Abstract
Objective
The Incredible Years® Series (IY®) intervention has demonstrated efficacy for reduction in conduct disorder (CD) symptomatology among clinically-affected youth in multiple randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Since children with family psychiatric histories of antisocial behavior are at markedly elevated risk for enduring symptoms of antisocial behavior (in comparison with their family-history-negative counterparts), we examined whether intervention effects across studies prevail in that subgroup or are relatively restricted to children without inferred risk.
Method
We conducted a re-analysis of 5 RCTs of IY® involving 280 clinically-affected children, 3–8 years of age, for whom family psychiatric history of externalizing behavior among first- and second-degree relatives was ascertained from at least 1 parent.
Results
IY® equally benefitted children with CD with and without family psychiatric histories of externalizing behavior. Both family psychiatric history of externalizing behavior and parental depressive symptomatology predicted higher severity of CD symptomatology at baseline.
Conclusion
The beneficial effects of IY® are evident among children with CD, irrespective of whether their conditions are more or less attributable to inherited susceptibility to enduring antisocial syndromes. A next phase of research should address whether earlier implementation of group-based education for parents of young children at elevated familial risk for antisocial behavior syndromes—prior to the development of disruptive patterns of behavior--would result in even more pronounced effects, and thereby constitute cost-effective, targeted preventive intervention for CD.
Keywords: antisocial, children, behavioral, Incredible Years, maltreatment
INTRODUCTION
Externalizing disorders in childhood have long been known to predict life-course--persistent antisocial behavioral disorders, 1,2 which entail massive social costs associated with health and social service provision, law enforcement, and criminal justice.3,4
Evidence from social scientific and behavioral genetic research supports additive, 5 interactive, 6–8 and direct effects of stressful life events 9,10 and genetic predisposition 11–14 on childhood conduct disorder (CD). The complexity of gene-environment interaction has informed a “differential susceptibility” model of child development in which genetic differences confer sensitivity to both enriching and toxic aspects of the developmental environment. 15 Ideally, the primary caregiver acts as a protective buffer against environmental toxicity and as a positive moderator of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral susceptibilities 16,17 during sensitive developmental periods. 18 Positive parenting appears to promote adaptive executive-functioning and self-regulation, even in children with an emotionally reactive temperament in infancy. 19
The central role of the primary caregiver in buffering stress and enriching the developmental environment has informed the creation of evidence-based parenting interventions to prevent and treat childhood conduct problems as early as possible. 20 One of the best-validated and most cost-effective parent-training interventions is the Incredible Years® Series (IY®), which includes a core group-based parent-training component and complementary interventions for teachers and children. Through multiple randomized controlled trials (RCTs)21–30 and independent replications,31–37 IY® has been shown to decrease problem behavior in children, improve core parenting skills, enhance positive teaching practices, and consequently interrupt longer-term trajectories of externalizing behavior in a substantial proportion of children for whom the intervention is implemented.23,26,28
It is not yet known, however, whether IY® effects (typically assessed over months) might be relatively restricted to the large subset of children whose conduct problems would otherwise resolve naturalistically (i.e., without treatment and over a matter of years), and might conversely be less effective among children with the highest likelihood of life-course--persistent antisocial syndromes. This important subgroup is characterized by high levels of genetic influence that contribute to differential heritability of antisocial behavior observed between childhood and adulthood14; inherited influences contribute the highest portion of population-attributable risk for syndromes of antisocial behavior that persist through early adulthood.14,38 Targeting intensive intervention toward those children who are most likely to exhibit long-term antisocial syndromes is a major public health priority.
In this study we focus on intervention that has succeeded, but we also want to determine whether success is restricted to families with lower levels of intergenerational risk for persistent antisocial development, especially given that the time scale of resolution of childhood-limited antisocial syndromes far exceeds that of most intervention studies. Prior studies of the IY® intervention have shown the impact of the intervention is robust across a wide range of parent and environmental characteristics,39 but familial liability to antisocial development has never previously been explored. A number of previous studies have explored effects of maternal depression, parental substance abuse, and parental cognitive disabilities on the impact of parenting interventions and child outcome; however, such studies have not systematically explored the extent to which inherited liabilities associated with these disorders – versus environmental modifications brought about by intervention – jointly influence offspring outcome. We are aware of only one study that has directly tested the moderating effect of genetic risk for externalizing behavior on response to parent training intervention in general. In that study, an allelic variation in DRD4 in children was found to moderate the effects of parent-training on child externalizing behavior,40 underscoring a role of the 7-repeat allele in responsiveness to modification of the environment.
In this study we capitalized upon the availability of RCT data from 5 independent studies in which familial liability to antisocial outcome – as indexed by family psychiatric history – was collected but not analyzed in the ascertainment of intervention effects. Specifically, we re-analyzed IY® RCT data for the purpose of determining whether IY® was as effective among children with higher familial loading for externalizing behavior as for those without. Historic attempts to apply psychosocial interventions to inherited disability syndromes have been successful in improving adaptive functioning 41 but often sobering with respect to resolution of primary symptoms of the disorder (see, for example, 42–43). We therefore hypothesized that the effects of the intervention would be attenuated in the subgroup with elevated familial risk, controlling for severity of symptomatology at baseline. The ability of the intervention to exert positive effects on symptom burden – even in the context of familial susceptibility – would substantially enhance its relevance as a promising preventive intervention for children at serious risk for enduring antisocial syndromes.
METHOD
Data from 5 RCTs of IY® in which history of parental externalizing behavior was systematically acquired were included in this re-analysis. Inclusion characteristics for the studies are summarized as follows: (a) The child was between 3 and 8 years old (RCT4–5: between 4 and 7 years old); (b) the child had no debilitating physical impairment, intellectual deficit, or history of psychosis and was not receiving any form of psychological treatment at the time of referral; (c) the primary referral problem was child misconduct (e.g., noncompliance, aggression, oppositional behaviors) that had been occurring for at least 6 months; (d) parents had to have reported a clinically significant number of child behavior problems (more than 1 SD above the mean [RCT 4–5: 2 SD above the mean] on the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory [ECBI]20); (e) (RCT 4–5 only) the child met criteria for ODD or CD in accordance with the DSM-III-R; and (f) pre- and posttreatment behavioral data was available for all subjects. Intervention participants were included in the analysis irrespective of their subjective level of engagement in treatment or their improvement in intermediary indices of parenting skill. Detailed information about these samples can be found in the original published reports referenced in Table 1.
Table 1.
Original Samples | Analytic Sample (n) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trial | Conditions | Tx | Cx | Excl. | Reason excluded |
RCT122–23,26 | PT | 27 | |||
GD | 23 | No video modeling | |||
IT | 27 | Self-administered | |||
WLC | 28 | ||||
RCT230 | IT + CONSULT | 16 | Self-administered | ||
IT | 17 | Self-administered | |||
WLC | 13 | ||||
RCT324 | PT + ENHANCE | 37 | |||
PT | 41 | ||||
RCT425 | CT | 25 | No parent training | ||
PT | 35 | ||||
CT+PT | 24 | ||||
WLC | 15 | ||||
RCT527 | CT | 30 | No parent training | ||
PT | 25 | ||||
PT+TT | 24 | ||||
CT+TT | 23 | No parent training | |||
PT+CT+TT | 24 | ||||
WLC | 27 | ||||
TOTAL | 237 | 83 | 161 |
Note: CONSULT=two individual therapist consultations; CT=child training; Cx = control group; ENHANCE=couple’s counseling enhancement delivered after posttreatment measure; GD=group discussion (no video); IT=individually self-administered video training; PT=videotape group parent training with therapist; TT=teacher training; Tx = treatment group; WLC=waitlist control.
The version of the IY curriculum that has emerged as the standard for implementation minimally includes the following elements: 1) the engagement of parents in group-based parenting education; 2) delivery by a certified parent-group leader or facilitator; and 3) use of video vignettes as a key pedagogical method. We note that in the 5 studies, small subsets of children were randomized to conditions that did not contain all 3 elements. We restricted our inclusion of intervention group children to those for whom all three key components were delivered. The 5 samples and our process of inclusion and exclusion in this re-analysis are described in Table 1. We included all of the control subjects described in the original reports.
The purpose of randomizing a sample is to ensure a comparable distribution of participant characteristics that might impact the outcome of treatment. While our combined treatment/control groups are not strictly randomized insofar as they are drawn from multiple randomized trials, we tested the association of treatment condition with baseline (pretreatment) child and parent characteristics that are known to moderate child externalizing behavior and clinical response to early childhood interventions, namely maternal education, maternal race/ethnicity, child sex, and family history of externalizing problems. T-tests and chi-squared tests revealed no significant differences between the pooled treatment and control groups on these variables. We therefore consider our analytic sample a quasi-randomized sample well-suited to testing our hypotheses.
Sample Description
Table 2 describes demographic characteristics, familial risk, and child externalizing behavior of the sample in which we differentiate children residing in 2-parent households (in which family history of both parents was provided) from those residing in single-parent households (in which only maternal family psychiatric history was reported). T-tests and chi-squared tests revealed no significant differences between treatment and control participants meeting inclusion criteria for this re-analysis, with respect to baseline externalizing behavior scores and parental Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores.
Table 2.
Two bio-parent households | Single bio-mother households | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Control (n=58) | Treatment (n=157) | Control (n=15) | Treatment (n=50) | |
Child’s sex | ||||
Female | 22.4 (13) | 19.8 (31) | 26.7 (4) | 26.0 (13) |
Male | 77.6 (45) | 80.3 (126) | 73.3 (11) | 74.0 (39) |
Maternal education | ||||
4-year college or more | 55.2 (32) | 49.7 (78) | 33.3 (5) | 26.0 (13) |
Some college | 31.0 (18) | 29.3 (46) | 46.7 (7) | 44.0 (22) |
HS diploma only | 13.8 (8) | 20.4 (32) | 20.0 (3) | 30.0 (15) |
Without HS diploma | 0.0 (0) | 0.6 (1) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) |
Maternal race | ||||
Caucasian | 93.1 (54) | 95.5 (150) | 93.3 (14) | 88.0 (44) |
Hispanic | 1.7 (1) | 2.6 (4) | 6.7 (1) | 4.0 (2) |
Black | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 4.0 (2) |
Asian | 3.5 (2) | 0.6 (1) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) |
Native American | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) |
Pacific Islander | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) | 0.0 (0) |
Other/mixed race | 1.7 (1) | 1.3 (2) | 0.0 (0) | 4.0 (2) |
Mother Depression at Baseline (BDI) | 7.0±0.8 (58) | 7.8±0.4 (155) | 12.3±1.9 (15) | 9.9±1.3 (50) |
Father Depression at Baseline (BDI) | 5.1±0.6 (56) | 5.2±0.4 (46) | n/a | n/a |
Note: BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; HS = high school
Child Behavior
The externalizing domain T-score from the Achenbach System for Empirically-Based Assessment Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was used to characterize child externalizing behavior at baseline and posttreatment.44
Family History of Externalizing Disorders
An intake interview with questions about family history was administered to each parent involved in the parent-training intervention. Mothers and fathers were asked about their own and their parents’ mental health history. The child’s family members were deemed to reflect familial risk if the informant endorsed a history of alcohol problems, drug problems, or incarceration. Studies of twins and families have demonstrated highly overlapping components of genetic liability for these conditions, which extend generally to antisocial behavior. 45,46 The validity of brief family history methods for ascertaining familial risk for substance use and antisocial disorders has been strongly supported in previous research among populations similar to those comprising the IY® RCTs.47,48 We coded the child’s familial risk for externalizing behavior into 3 categories, across 2 strata: (0) “Absent”, i.e. no parent or grandparent with a reported externalizing behavior problem, (1) externalizing problems present in grandparents only, (2) externalizing problems present in parents only, (3) externalizing problems present among parents and grandparents (thus first- and second-degree relatives, suggesting slightly higher continuity/penetrance of intergenerational risk in data acquired from single parent households); the 2 strata were (a) single-biological-parent households (in which family history was only available from a single parent and restricted to that side of the child’s family), and (b) 2-biological-parent households.
DATA ANALYSIS
We conducted separate analyses for single-parent and 2-parent households since the former reflected only half of the familial liability information available in the latter.
Univariate
We examined differences between single-bio-mother and 2-bio-parent households with respect to child gender, maternal education, child race or ethnicity, and maternal depression scores derived from the BDI at baseline, and mother-reported externalizing behavior at baseline.
In order to examine the association of the familial risk for externalizing behavior with child externalizing behavior as well as the differential effect of the intervention on change in child externalizing scores, we performed paired t-tests on pre-/posttreatment CBCL mother- and father-reported externalizing T-scores by household type, intervention/control group, and familial risk level (Table 3).
Table 3.
Mother-reported CBCL externalizing | Father-reported CBCL externalizing | |||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Posttreatment | Change | Baseline | Posttreatment | Change | |||||||||||||
INTERVENTION GROUP | n | mean | SD | n | mean | SD | mean | paired t | p | n | mean | SD | n | mean | SD | mean | paired t | p |
Two-parent households | ||||||||||||||||||
NO first or second degree risk | 48 | 64.6 | 7.4 | 48 | 56.8 | 8.8 | 7.8 | 7.3 | <.0001 | 46 | 63.1 | 7.6 | 46 | 54.9 | 8.2 | 8.2 | 7 | <.0001 |
Presence of externalizing disorder in any first- or second- degree relative |
108 | 67.5 | 8.1 | 108 | 60.1 | 9.8 | 7.5 | 10.5 | <.0001 | 93 | 64.9 | 8.5 | 93 | 58.6 | 9.8 | 6.3 | 7.3 | <.0001 |
First-and second-degree risk | 44 | 69.8 | 6.4 | 44 | 62.7 | 7.7 | 7.1 | 7.2 | <.0001 | 36 | 67.4 | 7.1 | 36 | 60.7 | 8 | 6.8 | 4.4 | <.001 |
First-degree risk only | 13 | 68.8 | 8.5 | 13 | 61.8 | 11.3 | 7.1 | 3.1 | <.05 | 10 | 64.3 | 13.7 | 10 | 58.8 | 14 | 5.5 | 5.9 | <.001 |
Second-degree risk only | 51 | 65.3 | 8.8 | 51 | 57.3 | 10.4 | 7.9 | 7.1 | <.0001 | 47 | 63.1 | 7.7 | 47 | 57 | 9.9 | 6 | 5 | <.0001 |
Single Mother | ||||||||||||||||||
NO first or second degree risk | 18 | 68.4 | 8.6 | 18 | 60.7 | 9.9 | 7.7 | 3.1 | <.01 | |||||||||
Presence of externalizing disorder in any first- or second- degree relative |
32 | 70.2 | 7.1 | 32 | 60.5 | 7.2 | 9.7 | 7.2 | <.0001 | |||||||||
First- and second-degree risk | 14 | 70.6 | 7.3 | 14 | 58.9 | 6.8 | 11.8 | 5.5 | <.001 | |||||||||
First-degree risk only | 5 | 75.2 | 6.6 | 5 | 72 | 8.8 | 13 | 6 | <.01 | |||||||||
Second-degree risk only | 13 | 68.8 | 7.7 | 13 | 62.8 | 8.1 | 6.1 | 3.2 | <.01 | |||||||||
CONTROL GROUP | ||||||||||||||||||
Two-parent households | ||||||||||||||||||
NO first-or second-degree risk | 18 | 64.4 | 7.9 | 18 | 62.7 | 8.2 | 1.7 | 1.2 | ns | 17 | 64.9 | 9.1 | 17 | 62.7 | 7.5 | 2.2 | 1.9 | ns |
Presence of externalizing disorder in any first- or second- degree relative |
40 | 68.6 | 8.6 | 40 | 64.7 | 8.7 | 3.9 | 4.5 | <.0001 | 36 | 65.8 | 8.5 | 36 | 63 | 8.7 | 2.8 | 2.2 | <.05 |
First- and second-degree risk | 11 | 69.5 | 10.2 | 11 | 66.1 | 8.7 | 3.4 | 2.1 | ns | 10 | 67.9 | 8.6 | 10 | 65.3 | 8.9 | 2.6 | 1.4 | ns |
First-degree risk only | 4 | 61 | 12.3 | 4 | 55.5 | 8.2 | 5.5 | 1.3 | ns | 4 | 65.8 | 7.6 | 4 | 60.5 | 11.6 | 5.3 | 1.6 | ns |
Second-degree risk only | 25 | 69.4 | 6.8 | 25 | 65.5 | 8.3 | 3.9 | 3.7 | <.01 | 22 | 64.8 | 8.8 | 22 | 62.4 | 8.3 | 2.4 | 1.4 | ns |
Single Mother | ||||||||||||||||||
NO first-or second-degree risk | 6 | 66.5 | 6.7 | 6 | 64 | 5.4 | 2.5 | 1.9 | ns | |||||||||
Presence of externalizing disorder in any first- or second- degree relative |
9 | 75.3 | 5.1 | 9 | 71.1 | 8.9 | 4.2 | 1.9 | ns | |||||||||
First and second-degree risk | 5 | 75.2 | 6.6 | 5 | 72 | 8.8 | 3.2 | 1.3 | ns | |||||||||
First-degree risk only | 1 | 79 | --- | 1 | 70 | --- | 9 | --- | --- | |||||||||
Second-degree risk only | 3 | 74.3 | 2.9 | 3 | 70 | 12.5 | 4.3 | 0.7 | ns |
Note: CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist; ns = not significant.
Multivariate
Since familial liability indexed by family history represents only 1 of many domains of influence on behavioral outcome and the impact of intervention, we conducted (separately for pre-intervention and post-intervention data) a set of linear regression analyses that controlled for relevant variables for which data were available, including baseline depressive symptomatology of the parental reporter,49,50 maternal education, maternal race/ethnicity, and child sex. The results of these preparatory analyses (essentially supporting the appropriateness of inclusion of the variables in the tests of the study’s primary hypotheses) are summarized in supplementary tables S1 and S2 (available online).
Next, we proceeded with the test of the study’s central hypothesis, using analysis of covariance to test treatment efficacy while controlling for baseline child externalizing behavior, familial risk, and relevant demographic characteristics, separately considering maternal and paternal reports (when available). P values reported for these central analyses of the study were not adjusted for number of statistical tests, since they represent the test of the memory hypothesis, and since the other statistical tests reported fundamentally establish the validity of these primary tests. In the 2-bio-parent model, we included the interaction between familial risk and treatment group. Effective sample size (n = 65) prevented inclusion of the interaction term in the single-bio-parent multivariate model. SAS Proc GLM was used for all multivariate analyses.
Additionally, we sought to characterize the impact of family history on clinically significant treatment response. A child’s response to the intervention was considered clinically significant if his or her caregiver reported externalizing behavior above the clinical threshold of 60 before the intervention and below the clinical threshold after the intervention. We used SAS Proc Logistic to model clinically significant response on treatment condition and family history of externalizing problems.
RESULTS
Household Differences
Children in single-bio-mother households were significantly more likely than children in 2-bio-parent households to have a lower level of maternal education (Mantel-Haenszel χ2[1, 280] = 8.44, p < .01). There was no significant difference in the race or ethnicity of children in single-bio-mother and 2-bio-parent households (χ2[1, 280] = 2.65, p = .10). Maternal depression scores were significantly higher in single-bio-mother versus 2-bio-parent households (t [279] = 2.42, p < .05; Table 2). Mother-reported baseline child externalizing behavior was significantly higher in single-bio-mother than 2-bio-parent households (t [279] = 2.88, p < .01), as was maternal familial history of externalizing disorders (Mantel-Haenzel χ2[1, 280] = 18.06, p < .0001).
Intervention effects and their interaction with familial risk
Table 3 summarizes the positive responses associated with IY intervention in child externalizing behavior scores which occured irrespective of familial risk group. All 15 household and risk groups who received the intervention showed a significant decrease in child externalizing behavior from baseline to posttreatment with a magnitude of change ranging from 3.1 to 10.5 points. Only 3 of 15 household and risk groups who received no intervention showed a significant decrease in child externalizing behavior.
Linear regression analysis examining predictors of CBCL – externalizing scores separately at each time point revealed that membership in the treatment group was not significantly associated with a difference in child externalizing behavior at baseline in single-bio-mother or 2-bio-parent households. In 2-bio-parent households (Table S1, available online), combined first- and second-degree familial risk was significantly associated with higher mother- and father-reported CBCL externalizing T-scores at baseline and posttreatment. In 2-bio-parent households, maternal race was associated with higher father-reported externalizing behavior at baseline but not posttreatment. Paternal depression predicted increased father-reported externalizing behavior at baseline and posttreatment. Neither demographic characteristics, familial liability, nor maternal depression predicted differences in child externalizing behavior in single bio-mother households (Table S2, available online). Intervention condition (treatment versus control) consistently predicted post-intervention but not pre-intervention externalizing scores across family and rater type.
Treatment Effects
Analyses of covariance exhibited significant intervention effects in both single-bio-parent and 2-bio-parent households, as shown in Table 4. In two-bio-parent households, familial history of externalizing behavior did not interact with treatment in predicting child externalizing behavioral outcome. Child sex was a significant predictor of change in father-reported externalizing behavior across time. The magnitude of reduction in externalizing behavior in each subject group is appreciable from estimations of the influence of treatment on pre- and post-intervention scores presented in Table 3, and is on the order of 1 standard deviation of the mean T-score at baseline (i.e. an effect size of 1).
Table 4.
Two-bio-parent households | Single-bio-mother households | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Maternal Report | Paternal Report | Maternal Report | |||||
DF | F Value | Pr > Fa | F Value | Pr > Fa | F Value | Pr > Fa | |
Baseline CBCL externalizing behavior | 1 | 149.15 | <.0001 | 94.90 | <.0001 | 16.54 | <.001 |
Treatment | 1 | 7.48 | <.01 | 7.84 | <.01 | 8.91 | <.01 |
Family history of externalizing disorders | 3 | 0.40 | ns | 0.19 | ns | 0.84 | ns |
Treatment * Family hx of externalizing disorders | 3 | 0.83 | ns | 0.60 | ns | --- | --- |
Child’s sex | 1 | 0.06 | ns | 4.03 | <.05 | 2.22 | ns |
Maternal Education | 2 | 0.07 | ns | 1.35 | ns | 0.71 | ns |
Maternal Race | 1 | 0.26 | ns | 0.53 | ns | 0.76 | ns |
Reporter Depression at Baseline | 1 | 0.39 | ns | 2.75 | ns | 0.01 | ns |
Note: CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist; DF = degree of freedom; hx = history; NS = not significant.
Pr>F p value; the probability of obtaining the F value if the null hypothesis is true.
Categorical Designation of Clinical Affectation
To complement our quantitative analyses, and to contextualize the range of clinical disability in which quantitative shifts occurred, we compared the proportion of children in treatment and control groups who moved from clinical-level affectation to sub-clinical--level affectation during the study period. Among children in 2-bio-parent households who demonstrated clinical-level externalizing behavior at baseline (CBCL externalizing T-score ≥ 60), those in the treatment group were respectively 3.4 (by mother report, 95% confidence interval 1.5-7.9) and 3.2 (by father report, 95% confidence interval 1.3-7.5) times more likely than controls to be rated below clinical-level symptomatology by parent report at posttreatment. Children with no family history of externalizing behavior were more likely to cross the clinical threshold as would be expected by virtue of the fact that on average, they were affected less severely at baseline. Children in single-bio-parent households randomized to intervention were 5.1 (95% confidence interval 0.9-28.5) times more likely than those in the control group to cross the threshold from clinical to subclinical affectation; family history of externalizing behavior did not significantly moderate clinically significant response in single-bio-parent households.
DISCUSSION
This study comprises secondary familial risk analyses of a combined meta-analytic sample drawn from 5 RCTs of the IY® parent training intervention to treat conduct problems in children aged 3 to 8. The findings support and extend previously reported findings on the impact of the IY intervention—namely that children with clinical-level externalizing behavior benefit from the intervention. Moreover the present analyses confirm that the treatment effect occurs irrespective of the presence of family history of adult antisocial behavior. This clarification of effect offers hope that children at serious risk based on inherited liability are as likely to benefit from effective parent training as those without such liability. Since life-course--persistent antisocial behavior is strongly influenced by genetic factors, this finding underscores the relevance of successful parent training for children with such profiles of risk, and supports the possibility that judicious implementation of parent training for such children could serve to offset risk incurred by inherited liabilities.
A limitation of our study was that single-parent households were informative only with respect to the parent living in the home; this was an important reason for segregating the sample and examining separately a sample that was more fully informative based on the family history data collected. The results were highly congruent, whether considering single-parent or dual-parent households. A second limitation is that this analysis was restricted to published RCTs and does not address unpublished results in which this intervention may not have had a positive impact. A third limitation of our study is a lack of follow-up measurement beyond immediate posttreatment. Ideally, parent-training interventions impart change through iterative effects as parents practice new parenting skills and children adapt to positive parenting practices. One year51 and 2-year52 outcome studies of the IY® interventions show that treatment effects substantially persist over time, and promising results from recent research on other parent-focused interventions have similarly demonstrated enduring gains over time.53
We conclude that the IY intervention, as delivered in multiple RCTs, resulted in significant reductions in externalizing behavior irrespective of whether a child’s clinical condition was associated with familial loading for antisocial behavior. The effects observed among clinically affected children age 3 to 8 years additionally inform a rationale for providing such training to parents of children at elevated risk prior to the development of disruptive patterns of behavior. Constantino and colleagues have previously reported that families of infants at elevated risk for antisocial behavior can be successfully engaged in group-based preventive intervention involving parent training.54 Magnitude of impact of such targeted early parenting education on developmental outcomes is being addressed in a next generation of controlled studies.55
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The data collection was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) National Center for Nursing Research Grant #5 R01 NR01075-11 and National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Research Scientist Development Award MH00988 (CW-S). The re-analysis was supported by grants from the Administration for Children and Families (60329, JNC), by an anonymous donor, and from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention through the Brown Center for Violence and Injury Prevention (R49CE001510-01).
The views expressed do not necessarily reflect the opinions of the funder.
The authors wish to thank the families who participated in the original intervention trials, whose efforts made this study possible.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Supplemental material cited in this article is available online.
Disclosure: Dr. Webster-Stratton disseminates these treatments and stands to gain from favorable reports. Because of this, she has voluntarily agreed to distance herself from certain critical research activities, including recruitment, consenting, primary data handling, and data analysis. The University of Washington has approved these arrangements. Dr. Constantino has received royalties for the authorship of the Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2), which is published and distributed by Western Psychological Services. Mr. Presnall reports no financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Contributor Information
Ned Presnall, Washington University in St. Louis.
Carolyn H. Webster-Stratton, University of Washington.
John N. Constantino, Washington University in St. Louis.
References
- 1.Robins LN. Deviant children grown up: a sociological and psychiatric study of sociopathic personality. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Harrington H, Milne BJ. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: follow-up at age 26 years. Dev Psychopathol. 2002 Winter;14(1):179–207. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402001104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Scott S, Knapp M, Henderson J, Maughan B. Financial cost of social exclusion: follow up study of antisocial children into adulthood. BMJ. 2001 Jul 28;323(7306):191. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7306.191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wolfgang ME, Figlio RM, Sellin T. Delinquency in a birth cohort. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Jonson-Reid M, Presnall N, Drake B, et al. Effects of child maltreatment and inherited liability on antisocial development: an official records study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010 Apr;49(4):321–332. quiz 431. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, et al. Nature X nurture: genetic vulnerabilities interact with physical maltreatment to promote conduct problems. Dev Psychopathol. 2005 Winter;17(1):67–84. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Taylor A, et al. MAOA, maltreatment, and gene-environment interaction predicting children's mental health: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2006 Oct;11(10):903–913. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Moffitt TE. The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: gene-environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychol Bull. 2005 Jul;131(4):533–554. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Widom CS. Does violence beget violence? A critical examination of the literature. Psychol Bull. 1989 Jul;106(1):3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Widom CS. The cycle of violence. Science. 1989 Apr 14;244(4901):160–166. doi: 10.1126/science.2704995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.DiLalla LF, Gottesman II. Biological and genetic contributors to violence--Widom's untold tale. Psychol Bull. 1991 Jan;109(1):125–129. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.125. discussion 130-122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Iacono WG, McGue M, Patrick CJ. Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: a twin-family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004 Sep;61(9):922–928. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Turkheimer E, et al. Intergenerational transmission of childhood conduct problems: a Children of Twins Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 Jul;64(7):820–829. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Viding E, McCrory EJ. Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol. 2012 Aug;24(3):969–983. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200048X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Belsky J, Jonassaint C, Pluess M, Stanton M, Brummett B, Williams R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol Psychiatry. 2009 Aug;14(8):746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gunnar M, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002 Jan-Feb;27(1–2):199–220. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00045-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mills-Koonce WR, Propper C, Gariepy JL, Barnett M, Moore GA, Calkins S, Cox MJ. Psychophysiological correlates of parenting behavior in mothers of young children. Dev Psychobiol. 2009;51(8):650–661. doi: 10.1002/dev.20400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ursache A, Blair C, Stifter C, Voegtline K. Emotional Reactivity and Regulation in Infancy Interact to Predict Executive Functioning in Early Childhood. Dev Psychol. 2012 May 7; doi: 10.1037/a0027728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Eyberg SM, Nelson MM, Boggs SR. Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2008 Jan;37(1):215–237. doi: 10.1080/15374410701820117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Webster-Stratton C, Reid MJ, Beauchaine TP. Combining Parent and Child Training for Young Children with ADHD. Journal of Clincial Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2011;40(2):1–13. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2011.546044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Webster-Stratton C, Kolpacoff M, Hollinsworth T. Self-administered videotape therapy for families with conduct-problem children: comparison with two cost-effective treatments and a control group. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988 Aug;56(4):558–566. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.4.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Webster-Stratton C. Long-term follow-up of families with young conduct problem children: from preschool to grade school. J of Clin Child Psychol. 1990;12(2):144–149. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Webster-Stratton C. Advancing videotape parent training: a comparison study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1994 Jun;62(3):583–593. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.62.3.583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M. Treating children with early-onset conduct problems: a comparison of child and parent training interventions. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1997 Feb;65(1):93–109. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.65.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Webster-Stratton C, Hollinsworth T, Kolpacoff M. The long-term effectiveness and clinical significance of three cost-effective training programs for families with conduct-problem children. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989 Aug;57(4):550–553. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.4.550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Webster-Stratton C, Reid MJ, Hammond M. Treating children with early-onset conduct problems: intervention outcomes for parent, child, and teacher training. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004 Mar;33(1):105–124. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Webster-Stratton C, Rinaldi J, Jamila MR. Long-Term Outcomes of Incredible Years Parenting Program: Predictors of Adolescent Adjustment. Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2011 Feb;16(1):38–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00576.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Webster-Stratton C, Reid MJ, Beauchaine TP. One-year follow-up of combined parent and child intervention for young children with ADHD. Journal of Clincial Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2013;42(2):251–261. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.723263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Webster-Stratton C. Enhancing the effectiveness of self-administered videotape parent training for families with conduct-problem children. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 1990 Oct;18(5):479–492. doi: 10.1007/BF00911103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Menting AT, de Castro BO, Wijngaards-de Meij LD, Matthys W. A trial of parent training for mothers being released from incarceration and their children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2014;43(3):381–396. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.817310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hutchings J, Gardner F, Bywater T, Daley D, Whitaker C, Jones K, et al. Parenting intervention in Sure Start services for children at risk of developing conduct disorder: Pragmatic randomized controlled trial. British Medical Journal. 2007;334(7595):1–7. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39126.620799.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Posthumus JA, Raaijmakers MAJ, Maassen GH, Engeland H, Matthys W. Sustained effects of Incredible Years as a preventive intervention in preschool children with conduct problems. Jounal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(4):487–500. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9580-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Drugli MB, Larsson B, Fossum S, Morch W. Five-to six-year outcome and its prediction for children with ODD/CD treated with parent training. Jounal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2010;51(5):559–566. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02178.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Scott S, Briskman J, O'Connor TG. Early prevention of antisocial personality: long-term follow-up of two randomized controlled trials comparing indicated and selective approaches. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:649–657. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13050697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gardner F, Burton J, Klimes I. Randomised controlled trial of a parenting intervention in the voluntary sector for reducing child conduct problems: outcomes and mechanisms of change. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006 Nov;47(11):1123–1132. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01668.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Scott S, Spender Q, Doolan M, Jacobs B, Aspland H. Multicentre controlled trial of parenting groups for childhood antisocial behaviour in clinical practice. BMJ. 2001 Jul 28;323(7306):194–198. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7306.194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lyons MJ, True WR, Eisen SA, et al. Differential heritability of adult and juvenile antisocial traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995 Nov;52(11):906–915. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950230020005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Menting AT, Orobio de Castro B, Matthys W. Effectiveness of the Incredible Years parent training to modify disruptive and prosocial child behavior: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013 Dec;33(8):901–913. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IMH, Pijlman FT, Mesman J, Juffer F. Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers' externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev Psychol. 2008 Jan;44(1):293–300. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Chou CY, Ma MC, Yang TT. Determinants of subjective health-related quality of life (HRQoL) for patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2014 Apr;154(1–3):83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Charman T. Commentary: Glass half full or half empty? Testing social communication interventions for young children with autism--reflections on Landa, Holman, O'Neill, and Stuart (2011) J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011 Jan;52(1):22–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02359.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kazdin AE. Treatments for aggressive and antisocial children. Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America. 2000 Oct;9(4):841–858. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the Child Behavior Checkllist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Markon KE, Krueger RF. Categorical and continuous models of liability to externalizing disorders: a direct comparison in NESARC. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005 Dec;62(12):1352–1359. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Witkiewitz K, King K, McMahon RJ, et al. Evidence for a multi-dimensional latent structural model of externalizing disorders. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2013 Feb;41(2):223–237. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9674-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Rice JP, Reich T, Bucholz KK, et al. Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 1995 Aug;19(4):1018–1023. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb00983.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Waldron M, Madden PA, Nelson EC, et al. The interpretability of family history reports of alcoholism in general community samples: findings in a midwestern U.S. twin birth cohort. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research. 2012 Jun;36(6):1091–1098. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01698.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gartstein MA, Bridgett DJ, Dishion TJ, Kaufman NK. Depressed Mood and Maternal Report of Child Behavior Problems: Another Look at the Depression-Distortion Hypothesis. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2009 Mar;30(2):149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M. Maternal depression and its relationship to life stress, perceptions of child behavior problems, parenting behaviors, and child conduct problems. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 1988 Jun;16(3):299–315. doi: 10.1007/BF00913802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Webster-Stratton C. The long-term effects of a videotape modeling parent-training program: comparison of immediate and 1-year follow-up results. Behavior Therapy. 1982;13(5):702–714. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Reid MJ, Webster-Stratton C, Hammond M. Follow-up of children who received the incrdible years intervention for oppositional-defiant disorder: maintenance and predcition of 2-year outcome. Behavior Therapy. 2003;34(4):471–491. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Eyberg S, Boggs S, Jaccard J. Does Maintenance Treatment Matter? Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2014 Jan 11; doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9842-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Constantino JN. Mental Health in Public Health: The Next 100 Years, (Cottler LB, American Psychopathological Association, eds.) Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. Child mental health: status of the promise, the reality, and the future of prevention; pp. xvii–348. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Meyer A, Fortunato C. Parenting Interventions in Early Head Start: The Buffering Toxic Stress Consortium. ZERO TO THREE. 2013 Nov;34(2):73–80. 2013. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.