Abstract
Background
Excessive crying in early infancy has been associated with behavioural problems among preschool children from high income countries but studies in low income and middle income countries are scarce.
Methods
The 2004 Pelotas Birth Cohort is a population-based study planned to enrol all live births occurring in Pelotas that year and comprises 4231 children who so far have been followed up at 3, 12, 24, 48 and 72 months of age. Several familial, maternal and child characteristics were gathered in every follow-up. At the 3-month follow-up, infants whose mothers perceived them as crying more than others of the same age were classified as ‘crying babies’. Child behavioural problems were assessed through the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) applied to the mother at the 48-month follow-up. Crude and adjusted ORs with 95% CIs were calculated by logistic regression.
Results
Prevalence of excessive crying at 3 months was 11.9% (10.9% to 13.0%). Among children with excessive crying at 3 months the proportion in the clinical range for CBCL total, internalising and externalising problems at 4 years of age was 31.2%, 12.9% and 37.5%, respectively, against 20.6%, 6.8% and 29.6%, respectively, among non-crying babies. After controlling for confounders crying babies presented increased risk of being in clinical range of CBCL total (OR=1.34; 1.03 to 1.74), internalising (OR=1.55; 1.09 to 2.21) and externalising problems (OR=1.29; 1.01 to 1.64) than infants without excessive crying.
Conclusions
Excessive crying in early infancy may represent one important risk factor for developing behavioural problems in later phases of early childhood.
Keywords: CHILD HEALTH, Cohort studies, EPIDEMIOLOGY, PRIMARY HEALTH CARE, PUBLIC HEALTH
Introduction
Studies have shown that psychosocial problems in childhood are associated with psychological disorders later in life and that early detection and treatment of psychosocial problems in childhood are important for prevention of psychosocial disorders in adults. Children with behaviour problems are recognised as at increased risk for problems with regulating affect, behaviour and cognition in adulthood.1–6
The prevalence of excessive crying during the first 3 months in representative community-based samples from high income countries has been reported to range between 14% and 29%.7 8 There is no consensus regarding the definition of excessive crying. A frequently used definition is the excessive paroxysmal crying, that is most likely to occur about the same time every day (usually in the late afternoon or evenings) without any identifiable cause in an otherwise healthy baby aged 2 weeks to 4 months and lasting more than 3 h per day, occurring in more than 3 days in any week for 3 weeks (rule of three) that is typically known as colic.9 Colic affects between 9% and 12% of infants from community samples.8 Others give less emphasis to the amount of crying and give relevance to maternal or parental stress due to the child unresponsiveness to soothing10 or to the maternal perception of the intensity of crying.11
Negative consequences of excessive crying on maternal and child health have been described: it is associated with early weaning from breast milk,12–14 frequent changes of formulae,15 and maternal mental symptoms,16 besides being the most common proximal risk factor for shaken baby syndrome.17
The association between excessive crying in the first months of life and preschool children behaviour has been the subject of several studies in high income countries,18–21 whereas scarce information from low income and middle income countries is available. To explain the association between early regulatory problems (excessive crying, sleeping and feeding problems) and behavioural problems it has been suggested that the former may be early markers for similar processes of inadequate or undercontrolled behaviour in childhood.19 The aim of this study was to assess the prevalence of excessive crying at the age of 3 months and to test the hypothesis that excessive crying at this age is associated with behavioural problems when children reach the age of 4 years.
Methods
A birth cohort was started in 2004 in the city of Pelotas (Southern Brazil) when all live births were included between January 1st and December 31st. Only newborns to mothers living in the urban area of Pelotas were eligible to the study. Of a total of 4263 mothers complying with this inclusion criterion, 32 (0.8%) refused to participate in the perinatal study. So far, visits have been carried out to the full cohort when children were about 3, 12, 24, 48 and 72 months of age, with follow-up rates of 95.7%, 94.0%, 93.5%, 92.0%, and 90.2%, respectively. More details of the study can be found elsewhere.22–24 This paper uses information collected at the perinatal study and at the 3-month and the 48-month visits.
Exposure
Excessive crying was assessed at the 3-month visit by asking the mother whether “compared to babies of the same age, her baby cried more, less or as the same”. Infants whose mothers perceived them as crying more than others of the same age were classified as ‘crying babies’.11
Outcome
Child behaviour was assessed through the application to the mother of the 4–18 year version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)25 when children were 4 years of age. The CBCL is designed to be self-completed (by parents), however to maximise response rates, standardisation, accuracy and to prevent embarrassment or error among less literate mothers, questions were read out by trained psychologists in the identical order and form as in the original instrument. The CBCL items are rated on a three-point Likert scale.26 A profile of childhood psychological problems provided scores on eight empirically derived scales: withdrawn, somatic problems, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, aggressive behaviour and rule-breaking behaviour. Data from these scales were summed to provide an overall score (total problems), and were also grouped in two broad dimensions (internalising and externalising problems). A dichotomous classification of CBCL scores comparing ‘clinical’ (positive screening) and ‘non-clinical’ (negative screening) groups was used. The clinical group was identified as those children with a t score higher than 63 points in the CBCL total, externalising or internalising scales according to the CBCL manual.25 In addition, CBCL scores were analysed as a continuous variable in original score units, whereby a higher score reflects greater problems. Mean CBCL scores were compared between crying and non-crying babies using Student t test. Several studies have supported the CBCL's psychometric properties, showing good reliability and validity in clinical and non-clinical populations. The CBCL was validated among Brazilian children by Bordin et al.26
Antenatal and perinatal covariables
Information on potential confounders was gathered at the perinatal and 3-month visits. In the perinatal study, mothers were interviewed and newborns were examined during their stay at the hospital of birth. Maternal age (in complete years at delivery), skin colour (as observed by the interviewer and classified as white, black or mixed), complete years of formal education, smoking (at least 1 cigarette/day daily in at least 1 trimester of pregnancy), number of antenatal care appointments (extracted from the mother's card), type of delivery (vaginal or caesarean section), marital status (living with or without a partner), alcohol consumption during pregnancy, total family income in the month preceding parturition (subsequently divided in quintiles), and maternal heavy caffeine consumption (≥300 mg/day) in the third trimester of pregnancy were gathered at perinatal interview. Presence of maternal mood symptoms during pregnancy was defined through the positive answer to the question: “During pregnancy, did you feel depressed or have any nervous condition?”
Newborns were weighed by the hospital staff using digital paediatric scales with 10 g precision, calibrated weekly to standard weights and birth length was measured by research team using a locally made infantometer. Gestational age was calculated using the first day of the last normal menstrual period or estimated by obstetric ultrasound obtained before 20 weeks of gestation or was estimated from physical and neurological assessment of the newborn (Dubowitz et al’s27 method). Information on preterm birth (<37 weeks of gestational age), firstborn (yes or not), Apgar's score at the fifth minute of life (<7 and ≥7), and type of hospital admission for the newborn (‘staying with the mother’ or “neonatal intermediate or intensive care”) was collected.
Three-month postpartum covariables
At 3 months post partum, maternal mental health was evaluated through the application of the Self-Reporting Questionnaire (SRQ-20).28 29 A validation study in Brazil defined the cut-off ≥8 to identify women in higher risk of common mental disorders.30 Information on maternal heavy caffeine consumption (≥300 mg/day), breastfeeding pattern (exclusive, predominant, partial or weaning) was collected. Bed sharing (mother and child sharing the same surface at night to sleep),31 and use of pacifier was also gathered at 3 months of age.
Statistical analyses
Only infants from singleton pregnancies were included in the analyses. Prevalence rates of excessive crying with 95% CIs (PR; 95% CI) were calculated for every maternal and child characteristic. The association between excessive crying and CBCL scores was assessed through logistic regression. A hierarchical model was used to control for confounders. Variables associated with excessive crying and positive CBCL score at a p level ≤0.20 were kept in the regression model as confounders. In the first step (model 1) the crude effect of excessive crying was measured. In the second step (model 2) the association between excessive crying and behavioural problems was adjusted for maternal antenatal (antenatal care, smoking in pregnancy, caffeine intake in third trimester and maternal mood symptoms in pregnancy) and perinatal variables (family income, maternal age and maternal education), as well as for the newborn characteristics (Apgar at 5th minute and firstborn child). In the third step (model 3), potential confounders from the 3-month follow-up were added to variables of model 2 (maternal SRQ-20, breastfeeding pattern, and bed sharing). ORs with 95% CIs were obtained with Stata V.12.0.
Ethics
The perinatal study and all cohort follow-ups had the research protocol approved by the Medical Research Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Pelotas, affiliated to the Brazilian National Commission for Research Ethics (CONEP). Mothers were requested to fill a written consent for participation in the study.
Results
A total of 3674 mothers replied to the question on excessive crying and to the CBCL questionnaire and were included in the analyses. Prevalence of excessive crying was 11.9% (95% CI 10.9% to 13.0%). Table 1 presents the sample distribution and prevalence of excessive crying according to independent variables. Among the maternal antenatal characteristics, excessive crying was negatively associated with younger age and higher level of formal education. Maternal mood symptoms during pregnancy and positive SRQ-20 at 3 months post partum were strongly associated with excessive crying.
Table 1.
N (%) | Prevalence of excessive crying | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
Antenatal and perinatal maternal and child characteristics | |||
Family income (n=3578) | p=0.070* | ||
1 (poorest) | 707 (19.8) | 13.1 | (10.7 to 15.7) |
2 | 716 (20.0) | 12.3 | (9.9 to 14.7) |
3 | 718 (20.1) | 13.1 | (10.6 to 15.6) |
4 | 752 (21.0) | 10.8 | (8.6 to 13.0) |
5 (richest) | 685 (19.1) | 10.4 | (8.1 to 12.7) |
Maternal age (n=3576) | p=0.020* | ||
<19 | 677 (18.9) | 8.3 | (6.2 to 10.4) |
20–34 | 2405 (67.3) | 12.9 | (11.5 to 14.2) |
≥35 | 494 (13.8) | 12.4 | (9.4 to 15.3) |
Maternal skin colour (n=3578) | p=0.574† | ||
White | 2613 (73.0) | 11.8 | (10.5 to 13.0) |
Black and mixed | 965 (27.0) | 12.4 | (10.4 to 14.7) |
Maternal education (years of study; n=3578) | p=0.002* | ||
0–4 | 534 (15.1) | 16.5 | (13.3 to 19.6) |
5–8 | 1475 (41.6) | 12.3 | (10.6 to 13.9) |
9–11 | 1189 (33.5) | 10.0 | (8.3 to 11.7) |
≥12 | 347 (9.8) | 10.7 | (7.4 to 13.9) |
Live with partner (n=3578) | p=0.266† | ||
No | 560 (15.7) | 10.5 | (8.0 to 13.1) |
Yes | 3018 (84.4) | 12.2 | (11.0 to 13.4) |
Number of antenatal care appointments (n=3523) | p=0.139† | ||
0–4 | 363 (10.6) | 14.9 | (11.4 to 19.0) |
5–10 | 2380 (69.3) | 11.9 | (10.7 to 13.3) |
≥11 | 691 (20.1) | 10.7 | (8.5 to 13.3) |
Smoking (n=3578) | p=0.125† | ||
No | 2608 (72.9) | 11.4 | (10.2 to 12.6) |
Yes | 970 (27.1) | 13.3 | (11.2 to 15.4) |
Alcohol in pregnancy (n=3578) | p=0.682† | ||
No | 3456 (96.6) | 11.9 | (10.8 to 13.0) |
Yes | 122 (3.4) | 13.1 | (7.0 to 19.2) |
Type of delivery (n=3578) | p=0.711† | ||
Vaginal | 1974 (55.2) | 11.8 | (10.3 to 13.2) |
C-section | 1604 (44.8) | 12.2 | (10.6 to 13.8) |
Caffeine intake 3rd trimester (n=3310) | p=0.150† | ||
<300 mg/day | 2724 (82.3) | 11.5 | (10.3 to 12.7) |
≥300 mg/day | 586 (17.7) | 13.7 | (10.9 to 16.4) |
Maternal mood symptoms in pregnancy (n=3577) | p<0.001† | ||
No | 2691 (75.2) | 10.7 | (9.5 to 11.9) |
Yes | 886 (24.8) | 15.7 | (13.3 to 18.1) |
Sex of the newborn (n=3578) | p=0.219† | ||
Male | 1861 (52.0) | 12.6 | 11.1 to 14.1 |
Female | 1717 (48.0) | 11.2 | (9.7 to 12.7) |
Gestational age at birth (n=3574) | p=0.752* | ||
<34 | 85 (2.4) | 15.3 | 7.5 to 23.1 |
34–36 | 372 (10.4) | 12.1 | 8.8 to 15.4 |
37–41 | 2884 (80.7) | 11.8 | 10.6 to 12.9 |
≥42 | 233 (6.5) | 12.9 | 8.5 to 17.2 |
Low birth weight (n=3577) | p=0.603† | ||
No | 3290 (92.0) | 11.9 | 10.7 to 13.0 |
Yes | 287 (8.0) | 12.9 | 9.0 to 16.8 |
Neonatal intensive care (n=3569) | p=0.234† | ||
No | 3393 (95.1) | 11.8 | 10.7 to 12.9 |
Yes | 176 (4.9) | 14.8 | 9.5 to 20.1 |
Apgar 5th minute (n=3560) | p=0.057† | ||
<7 | 55 (1.5) | 3.6 | 0.0 to 8.7 |
≥7 | 3505 (98.5) | 12.0 | 10.9 to 13.1 |
Firstborn (n=3577) | p<0.001† | ||
No | 2006 (56.1) | 14.0 | (12.5 to 15.5) |
Yes | 1571 (43.9) | 9.3 | (7.9 to 10.7) |
Maternal and infant characteristics at 3 months post partum | |||
Caffeine intake (n=3316) | p=0.718† | ||
<300 mg/day | 2817 (85.0) | 11.9 | 10.7 to 13.1 |
≥300 mg/day | 499 (15.0) | 12.4 | 9.5 to 15.3 |
SRQ≥8 (n=3569) | p<0.001† | ||
No | 2649 (74.2) | 9.3 | (8.2 to 10.4) |
Yes | 920 (25.8) | 19.5 | (16.9 to 22.0) |
Breastfeeding (n=3578) | p=0.154* | ||
Weaned | 945 (26.4) | 12.5 | 10.4 to 14.6 |
Partial | 990 (27.7) | 13.1 | 11.0 to 15.2 |
Predominant | 670 (18.7) | 12.2 | 9.8 to 14.7 |
Exclusive | 973 (27.2) | 10.0 | 8.0 to 11.9 |
Bed sharing (n=3578) | p=0.001† | ||
No | 1915 (53.5) | 10.3 | 8.9 to 11.6 |
Yes | 1663 (46.5) | 13.8 | 12.2 to 15.5 |
Use of pacifier (n=3578) | p=0.557† | ||
No | 1220 (34.1) | 12.4 | 10.5 to 14.2 |
Yes | 2358 (65.9) | 11.7 | 10.4 to 13.0 |
*Test for trend.
†χ2 test.
SRQ, Self-Reporting Questionnaire.
None of the newborn characteristics were associated with excessive crying except of not being a firstborn (table 1). At 3 months, infants who shared the bed with their mothers to sleep during the night presented higher prevalence of excessive crying.
Among children with excessive crying at 3 months the proportion in the clinical range for CBCL total, internalising and externalising problems at 4 years of age was 31.2%, 12.9% and 37.5%, respectively. Among non-crying babies CBCL total, internalising and externalising problems were 20.6%, 6.8% and 29.6%, respectively. Table 2 shows the means, SDs and ranges for CBCL total, internalising and externalising problems at the age of 4 years, according to the exposure status at 3 months old.
Table 2.
CBCL scales | Crying babies | Non-crying babies | p Value* | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Range | Mean | SD | Range | ||
Total CBCL | 39.5 | 18.7 | 1–135 | 34.0 | 15.8 | 5–132 | <0.001 |
Internalising problems | 7.6 | 5.7 | 0–43 | 6.1 | 4.4 | 0–32 | <0.001 |
Externalising problems | 17.0 | 7.9 | 1–46 | 15.3 | 7.0 | 1–52 | <0.001 |
*Student t test.
CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist.
In the crude analysis crying babies had higher risk of being in the clinical range for CBCL total, internalising and externalising problems than babies who did not cry a lot (table 3). In the full-adjusted model allowing for antenatal, perinatal and 3-month variables, the magnitude of the association between excessive crying and behavioural problems decreased. However, the association remained statistically significant and babies with excessive crying showed higher risk of being in the clinical range of CBCL total (OR=1.34; 95% CI 1.03 to 1.74), internalising (OR=1.55; 95% CI 1.09 to 2.21) and externalising problems (OR=1.29; 95% CI 1.01 to 1.64) than infants without excessive crying (table 3).
Table 3.
Models | CBCL total | Internalising syndrome | Externalising syndrome | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR (95% CI) | p Value | OR (95% CI) | p Value | OR (95% CI) | p Value | ||
Excessive crying | 1 | 1.75 (1.40 to 2.18) | <0.001 | 2.04 (1.49 to 2.80) | <0.001 | 1.50 (1.21 to 1.85) | <0.001 |
Model 1+antenatal and perinatal variables* | 2 | 1.57 (1.22 to 2.02) | <0.001 | 1.77 (1.26 to 2.50) | 0.001 | 1.43 (1.12 to 1.81) | 0.003 |
Model 2+3-month variables† | 3 | 1.34 (1.03 to 1.74) | 0.028 | 1.55 (1.09 to 2.21) | 0.015 | 1.29 (1.01 to 1.64) | 0.042 |
*Adjusted for family income, maternal age, maternal education, antenatal care, smoking in pregnancy, caffeine intake in third trimester, maternal mood symptoms in pregnancy, Apgar at fifth minute, and firstborn child. †Adjusted for variables of the second model+maternal SRQ-20, breastfeeding pattern, and bed sharing at 3 months.
CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; SRQ, Self-Reporting Questionnaire.
Discussion
This study showed that excessive crying is a prevalent problem and that mothers of crying babies at the age of 3 months reported more problem behaviours when their children reach 4 years of age than mothers of non-crying babies. The reported PR of excessive crying was lower than the described in studies from high income countries. Genetic predisposition, care giving practice or soothing techniques, as well as ethnic differences in maternal perceptions of crying might be among the explanations for these differences. Overall PR may also vary strongly according to the operational definition of excessive crying employed, as well as according to the way the information is collected (retrospective or prospective parental report, diaries or taping registration). In a population-based cross-sectional study involving 3345 Dutch infants at 1–6 months of age, 10 definitions that had been used in previous studies were compared, showing that the overall PR of excessive crying may vary eightfold depending on the definition used.8 In a population-based study conducted with 1826 infants aged 2–3 months in Amsterdam, overall prevalence of ‘crying for three or more hours/day’ in the previous week, ‘crying a lot’, and ‘difficult to comfort’ were 7.6%, 14.0%, and 10.3%, respectively.11 One-fifth (20.3%) of the mothers answered affirmatively to at least one criterion, whereas 14% met all three criteria.11 As seen by the 95% CI the PR under the criterion ‘baby cried a lot’ (14.0%; 12.4% to 15.6%)11 was similar to the observed in the current study.
As perceived by their mothers, crying babies presented a higher prevalence of behavioural problems for both domains of the CBCL at 4 years of age in comparison to non-crying babies. Externalising syndromes were more frequently reported than internalising problems probably because externalising disorders are more easily observable than internal states.32 Perception of the child behaviour as a problem may also be influenced by the presence of maternal depression symptoms and stress.32–34 It is also possible that the lower rate of internalising problems reported is due to the fact that tears and fears may simply be regarded by mothers as common and age-appropriate emotions for 4-year-olds.
The increased risk for problem behaviour observed among crying babies is in agreement with findings from high income countries, and may have important implications for maternal counselling on childcare. A meta-analysis including 22 longitudinal studies to test the association between infant regulatory problems (excessive crying, sleeping and feeding problems) and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood showed that the presence of previous regulatory problems was associated with behavioural problems in childhood and that crying problems led to the highest effect sizes.19
Although excessive crying is usually a benign and self-limiting neurodevelopmental phenomenon, with no need of additional investigations for a child with no signs of illness on a thorough history and examination,35 36 early detection and management of babies who cry a lot may play a role in the primary prevention of behavioural problems in childhood and in adult life. Several methods have been tried to prevent or manage crying babies in the first months of life. Studies comparing parenting style showed that proximal care (prolonged holding, frequent breast feeding, rapid response to infant frets and cries, and bed sharing with infants at night) or even moderate levels of physical contact from birth result in infants with less crying problems than more structured approaches of caring.37 38
At the opposite, St James-Roberts et al39 compared the effectiveness of two interventions introduced from birth (supplementary carrying, increased parental responsiveness) with a control group in reducing the amounts of crying in general community infants at 2, 6, and 12 weeks age. The authors found no differences in amounts of crying and fussing between the three groups of infants.39 Additionally, a comprehensive literature review on the effect of behavioural interventions to reduce parental distress caused by excessive infant crying in the first 6 months of life showed that despite substantial investment in recent years in implementation and evaluation of first wave behavioural interventions for infant sleep in the first 6 months, these strategies have not shown to decrease infant crying, prevent sleep and behavioural problems in later childhood, or protect against postnatal depression, besides being subject to important methodological constraints.40
The strength of this study is the large number of participants with a high rate of follow-up and the population-based design, besides the fact that the predictor variable was assessed in the past when infants were 3 months old, which avoids selective recall bias. This study has several limitations. First, the outcome had to rely on mother-reports of problem behaviour, as the children were too young for self-reports or assessments by teachers or other informants. Second, is the lack of objective, non-maternal evidence that infants reported as crying babies actually cried more than others of the same age. Data on parental behaviour and infant crying collected by parents using behaviour diaries as compared with audio recordings and researcher observations showed that diaries are the leading method for studies of infant crying.37 41 42 However, audiotape recordings are too expensive for use in large samples and prospective diaries in general are not completed in low socioeconomic groups giving place for misclassification bias. Third, similar crying may not be interpreted in the same way by all mothers,43 and babies with excessive crying are not a homogenous group, but rather are part of subgroups that differ in their natural course or aetiology.8 35 44 And fourth, the effect of comorbidity of other regulatory problems in the children (problems with feeding and sleeping) that can contribute to the occurrence of behavioural problems later in childhood,19 was not assessed at the study.
Conclusion
This study showed that while most babies passed through the crying period without long-term effect, a small subgroup of crying babies will screen positively to behavioural problems in later childhood. Considerable gaps and controversies still remain in regard to the effectiveness of available interventions, and further research into prevention and early intervention is required.45 46
Meanwhile, simple general rules such as feeding a hungry baby, changing wet diapers, and comforting a baby who is cold and crying as a result of these, as well as parental attention, including eye contact, talking, touching, rocking, walking and playing, may be effective in some infants and is never harmful.17 38
What is already known on this subject.
Studies with children from high income countries show that excessive crying is a precursor of many problems that emerge in later phases of childhood but studies on this subject from low income and middle income countries are scarce at the literature.
What this study adds.
After controlling for several confounders, mothers of crying babies at the age of 3 months belonging to a large population-based birth cohort study in a middle income country reported more internalising and externalising problem behaviours when their children reached 4 years of age than mothers of non-crying babies.
Footnotes
Contributors: ISS conceptualised and designed the study, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. AM designed the data collection instruments, coordinated and supervised data collection, carried out the analyses, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. MFC carried out the initial analyses, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. LA and FCB critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted.
Funding: This article is based on data from the study “Pelotas Birth Cohort, 2004” conducted by Postgraduate Program in Epidemiology at the Universidade Federal de Pelotas. The 2004 birth cohort study is currently supported by the Wellcome Trust through the program entitled Major Awards for Latin America on Health Consequences of Population Change (Grant no. 086974/Z/08/Z). The World Health Organization (Grant no. 03014HNI), National Support Program for Centers of Excellence (PRONEX) (Grant no. 04/0882.7), Brazilian National Research Council (CNPq) (Grant nos. 481012-2009-5; 484077-2010-4; 470965-2010-0; and 481141-2007-3), Brazilian Ministry of Health (Grant no. 25000.105293/2004-83), and Children's Pastorate supported previous phases of the study. IS and AM are supported by the CNPq.
Competing interests: None.
Patient consent: Obtained.
Ethics approval: Medical Research Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Pelotas, affiliated to the Brazilian National Commission for Research Ethics (CONEP).
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
References
- 1.Althoff RR, Verhulst FC, Rettew DC, et al. . Adult outcomes of childhood dysregulation: a 14-year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:1105–16. 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Aebi M, Plattner B, Metzke CW, et al. . Parent- and self-reported dimensions of oppositionality in youth: construct validity, concurrent validity, and the prediction of criminal outcomes in adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013;54:941–9. 10.1111/jcpp.12039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bellani M, Negri GA, Brambilla P. The dysregulation profile in children and adolescents: a potential index for major psychopathology? Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci 2012;21:155–9. 10.1017/S2045796011000849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Holtmann M, Buchmann AF, Esser G, et al. . The Child Behavior Checklist-Dysregulation Profile predicts substance use, suicidality, and functional impairment: a longitudinal analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011;52:139–47. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02309.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Reef J, Diamantopoulou S, van Meurs I, et al. . Child to adult continuities of psychopathology: a 24-year follow-up. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;120:230–8. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01422.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Scott J, Martin G, Welham J, et al. . Psychopathology during childhood and adolescence predicts delusional-like experiences in adults: a 21-year birth cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2009;166:567–74. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim JS. Excessive crying: behavioral and emotional regulation disorder in infancy. Korean J Pediatr 2011;54:229–33. 10.3345/kjp.2011.54.6.229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Reijneveld S, Brugman E, Hirasing R. Excessive infant crying: the impact of varying definitions. Pediatrics 2001;108:893–7. 10.1542/peds.108.4.893 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wessel M, Cobb J, Jackson E, et al. . Paroxysmal fussing in infancy, sometimes called colic. Pediatrics 1954;14:421–35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Geertsma M, Hyams J. Colic: a pain syndrome of infancy? Pediatr Clin North Am 1989;36:905–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.van der Wal MF, van den Boom DC, Pauw-Plomp H, et al. . Mothers’ reports of infant crying and soothing in a multicultural population. Arch Dis Child 1998;79:312–17. 10.1136/adc.79.4.312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Forsyth B, Leventhal J, McCarthy P. Mothers’ perceptions of problems of feeding and crying behaviors. A prospective study. Am J Dis Child 1985;139:269–72. 10.1001/archpedi.1985.02140050063023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cunningham A, Jelliffe D, Jelliffe E. Breast-feeding and health in the 1980s: a global epidemiologic review. J Pediatr 1991;118:659–66. 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)80023-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Barros F, Halpern R, Victora C, et al. . Promotion of breast-feeding in urban localities of southern Brazil: a randomized intervention study. Rev Saude Publica 1994;28:277–83. 10.1590/S0034-89101994000400006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Iacovou M, Ralston R, Muir J, et al. . Dietary management of infantile colic: a systematic review. Matern Child Health J 2012;16:1319–31. 10.1007/s10995-011-0842-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Staehelina K, Kurtha E, Schindlera C, et al. . Predictors of early postpartum mental distress in mothers with midwifery home care—results from a nested case-control study. Swiss Med Wkly 2013;143:w13862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Barr RG, Rivara FP, Barr M, et al. . Effectiveness of educational materials designed to change knowledge and behaviors regarding crying and shaken-baby syndrome in mothers of newborns: a randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 2009;123:972–80. 10.1542/peds.2008-0908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Barnevik Olsson M, Carlsson LH, Westerlund J, et al. . Autism before diagnosis: crying, feeding and sleeping problems in the first two years of life. Acta Paediatr 2013;102:635–9. 10.1111/apa.12229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hemmi MH, Wolke D, Schneider S. Associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood: a meta-analysis. Arch Dis Child 2011;96:622–9. 10.1136/adc.2010.191312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Korja R, Huhtala M, Maunu J, et al. . Preterm infant's early crying associated with child's behavioral problems and parents’ stress. Pediatrics 2014;133:e339–45. 10.1542/peds.2013-1204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schmid G, Schreier A, Meyer R, et al. . A prospective study on the persistence of infant crying, sleeping and feeding problems and preschool behaviour. Acta Paediatr 2010;99:286–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Barros AJ, da Silva dos Santos I, Victora CG, et al. . [The 2004 Pelotas birth cohort: methods and description]. Rev Saude Publica 2006;40:402–13. 10.1590/S0034-89102006000300007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Santos IS, Barros AJ, Matijasevich A, et al. . Cohort profile: the 2004 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:1461–8. 10.1093/ije/dyq130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Santos IS, Barros AJ, Matijasevich A, et al. . Cohort profile update: 2004 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study. Body composition, mental health and genetic assessment at the 6 years follow-up. Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1437–1437a-f. 10.1093/ije/dyu144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Achenbach T. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bordin I, Mari J, Caeiro M. Validação da versão Brasileira do “Child Behavior Checklist” (CBCL) (Inventário de Comportamentos da Infância e Adolescência): dados preliminares. Revista ABP-APAL 1995;17:55–66. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dubowitz LM, Dubowitz V, Goldberg C. Clinical assessment of gestational age in the newborn infant. J Pediatr 1970;77:1–10. 10.1016/S0022-3476(70)80038-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Harpham T, Reichenheim M, Oser R, et al. . Measuring mental health in a cost-effective manner. Health Policy Plan 2003;18:344–9. 10.1093/heapol/czg041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.World Health Organization. Useŕs guide to the Self-Reporting Questionnaire—SRQ. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mari JJ, Williams P. A validity study of a psychiatric screening questionnaire (SRQ-20) in primary care in the city of Sao Paulo. Br J Psychiatry 1986;148:23–6. 10.1192/bjp.148.1.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Canadian Pediatric Society. Recommendations for safe sleeping environments for infants and children. Paediatr Child Health 2004;9:659–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kolko DJ, Kazdin AE. Emotional/behavioral problems in clinic and nonclinic children: correspondence among child, parent and teacher reports. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1993;34:991–1006. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01103.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Berg-Nielsen TS, Vika A, Dahl AA. When adolescents disagree with their mothers: CBCL-YSR discrepancies related to maternal depression and adolescent self-esteem. Child Care Health Dev 2003;29:207–13. 10.1046/j.1365-2214.2003.00332.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.De Los Reyes A, Goodman KL, Kliewer W, et al. . Whose depression relates to discrepancies? Testing relations between informant characteristics and informant discrepancies from both informants’ perspectives. Psychol Assess 2008;20:139–49. 10.1037/1040-3590.20.2.139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Douglas P, Hill P. Managing infants who cry excessively in the first few months of life. BMJ 2011;343:d7772 10.1136/bmj.d7772 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Douglas PS, Hill PS. The crying baby: what approach? Curr Opin Pediatr 2011;23:523–9. 10.1097/MOP.0b013e32834a1b78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.St James-Roberts I, Alvarez M, Csipke E, et al. . Infant crying and sleeping in London, Copenhagen and when parents adopt a “proximal” form of care. Pediatrics 2006;117:e1146–55. 10.1542/peds.2005-2387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kheir AE. Infantile colic, facts and fiction. Ital J Pediatr 2012;38:34 10.1186/1824-7288-38-34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- 39.St James-Roberts I, Hurry J, Bowyer J, et al. . Supplementary carrying compared with advice to increase responsive parenting as interventions to prevent persistent infant crying. Pediatrics 1995;95:381–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Douglas PS, Hill PS. Behavioral sleep interventions in the first six months of life do not improve outcomes for mothers or infants: a systematic review. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2013;34:497–507. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31829cafa6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.St James-Roberts I, Hurry J, Bowyer J. Objective confirmation of crying durations in infants referred for excessive crying. Arch Dis Child 1993;68:82–4. 10.1136/adc.68.1.82 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wake M, Morton-Allen E, Poulakis Z, et al. . Prevalence, stability and outcomes of cry-fuss and sleep problems in the first two years of life: prospective community-based study. Pediatrics 2006;117:836–42. 10.1542/peds.2005-0775 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.James-Roberts I, Conroy S, Wilsher K. Bases for maternal perceptions of infant crying and colic behavior. Arch Dis Child 1996;75:375–84. 10.1136/adc.75.5.375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Miller J, Newell D. Prognostic significance of subgroup classification for infant patients with crying disorders: a prospective cohort study. J Can Chiropr Assoc 2012;56:40–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Douglas PS, Miller Y, Bucetti A, et al. . Preliminary evaluation of a primary care intervention for cry-fuss behaviours in the first 3–4 months of life (?The Possums Approach. Aust J Prim Health 2013 Sep 2. doi: 10.1071/PY13011. [Epub ahead of print] 10.1071/PY13011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Douglas PS, Hill PS. A neurobiological model for cry-fuss problems in the first three to four months of life. Med Hypotheses 2013;81y:816–22. 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]