Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
editorial
. 2015 Jul 1;38(7):997–999. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4792

Analog Flashbacks

Edward F Pace-Schott 1,
PMCID: PMC4481017  PMID: 26085293

In the growing field of research on sleep and emotion regulation, an interesting dichotomy has arisen between findings suggesting that sleep following traumatic events could, via sleep-dependent memory processes, detrimentally strengthen and perpetuate traumatic memories,13 and those that suggest that post-trauma sleep disruption contributes to emotional dysregulation and psychopathology.49 These findings have led to conflicting speculation that sleep should either be curtailed on the night following the traumatic event24 or that correcting sleep disturbance should be treated as an essential first step in preventing subsequent anxiety symptoms.7,9 In this issue of SLEEP, Porcheret and colleagues report that a night of total sleep deprivation (TSD) following presentation of series of emotionally disturbing film clips (e.g., suicide) reduced the rate at which spontaneous memories of these films intruded into participants' consciousness over the subsequent 6 days.10 The authors suggest that these intrusive memories are analogous to those that occur in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and hypothesize that sleep deprivation following an actual trauma might reduce the consolidation of such intrusive memories expressed symptomatically in PTSD as flashbacks, dissociation, and nightmares.10

These alternate approaches to preventing PTSD symptoms are, of course, not mutually exclusive, since a single night's TSD could immediately be followed by efforts to optimize sleep quality. Evidence exists that sleep disturbances during the weeks and months immediately following a traumatic event may be especially important in the development of PTSD.47 Similarly, sleep difficulties preceding traumatic experiences increase risk of later PTSD.11,12 However, because memory consolidation13 and processing of emotional memories14 during sleep are ongoing processes, it is possible that a single night's TSD would produce only a small effect. Evidence that brief peritraumatic interventions may not prove effective comes from negative findings in pharmacological interventions that were intended to weaken initial consolidation of traumatic memories. For example, trials that used propranolol immediately post trauma to block noradrenergic facilitation of fear consolidation, although initially promising,15 did not produce a clinically significant effect in a larger sample.16 Nonetheless, reports of reduced PTSD in traumatized children initially treated with opiates,17 as well as positive results in reconsolidation-blockade studies using propranolol18,19 suggest that pharmacological interventions designed to weaken traumatic memories continue to hold promise. Thus a TSD intervention might also do so and should be tested in clinical samples. Before attempting an actual peritraumatic TSD trial, more easily accomplished paradigms might first be tried, such as reconsolidation blockade or a retrospective chart review comparing individuals kept awake incidentally following a traumatic stressor to those who slept.

Another caveat when considering a single night of TSD as prophylactic treatment for trauma-memory intrusion is the above-noted phenomenon of reconsolidation, whereby retrieved memories temporarily re-enter a labile state.20 Whereas such lability offers the opportunity for modification or even elimination of an unwanted memory,21,22 it also presents the possibility for retrieved fear memory to re-stabilize, generalize, or even worsen. Therefore, reconsolidation blockade sessions might need to be added to the above-envisioned compound therapy. An important additional consideration in any research on preventive strategies for PTSD is that a history of trauma preceding the index event is extremely common23 and may greatly increase the risk of developing symptoms24 and complicate treatments focused exclusively on a single event.

Because of the difficulties involved in controlled peritrauma research, experimental paradigms that reproduce elements of the human response to trauma in healthy individuals are very important for exploratory studies of potential clinical interventions.10,25 Such paradigms are additionally important because experimental manipulations in humans cannot ethically induce levels of stress shown to produce PTSD-like effects in rats.26 The use of analog-trauma films to elicit intrusive memories has had a particularly productive history in work by Emily Holmes and colleagues.10,27,28 For example, this group has characterized individual differences in vulnerability to such intrusions29,30 and developed techniques for inhibiting their occurrence.31,32 These researchers also showed that memories that later elicited intrusions produced greater activation of limbic structures when encoded during trauma films,33 conditions also known to enhance voluntary recall of emotional over neutral material.34,35 Such limbic activation at encoding may thus also explain intrusions following an actual traumatic stressor. Surprisingly however, in several studies, intrusion frequency was unrelated to measures of recall and recognition.10,36 Therefore, such analog intrusions, as well as the traumatic memories they model, must differ in fundamental ways from the voluntary recall of declarative memory, which is the form of emotional memory usually tested in sleep-dependent memory consolidation protocols.13

Kuriyama et al.1 performed a similar study in which they showed prevention, by TSD, of the generalization of fear from a trauma film to a neutral one. Fear generalization is also characteristic of PTSD.37 However, it should be noted that neither of these studies necessarily show impact on a pathological process, and the phenomena observed might or might not be the same as those that become dysregulated in psychopathology. One potential confound in the study by Porcheret and colleagues10 is a possible salience effect at memory encoding,38,39 the consolidation of which might itself also be sleep-dependent40 and lead to enhanced involuntary recall. For example, one might see similar effects if the pre-intervention film showed highly arousing but pleasantly valenced stimuli, and this would be an important experimental control in future studies using TSD manipulations.

Another important consideration when interpreting the effects of sleep on emotional memory processes is their apparent sensitivity to small differences in experimental procedures. For example, Kuriyama et al.41 replicated their previous finding when participants were told to recall events of the trauma film. However, they then found that TSD produced more rather than less generalization of fear when participants performed an active memory suppression procedure following their initial exposure.41 Generalization and sensitization phenomena are more nuanced aspects of emotional memory that can be affected by sleep and TSD even when the actual strength of a memory is unaffected.42 Moreover time-of-day and morningness-eveningness along with possible gender interactions,4244 in addition to sleep itself,4548 can affect the extinction of conditioned fear. Therefore, standardized paradigms, such as used by Porcheret et al.10 are necessary to dissect these more subtle influences by selective manipulation of specific factors in otherwise identical protocols.

CITATION

Pace-Schott EF. Analog flashbacks. SLEEP 2015;38(7):997–999.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

Dr. Pace-Schott has indicated no financial conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Kuriyama K, Soshi T, Kim Y. Sleep deprivation facilitates extinction of implicit fear generalization and physiological response to fear. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:991–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Baran B, Pace-Schott EF, Ericson C, Spencer RM. Processing of emotional reactivity and emotional memory over sleep. J Neurosci. 2012;32:1035–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wagner U, Hallschmid M, Rasch B, Born J. Brief sleep after learning keeps emotional memories alive for years. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:788–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mellman TA, Bustamante V, Fins AI, Pigeon WR, Nolan B. REM sleep and the early development of posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1696–701. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Koren D, Arnon I, Lavie P, Klein E. Sleep complaints as early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder: a 1-year prospective study of injured survivors of motor vehicle accidents. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:855–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mellman TA, Pigeon WR, Nowell PD, Nolan B. Relationships between REM sleep findings and PTSD symptoms during the early aftermath of trauma. J Trauma Stress. 2007;20:893–901. doi: 10.1002/jts.20246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Pace-Schott EF, Germain A, Milad MR. Sleep and REM sleep disturbance in the pathophysiology of PTSD: the role of extinction memory. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2015;5:3. doi: 10.1186/s13587-015-0018-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Germain A, Buysse DJ, Nofzinger E. Sleep-specific mechanisms underlying posttraumatic stress disorder: integrative review and neurobiological hypotheses. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12:185–95. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Germain A. Sleep disturbances as the hallmark of PTSD: where are we now? Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170:372–82. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Porcheret K, Holmes EA, Goodwin GM, Foster RG, Wulff K. Psychological effect of an analogue traumatic event reduced by sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2015;38:1017–25. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.van Liempt S, van Zuiden M, Westenberg H, Super A, Vermetten E. Impact of impaired sleep on the development of PTSD symptoms in combat veterans: a prospective longitudinal cohort study. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30:469–74. doi: 10.1002/da.22054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gehrman P, Seelig AD, Jacobson IG, et al. Predeployment sleep duration and Insomnia symptoms as risk factors for new-onset mental health disorders following military deployment. Sleep. 2013;36:1009–18. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rasch B, Born J. About sleep's role in memory. Physiol Rev. 2013;93:681–766. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00032.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Walker MP. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:168–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pitman RK, Sanders KM, Zusman RM, et al. Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with propranolol. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:189–92. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01279-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hoge EA, Worthington JJ, Nagurney JT, et al. Effect of acute posttrauma propranolol on PTSD outcome and physiological responses during script-driven imagery. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2012;18:21–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2010.00227.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sheridan RL, Stoddard FJ, Kazis LE, et al. Long-term posttraumatic stress symptoms vary inversely with early opiate dosing in children recovering from serious burns: effects durable at 4 years. J Trauma Acute Care Surg. 2014;76:828–32. doi: 10.1097/TA.0b013e3182ab111c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Brunet A, Poundja J, Tremblay J, et al. Trauma reactivation under the influence of propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress symptoms and disorder: 3 open-label trials. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2011;31:547–50. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e318222f360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lonergan MH, Olivera-Figueroa LA, Pitman RK, Brunet A. Propranolol's effects on the consolidation and reconsolidation of long-term emotional memory in healthy participants: a meta-analysis. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38:222–31. doi: 10.1503/jpn.120111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nader K, Einarsson EO. Memory reconsolidation: an update. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1191:27–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05443.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Nader K, Hardt O, Lanius R. Memory as a new therapeutic target. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15:475–86. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.4/knader. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Quirk GJ, Pare D, Richardson R, et al. Erasing fear memories with extinction training. J Neurosci. 2010;30:14993–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4268-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Karam EG, Friedman MJ, Hill ED, et al. Cumulative traumas and risk thresholds: 12-month PTSD in the World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:130–42. doi: 10.1002/da.22169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Delahanty DL, Nugent NR. Predicting PTSD prospectively based on prior trauma history and immediate biological responses. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1071:27–40. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Spring J, Wood N, Mueller-Pfeiffer C, Milad MR, Pitman RK, Orr SP. Pre-reactivation propranolol fails to reduce skin conductance reactivity to fear-conditioned stimuli. Psychophysiology. 2015;52:407–15. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sanford LD, Suchecki D, Meerlo P. Stress, arousal, and sleep. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2015;25:379–410. doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Holmes EA, Bourne C. Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: a review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2008;127:553–66. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Holmes EA, Grey N, Young KA. Intrusive images and “hotspots” of trauma memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: an exploratory investigation of emotions and cognitive themes. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2005;36:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Malik A, Goodwin GM, Hoppitt L, Holmes EA. Hypomanic experience in young adults confers vulnerability to intrusive imagery after experimental trauma: relevance for bipolar disorder. Clin Psychol Sci. 2014;2:675–84. doi: 10.1177/2167702614527433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Clark IA, Mackay CE, Holmes EA. Low emotional response to traumatic footage is associated with an absence of analogue flashbacks: an individual participant data meta-analysis of 16 trauma film paradigm experiments. Cogn Emot. 2015;29:702–13. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.926861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Holmes EA, James EL, Coode-Bate T, Deeprose C. Can playing the computer game “Tetris” reduce the build-up of flashbacks for trauma? A proposal from cognitive science. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Woud ML, Holmes EA, Postma P, Dalgleish T, Mackintosh B. Ameliorating intrusive memories of distressing experiences using computerized reappraisal training. Emotion. 2012;12:778–84. doi: 10.1037/a0024992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bourne C, Mackay CE, Holmes EA. The neural basis of flashback formation: the impact of viewing trauma. Psychol Med. 2013;43:1521–32. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:1–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat Rev. 2006;7:54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Holmes EA, Brewin CR, Hennessy RG. Trauma films, information processing, and intrusive memory development. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133:3–22. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sauerhofer E, Pamplona FA, Bedenk B, et al. Generalization of contextual fear depends on associative rather than non-associative memory components. Behav Brain Res. 2012;233:483–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wiswede D, Russeler J, Hasselbach S, Munte TF. Memory recall in arousing situations - an emotional von Restorff effect? BMC Neurosci. 2006;7:57. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ohman A, Flykt A, Esteves F. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130:466–78. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Payne JD, Stickgold R, Swanberg K, Kensinger EA. Sleep preferentially enhances memory for emotional components of scenes. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:781–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02157.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kuriyama K, Honma M, Yoshiike T, Kim Y. Memory suppression trades prolonged fear and sleep-dependent fear plasticity for the avoidance of current fear. Scientific Reports. 2013;3:2227. doi: 10.1038/srep02227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pace-Schott EF, Germain A, Milad MR. Effects of sleep on memory for conditioned fear and fear extinction. Psychol Bull. 2015 Apr 20; doi: 10.1037/bul0000014. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pace-Schott EF, Rubin ZS, Tracy LE, Spencer RMC, Orr SP, Verga PW. Emotional trait and memory associates of sleep timing and quality. Psychiatry Res. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.069. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pace-Schott EF, Spencer RM, Vijayakumar S, et al. Extinction of conditioned fear is better learned and recalled in the morning than in the evening. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:1776–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.07.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pace-Schott EF, Milad MR, Orr SP, Rauch SL, Stickgold R, Pitman RK. Sleep promotes generalization of extinction of conditioned fear. Sleep. 2009;32:19–26. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Pace-Schott EF, Verga PW, Bennett TS, Spencer RM. Sleep promotes consolidation and generalization of extinction learning in simulated exposure therapy for spider fear. J Psychiatr Res. 2012;46:1036–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Spoormaker VI, Schroter MS, Andrade KC, et al. Effects of rapid eye movement sleep deprivation on fear extinction recall and prediction error signaling. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2362–76. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Spoormaker VI, Sturm A, Andrade KC, et al. The neural correlates and temporal sequence of the relationship between shock exposure, disturbed sleep and impaired consolidation of fear extinction. J Psychiatr Res. 2010;44:1121–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES