive) [5]. Another difference is that besides the caudal auditory pathway regions’ (e.g. Field L) input into the posterior vocal pathway as in songbirds, in the parrot, auditory neurons within the somatosensory nucleus basorostralis [B] also send input into the overlying posterior vocal pathway song nucleus LAN (lateral nucleus of the anterior nidopallium; analog of songbird NIf, interfacial nucleus of the nidopallium) [24,32,36]. However, the tracing studies in budgerigars have only identified a sub-region of the anterior vocal nuclei activated by vocalizing-driven gene expression, called the core anterior nuclei in Jarvis and Mello [24]. The vocalizing-driven gene expression studies revealed a surround region of activation for the anterior pathway nuclei, not specifically noted in tracing studies published before or after. Further, the parrot posterior vocal motor pathway nuclei NLC (central nucleus of the lateral nidopallium) and AAC (central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium) contain anatomically distinct dorsal and ventral subdivisions as described in tracing studies [32,34] that also show vocalizing-induced gene expression [24,35]. However, the areas surrounding the NLC described as the “shell”, “NLs” (supracentral nucleus of the lateral nidopallium), and “NLv” (ventral lateral nidopallium) in Durand et al. [34] appear to encompass the motor areas surrounding the NLC described in Feenders et al. [35]. This has caused considerable confusion in the field as to the organization and terminology of the parrot song nuclei and attempts to resolve the differences and similarities have been few [24,31,34]. Although, differences in connectivity, position, or shape of song nuclei have been noted between parrots compared to songbirds and hummingbirds [5,24,32,34], no differences have been noted in the presence or absence of song system nuclei.
To better understand the organization of the song system in parrots, its evolution, and specialized molecular properties relative to other vocal learning avian species, we performed analyses of brain gene expression profiles in nine different parrot species representing all the three superfamilies, Strigopoidea, Cacatuoidea, and Psittacoidea, for genes with known specialized expression in vocal learning nuclei, and compared those findings with vocalizing-driven gene expression and neural tracing experiments. The set of genes that show specialized expression, meaning higher or lower expression in the song nuclei compared to the surrounding regions, include among many others Parvalbumin (PVALB), glutamate receptors, FOXP1, and SLIT1 [37–39]. These analyses led us to hypothesize that the parrot vocal learning system contains “core” and “shell” region specializations, with the core system similar to the song system of songbirds and hummingbirds and the shell system unique to parrots. This dual system evolved early in the parrot lineage and has lasted and expanded for millions of years in different species.
Results
We performed a series of experiments that led us to conclude that brain regions that include song nuclei of parrots have three concentric levels of specializations: (1) the inner core song nuclei; (2) the outer shell song nuclei that surround the cores; and (3) the outer non-vocal motor regions that surround the song nuclei shells. We described our more in depth findings in budgerigars first, and then comparisons with other parrot species.
Gene expression profiles reveal core and shell regions within the cortical-like song nuclei of budgerigars
We first performed anatomical gene expression analyses for the PVALB gene, which generates a calcium-binding protein with a role in neuroprotection and critical period plasticity, preferentially in GABA-expressing interneurons [40,41]. In contrast to our prior study [38], here we processed sections for PVALB mRNA expression serially throughout the budgerigar forebrain. We found that PVALB expression revealed core (very high) and shell (moderate) regions of expression that included the posterior song nuclei NLC and AAC and surrounding motor areas relative to the surrounding nidopallium (N) and arcopallium (A), respectively (Fig 2a; serial sections in 2b). The PVALB-defined cores were shaped like spheres, had Nissl-defined boundaries, were positioned dorsally within the shells, and were largest in size in sections in the middle of the song nuclei (Fig 2b, sections viii to xiii; S1 Fig), gradually diminishing in size in the anterior and posterior directions. The PVALB-defined shells surrounded the cores dorsally and ventrally (Fig 2a). The shell regions were much larger in ventral, anterior, and posterior directions, than dorsally (Fig 2b, sections i to vii and xiv to xvi). The boundaries of the PVALB-defined shells were not detectable in Nissl staining, but within them we noted less prominent Nissl-defined boundaries, which correspond to what we call in this study shell song nuclei (Fig 2; S1 Fig).
We also noted higher PVALB expression in the primary sensory neural populations of the telencephalon and thalamus. These included the somatosensory basorostralis [B], visual entopallium [E], auditory L2 [not shown], and visual intercalated hyperpallium [IH] in the telencephalon and the visual nucleus rotundus (Rt) in the thalamus. There was also higher expression at the boundaries of the ventral mesopallium (MV) with the adjacent nidopallium and the dorsal mesopallium (MD) with the intercalated hyperpallium (Fig 2a and 2b; terminology is according to a recently revised nomenclature and understanding of avian brain organization [42,43]).
In contrast to the posterior song nuclei, we discovered high PVALB expression in only the well-defined core regions of the anterior song nuclei MO (oval nucleus of the mesopallium) and NAO (oval nucleus of the anterior nidopallium) (Fig 3a–3c). The MO core nucleus was distinguishable with Nissl staining, shaped like an egg sitting on its side, with its narrower end pointed towards the midline, comprised of large, loosely distributed cells relative to the surrounding mesopallium (S2 Fig). The NAO core was positioned more anterior and sat directly adjacent to the somatosensory basorostralis also of higher PVALB expression. The MO and NAO shells, like NLC and AAC shells, were partly distinguishable in Nissl from the surrounding motor mesopallium and nidopallium, respectively, by their shared cellular orientation (S2 Fig) as previously noted [31,32,34]. We did not detect core and shell region specializations of PVALB expression within the striatal song nucleus, MMSt (magnocellular nucleus of the medial striatum). However, we noted a uniformly moderate higher expression than the surrounding striatum (St) in sections that spanned the MMSt song nucleus rostro-caudally (Fig 3c, sections ix to xv).
Functional activation of core and shell regions
To determine the functional makeup of the PVALB-defined core and shell regions, and whether core and shell song nuclei regions were confined to the Nissl-defined boundaries we identified, we performed side-by-side comparisons of comparable serial brain sections that we had collected with vocalizing-driven (warble song) and non-singing motor-driven (hopping) DUSP1, EGR-1 (also known as ZENK), and C-FOS IEG expression [24,35,44,45]. DUSP1 is an activity-dependent dual sensitivity phosphatase that shows motor-driven gene expression only in song nuclei of vocal learners and sensory-driven expression in the primary sensory neurons in all species [45], whereas EGR-1 and C-FOS are activity-dependent transcription factors that show behaviorally-driven expression in many cell types, including in song nuclei and the surrounding motor and auditory regions [24,35,44,46].
We found that the functionally defined, vocalizing-driven EGR-1 and C-FOS boundaries encompassed the entire PVALB-defined cores and part of the PVALB-defined shells of the NLC and AAC (Fig 4a, sections i, iii; Fig 5a, sections i-ii). The vocalizing-driven DUSP1 expression overlapped similarly with PVALB, with higher expression in AAC than EGR-1 or C-FOS (Fig 4a, sections ii-ii). The remainder of the PVALB-defined shells extended beyond the vocalizing-driven functionally-defined boundaries of the NLC and AAC, and overlapped with the non-vocal movement-driven EGR-1 and C-FOS gene expression regions, including what has been called the supra lateral nidopallium (SLN) around the NLC shell and the lateral intermediate arcopallium (LAI) around the AAC shell (Fig 4a, sections v-vi; Fig 5a, sections i-iii). In animals that heard warble song but did not vocalize and sat relatively still, there was no detectable EGR-1 or C-FOS (DUSP1 not shown) induction in the PVALB-defined song nuclei and surround (Fig 4a, section iv; Fig 5a, section iv). Therefore, we conclude that there are two levels of PVALB-defined specializations involving the posterior NLC and AAC song nuclei: the PVALB-defined song nuclei cores (high expression) and shells (moderate to low expression) that are vocally active; and the remainder of the PVALB-defined shells (moderate expression) that are non-vocal motor active regions (Fig 4a, section vii).
In the anterior song nuclei MO and NAO, the functionally defined, vocalizing-driven EGR-1 and C-FOS regions encompassed the entire PVALB-defined core (Fig 4b, sections i,iii,iv; Fig 5b, sections i-iii). The vocalizing-driven EGR-1 and C-FOS expression defined the boundaries of the song nuclei shells (Fig 4b, section i) with the movement-driven expression-defined regions of the anterior ventral mesopallium (AMV) and anterior nidopallium (AN) (Fig 4b, section v). Vocalizing-driven DUSP1 expression was highest in the cores but exhibited lower expression in the anterior song nuclei shells (Fig 4b, section ii). Therefore, in contrast to the posterior song nuclei, there is only one level of PVALB-defined specialization involving the anterior song nuclei: the PVALB-defined cores (high expression). The anterior song nuclei shell regions share non-specialized PVALB expression with the surrounding motor regions (Fig 4b, section vi).
Songbirds and hummingbirds do not have shell song nuclei
Core versus shell gene expression specializations have not been described in the ‘vocal-motor active’ brain regions of songbirds or hummingbirds. To validate whether the vocal-motor pattern we found is unique to parrots, we examined published and previously unpublished serial sections containing song nuclei that were hybridized for PVALB and for vocalizing-driven DUSP1, C-FOS, or EGR-1 expression from our studies in songbirds and hummingbirds [35,38,44,45] namely the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) and the Anna’s hummingbird (Calypte anna), respectively.
In the zebra finch we did not find evidence of differential core and shell PVALB expression “within” the song nuclei. Instead, in support of previous conclusions [38], the differential PVALB expression was uniformly high throughout each song nucleus (Fig 6a and 6b), and restricted to the vocalizing-driven gene expression defined boundaries (Fig 6c and 6d); these were HVC (a vocal nucleus, no abbreviation), RA (robust nucleus of the arcopallium), and LMAN (lateral part of the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium), analogs of parrot NLC, AAC, and NAO, respectively. We did not note differential PVALB expression within the MO analog of songbirds in the mesopallium, which is relatively much smaller in songbirds (Fig 6b and 6d). Previous studies have characterized areas surrounding songbird song nuclei such as the HVC shelf, LMAN shell, and RA cup, parts of which are known to be auditory [47–50] and/or non-vocal motor [35,45] in function, or proposed to be accessory to the song system [51,52]. In this regard, we noted that the banana-shaped intermediate lateral arcopallium (LAI) directly adjacent to RA, part of which controls movement [35,50], had moderately higher levels of PVALB expression relative to the remaining arcopallium (Fig 6a) similar to the non-vocal motor LAI regions adjacent to the parrot AAC shell. We did not note any additional PVALB specializations restricted to the non-vocal HVC shelf or LMAN shell.
In the hummingbird, as in songbirds, differential PVALB expression in each song nucleus was uniformly high relative to the immediate adjacent surrounding non-song regions (Fig 6e and 6f); these were VLN (vocal nucleus of the lateral nidopallium), VA (vocal nucleus of the arcopallium), and VAN (vocal nucleus of the anterior nidopallium), analogs of parrot NLC, AAC, and NAO, respectively. PVALB expression was restricted to the vocalizing-driven gene expression defined boundaries (Fig 6g and 6h), and not present in VAM (vocal nucleus of the anterior mesopallium), analog of parrot MO (Fig 6f and 6h). As in songbirds and parrots, the immediate surrounding Ai (intermediate arcopallium) region that includes the movement activated part (LAI) [35] exhibited intermediate PVALB levels (Figs 4, 6e). In contrast, the hummingbird VAN (budgerigar NAO analog) includes a dorsocaudal region lacking vocalizing-driven EGR-1 expression (Fig 6h) as noted previously [27]. But this region exhibits higher PVALB expression (Fig 6f), similar to the more anterior and larger sized vocalization-driven EGR-1 expression part (Fig 6h). Overall, these findings suggest that the parrot shell song regions are unique among avian vocal learners.
Multiple genes show shell and core region specializations in budgerigar
We wondered if our findings were specific to PVALB or would other genes that are differentially expressed in song nuclei of songbirds also show differential expression in core versus shell regions of parrots. Including PVALB, we identified 10 genes from the past and recent literature, including that of our own studies, with differential mRNA or protein expression in core versus shell song nuclei or adjacent non-vocal motor regions of budgerigars (Table 1).
Table 1. Summary of gene expression specializations in budgerigar.
NLC core | NLC shell | NLC motor | AAC core | AAC shell | AAC motor | MO core | MO shell | NAO core | NAO shell | MMSt | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | |||||||||||
PVALB | +++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | +++ | + | ||
SLIT1 | --- | ||||||||||
FOXP1 | ++ | + | |||||||||
NR2A | +++ | + | |||||||||
GLUR1 | -- | ||||||||||
NADPH-d | +++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | +++ | + | ++ | |
AR | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | ++ | +++ | ++ | ||||
mENK fiber | +++ | ++ | +++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | +++ | + | ++ | ||
mENK cell | ++ | ++ | ++ | ||||||||
TH fiber | -- | - | -- | -- | -- | - | - | + | |||
CGRP-LI fibers | ++ | ++ | + | + | +++ | +++ | + | + | |||
Activity | |||||||||||
DUSP1 | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | + | +++ | ++ | |||
C-FOS | +++ | +++ | +++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ |
EGR1 | +++ | +++ | +++ | + | + | + | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ | +++ |
The data represent qualitative analyses of gene expression (mRNA or protein) labeling in the song nucleus relative to the surround of that same brain subdivision conducted in this and other studies. + = Relative level of expression higher than the surrounding non-vocal or non-motor areas;— = lower expression; no value entered means no difference relative to the surrounding areas. The other studies are: PVALB, Hara et al. [38]; SLIT1, Wang et al. [39]; FOXP1, Haesler et al. [54]; EGR-1, Feenders et al. [35]; NR2A, Wada et al. [56]; GLUR1, Wada et al. [56]; NADPH-d, Brauth et al. [57]; AR, Matsunaga and Okanoya [60]; mENK, Durand et al. [58]; TH, Roberts et al. [61]; CGRP-LI, Durand et al. [59]; DUSP1, Horita et al. [45], C-FOS, Jarvis [44]; EGR-1, Jarvis and Mello [24]. The NR2A mRNA levels are also consistent with protein levels detected by immunocytochemistry [85].
SLIT1 is an axon guidance molecule that we had recently found was downregulated in songbird RA, hummingbird VA, and budgerigar dorsal AAC (AACd) [37,39]. We found here that the budgerigar region with SLIT1 downregulation corresponds to our PVALB-defined AAC core, and that SLIT1 expression in the PVALB-labeled AAC shell was not different relative to the surrounding arcopallium (Fig 7a and 7b). The FOXP1 transcription factor, a co-factor of FOXP2 that is required for speech and song acquisition in humans and songbirds [53], was reported to be differentially expressed in the NLC analog of vocal learners [35,54]; we found here that it is expressed in a gradient of higher to lower expression from the NLC core to the shell, but only for the part of the shell that corresponds to the vocalizing-driven song nucleus (Fig 7c and 7d). Interestingly, FOXP1 has a ring of lower expression around the MMSt song nucleus, but this is thought to be a non-vocal motor part of the striatum around MMSt [35,55]. NR2A glutamate receptor upregulation (Fig 8a and 8b) and GLUR1 downregulation in NLC [56] (Fig 8c and 8d) corresponds to our PVALB-defined NLC core. The NADPH-d enzyme pattern [57] resembles the PVALB pattern, with the exception that the NAO shell shows some specialized expression as well (Fig 9a and 9c). Methionine Enkephalin (mENK) fiber staining [58] also shows a similar pattern (Fig 9b and 9d). The CGRP-LI (Calcitonin gene-related peptide-like immunoreactivity) fiber staining [59] shows a diverse pattern, with comparable high density of cell body and fiber labeling in the NLC and AAC cores and shells, and barely any in the surrounding motor areas (Fig 10a and 10b); and a much higher density of fiber labeling than cell body labeling in the MO and NAO cores, less in the shells, and very little in the surrounding motor areas (Fig 10c and 10d). Matsunaga et al. [60] found higher androgen receptor (AR) expression in what we define here as both NLC, AAC, and NAO core and shells, and only the MO core (Table 1). Roberts et al. [61] found higher density of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) fiber staining in the ventral part of NLC, which appears to correspond here to the PVALB-defined NLC shell and the motor part around the NLC shell compared to the NLC core where TH fibers were sparse (Table 1).
In songbirds and hummingbirds, we note that the song nuclei patterns of SLIT1 [39], FOXP1 [35,54], NR2A [56], and GLUR1 [56] are similar to the core regions of the analogous nuclei in budgerigars. Overall, these results indicate that in budgerigars, the core and shell song regions, and the adjacent motor regions, have diverse patterns of specialized gene expression, and that the cores share more similar gene expression profiles with songbird and hummingbird song nuclei.
Core and shell song regions show differences in connectivity
To investigate whether the gene expression-defined core and shell regions of the budgerigar song nuclei have similar or different neural connectivity, we collected and analyzed published and unpublished neural tracing data of male budgerigars, consisting of several thousand brain sections and performed side-by-side comparisons with the serial brain sections processed for PVALB, SLIT1, FOXP1, and vocalizing-driven IEG expression [24,31–34,38,45,54] (see methods, Table 2). Broadly, we noted that what had been called NLC in the prior connectivity studies consisted of both core and shell NLC regions defined by the gene expression profiles here (Table 3). What had been called dorsal AAC (AACd) corresponds to our AAC core; what had been called ventral AAC (AACv) contains part of our AAC shell. The previously named NLs overlying the NLC lies within the motor activated part outside of our NLC shell. What had been called NAO and MO corresponds only to our NAO and MO cores respectively (Table 3). The NAO and MO shell regions in previous connectivity studies were assumed to not belong to the song nuclei, which is understandable considering that they did not readily look conspicuous in Nissl staining. The regions identified in the connectivity studies were not tested functionally with vocal behavior, but they were tested with vocalizing-driven IEG expression [24], and when compared, the connectivity of the shells had some significant differences to the core nuclei. We present specific examples of one posterior and one anterior song nucleus below.
Table 2. Summary of neural connectivity findings.
Injection site | n = | AAC core | AAC shell | NLC core | NLC shell | MO core | MO shell | NAOcore | NAO shell |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AAC core | 3 | +++ | +o | ooo | +++o | + | ++ | ||
AAC shell | 1 | +++ | ooo | ++o | ++oo | ||||
NLC core | 1 | +++ | +++ | o | |||||
NLC shell | 4 | +++ | +++ | ? | ? | ? | ? | ||
MO core | 3 | + | ++ | +++ | ++o | +++ | + | ||
MO shell | 3 | + | + | +o | +++ | +o | +++o | ||
NAO core | 4 | oo | ++ | +++ | ++ | +++ | + | ||
NAO shell | 4 | +o | + | +oo | +++ |
Sample sizes are number of animals with injections. Listed are qualitative analyses of relative degree of staining of fibers (+) and/or labeled cells (o) present in the song nucleus; no value indicates that no labeled fibers or cells were visualized in these areas. See Figs 11–14 for representative examples of tracer injection sites and spread of tracer and results for explanation of findings and sources of experiments.
Table 3. List of abbreviated names of song nuclei used in describing the parrot song system by various studies interpreted relative to our study.
Paton et al. 1981 | Striedter, 1994 | Durand et al. 1997 | Jarvis et al. 2000 | Reiner et al 2004, Jarvis et al 2005 | Present study |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
HVc | NLc | NLc | NLc | NLC | NLC core |
NLc | NLc | NLc | NLC | NLC shell | |
RA | AAc | AAcd | AAcd | AAcd | AAC core |
AAc | AAcv* | AAcv* | AAcv | AAC shell | |
HVo | HVo | HVo core* | MO core | MO core | |
HVo surround* | MO surround | MO shell | |||
MAN | NAo | NAo | NAo core* | NAO core | NAO core |
MAN | NAs* | NAom* | NAo surround* | NAO surround | NAO shell |
* NAs defined in Striedter [32] is a small portion of what we find as the NAO shell, in which authors said was analogous to songbird LMAN. NAom defined in Durand et al. [34] is another small portion of what we define as NAO shell. NAo core and surround and HVo core and surround in Jarvis et al. [24] was together called the NAo and HVo complex. AAcv in Durand et al. [34] and Jarvis et al. [24] comprised the ventral part of the AAC shell described here.
When analyzed in the context of our differential and functionally-defined (vocalizing-driven) gene expression profiles and Nissl-defined patterns, we found that targeted biocytin injections into the AAC core (Fig 11a) with very little leakage in the shell retrogradely labeled a high density of cell bodies and some anterogradely labeled fibers within the PVALB-defined (from strongest to weakest) NLC core, MO core, and NAO core relative to the shell regions (Fig 11b–11j; Table 2). We did not observe fiber labeling in the MMSt region (Fig 11h) consistent with prior studies [34]. Targeted biocytin injections within the AAC shell labeled cell bodies and some fibers in the NLC shell and NAO shell, and mostly fibers in the MO core (Figs 6b, 6c and 7 of Durand et al. [34]; Table 2). Conversely, targeted biocytin injections within the NLC shell (Fig 12a) with little to no leakage in the core (Fig 12a and 12b) labeled a high density of fibers in the AAC shell and NLC shell (Fig 12c–12h; Table 2), supporting the shell-to-shell connectivity. We did not observe labeled fibers within the AAC core (Fig 12i). These findings are consistent with those of Striedter [32], who showed topographic connectivity between the AAC and NLC song nuclei, which we now define as the AAC and NLC cores and shells.
Targeted biocytin injections in the PVALB-defined MO core (Fig 13a and 13b) labeled a higher density of fibers within (from strongest to weakest) the NAO core (Fig 13c and 13e), NLC core (Fig 13h and 13k), and AAC core (Fig 13h and 13i) relative to their shells (Fig 13f, 13j and 13l). There were also fiber projections and filled cell bodies in the MO shell (Fig 13c and 13d) and MMSt of the striatum (Fig 13g). Targeted BDA (biotinylated dextran amine) injections within the MO shell (Fig 14a) with little to no leakage in the core, labeled fibers and some cell bodies within the MO core (Fig 14b) confirming the local connectivity finding. However, there were greater amounts of labeled fibers and some cell bodies in the NAO shell (Fig 14c), than in the NAO core (Fig 14d), and fibers only in the NLC and AAC shells (Fig 14e–14j; Table 2). These findings suggest a strong core-to-core connectivity of the MO with the other core song nuclei, strong shell-to-shell connectivity of MO with the other shell song nuclei, and a weaker local MO and NAO shell with core connectivity.
Available published data for the remaining song nuclei also support parallel core and shell systems. For example, injections of dual anterograde+retrograde fluorescent dextran amines targeted to the NAO core (i.e. NAom) by Durand et al [34], labeled cells mostly confined to what we define as the AAC core, but with a smaller minority in the shell (Table 2; and Fig 15A and 15B of that study). Injections of fluorescent dextran amines in the NAO shell labeled cells and fibers in the AAC shell (Table 2; Fig 15A and 15D of that study).
Taken together, these findings indicate that the core regions of NLC, AAC, MO, and NAO are primarily interconnected to each other whereas the shell regions are primarily interconnected to each other. There are sparser connections between cores and shells within and among each song nucleus.
Identification of song nuclei specializations in other parrot species
We wondered if the core and shell song nuclei organization was unique to budgerigars, or to parrots generally. Using PVALB mRNA expression, we discovered the presence of similarly labeled song nuclei-like regions in eight other parrot species, namely the yellow lored Amazon (Amazona xantholora), yellow crowned Amazon (Amazona ochrocephala), peach-faced lovebird (Agapornis roseicollis), African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), the peach-fronted conure (Aratinga aurea), and the blue and gold macaw (Ara ararauna), all from the Psittacidae Family, the cockatiel (Nymphicus hollandicus) from the Cacatuidae Family, and the kea (Nestor notabilis) from the Nestoridae Family (Figs 15–17) [62]. Similar to budgerigars, we distinguished PVALB-defined NLC and AAC core (high expression) and shell (moderate expression) regions (Fig 15), and only the MO and the NAO cores (Figs 16 and 17). The kea, considered the most distantly related of the group and representing the most basal split with the other species [8], contained distinct PVALB- and Nissl-defined NLC, ACC, and MO core regions similar to other species (Figs 15g and 16g). However, the kea PVALB- defined MO core was rectangular in shape (Fig 16g) in contrast to the more typical oval shape found in other species, and the kea did not possess a detectable NAO core with high PVALB expression that we could identify (Fig 17g) in serial sections, although a tiny core region was distinguishable with Nissl staining. We also noted that differential expression of PVALB in the kea NLC and AAC shell regions (presumably including motor areas) was barely detectable relative to the surrounding brain regions (Fig 15g).
Different species had variation in specialized SLIT1 expression in the AAC song nucleus. Similar to budgerigars, SLIT1 mRNA was downregulated in the AAC core relative to the AAC shell and the surrounding arcopallium in cockatiels (Fig 18a and 18b). However, in the peach-faced lovebird, peach-fronted conure, the two Amazon species, the African Grey, the kea, and in the blue and gold macaw not only was SLIT1 downregulated in the AAC core region, but it was expressed at noticeably lower levels in the AAC shell relative to the surrounding arcopallium (Fig 18c–18i). This resulted in two gene expression boundaries in the in situ hybridizations, one for the core and another one for the shell. The boundaries of the shell appeared to coincide with the PVALB-defined shell boundaries in the adjacent sections. We surmise that the SLIT1 specialized shell expression in these seven parrot species could possibly contain both the song nucleus and adjacent motor areas.
Core and shell cross-section area ratios show species differences
We noted considerable relative size differences in the core and shell regions among species (Figs 15–17). Thus, we analyzed the area ratios of the core and shell regions of NLC, AAC, MO and NAO in all nine species by sampling serially adjacent brain sections (n = 3–4 per animal) where the core region was the largest (in the center part of the nucleus) using PVALB and Nissl-defined boundaries. We separately quantified males (Fig 19a) and females (Fig 19b), but did not have a large enough sample size of each sex for each species to reliably determine if there were species differences between the sexes. We found that budgerigars and cockatiels had the highest core to shell area ratios overall (i.e. cores were larger), followed by lovebird, conures, and the two Amazons, whereas the African Grey, kea and the blue and gold macaw were among the lowest (i.e. cores were smaller; Fig 19a and 19b). The nidopallial song nuclei NLC and NAO generally had the smallest core to shell area ratios, the arcopallium song nucleus AAC intermediate levels, and the mesopallium song nucleus MO the highest ratios (i.e. core was larger; Fig 19a and 19b). Females of some of the larger brained species (conure, kea, and macaw) did not follow this same pattern (Fig 19b).
To test for isometric versus allometric scaling of core and shell sizes relative to brain size, we measured brain section area from the same sections as a proxy for brain size; we did not have sufficient brain sections to measure volume. We found that the core and shell regions measured did not increase in size isometrically (e.g. not the same slopes in Fig 19c–19f). Rather, the shells had a significant log-linear relationship with their brain section size, which was stronger for the anterior song nuclei, whereas the cores did not. If anything, the NLC core had a trend for the opposite relationship (Fig 19c). Except for NLC, the slopes of shell values were less than 1 (B = ~0.6), meaning a negative allometric relationship with brain section size. Despite the cores not having a scaling relationship, there was a strong log-linear relationship of shell:core ratios with brain section size (Fig 19g–19j). This relationship was negatively allometric (B << 1) for the posterior nuclei NLC and AAC, and weakly positively allometric if at all (B > or ~1) for the anterior nuclei MO and NAO. Only the relationship of AAC with brain section size become stronger, indicating that except for this nucleus, most of the scaling relationship with brain size is due to the shells changing size.
Overall, these findings demonstrate a significant plasticity in relative sizes of song nuclei among parrot species, with the shell regions being larger in larger brain species that are also typically considered to have more advanced vocal and cognitive abilities. The core varies, but not with brain section size alone, indicative of another variable, perhaps behavior, that its size could be associated with.
Discussion
Our study provides several advances in our understanding of the parrot song system. First, unlike other vocal learning bird lineages, the parrot song system has core and shell regions. Second, the PVALB-defined shell specializations include song and adjacent motor areas for both posterior song nuclei. Third, the core and shell regions show differences in neural connectivity, with stronger connections among the core regions and among the shell regions (summarized in Fig 20). Fourth, for both gene expression specializations and neural connectivity, the parrot core system is more similar to the song systems of songbirds and hummingbirds. Fifth, apparent PVALB-, SLIT1-, and Nissl-defined core and shell song nuclei exist in other parrot species besides the budgerigar, where there are relative size differences of the core and shell regions among species. Finally, the existence of song nuclei in the kea, but with some notable differences from the other species, demonstrates that song nuclei evolved early in parrots more than 29 million years ago before the kea split from the other parrot lineages (based on a dated genome-scale phylogenetic tree [8]), with subsequent divergences of their systems. Based on these results, we hypothesize that the parrot brain consists of a song system embedded within a song system. Below we discuss the implications of this hypothesis.
Our study helps clear up contradictions between past studies, in that different investigators had been studying the core, shell, and/or both without consistent recognition of the differences between them (Table 3). In the initial discovery of the budgerigar song system, Paton et al. [33] identified the posterior song pathway and called the nuclei by their songbird analogs HVC and RA, which we note in their figures appears to precisely correspond to our NLC core and AAC core respectively (Table 3). Paton et al. [33] also identified a large region that they named MAN similar to the comparable nucleus songbirds, which corresponds to our NAO core and shell. Striedter [32] renamed the posterior nuclei to NLC and AAC due to the belief that the parrot song system had evolved independently of songbirds, and expanded the regions to encompass most but not all of our shells. In the same study, Striedter [32] also identified our MO core (called HVo then) and NAO core, but did not consider them analogs of any songbird song nuclei, and instead considered a region he called NAs adjacent to the NAO core (part of our NAO shell) as the LMAN analog of songbirds (Table 3). Durand et al. [34] also identified the boundaries of NLC and AAC but did not distinguish the core and shell regions. Further, Durand claimed that NAom (a medial part of our NAO shell) adjacent to NAo (our core) is part of the parrot song system. Jarvis and Mello [24], using vocalizing-driven IEG expression, functionally identified the MO and NAO core and shells, calling them core and surround complexes, but did not recognize a core and shell for the NLC and AAC nuclei (Table 3). Since then, different studies have used either the Jarvis and Mello [24] or Durand et al. [34] naming systems without reconciling the two. In 2004, a new nomenclature for the avian brain was published that more closely matched their mammalian homologs [63,64]. The new nomenclature resulted in changes to the names (but not abbreviations, except for MO and MMSt), but it did not resolve the discrepancies. In the current study, authors from both groups reconciled the differences based on the findings of this study (Table 3).
We did not find gene expression specializations for the two smaller parrot cortical song nuclei, LAN (lateral nucleus of the anterior nidopallium) in the nidopallium and LAM (lateral nucleus of the anterior mesopallium) in the mesopallium (analogs of songbird NIf and Av) [24], except for distinct mENK labeled fibers and cells (Fig 12B of Durand et al. [58]). We note that variation in mENK expression among animals could be related to singing as it was later discovered that mENK expression levels in several songbird song nuclei are singing regulated [46].
One fundamental shared connection among the vocal learning birds is the parrot’s AAC core, songbird’s RA, and hummingbird’s VA specialized direct projection to the brainstem vocal lower motor neurons [34]; a difference is that the parrot AAC shell does not make this projection and instead projects to adjacent forebrain song nuclei (Fig 20). The direct projection to vocal motor neurons is thought to be critical for the evolution of vocal learning, as vocal non-learning species either do not have it or have a very sparse projection [3,6,65–68]. Given this interpretation, and the more rudimentary presence of the shell regions and absence of a detectable specialized NAO core region, but clear presence of other core regions in the kea, the “core song system” could represent a more “ancestral” state in the evolution of vocal learning in parrots that is similar with songbirds and hummingbirds, and the “shell song system” represent a more recently evolved state unique to parrots. If there were three independent gains of vocal learning, then parrots, songbirds, and hummingbirds would have each evolved the “core system” independently, with subsequent evolution of the shell system in only parrots. If there were only two independent gains, then the common ancestor of parrots and songbirds, and the ancestor of hummingbirds would have each evolved a core-like system independently, and thereafter suboscines and possibly New Zealand Wrens lost the core system and the vocal learning trait, whereas parrots went in the opposite direction and enhanced the trait with subsequent evolution of the “shell system”. In support of a possible loss, it was recently found that the suboscine Eastern phoebe (Sayornis phoebe) has a rudimentary RA (AAC-like) song nucleus not found in other bird species, such as quails [69], and at least another suboscine, a Bellbird (Procnias contigas), shows evidence for vocal learning [70] whereas multiple others do not [71]. We also note that different types of specializations in the hummingbird NLC (VLN) and NAO (VAN) analogs exist, which could also be due to subsequent changes after their song system first evolved. Future experiments will be necessary to resolve the two hypotheses.
The remarkable relative size differences between core and shell regions among parrot species could reflect functional and/or brain size differences. Parrot species generally considered to be more limited in their vocal imitation abilities, such as the budgerigar, peach-faced lovebirds, and cockatiels, have larger and conspicuous core nuclei relative to the shell nuclei, whereas those thought to have considerably more complex communication abilities such as the blue and gold macaws, peach-fronted conures, African Grey [15,17], yellow lored Amazon, and yellow crowned Amazon have noticeably smaller cores, and correspondingly larger shell regions. Consistent with this notion, recent behavioral studies by Walløe [72] have indicated that the peach-fronted conures show rapid vocal modification abilities compared to other species such as the budgerigars. We noted that except for the kea MO, the shapes of the song nuclei across species were very similar in serial sections, suggesting that shape differences are unlikely to confer differences in the relative sizes of the core and shell regions. Another possibility is that the differences in core and shell ratios are due to allometric scaling, where the shell could become disproportionally larger than the cores as the brain and body evolved to become larger. However, because we find that not all shell regions scale allometrically (i.e. NLC), the shell scaling of other song nuclei is negatively allometric, and the cores from the very same sections do not show significant scaling with brain section size, this suggests that the shell and core systems can be scaled somewhat independently of each other and from brain size. Further, allometric scaling for the shell also does not mean that larger brain regions (shells) are not involved in more complex aspects of vocal communication. The kea, also a larger brain parrot, does not show a robust shell specialization. Very little has been reported on kea vocal communication abilities, but one study has shown that there is geographic variation in their contact calls, and between juveniles and adults, indicating the presence of vocal learning [73]. Behavioral and/or neuroanatomical studies on most parrot species (with the exception of the budgerigar) including the endangered kea are few. Hence, additional experiments on volume measurements (beyond areas) with a larger number of animals from both sexes and from a wide variety of parrot species that differ in communication abilities would be necessary to further test these hypotheses on size differences.
In most species, the lowest core to shell area ratios (meaning shell is bigger) were in NLC. The NLC in budgerigars is required for production, but not memory of English words and natural vocalizations [74]. NLC lesions also disrupt the amplitude at which the carrier frequency of amplitude-modulated vocalizations are produced [74]; we note here that the lesions in that study included parts of both the NLC core and shell, making it difficult to pinpoint precise functional significance of the core and shell areas. Perhaps the strongest evidence regarding functional diversification of the NLC core and shell comes from a preliminary study by Striedter and Lei [75] where bilateral lesions of dorsal NLC (core in this study) and ventral NLC (shell in this study) were performed and learned contact calls examined. The authors found that NLC core lesions produced dramatic shifts in vocal frequency within a short period of time, with little recovery after lesions. However, NLC shell lesions produced a slow decrease in the size of the contact call repertoire. This suggests that further experiments are likely to discover different functions of the core and shell systems in parrot vocal communication.
Identification of core and shell functional differences is informed by the gene expression specializations. SLIT1 belongs to a class of proteins involved in axon guidance and cell migration, and in a recent study we also showed that its mRNA is downregulated in the human laryngeal motor cortex [37], the analogue of AAC. The protein serves as a ligand to the ROBO1 receptor [76], with a role in brain development [77], and is differentially upregulated by the human FOXP2 transcription factor (involved in speech acquisition) more than the chimpanzee FOXP2 [78]. Mutations in ROBO1 cause speech sound disorders and dyslexia [79]. One potential hypothesis is that SLIT1 is a candidate gene involved in the formation and maintenance of the specialized direct projection of the ACC core to brainstem vocal motor neurons [39]; SLIT1 is mostly repulsive for forming connections, and its down-regulation could be a mechanism to allow a direct projection to form.
Although all nine parrot species examined showed SLIT1 downregulation in the AAC core, a subset of them expressed intermediate specialized levels of SLIT1 in the PVALB-defined shell that includes non-vocal motor areas. Together with the limited specializations of mENK and TH noted here in the adjacent motor regions of this or other parrot song nuclei suggest that the brain areas involved in movement control may also have some specialized connectivity and functions. Only vocal learning birds (parrots in particular) and mammals (humans, elephant, and a sea lion [said to be a non-learner exception among pinnepeds although not demonstrated]) show auditory-motor entrainment, synchronizing body movements to the rhythm of the downbeat in music (i.e. dance to music heard) [16,80–82]. Similar types of movements in budgerigars are associated with activation of the motor regions adjacent to the song nuclei shells [35] as noted here. Thus, it is tempting to speculate that the gene expression specializations in the non-vocal motor regions adjacent to the song nuclei could be related to their more unique ability to learn to dance. This hypothesis can be tested in experiments targeting manipulations of the specialized gene expression part of the adjacent motor brain regions.
The differential regulation of FOXP1 in the NLC core and shell, and NR2A and GLUR1 genes in the core, could mean that the downstream consequences on the genes that they target (FOXP1’s direct regulation or NR2A’s and GLUR1’s indirect single transduction regulation of genes) and their ultimately influence on behavior would be different than in the surrounding regions. The higher levels of gene products with functions in Ca2+ buffering, neuroprotection and cell metabolism in all the cores (PVALB, NADPH-d, and CGRP), suggest that the cores could be more active than the surrounds [38], as in songbird song nuclei compared to surrounding non-song areas.
Interestingly, except for higher DUSP1 in MO and NAO cores, there is no noticeable difference in the relative degree of vocalizing-driven EGR1 or C-FOS mRNA induction between the core and shell region of any song nucleus in budgerigars. One explanation could be that both core and shell regions are equally required for production of warble song. Alternatively, IEG mapping techniques could be inadequate to parse out more finely tuned functions of the core and shell regions in vocal production. It is also possible that behavioral experiments that are designed for parrots to perform more specific vocal behaviors would be required to answer these questions. In either case, future IEG and other functional studies can now be guided by our discoveries to help determine potential functional differences between the core and shell regions.
In conclusion, we believe our study presents a new and viable hypothesis concerning the nature of unique specializations that characterize the parrot song system, which distinguish it from those of other vocal learning species. This hypothesis could inform future studies aimed at understanding the mechanisms of mimicry of human speech and the neural substrates that govern motor imitation in parrots, and provide a basis for deepening our understanding of the neural underpinnings of vocal learning.
Methods
Tissue collection and sectioning
We obtained brains from budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) (n = 5; 3 males, 2 females) from the University of Copenhagen, Denmark colony, and from Duke University, USA (n = 25, both males and females that were also used in other studies); peach-fronted conures (Aratinga aurea) (n = 5; 3 males, 2 females), cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) (n = 5; 3 males, 2 females), and peach-faced lovebirds (Agapornis roseicollis) (n = 5; 3 males, 2 females) from the University of Copenhagen; a male yellow lored Amazon parrot (Amazona xantholora), a male yellow crowned Amazon parrot (Amazona ochrocephala), a female blue and gold macaw (Ara ararauna) and a female kea (Nestor notabilis) from the Denmark Zoo (Copenhagen Zoo, Frederiksberg, Denmark); and a female African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) from the Diagnostic Pathology Laboratory of the Dutch Research Institute for Avian and Exotic Animals (NOIVBD, Netherlands). The birds in the University of Copenhagen colony were briefly anaesthetized with CO2, whereas the birds collected from the Copenhagen Zoo were anaesthetized with sevoflurane and pentobarbital IV, or with medetomidine and ketamine before decapitation. The remaining birds used for behaviorally regulated gene expression were not anaesthetized prior to decapitation, in order to not disrupt the behaviorally-regulated gene expression. All collected brains were dissected within minutes of decapitation, frozen immediately in a dry ice-ethanol bath, and stored at -80°C. Brains were kept frozen on dry ice during transportation. We embedded the frozen brain in Tissue-Tek OCT Compound (Sakura, Finetek, Torrance, CA) in a plastic block mold and quickly froze the Tissue Tek around the brain on a dry ice-ethanol bath. Brains were sectioned in sagittal and coronal planes at 16μm in 6–20 series on a cryostat and the sections mounted on plus charged slides (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). Only the coronal plane sections are shown, where it was easier to identify core and shell regions.
Ethics statement
All procedures on live animals were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Duke University and University of Copenhagen. Tissue collection and export were approved by the Tissue Collection permits of Denmark and Netherlands, and for import by the United States Department of Agriculture (Permit # 119548).
Radioactive in situ hybridization
To localize mRNA expression of the PVALB and SLIT1 genes we used radioactive 35S in situ hybridization following previously described procedures [54,83]. To make the PVALB riboprobe we used a clone from our zebra finch full-length cDNA collection [38, 46] (GenBank Accession # DQ215755), and a SLIT1 clone containing 1.8Kb of the coding region that we cloned [39] (GenBank Accession # KF738084). Sections were fixed in 3% paraformaldehyde, rinsed in 1X phosphate-buffer saline (PBS), dehydrated in an increasing ethanol series, and then air-dried. We generated radioactive 35S-UTP-labeled sense and antisense riboprobes by reverse transcription, and hybridized each slide containing brain sections with 1x106 cpm at 60°C (PVALB, SLIT1; the lower than 65°C temperature is to obtain cross-species hybridization). We exposed the slides to NTB emulsion (Kodak, USA) diluted 1:1 in distilled water for ~14–30 days at 4°C and processed the slides with D-19 developer (Kodak) and fixer (Kodak). We visualized the bound riboprobe as silver grains, and the cell bodies by counterstaining with cresyl-violet acetate solution (Sigma, USA). Tissue incubated with the sense probe showed no significant signal above background.
Quantification of song nuclei size ratios
To perform quantification of area measurements, we captured digital images of brain sections of emulsion dipped (in darkfield) plus Nissl-stained (in brightfield) slides using an Olympus MVX10 microscope (Olympus, Japan) connected to a DP71 camera (Olympus) and DP Controller software. We quantified the core and shell area boundaries from all available sections from each animal per species. However, to remove any size measurement biases due to the number of available sections, we selectively analyzed the core and shell regions in sections where the core region was the largest, in 3–4 adjacent sections per brain region in each animal. We measured the area sizes using the Freehand tool in the Fiji software from ImageJ with the scale bar set in millimeters for spatial calibration. PVALB expression specializations and cresyl violet were used to visualize the posterior song nuclei (NLC and AAC) core and shell regions and the anterior song nuclei (MO and NAO) core regions, whereas only cresyl violet was used to visualize the shell regions of MO and NAO, as PVALB does not distinguish their shell boundaries. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh (Version 22.0).
Analysis of connectivity of song nuclei in budgerigars
To investigate connectivity of the core and shell regions of NLC, AAC, MO, and NAO, we imaged and analyzed slides (> 4000) containing serial brain sections of budgerigars (n = 29) from neural tracing studies previously conducted in the Brauth lab [31,34] and compared them to serial brain sections of gene expression studies previously conducted in the Jarvis lab [24,35,38,44,45,54,84]. These include budgerigar brains with focal injections of 10–15% BDA in saline, and Biocytin into the different parts of what we have identified as NLC, AAC, MO, and NAO core and shell (Table 3). These serial sections included many previously unpublished images. We took digital images using a described procedure [46]. For NAO, we could not image brain sections with focal injections of fluorescent tracers [34] because the tissue still available had been stained with cresyl violet, which removed the tracer; thus, we examined the published study [34] to find examples of focal injections within NAO that we identified here as core and shell regions.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Anne Marie Wolthers Hansen for taking care of the birds at the University of Copenhagen, Electra Jarvis for helping with processing slides for Nissl staining, Syrus Jarvis for helping with preparation of images for figures, and Rebecca A. Senft for helping in sectioning the kea and macaw brains. We also thank Kazuhiro Wada, Miriam Rivas, and Erina Hara for providing us with in situ hybridization slides of DUSP1, EGR-1, and PVALB from their published work. We thank the reviewers for their feedback, particularly on suggestions of isometric versus allometric scaling.
Anatomical Abbreviations
- A
arcopallium
- AAC
central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium
- AACd
dorsal part of the central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium
- AACv
ventral part of the central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium
- ACM
caudal medial arcopallium
- AH
anterior hyperpallium
- Ai
intermediate arcopallium
- AMV
anterior ventral mesopallium
- AN
anterior nidopallium
- Area X
a vocal nucleus
- AR
androgen receptor
- Av
nucleus avalanche
- B
basorostralis
- Cb
cerebellum
- CM
caudal mesopallium
- CSt
caudal striatum
- CMM
caudomedial mesopallium
- DLN
dorsal lateral nidopallium
- DLM
dorsal lateral nucleus of the thalamus
- DM
dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain
- DMm
magnocellular nucleus of the dorsal thalamus
- E
entopallium
- GP
globus pallidus
- H
hyperpallium
- Hp
hippocampus
- HVC
a vocal nucleus (no abbreviation)
- HVo
oval nucleus of the ventral hyperstriatum
- HVoc
HVo complex
- IH
intercalated hyperpallium
- IEG
immediate early gene
- LAI
lateral intermediate arcopallium
- LAN
lateral nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- LAM
lateral nucleus of the anterior mesopallium
- LMAN
lateral part of the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- M
mesopallium
- MAN
magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- MLd
dorsal part of the lateral mesencephalic nucleus
- MMSt
magnocellular nucleus of the medial striatum
- MO
oval nucleus of the anterior mesopallium
- MD
dorsal mesopallium
- MV
ventral mesopallium
- nXIIts
12th nucleus, tracheosyringeal part
- N
nidopallium
- NAo
oval nucleus of the anterior neostriatum
- NAO
oval nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- NAoc
NAo complex
- NAom
medial division of the oval nucleus of the anterior neostriatum
- NAs
supralaminar area of the frontal neostriatum
- NCM
caudomedial nidopallium
- NDC
caudal dorsal nidopallium
- NIDL
dorsal lateral intermediate nidopallium
- Nif
interfacial nucleus of the nidopallium
- NLc
central nucleus of the anterior neostriatum
- NLC
central nucleus of the lateral nidopallium
- NLs
supracentral nucleus of the lateral nidopallium
- NLv
ventral lateral nidopallium
- TeO
optic tectum
- Ov
nucleus ovoidalis
- PH
posterior hyperpallium
- RA
robust nucleus of the arcopallium
- Rt
nucleus rotundus
- SLN
supra lateral nidopallium
- St
striatum
- VA
vocal nucleus of the arcopallium
- VAM
vocal nucleus of the anterior mesopallium
- VAN
vocal nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- VANp
posterior part of the vocal nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- VASt
vocal nucleus of the anterior striatum
- VMM
vocal nucleus of the medial mesopallium
- VMN
vocal nucleus of the medial nidopallium
- VLN
vocal nucleus of the lateral nidopallium
Data Availability
All relevant data are included within the paper and Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This study was funded by grants provided by the NIH Director’s Pioneer Award and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute to E.D.J., a Doctoral scholarship from the University of Copenhagen (Faculty of Science) to S.W., and the Frame work Grant from the Danish Council for Independent Research to T.D. (HHMI: http://www.hhmi.org/; NIH: http://commonfund.nih.gov/pioneer/index). The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Nottebohm F. The origins of vocal learning. Am Nat. 1972;106: 116–140. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Janik VM, Slater PJB. Vocal learning in mammals. Adv Study Behav. 1997;26: 59–99. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Arriaga G, Zhou EP, Jarvis ED. Of mice, birds, and men: The mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song learning birds. PLoS ONE. 2012;7: e46610 10.1371/journal.pone.0046610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Fitch WT, Jarvis ED. Birdsong and other animals models for human speech, song, and vocal learning In: Arbib MA, editor. Language, Music, and the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2013. pp. 499–539. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Jarvis ED. Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1016: 749–777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Fitch WT. The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4: 258–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Suh A, Paus M, Kiefmann M, Churakov G, Franke FA, Brosius J, et al. Mesozoic retroposons reveal parrots as the closest living relatives of passerine birds. Nature Commun. 2011;2: 443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Jarvis ED, Mirarab S, Aberer AJ, Li B, Houde P, Li C, et al. Whole-genome analyses resolve early branches in the tree of life of modern birds. Science. 2014;December 12; 346(6215):1320–1321. 10.1126/science.1253451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Cruickshank AJ, Gautier J-P, Chappuis C. Vocal mimicry in wild African Grey parrots Psittacus erithacus . Ibis. 1993;135: 293–299. [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gramza AF. Vocal mimicry in captive budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Z Tierpsychol. 1970;27: 971–983. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Kelley LA, Coe RL, Madden JR, Healy SD. Vocal mimicry in songbirds. Anim Behav. 2008;76: 521–528. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Kelley LA, Healy SD. Vocal mimicry in male bowerbirds: who learns from whom? Biol Lett. 2010;6: 626–629. 10.1098/rsbl.2010.0093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kelley LA, Healy SD. Vocal mimicry. Curr Biol. 2011;21(1): R9–R10. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Klatt DH, Stefanski RA. How does a mynah bird imitate human speech? The J Acoust Soc Am. 1974;55: 822–832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Pepperberg I. The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Schachner A. Auditory-motor entrainment in vocal mimicking species: Additional ontogenetic and phylogenetic factors. Commun Integr Biol. 2010;3: 290–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Pepperberg IM. Vocal learning in Grey parrots: A brief review of perception, production, and cross-species comparisons. Brain Lang. 2010;115: 81–91. 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Colbert-White EN, Covington MA, Fragaszy DM. Social context influences the vocalizations of a home-raised African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus erithacus). J Comp Psychol. 2011;125: 175–184. 10.1037/a0022097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Poole JH, Tyack PL, Stoeger-Horwath AS, Watwood S. Anim Behav. Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature. 2005;434: 455–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Richards DG, Wolz JP, Herman LM. Vocal mimicry of computer-generated sounds and vocal labeling of objects by a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus . J Comp Psychol.1984;98: 10–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S. Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina . Can J Zool. 1985;1050–1056. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Ridgway S, Carder D, Jeffries M, Todd M. Spontaneous human speech mimicry by a cetacean. Curr Biol. 2012;22: R860–861. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Stoeger AS, Mietchen D, Oh S, de Silva S, Herbst CT, Kwon S, et al. An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Curr Biol. 2012;22: 2144–2148. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Jarvis ED, Mello CV. Molecular mapping of brain areas involved in parrot vocal communication. J Comp Neurol. 2000;419: 1–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Jarvis ED. Brains and birdsong In: Marler P, Slabberkoom H, editors. Nature's music: The Science of Birdsong. New York: Elsevier- Academic Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 26. Gahr M. Neural song control system of hummingbirds: comparison to swifts, vocal learning (songbirds) and nonlearning (suboscines) passerines, and vocal learning (budgerigars) and nonlearning (dove, owl, gull, quail, chicken) nonpasserines. J Comp Neurol. 2000;426: 182–196. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Jarvis ED, Ribeiro S, da Silva ML, Ventura D, Vielliard J, Mello, CV. Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain. Nature. 2000;406: 628–632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Katz LC, Gurney ME. Auditory responses in the zebra finch's motor system for song. Brain Res. 1981;221: 192–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. McCasland JS, Konishi M. Interaction between auditory and motor activities in an avian song control nucleus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1981;78: 7815–7819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F. For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 1988;21: 775–788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Brauth SE, Liang W, Roberts TF. Projections of the oval nucleus of the hyperstriatum ventrale in the budgerigar: relationships with the auditory system. J Comp Neurol. 2001;432: 481–511. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Striedter GF. The vocal control pathways in budgerigars differ from those in songbirds. J Comp Neurol. 1994;343: 35–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Paton JA, Manogue KR, Nottebohm F. Bilateral organization of the vocal control pathway in the budgerigar, Melopsittacus undulatus . J Neurosci. 1981;1: 1279–1288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Durand SE, Heaton JT, Amateau SK, Brauth SE.Vocal control pathways through the anterior forebrain of a parrot (Melopsittacus undulatus). J Comp Neurol.1997;377: 179–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Feenders G, Liedvogel M, Rivas M, Zapka M, Horita H, Hara E, et al. Molecular mapping of movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a motor theory for vocal learning origin. PLoS ONE. 2008;3: e1768 10.1371/journal.pone.0001768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Farabaugh SM, Wild JM.Reciprocal connections between primary and secondary auditory pathways in the telencephalon of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus). Brain Res. 1997;747(1): 18–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Pfenning AR, Hara E, Whitney O, Rivas MR, Wang R, Roulhac PL, et al. Convergent transcriptional specializations in the brains of humans and song-learning birds. Science. 2014; December 12; 346(6215):1256846 10.1126/science.1256846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Hara E, Rivas MV, Ward JM, Okanoya K, Jarvis ED. Convergent differential regulation of parvalbumin in the brains of vocal learners. PLoS One 7 2012; e29457 10.1371/journal.pone.0029457 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Wang R, Chen CC, Hara E, Rivas MV, Roulhac PL, Howard JT, et al. Convergent differential regulation of SLIT-ROBO axon guidance genes in the brains of vocal learners. J Comp Neurol. 2014;523(6): 892–906. 10.1002/cne.23719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Celio MR. Parvalbumin in most gamma-aminobutyric acid-containing neurons of the rat cerebral cortex. Science.1986;231: 995–997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Braun K, Scheich H, Zuschratter W, Heizmann CW, Matute C, Streit P. Postnatal development of parvalbumin-, calbindin- and adult GABA-immunoreactivity in two visual nuclei of zebra finches. Brain Res. 1988;475: 205–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Chen CC, Winkler CM, Pfenning AR, Jarvis ED. Molecular profiling of the developing avian telencephalon: regional timing and brain subdivision continuities. J Comp Neurol. 2013;521: 3666–3701. 10.1002/cne.23406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Jarvis ED, Yu J, Rivas MV, Horita H, Feenders G, Whitney O, et al. Global view of the functional molecular organization of the avian cerebrum: mirror images and functional columns. J Comp Neurol. 2013;521: 3614–3665. 10.1002/cne.23404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Jarvis ED. Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans: a synopsis. J Ornithol. 2007;143: S35–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Horita H, Kobayashi M, Liu WC, Oka K, Jarvis ED, Wada K. Specialized motor-driven dusp1 expression in the song systems of multiple lineages of vocal learning birds. PLoS One. 2012;7: e42173 10.1371/journal.pone.0042173 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Wada K, Howard JT, McConnell P, Whitney O, Lints T, Rivas MV, et al. A molecular neuroethological approach for identifying and characterizing a cascade of behaviorally regulated genes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103: 15212–15217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Jarvis ED, Nottebohm F. Motor-driven gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94: 4097–4102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Mello CV, Clayton DF. Song-induced ZENK gene expression in auditory pathways of songbird brain and its relation to the song control system. J Neurosci. 1994;14: 6652–6666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Mello CV, Vates GE, Okuhata S, Nottebohm F. Descending auditory pathways in the adult male zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Neurol. 1988;395: 137–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Mandelblat-Cerf Y, Las L, Denisenko N, Fee MS. A role for descending auditory cortical projections in songbird vocal learning. 2014;eLife 3 10.7554/eLife.02152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Bottjer SW. Developmental regulation of basal ganglia circuitry during the sensitive period for vocal learning in songbirds. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004; 1016: 395–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Bottjer SW, Altenau B. Parallel pathways for vocal learning in basal ganglia of songbirds. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13: 153–155. 10.1038/nn.2472 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Fisher SE, Scharff C. FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends Genet. 2009;25(4): 166–177. 10.1016/j.tig.2009.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Haesler S, Wada K, Nshdejan A, Morrisey EE, Lints T, Jarvis ED, et al. FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. J Neurosci. 2004;24: 3164–3175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Whitney O, Voyles T, Hara E, Chen Q, White SA, White TF. Differential FoxP2 and FoxP1 expression in a vocal learning nucleus of the developing budgerigar. Dev Neurobiol. 2014; 10.1002/dneu.22247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Wada K, Sakaguchi H, Jarvis ED, Hagiwara M. Differential expression of glutamate receptors in avian neural pathways for learned vocalization. J Comp Neurol. 2004;476: 44–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Brauth SE, Liang W, Amateau SK, Roberts TF. Sexual dimorphism of vocal control nuclei in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) revealed with Nissl and NADPH-d staining. J Comp Neurol. 2005;484: 15–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Durand SE, Liang W, Brauth SE. Methionine enkephalin immunoreactivity in the brain of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus): similarities and differences with respect to oscine songbirds. J Comp Neurol. 1998;393: 145–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Durand SE, Brauth SE, Liang W. Calcitonin gene-related peptide immunoreactive cells and fibers in forebrain vocal and auditory nuclei of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus). Brain Behav Evol. 2001;58: 61–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Matsunaga E, Okanoya K. Vocal area-related expression of the androgen receptor in the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) brain. Brain Res. 2008;1208: 87–94. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Roberts TF, Cookson KK, Heaton KJ, Hall WS, Brauth SE. Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase-containing neurons and fibers in the brain of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus): general patterns and labeling in vocal control nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 2001;429: 436–454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Joseph L, Toon A, Schirtzinger EE, Wright TF, Schodde R. A revised nomenclature and classification for family-group taxa of parrots (Psittaciformes). Zootaxa. 2012;3205: 26–40. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Jarvis ED, Gunturkun O, Bruce L, Csillag A, Karten H, Kuenzel W, et al. Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6: 151–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce LL, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, et al. Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 2004;473: 377–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Fitch WT. Evolutionary Developmental Biology and Human Language Evolution: Constraints on Adaptation. Evo Biol. 2012;39(4): 613–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Fitch WT. The evolution of language. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Petkov CI, Jarvis ED. Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates. Front Evol Neurosci. 2012;4: 12 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Fitch WT, Huber L, Bugnyar T. Social cognition and the evolution of language: constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron. 2010;65: 795–814. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Liu W, Wada K, Jarvis ED, Nottebohm F. Rudimentary substrates for vocal learning in a suboscine. Nat Commun. 2013;4:2082: 1–12. 10.1038/ncomms3082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Kroodsma D, Hamilton D, Sanchez JE, Byers BE, Fandino-Marino H, Stemple DW, et al. Behavioral Evidence for Song Learning in the Suboscine Bellbirds (Procnias Spp.; Cotingidae). Wilson J Ornithol. 2013;125: 1–14. 10.1159/000360034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Touchton JM, Seddon N, Tobias JA. Captive Rearing Experiments Confirm Song Development without Learning in a Tracheophone Suboscine Bird. PLoS One. 2014;9: e95746 10.1371/journal.pone.0095746 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Harpøth SW. Vocal modification abilities and brain structures in parrots- how do they correlate? Ph.D. Thesis, Department of Biology, Graduate School at the Faculty of Science, University of Copenhagen. 2013.
- 73. Bond AB, Diamond J. Geographic and ontogenetic variation in the contact calls of the kea (Nestor notabilis). Behaviour. 2005;142: 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Lavenex PB. Lesions in the budgerigar vocal control nucleus NLc affect production, but not memory, of english words and natural vocalizations. J Comp Neurol. 2000;421: 437–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Striedter GF, Lei K. Vocal performance and plasticity functions are segregated into dorsal and ventral subdivisions of a single nucleus in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Poster number 818.10/X22. Society for Neuroscience Atlanta, GA; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 76. Wong K, Park HT, Wu JY, Rao Y. Slit proteins: molecular guidance cues for cells ranging from neurons to leukocytes. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2002;12: 583–591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Kim M, Roesener AP, Mendonca PR, Mastick GS. Robo1 and Robo2 have distinct roles in pioneer longitudinal axon guidance. Dev Biol. 2011;358: 181–188. 10.1016/j.ydbio.2011.07.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Konopka G, Bomar JM, Winden K, Coppola G, Jonsson ZO, Gao F, et al. Human-specific transcriptional regulation of CNS development genes by FOXP2. Nature. 2009;462: 213–217. 10.1038/nature08549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Hannula-Jouppi K, Kaminen-Ahola N, Taipale M, Eklund R, Nopola-Hemmi J, Kaariainen H, et al. The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PLoS Genetics. 2005;1: e50 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Cook P, Rouse A, Wilson M, Reichmuth C. A California Sea Lion (Zalophus californianus) Can Keep the Beat: Motor Entrainment to Rhythmic Auditory Stimuli in a Non Vocal Mimic. J Comp Psychol. 2013;127: 412–427. 10.1037/a0032345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Bregman MR, Schulz I. Experimental evidence for synchronization to a musical beat in a nonhuman animal. Curr Biol. 2009:CB 19: 827–830. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Schachner A, Brady TF, Pepperberg IM, Hauser MD. Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species. Curr Biol. 2009:CB 19: 831–836. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Chen CC, Wada K, Jarvis ED. Radioactive in situ hybridization for detecting diverse gene expression patterns in tissue. J Vis Exp. 2012;3764 [pii] 10.3791/3764 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Horita H, Wada K, Rivas MV, Hara E, Jarvis ED. The dusp1 immediate early gene is regulated by natural stimuli predominantly in sensory input neurons. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518: 2873–2901. 10.1002/cne.22370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Ball GF. Neurochemical specializations associated with vocal learning and production in songbirds and budgerigars. Brain Behav Evol. 1994;44: 234–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are included within the paper and Supporting Information files.