Abstract
Purpose
To determine which vision tests predict mortality within 10 years in a community-based elderly sample.
Methods
Nine hundred residents of Marin County, CA, aged 58 to 101 (mean 75 years at baseline), underwent a battery of tests, including high contrast acuity, low contrast acuity, low contrast/low luminance acuity, acuity in glare, contrast sensitivity, color vision, stereopsis, standard and attentional fields. The association between the vision tests and mortality within 10 years of baseline was assessed with Cox Proportional Hazards models controlling for age, sex, education level, depression, cognitive status and self-reported medical conditions.
Results
Forty-three percent of the sample died within 10 years of baseline. When controlling for mortality-related covariates, impairment in any of the vision measures was associated with increased risk of death. However, non-standard vision measures (i.e. impairment in low contrast/low luminance acuity, standard field integrity and the impact of the attentional task on field integrity) were more highly associated with mortality than standard high contrast acuity.
Conclusions
In agreement with other studies, we find that visual impairment is a significant predictor of death. However, the strongest relationship was found for measures other than high contrast acuity. These results suggest that non-standard vision measures may be more sensitive indicators of generalized aging in the oldest-old.
Keywords: Aging, Attentional visual fields, Mortality, SKILL Dark Acuity, Visual impairment
Introduction
Many researchers have investigated the relationship between vision and mortality in aging individuals. Most report statistically significant increases in the risk of death with eye diseases1-17 (But see also18) and visual impairment.4,9,15,19-30 (But see also17)
To our knowledge, of the studies that have assessed the relationship between visual impairment and mortality, only one has included a vision measure other than high contrast acuity. Pedula et.al. (2006) found that women with reductions in high contrast acuity or contrast sensitivity had a higher mortality rate than those with good vision on either measure.26
Some authors have begun to question the strong reliance on standard acuity for vision assessment (for a review, see31). Clearly, high contrast acuity is the “gold standard” used by clinicians and researchers alike, but recent research suggests that it may not be the best predictor of everyday function, particularly when considering the rapidly growing elderly population.32-38 Studies have shown that, although elders maintain good high contrast acuity into old age, the same individuals can show a wide range of performance on non-standard vision tests.36,39,40
The purpose of the current study is to confirm the previously reported association between visual impairment and mortality, but also to extend our understanding of this relationship by including vision measures other than standard high contrast acuity. Models were adjusted for age, sex, self-reported medical conditions, depression and cognitive status, since these variables are associated with mortality. (e.g. 41-44)
Materials and Methods
Participants
Smith-Kettlewell Institute (SKI) Vision Study32,39 participants were a sub-sample of the Buck Center for Research in Aging's (BCRA) population-based study of health and functioning (H&F) of older adults in Marin County, CA. The BCRA Study has been described in detail previously.45 Briefly, the BCRA cohort (non-institutionalized residents of Marin County, CA, aged 55 and older) was identified through Health Care Financing Administration of Medicare-eligible residents (those 65 years and older) and through random digit dialing. Sixty-nine percent (N=2018) of those eligible agreed to participate in the baseline wave of the BCRA H&F Study (between 1989-91), and N=1521 participated in the follow-up BCRA H&F wave (1992-95).
Potential subjects for the SKI Study Vision sample were identified from those who had participated in the BCRA H&F follow-up (N=1521). Of these, 92.8% were eligible and invited to participate in the SKI Vision Study (N=1410). This included all participants aged 80 years and older, and a random sample of 88% of those younger than 80. Of the 1410 subjects eligible to participate, 9% had died since completing the H&F follow-up, 27% refused, and <1% could not be located. Nine hundred elders (63.8%) participated in the SKI Study at baseline (11/92-12/95), and the remaining sample was tested at three subsequent follow-up (FU) waves of testing (i.e. FU1: 1/98-7/99, N=597; FU2: 1/00-3/02, N=452; FU3: 2/05-12/06, N=254), and the final wave began in 7/08.
This study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) for Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute and the California Pacific Medical Center. At each wave of testing, an explanation of all test procedures was given and each participant signed the IRB-approved consent form.
Measures
Vision Measures
The SKI Study was intended to assess vision in a group of elderly individuals under everyday viewing conditions; all testing was completed binocularly with the participant's presenting (habitual) correction (glasses or contact lenses).
The vision test battery used for the SKI Study has been described in detail elsewhere.39 The vision tests used for the current analysis are listed below.
High- and low-contrast distance acuity were assessed using Bailey-Lovie charts46,47 at a 3 m viewing distance.
Low contrast/low luminance acuity was assessed with the dark chart of the Smith-Kettlewell Low Luminance (SKILL) Card48 at a 40 cm viewing distance. The SKILL Dark chart consists of black letters on a dark grey background.
Low contrast acuity in the presence of surrounding glare was assessed with the Berkeley Glare Test49 at 40 cm.
Contrast Sensitivity was assessed using the Pelli-Robson Chart50 at a 3 m viewing distance. The chart was scored letter by letter (each letter corresponds to 0.05 log unit).51
Stereopsis was assessed using the Frisby Test.52 This test measures the type of depth perception that requires the two eyes to operate together, and is important for near tasks. The test consists of 3 plates of different thickness. At a viewing distance of 40 cm, the plates yield disparities of 340, 170, and 85 arcsec, with smaller numbers indicating better performance.
Color vision was assessed using the Farnsworth Panel D-15 Test.53 This test requires that participants demonstrate color discrimination by placing colored caps in the appropriate order starting at a fixed color. The calculated Color Confusion Score indicates the severity of the color defect. A score of 0 indicates no arrangement errors (i.e. perfect performance).54
Standard and attentional visual fields were measured with a modified Synemed perimeter.39 The test consisted of detection of bright, peripheral flashed targets without and with an added attentional task (silently counting central flashes). Standard field score was the percentage of locations missed (corrected for false-positive responses). The attentional field was scored as the percentage of locations missed (corrected for false positive responses AND the difference between actual and reported central attentional flashes). The difference between the attentional and standard field scores yielded a measure of the impact of attention on visual field integrity. This measure is somewhat similar to the useful field of view.55,56
All acuity measures (1-3 above) were scored as number of letters read correctly and converted to logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR). These acuities are also reported as a “Snellen equivalent” (e.g. 20/20).
For each vision measure, two visual impairment “pass/fail” criteria were computed, and participants were coded as 0 (pass) or 1 (fail). These criteria were devised to define visual impairment for the non-standard acuity measures that are comparable to those of standard high contrast acuity. For high contrast acuity, Criterion 1 was “worse than 20/40” or >0.30 logMAR, which is equivalent to 3 lines (0.30 log units) poorer than the “normal” visual acuity of 20/20 (0.0 logMAR). Criterion 2 was “worse than 20/70” or >0.54 logMAR, which is equivalent to 5.4 lines poorer than “normal” acuity.
The first criterion (20/40) is the requirement for driver's licensure, and has been used by many previous mortality studies. 4,9,15,17 The second criterion (20/70) is used to qualify an individual legally as visually impaired and thus eligible for the associated services. Separate analyses were run using the two visual impairment criteria.
A method similar to that used for high contrast acuity was employed to establish comparable criteria for the other vision measures used in this study. For each measure, a “normal” value was defined (i.e. a value expected in a young, healthy individual). For spatial vision measures (i.e. acuities and contrast sensitivity: 1-4 above), Criteria 1 and 2 were defined as 0.30 and 0.54 log units worse than this young, normative value. Normal values were defined for stereopsis, color vision, standard field integrity and the impact of attentional task on visual field integrity, and cut-off values were selected. Table 1 presents the normal values and the two visual impairment cut-off criteria used for each vision variable.
Table 1.
Vision Variables | “Normal” Values* | Visual Impairment Pass/Fail Criteria* | |
---|---|---|---|
Criterion 1 | Criterion 2 | ||
High Contrast Acuity | 20/20 (0.0 logMAR) | 20/40 (>0.30 logMAR) | 20/70 (>0.54 logMAR) |
Low Contrast Acuity | 20/27 (0.12 logMAR) | 20/53 (>0.42 logMAR) | 20/91 (>0.66 logMAR) |
Low Contrast/Low Luminance Acuity | 20/40 (0.30 logMAR) | 20/80 (>0.60 logMAR) | 20/138 (>0.84 logMAR) |
Low Contrast Acuity in Glare | 20/40 (0.30 logMAR) | 20/80 (>0.60 logMAR) | 20/138 (>0.84 logMAR) |
Contrast Sensitivity | 1.85 log units (1.4%) | <1.55 log units (2.8%) | <1.30 log units (5.0%) |
Stereopsis | 16 arcsec (pass all 3 plates) | >85 arcsec (fail ≥ 1 plate) | >320 arcsec (fail all 3 plates) |
D-15 Color Vision | Color confusion score = 0 | Color confusion score ≥30 | Color confusion score ≥100 |
Standard Visual Field | 0 errors | ≥ 5% errors | ≥ 10% errors |
Impact of Attentional Task on Field | 0 errors | ≥ 25% errors | ≥ 50% errors |
Acuity measures are presented in Snellen equivalent and (logMAR). Contrast Sensitivity is presented in log units and % Weber contrast.
Other Covariates
Baseline age and years of education were included as continuous variables. The remaining covariates were coded as dichotomous variables.
The Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D)57,58 was used to assess the presence of depressive symptoms. The CES-D for each participant was categorized as ‘not depressed’ (CES-D score <16) or ‘depressed’ (score of 16 or more).
The Short Portable Mental Status Questionnaire (SPMSQ)59 was used to indicate cognitive status. The SPMSQ was categorized as ‘no cognitive impairment’ (< 3 errors) or ‘cognitively impaired’ (3 or more errors).
Self-reported medical conditions were obtained by asking each participant “Have you been told by a doctor that you have….” (heart disease, hypertension, cancer, diabetes, stroke, arthritis). Each variable was categorized as ‘not reported’ or ‘reported’.
Mortality Status
Date of death was obtained from the Social Security Death Index, local newspaper obituaries, or reports from relatives. Years from baseline to date of death was calculated for each participant, and those known to be alive for over 10 years after their baseline measure or missing were censored.
Data Analysis
Stata/SE 9.2 for Macintosh (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX) was used for all data analyses. To assess the difference between survivors and those who died, t-tests were used for continuous variables, and chi square tests were used for categorical measures.
Initial univariate analyses to assess the risk of death within 10 years of baseline were conducted using Kaplan-Meier estimates, followed by multivariate Cox proportional hazards models adjusting for age, sex, and all possible covariates. Proportional hazards assumptions for the separate models were tested using Stata's linktest, as well as tests based on the Schoenfeld residuals.60
Preliminary analyses also included all possible “age by variable” and “sex by variable” interaction terms. None was significant (p<0.10) and therefore interaction terms were therefore excluded from the final analyses.
Results
The mean age of the 900 participants at baseline was 75.5 years (SD=9.3, range=58.4-101.9), and 46.0% of the participants were male. As noted previously,38 this is a highly educated sample, with 73.4% reporting more than 12 years of education. Within 10 years of the baseline test, 43.0% (N=387) of the participants had died. Mean time to death for these individuals was 5.18 years (SD=2.65, range=0.08-9.86). Table 2 presents the baseline characteristics for the total sample, those who were still alive, and those who died within 10 years of baseline. As expected from previous studies, those who died were more likely than the survivors to be older, male, less well-educated, cognitively impaired, and to self-report a history of several medical conditions. Participants who died within 10 years of baseline were not significantly more likely than survivors to be depressed, although there was a trend in this direction (p=0.07).
Table 2.
Baseline Characteristics | Total Sample (N=900) | Alive (> 10 years after Baseline) (N=513) | Died (within 10 years of Baseline (N=387) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age, mean (SD), years | 75.5 (9.3) | 71.4 (7.4) | 80.8 (8.9) | |
Sex (% male) | 46.0% | 42.3% | 50.9%* | |
Education, mean (SD), years | 14.7 (3.0) | 15.1 (2.6) | 14.2 (3.5) | |
% Depressed (CES-D Score >15) | 7.8% | 6.4% | 9.6%† | |
% Cognitive Impairment (SPMSQ>2) | 5.5% | 3.7% | 8.0% | |
% Self-Reported Medical History: | ||||
Hypertension | 42.4% | 38.5% | 47.6% | |
Heart Disease | 34.7% | 25.6% | 46.8% | |
Cancer | 30.6% | 28.0% | 34.1%* | |
Stroke | 8.3% | 4.3% | 13.7% | |
Diabetes | 7.7% | 6.2% | 9.6%† | |
Arthritis | 35.8% | 30.9% | 42.4% |
Alive vs. Died: All statistically significant (p<0.005) except where noted:
p<0.05,
p>0.05
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics for the vision variables separately for the survivors and for those who died. Means and standard deviations for each measure are presented, along with the percentage of participants who failed according to the two sets of visual impairment pass/fail criteria. Regardless of whether the data were analyzed as continuous measures (t-tests) or as dichotomous measures (chi squares), each vision measure was significantly worse in the group who died within 10 years of baseline when compared with survivors (P<0.0001).
Table 3.
Alive | Died | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Vision Variables | Mean (SD) | Criterion 1 % Fail | Criterion 2 % Fail | Mean (SD) | Criterion 1 % Fail | Criterion 2 % Fail |
High Contrast Acuity (logMAR) | 0.07 (0.18) | 6.2 | 1.7 | 0.25 (0.29) | 26.4 | 9.9 |
Low Contrast Acuity (logMAR) | 0.30 (0.21) | 19.9 | 5.4 | 0.53 (0.31) | 53.7 | 25.9 |
Low Contrast/Low Luminance Acuity (logMAR) | 0.61 (0.21) | 42.8 | 11.6 | 0.88 (0.34) | 77.8 | 46.5 |
Low Contrast Acuity in Glare (logMAR) | 0.72 (0.29) | 56.8 | 19.7 | 1.07 (0.47) | 85.1 | 58.2 |
Contrast sensitivity (log units) | 1.63 (0.27) | 27.9 | 6.0 | 1.32 (0.43) | 64.9 | 31.2 |
Stereopsis (plates passed) | 2.43 (0.93) | 35.3 | 9.3 | 1.67 (1.40) | 58.7 | 32.7 |
D-15 Color Vision (Color confusion Score) | 16.1 (42.2) | 15.3 | 4.4 | 40.5 (71.6) | 35.5 | 11.7 |
Standard Visual Field (% errors) | 3.4 (8.7) | 19.1 | 7.3 | 9.7 (14.7) | 54.3 | 33.4 |
Impact of Attentional Task on Field (% errors) | 21.5 (22.6) | 34.9 | 10.6 | 37.5 (26.6) | 64.2 | 27.4 |
Alive vs. Died: All statistically significant (p<0.0001). See Table 1 for the pass/fail criteria for each individual vision variable.
The results of Cox Proportional Hazards Models are presented in Table 4. This table present hazards ratios (HRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs), for each individual vision variable adjusted for covariates (age, sex, education level, cognitive impairment, depression, and each individual self-reported medical condition) for both visual impairment criteria.
Table 4.
Vision Variables | Visual Impairment Criterion 1 HR (95% CI) | Visual Impairment Criterion 2 HR (95% CI) |
---|---|---|
High Contrast Acuity | 1.27 (0.98-1.65)§ | 1.49 (1.05-2.13)‡ |
Low Contrast Acuity | 1.29 (1.01-1.64)‡ | 1.42 (1.09-1.85)† |
Low Contrast/Low Luminance Acuity | 1.47 (1.11-1.94)† | 1.83 (1.45-2.32)* |
Low Contrast Acuity in Glare | 1.45 (1.05-2.00)‡ | 1.53 (1.19-1.96)† |
Contrast Sensitivity | 1.32 (1.02-1.71)‡ | 1.59 (1.24-2.05)* |
Stereopsis | 1.33 (1.07-1.66)‡ | 1.56 (1.23-1.98)* |
D-15 Color Vision | 1.32 (1.05-1.65)‡ | 1.11 (0.80-1.54)§ |
Standard Visual Field Integrity | 1.68 (1.33-2.11)* | 1.92 (1.51-2.45)* |
Impact of Attentional Task on Field | 1.51 (1.20-1.91)* | 1.40 (1.11-1.78)† |
Models adjusted for all factors listed in Table 2 (age, sex, education, depression, cognitive impairment, and self-reported health conditions). HR= Hazard Ratio, CI = Confidence Interval. Significance levels:
p <0.001;
p<0.01;
p<0.05;
p>0.05.
See Table 1 for the pass/fail criteria for each vision variable.
When using Visual Impairment Criterion 1 (Table 4: high contrast acuity worse than 20/40, reduced by >0.30 log units with comparable reductions from normal in other vision measures), all individual vision variables except high contrast acuity are significantly associated with mortality even when controlling for all other covariates. HRs range from 1.30 (CI: 1.02-1.66) for low contrast acuity to 1.69 (CI:1.35-2.13) for standard visual field integrity. When Visual Impairment Criterion 2 is used (Table 4: high contrast acuity worse than 20/70, reduced by >0.54 log units with comparable reductions from normal in non-standard measures), all individual variables except color vision remain significant when other covariates are included. HRs range from 1.40 (CI: 1.11-1.78) for impact of attentional task on visual field integrity, to 1.94 (CI: 1.52-2.47) for standard visual field integrity.
Since many of the vision variables under investigation are significantly correlated (particularly the spatial vision measures), including all simultaneously in regression models would cause their effects to cancel out. Therefore, stepwise Cox proportional hazards regression models were used to determine the vision variables with the strongest relationship to mortality while adjusting for all other covariates. Both backward and forward stepping models were run, and these yielded the same significant vision variables for both visual impairment criteria: Low contrast, low luminance acuity, standard visual field, and the impact of attentional task on visual fields.
The final model including all covariates along with these three vision variables for the two Visual Impairment Criteria is presented in Table 5. HRs and CIs are provided for all variables. For Visual Impairment Criterion 1 (i.e. reduction in vision comparable to 20/40 acuity), age, sex, self reported heart disease, low contrast/low luminance acuity, standard visual field and impact of attentional task on visual field are statistically significant. The same variables were also significant for Criterion 2 (i.e. reduction in vision comparable to 20/70 acuity), along with self-reported history of stroke.
Table 5.
Variable | Visual Impairment Criterion 1 HRs (95% CIs) | Visual Impairment Criterion 2 HRs (95% CIs) |
---|---|---|
Age (per 5 years) | 1.32 (1.22-1.43)* | 1.33 (1.24-1.43)* |
Sex (male) | 1.48 (1.20-1.82)* | 1.49 (1.21-1.84)* |
Education (per year) | 0.99 (0.95-1.02)§ | 0.99 (0.96-1.03)§ |
Cognitive Impairment (SPMSQ >2 errors) | 1.23 (0.85-1.79)§ | 1.18 (0.81-1.72)§ |
Depression (CESD score >15) | 1.04 (0.74-1.47)§ | 1.08 (0.77-1.54)§ |
Hypertension (self report) | 1.20 (0.98-1.48)§ | 1.20 (0.97-1.48)§ |
Heart Disease (self report) | 1.64 (1.34-2.01)* | 1.54 (1.26-1.89)* |
Cancer (self report) | 1.05 (0.61-1.79)§ | 1.13 (0.67-1.92)§ |
Stroke (self report) | 1.33 (0.84-2.12)§ | 1.60 (1.01-2.53)‡ |
Diabetes (self report) | 1.08 (0.76-1.54)§ | 0.96 (0.67-1.37)§ |
Arthritis (self report) | 1.05 (0.59-1.86)§ | 1.02 (0.58-1.81)§ |
Low Contrast/Low Luminance Acuity | 1.39 (1.04-1.84)‡ | 1.70 (1.33-2.16)* |
Standard Visual Field Integrity | 1.61 (1.28-2.02)* | 1.69 (1.33-2.17)* |
Impact of Attentional Task on Field | 1.46 (1.16-1.84)† | 1.37 (1.08-1.73) † |
Adjusted for all variables listed in this table. HR= Hazard Ratio, CI = Confidence Interval.
Significance levels:
p <0.001;
p<0.01;
p<0.05;
p>0.05.
See Table 1 for the pass/fail criterion for each vision variable.
Discussion
Each of the vision measures assessed was associated with mortality at either visual impairment criterion. However, when controlling for age, sex and other mortality-related covariates, visual impairment defined as high contrast acuity poorer than 20/40 or 20/70 was not as strong a predictor as other vision measures with equivalent criteria as defined by reduction from young normal. In fact, for the 20/40 criterion commonly used, high contrast acuity was not a significant predictor of death when age is included in the model.
These results are consistent with those reported by Knudtson et al (2006), that visual impairment was associated with reduced survival for the younger (<65) but not older (65-84) age groups15. Similarly, Kulmala et. al. (2008) found that visual acuity was associated with mortality in 75-year-old participants, but not 80-year olds28. The mean age of our sample at baseline was 75.5 years, with ages ranging from 58 to 103 years. It is likely that the older age of our participants could account for the weaker association between high contrast acuity and mortality.
Most other studies evaluating the relationship between visual impairment and mortality used a visual impairment criterion of 20/40 best-corrected in better eye, 4,9,15,17 whereas our tests were all performed with habitual (presenting) correction under binocular viewing conditions. Since uncorrected refractive error is a common occurrence with older individuals,61-64 our 20/40 cut-off likely included a number of participants who would have been classified as unimpaired with the proper spectacle correction. This could account for our lack of a significant association between high contrast acuity and mortality when controlling for other covariates (Table 4, Criterion 1).
We do not regard this as weak point for our study, since it could be argued that testing vision under participants' normal everyday viewing conditions would be a more valid indication of their functional abilities,38 and some have recommended that the International Classification of Disease (ICD) criterion for defining visual impairment be changed to habitual (presenting) acuity.65, 66
The findings of the current study are somewhat different from the results reported by Pedula et al (2006), who also used habitual, binocular viewing conditions.26 When using all-cause mortality as the outcome measure, these investigators found that both high contrast acuity and contrast sensitivity were independent predictors. Although these authors state that Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity was used, the description provided appears to be for grating contrast sensitivity, so test differences could contribute to the lack of agreement between the studies. Furthermore, since the data in that study were evaluated in quartiles of increased impairment, it is difficult to compare directly to the current study or to address in relationship to clinically meaningful standards.
It is likely that the non-standard vision measures investigated in the current study serve as surrogates for some eye conditions that have been shown to be associated with mortality. We would hypothesize that failure on such tests in the presence of normal visual acuity is indicative of subclinical eye disease. As stated previously, eye diseases such as cataracts, age-related macular degeneration and glaucoma are significantly associated with mortality. We cannot directly test this hypothesis, because information on eye disease was not available for all participants. Although all participants were asked to provide permission to access their eye records, many records were unavailable due to: participant refusal, records were not provided by the participant's eye practitioner, and several participants reported never having seen an eye doctor. Furthermore, many of the records obtained were not concurrent with vision test dates in a manner that would allow us to definitively say whether the participant had eye disease at the baseline test.
Complete information on all three age-related eye conditions was available for 803 participants (89.2% of the sample). Partial information (incomplete for one or two eye conditions) was available for 73 participants (8.1% of the sample), and no information was available for an additional 24 participants (2.7% of the sample). When separate analyses including existing eye condition information were conducted (either excluding those with missing data or using imputation techniques to replace missing values), the pattern of results was similar to that reported in Tables 4 and 5. As expected, the associations between each of the spatial vision measures and mortality were reduced.
Further research is needed to assess the potential public health impact of this finding. We suggest that, in addition to regular medical and vision screenings, older individuals could be given simple non-standard vision tests, and those who do poorly could be evaluated in a more comprehensive health evaluation.
In conclusion, we find that non-standard vision tests, and in particular low contrast, low luminance acuity and visual field integrity in the absence or presence of an additional attentional task, are significant predictors of mortality within 10 years in this sample of community-dwelling elders. Furthermore, these non-standard vision tests appear to be more sensitive predictors of mortality than high contrast acuity in this elderly sample.
Acknowledgments
We are deeply grateful to Ruth Youngquist, the volunteers from The Buck Institute for Age Research, and the SKI Study participants. This research was supported by a National Institute of Health (NIH) Grant EY09588 to John Brabyn and by the Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute. These results were presented, in part, at the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, 2006 in Ft. Lauderdale, FL.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interest: The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.
References
- 1.Rogot E, Goldberg ID, Goldstein H. Survivorship and causes of death among the blind. J Chronic Dis. 1966;19:179–197. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(66)90048-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hirsch RP, Schwartz B. Increased mortality rates after cataract extraction. Arch Ophthalmol. 1983;101:1034–1037. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1983.01040020036004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Benson WH, Farver ME, Caplan RJ. Increased mortality rates after cataract surgery. Ophthalmology. 1988;95:1288–1292. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(88)33019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Klein R, Klein BEK, Moss SE. Age-related eye disease and survival: The Beaver Dam Eye Study. Arch Ophthalmol. 1995;113:333–339. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1995.01100030089026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hiller R, Podgor MJ, Sperduto RD, Wilson PWF, Chew EY, D'Agostino RB. High intraocular pressure and survival: The Framingham Studies. Am J Ophthamol. 1999;128:440–445. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9394(99)00187-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Klein R, Klein BEK, Moss SE, Cruickshanks KJ. Association of ocular disease and mortality in a diabetic population. Arch Ophthalmol. 1999;117:1487–1495. doi: 10.1001/archopht.117.11.1487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.West SK, Munoz B, Istre J, et al. Mixed lens opacities and subsequent mortality. Arch Ophthalmol. 2000;118:393–397. doi: 10.1001/archopht.118.3.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hennis A, Wu S, Li X, Nemesure B, Leske MC The Barbados Eye Studies Group. Lens opacities and mortality: The Barbados Eye Studies. Ophthalmology. 2001;108:498–504. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(00)00542-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wang JJ, Mitchell P, Simpson JM, Cumming RG, Smith W. Visual impairment, age-related cataract and mortality. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001;119:1186–1190. doi: 10.1001/archopht.119.8.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Williams SL, Ferrigno L, Mora P, Rosmini F, Maraini G. Baseline cataract type and 10-year mortality in the Italian-American Case-Control Study of Age-related Cataract. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156:127–31. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lee DJ, Gomez-Marin O, Lam BL, Zheng DD. Glaucoma and survival: The National Health Interview Survey 1986-1994. Ophthalmology. 2003;110:1476–1483. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(03)00408-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Clemons TE, Kurinij N, Sperduto RD, Bressler SB AREDS Research Group. Associations of mortality with ocular disorders and an intervention of high-dose antioxidants and zinc in the Age-Related Eye Disease Study: AREDS Report No. 13. Arch Ophthalmol. 2004;122:716–726. doi: 10.1001/archopht.122.5.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nucci C, Cedrone C, Culasso F, Cesareo M, Regine F, Cerulli L. Association between lens opacities and mortality in the Priverno Eye Study. Graufe's Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2004;242:289–294. doi: 10.1007/s00417-003-0837-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Buch H, Vinding T, la Cour M, Jensen GB, Prause JU, Nielsen NV. Age-related maculopathy: A risk indication for poorer survival in women. The Copenhagen City Eye Study. Ophthalmology. 2005;112:305–312. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2004.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Knudtson MD, Klein BEK, Klein R. Age-related eye disease, visual impairment and survival: The Beaver Dam Eye Study. Arch Ophthalmol. 2006;124:243–249. doi: 10.1001/archopht.124.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lee AJ, Wang JJ, Kifley A, Mitchell P. Open-angle glaucoma and cardiovascular mortality. Ophthalmology. 2006;113:1069–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2006.02.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cugati S, Cumming RG, Smith W, Burlutsky G, Mitchell P, Wang JJ. Visual impairment, age-related macular degeneration, cataract, and long-term mortality: The Blue Mountains Eye Study. Arch Ophthalmol. 2007;125:917–924. doi: 10.1001/archopht.125.7.917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Borger PH, van Leeuwen R, Hulsman CAA, et al. Is there a direct association between age-related eye diseases and mortality? The Rotterdam Study. Ophthalmology. 2003;110:1292–1296. doi: 10.1016/S0161-6420(03)00450-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Thompson JR, Gibson JM, Jagger G. The association between visual impairment and mortality in elderly people. Age Ageing. 1989;18:83–88. doi: 10.1093/ageing/18.2.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Appollonio I, Carabellese C, Magni E, Frattola L, Trabucchi M. Sensory impairment and mortality in an elderly community population: A six year follow-up study. Age Ageing. 1995;24:30–36. doi: 10.1093/ageing/24.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Giles LC, Andrews GR. Demographic, health, cognitive and sensory variables as predictors of mortality in very old adults. Psychol Aging. 2001;16:3–11. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McCarthy CA, Nanjan MB, Taylor HR. Vision impairment predicts 5 year mortality. Br J Ophthalmol. 2001;85:322–326. doi: 10.1136/bjo.85.3.322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lee DJ, Gomez-Marin O, Lam BL, Zheng DD. Visual acuity impairment and mortality in US adults. Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120:1544–1550. doi: 10.1001/archopht.120.11.1544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Freeman EE, Egleston BL, West SK, Bandeen-Roche K, Rubin G. Visual acuity change and mortality in older adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46:4040–4045. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-0687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Thiagarajan M, Evans JR, Smeeth L. Wormald RPL, Fletcher AE. Cause-specific visual impairment and mortality: Results from a population-based study of older people in the United Kingdom. Arch Ophthalmol. 2005;123:1397–1403. doi: 10.1001/archopht.123.10.1397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Pedula KL, Coleman AL, Hillier TA, et al. Visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and mortality in older woman: Study of osteoporotic fractures. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2006;54:1871–1877. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00983.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Berdeaux G, Brezin AP, Fagnani F, Lafuma A, Mesbah M. Self-reported visual impairment and mortality: A French nationwide perspective. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2007;14:80–87. doi: 10.1080/09286580600899691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kulmala J, Era P, Törmäkangas T, Pärssinen O, Rantanen T, Heikkinen E. Visual acuity and mortality in older people and factors on the pathway. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2008;15:128–134. doi: 10.1080/09286580701840388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Christ SL, Lee DJ, Lam BL, Zheng DD, Arheart KL. Assessment of the effect of visual impairment on mortality through multiple health pathways: Structural equation modeling. Invest Ophthalm Vis Sci. 2008;49(8):3318–3323. doi: 10.1167/iovs.08-1676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Karpa MJ, Mitchell P, Beath K, Rochtchina E, Cumming RG, Wang JJ. Direct and indirect effects of visual impairment on mortality risk in older persons. Arch Ophthalmol. 2009;127(10):1347–1353. doi: 10.1001/archophthalmol.2009.240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jessa Z, Evans B, Thomson D, Rowlands G. Vision screening of older people. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2007;27:527–546. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2007.00525.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Brabyn J, Schneck M, Haegerström-Portnoy G, Lott L. The Smith-Kettlewell Institute (SKI) longitudinal study of vision function and its impact among the elderly: An overview. Optom Vis Sci. 2001;78:264–269. doi: 10.1097/00006324-200105000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lord SR, Dayhew J. Visual risk factors for falls in older people. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:508–515. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49107.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.West SK, Rubin GS, Broman AT, Munoz B, Bandeen-Roche K, Turano K. How does visual impairment affect performance on tasks of everyday life? The SEE Project. Arch Ophthalmol. 2002;120:774–780. doi: 10.1001/archopht.120.6.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.De Boer MR, Pluijm SMF, Lips P, et al. Different aspects of visual impairment as risk factors for falls and fractures in older men and women. J Bone Miner Res. 2004;19:1539–1547. doi: 10.1359/JBMR.040504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Schneck ME, Haegerström-Portnoy G, Lott LA, Brabyn JA, Gildengorin G. Low contrast vision function predicts subsequent acuity loss in an aged population: The SKI Study. Vis Res. 2004;44:2317–2325. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fletcher DC, Schuchard RA. Visual function in patients with choroidal neovascularization resulting from age-related macular degeneration: The importance of looking beyond visual acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 2006;83:178–189. doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000204510.08026.7f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rubin GS, Ng ESW, Bandeen-Roche K, et al. A prospective, population-based study of the role of visual impairment in motor vehicle crashes among older drivers: The SEE Study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:1483–1491. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-0474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Haegerström-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age: Vision function beyond acuity. Optom Vis Sci. 1999;76:141–158. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199903000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Haegerström-Portnoy G, Schneck ME, Lott LA, Brabyn JA. The relation between visual acuity and other spatial vision measures. Optom Vis Sci. 2000;77:653–662. doi: 10.1097/00006324-200012000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Murphy JM, Monson RR, Olivier DC, Sobel AM, Leighton AH. Affective disorders and mortality. A general population study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44:473–480. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800170095012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Blazer DG, Hybels CF. What symptoms of depression predict mortality in community-dwelling elders? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:2052–2056. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52564.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Korten AE, Jorm AF, Jiao Z, et al. Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1999;53:83–88. doi: 10.1136/jech.53.2.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Stump TE, Callahan CM, Hendrie HC. Cognitive impairment and mortality in older primary care patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49:934–940. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49184.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Reed D, Satariano W, Gildengorin G, McMahon K, Fleshman R, Schneider E. Health and functioning among the elderly of Marin County, California: A glimpse of the future. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1995;50:M61–69. doi: 10.1093/gerona/50a.2.m61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bailey IL, Lovie JE. New design principles for visual acuity letter charts. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1976;53:740–745. doi: 10.1097/00006324-197611000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ferris FL, III, Kassof A, Bresnick GH, Bailey I. New visual acuity charts for clinical research. Am J Ophthalmol. 1982;94:91–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Haegerström-Portnoy G, Brabyn JA, Schneck ME, Jampolsky A. The SKILL Card: An acuity test of reduced luminance and contrast. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:207–218. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bailey IL, Bullimore MA. A new test of disability glare. Optom Vis Sci. 1991;68:911–917. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199112000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pelli DG, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clin Vision Sci. 1988;2:187–99. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Elliot DB, Bullimore M, Bailey IL. Improving the reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity test. Clin Vis Sci. 1991;6:471–475. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Frisby JP. The Frisby stereotest: Amended instructions. Br Orthopt J. 1980;37:108. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Farnsworth D. The Farnsworth Dichotomous Test for color blindness Panel D-15 Manual. New York: The Psychological Corp; 1947. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Adams AJ, Haegerström-Portnoy G. Color deficiency. In: Amos JF, editor. Diagnosis and Management in Vision Care. Boston: Butterworths; 1987. pp. 671–713. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and visual search: Expanding the useful field of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5:2210–2219. doi: 10.1364/josaa.5.002210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ball K, Owsley C. The useful field of view test: A new technique for evaluating age-related declines in visual function. J Am Optom Assoc. 1993;64:71–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Radloff LS. The CES-D: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psych Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Radloff LS, Teri L. Use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale with older adults. Clin Gerontol. 1986;5:119–136. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Pfeiffer E. A short portable mental status questionnaire for the assessment of organic brain deficit in elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1975;23:433–441. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1975.tb00927.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Cleves MA, Gould WW, Gutierrez RG. An Introduction to Survival Analysis Using Stata. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Weih LM, VanNewkirk MR, McCarty CA, Taylor HR. Age-specific causes of bilateral visual impairment. Arch Ophthalmol. 2000;118:264–269. doi: 10.1001/archopht.118.2.264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Muñoz B, West SK, Rubin GS, et al. Causes of blindness and visual impairment in a population of older Americans: The Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. Arch Ophthalmol. 2000;118:819–825. doi: 10.1001/archopht.118.6.819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Evans BJ, Rowlands G. Correctable visual impairment in older people: A major unmet need. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2004;24:161–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.2004.00197.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Vitale S, Cotch MF, Sperduto RD. Prevalence of visual impairment in the United States. JAMA. 2006;295:2158–2163. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.18.2158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Dandona L, Dandona R. Revision of visual impairment definitions in the International Statistical Classification of Diseases. [Accessed June 4, 2009];BMC Medicine. 2006 4:7. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-4-7. online at http://www.biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/4/7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.World Health Organization. ICD Update and Revision Platform: Change the definition of blindness. [Accessed November 11, 2009]; online at http://www.who.int/blindness/en/index.html.