Skip to main content
Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI logoLink to Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI
. 2013 Feb 6;3(1):109–126. doi: 10.3390/ani3010109

The “Super Chimpanzee”: The Ecological Dimensions of Rehabilitation of Orphan Chimpanzees in Guinea, West Africa

Lissa Ongman 1, Christelle Colin 2,3, Estelle Raballand 2,3, Tatyana Humle 1,*
PMCID: PMC4495513  PMID: 26487312

Abstract

Simple Summary

This study examines relevant behavioral indicators of rehabilitation success of orphaned chimpanzees, victims of the bushmeat and pet trade, and contributes to identifying future release candidates. Results highlight the importance of bush-outings in the development of species-specific behaviors. Neither trauma upon arrival nor contact with human caretakers predicted dietary knowledge among rehabilitants at the Chimpanzee Conservation Centre where the study took place. The studied orphans demonstrated a relatively broad dietary knowledge. We attributed this result to the combined effect of the multiregional origins of residents and the learning opportunities available during bush-outings, which we termed the “Super Chimpanzee” theory.

Abstract

To date few studies, especially among non-human primates, have evaluated or monitored rehabilitation effectiveness and identified key species-specific behavioral indicators for release success. This four-months study aimed to identify behavioral indicators of rehabilitation success among ten infant and juvenile orphaned chimpanzees cared for in peer groups at the Centre for Conservation of Chimpanzees (CCC), Guinea, West Africa. Behavioral data focused on foraging skills and activity budget. During bush-outings, rehabilitants spent on average nearly a quarter of their activity budget foraging, resting or traveling, respectively. Neither age, sex, the level of abnormal behaviors demonstrated upon arrival nor human contact during bush-outings predicted individual dietary knowledge. However, individuals who spent more time arboreal demonstrated a greater dietary breadth than conspecifics who dwelled more terrestrially. Although our data failed to demonstrate a role of conspecific observation in dietary acquisition, we propose that the mingling of individuals from different geographical origins may act as a catalyst for acquiring new dietary knowledge, promoted by ecological opportunities offered during bush-outings. This “Super Chimpanzee” theory opens up new questions about cultural transmission and socially-biased learning among our closest living relatives and provides a novel outlook on rehabilitation in chimpanzees.

Keywords: chimpanzee, rehabilitation, socially-biased learning, abnormal behaviors, reintroduction, sanctuary, bush-outings

1. Introduction

The persistence of habitat destruction and the bushmeat and pet-trade across wild chimpanzee range states has led to a marked increase in centers or sanctuaries caring for orphaned chimpanzees [1,2,3,4,5,6,7]. The Pan African Sanctuary Alliance (PASA) was created in 2000 in response to this conservation and welfare urgency, all the more exemplified by a 15% annual growth rate of sanctuary residents between 2000 and 2006 across PASA-member sanctuaries [6,8]. Due to what some refer to as a ‘moral’ and “ethical” obligation to the welfare of individuals who have been taken from their natural social and environmental settings, this situation has led to the creation of multiple chimpanzee sanctuaries across Africa [6]. Sanctuaries provide conditions for rehabilitation, which contribute to improving both an individual’s welfare and wellbeing by promoting species-specific behaviors. In this context, rehabilitation can be defined as “the process by which captive (primates) are treated for medical and physical disabilities until they regain health, are helped to acquire natural social and ecological skills, and are weaned from human contact and dependence, such that they are able to survive in the wild” ([9], p.5). This multifaceted and sometimes complex and lengthy process is rendered even more challenging when it entails additionally overcoming sometimes serious individual trauma frequently associated with capture and mistreatment at the hands of previous owners [10].

The developmental trajectory of young chimpanzees and other non-human primates is often affected by exposure and response to learning opportunities, which may be constrained by the individual’s ecological and social environment and psychological state [11,12,13]. Solitary confinement and early maternal removal can lead to more timid personalities, less social activity, less dominance and more susceptibility to stress in orphan chimpanzees [14]. One approach to decreasing the expression of aberrant or stereotypic behaviors is through environmental enrichment [15]. Many PASA-member sanctuaries often rehabilitate individual orphans in large naturalistic enclosures that encourage a natural fission-fusion group lifestyle either in peer-aged or multi age-class social groupings with multi-males and multi-female composition [16]. In these highly enriched environments, Wobber and Hare [16] showed that sanctuary bonobos (Pan paniscus) exhibited higher levels of cognition when tested on an assortment of cognitive tasks and a lower level of abnormal behaviors than their captive zoo counterparts.

Some sanctuaries are able to carry out bush-outings as part of their rehabilitation program; these usually benefit all or a subset of their younger residents. Bush-outings aim to enhance and develop species-specific behaviors such as nesting skills, anti-predatory behaviors, social interactions, food selection and processing. For instance, the diet provided in most captive environments does not encompass the wide range of floral and faunal species consumed typically in the wild, and individuals potentially destined for release need to recognize and process natural foods to survive independently of human provisioning [3]. Bush-outings also expose individuals to natural three-dimensional spaces creating opportunities for locomotion and for dietary knowledge expansion. In the wild, chimpanzees spend a large proportion of daylight hours foraging, i.e., selecting and manufacturing tools to access embedded foods, and/or selecting and processing food items for consumption [17,18]. Wild chimpanzees consume mostly fruit, although their diverse food repertoire also often includes leaves, flowers, bark, stems, seeds, honey, insects and small- to medium-sized mammals [19,20,21,22]. It is from infancy through adolescence that wild chimpanzees have the opportunity to acquire social and foraging skills necessary for adult life [23,24,25,26]. It is important that these social and foraging skillsets develop evenly allowing no deficit in either vital domain of behavior in adulthood.

In order to achieve a comprehensive foraging repertoire, immature primates can benefit from observing their mother, as well as older and more experienced members of their community and from socially-biased and environmental learning opportunities [27,28,29,30,31]. It is therefore vital during rehabilitation to promote an environment conducive to socially-biased learning and offer multiple learning opportunities for rehabilitant individuals to acquire the skills necessary for survival in the wild. In sanctuaries, where infant and juvenile chimpanzees are without a mother, orphans can learn socially essential survival skills by paying attention to typically unrelated conspecifics and in some cases their human caretakers. Since weaning in the wild promotes independent social and ecological behaviors [24], it is also critical to mimic weaning from human contact in sanctuary settings. Dependency on human contact after a certain age could potentially compromise the emotional, nutritional and social independence of an individual and hinder his/her appropriate social integration with conspecifics [32]. Dependency on human contact could also impact release success if released individuals seek human presence at the expense of exhibiting naturalistic species-specific social behaviors and interactions with conspecifics [9].

Research into assessing effective rehabilitation processes for sanctuary chimpanzees and determining criteria of rehabilitation and release success is currently lacking. Since rehabilitants were not constrained by food availability since provisioned when enclosed, this study aimed to first test whether there was a trade-off between social and foraging domains of behavior during bush-outings. We then analyzed the activity budgets of rehabilitated chimpanzees on bush-outings and assessed individual’s dietary knowledge. This study also sought to examine the influence of abnormal behaviors upon arrival, sex and age and other relevant variables on individual’s dietary acquisition (i.e., observation of conspecifics foraging) and knowledge. Finally we also investigated whether human contact negatively impacted dietary knowledge and observation of the foraging behavior of conspecifics.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Site, Subjects

All data were collected at the Centre for Conservation of Chimpanzees (CCC) in Somoria, Guinea, West Africa (Figure 1). The CCC is the sole PASA-member sanctuary recovering orphan chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) victims of the bushmeat and pet trade in Guinea. This sanctuary currently cares for 41 chimpanzees. Since the national legislation states that it is illegal to kill or possess a chimpanzee, individual chimpanzees, most often infant victims of the pet trade, are typically confiscated by relevant government authorities and handed over to the CCC, where their care and wellbeing in a semi-natural environment is ensured. Individuals come from all over Guinea and occasionally neighboring countries. The CCC is located at the northeastern edge of the Mafou core area of the Haut-Niger National Park (HNNP), one of the last remaining dry forest-savanna mosaics in West Africa [33]. The HNNP harbors open savanna, wooded savanna, bamboo forest, open dry forest, and riparian forest [34].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Map of the location of the Haut Niger National Park (HNNP) in Guinea, West Africa, highlighting the Mafou core area of the park and the relative location of the sanctuary in Somoria (adapted from [33]).

After a three months quarantine, new arrivals are introduced to peer groups. Study individuals were housed and rehabilitated in two separate peer groups—the “TDT” and the “Nursery”. At the time of the study, the “TDT” group contained three chimpanzees (one male, two females, average age = 3.67 yrs ± 1.53) whereas the “Nursery” group comprised seven chimpanzees (four males, three females, average age = 6.9 yrs ± 0.89) (Table 1). The average age upon arrival for all subjects was 1.8 yrs ± 0.92 (Table 1). Each individual had been rehabilitated for at least a year, and many had been cared for at the CCC for the majority of their infant lives (average time at sanctuary: “TDT” = 1.67 yrs ± 1.15, “Nursery” = 5.15 yrs ± 1.07) (Table 1). Upon confiscation, the geographical origin of capture was confirmed for five of the individuals (Fouta Djallon (n = 2), High Guinea (n = 1) and Forest Region of Guinea (n = 1) and Sierra Leone (n = 1)). All subjects reached the sanctuary presenting varying states of mental and physical health. A four-point Likert scale was used to quantify the level of abnormal behaviors upon arrival; a minimum of five experienced staff members scored independently each individual they had personally witnessed arrive at the sanctuary (Table 2). The use of this simple four-point scale aimed to ensure that the staff could easily score each individual regardless of the individual’s date of arrival. Data were then averaged across all scorers to produce a final score for each individual ranging from zero to three [35] (Table 1). The low standard deviation reported in Table 1 confirmed there was a strong consensus among the scorers. The averaged abnormal behavior score upon arrival (ABS) was used as a covariate in our models.

Table 1.

Summary of each subject’s sex, year of birth (YOB), current age (as of January 2012), age upon arrival to the Centre for Conservation of Chimpanzees (CCC), the number of years spent at sanctuary (as of January 2012), and average abnormal behavior likert-scale [35] score upon arrival (ABS).

Name Sex YOB Group Arrival Date (month/year) Age of Individual (years) Age at arrival (years) Years at sanctuary ABS ± 1SD
HAKIM Male 2004 Nursery 04/06 8 2 6 0 ± 0.0
AMA Female 2006 Nursery 05/08 6 2 4 1.6 ± 0.5
KIRIKOU Male 2006 Nursery 05/08 6 2 4 2.2 ± 0.4
LILI Female 2005 Nursery 03/07 7 2 5 0 ± 0.0
FLO Female 2005 Nursery 08/07 7 2 5 0.75 ± 0.5
DOUMA Male 2006 Nursery 06/07 6 1 5 0.25 ± 0.5
PANZA Male 2005 Nursery 09/06 8 1 7 0.25 ± 0.5
TANGO male 2009 TDT 06/10 4 1 3 0.2 ± 0.4
TYA Female 2010 TDT 01/11 2 1 1 0.25 ± 0.5
DEMOU Female 2007 TDT 03/11 5 4 1 0 ± 0.0

Table 2.

Summary of scale used to quantify the degree of aberrant or stereotypic behaviors exhibited by each orphan upon arrival at the sanctuary. Experienced keepers, staff members and long-term volunteers who were present at the time were asked to score each chimpanzee independently. The final score for each individual was averaged based on a minimum of five scorers.

Likert Scale Scale definition
3 Exhibits a range of abnormal behaviors, i.e., intense rocking, hair plucking and/or self-mutilation, these behaviors are seen throughout the day under both stressful and relaxed situations/environments.
2 Exhibits abnormal behaviors occasionally under stressful and less stressful conditions, e.g., rocking, on a daily basis. Abnormal behaviors seen several times a day.
1 Exhibits abnormal behaviors on occasion and only under stressful situations. Abnormal behaviors observed no more than once a day.
0 No abnormal behaviors

Once or twice daily, all CCC juveniles and infants go on bush-outings in the surrounding areas of the sanctuary. After being provisioned, the “Nursery” and the “TDT” chimpanzees are accompanied by one keeper and one to two volunteers during their bushwalks The CCC has a strict policy on human contact during bush-outings; contact typically only takes place when it is solicited and should not typically be initiated by a human caretaker unless necessary.

2.2. Data Collection During Bush-Outings

Ten minute continuous individual focal follows were undertaken during bush-outings to record the duration of activities performed (total individual average time observed: “Nursery” = 799 ± 29.80 min; “TDT” = 1,053 ± 14.71 min) [36,37]. Each behavioral category was coded using a basic behavioral ethogram (Table 3). A change in behavioral state was recorded when the intervening interval surpassed 3 seconds. During focal follows, we also recorded the species and part of the food item consumed (Table 4). In addition, any physical contact with a human and its duration were recorded throughout the focal observations. Finally, we conjointly recorded continuously the duration of the arboreal or terrestrial location of the individual. All behavioral recordings were conducted by L. Ongman with a Palmtop recorder uploaded with Observer XT 10.1 (®Noldus Information Technology).

Table 3.

Ethogram for data collection categories. All categories were recorded during focal follows, including behavior, location and human contact.

CATEGORIES Definition
BEHAVIORS
Social Behavior An individual is engaged in social play or allo-grooming with humans or conspecifics. This category also includes rarely observed social sexual events.
Feeding Individual eating a food item
Searching for food An individual actively searching for food (may be collecting seed pods or fruit).
Subsistence tool-use The use of an object, e.g., stick or stalk of vegetation, to probe or explore an opening, including termite mound holes, ground holes, or cracks. This behavioral category also included the use of a solid object to strike another to access potentially an embedded edible resource.
General Solitary Behaviors Time spent self-grooming, playing alone or with an object, and solitary sexual events such as masturbation
Observing Individual maintains gaze directed towards keeper or conspecific group member. Who is observed and their activity is also recorded. Gaze must be maintained for longer than 3 seconds and within 3 meters of the individual being observed.
Nest Making Manipulation of branches and/or terrestrial herbaceous vegetation (THV) with the purpose to construct or modify a nesting structure either for resting or play.
Travel Locomotion on the ground or in an arboreal setting (excludes individual displacement when actively searching for food—see above).
Resting The individual is sleeping, standing or sitting and is not actively playing, grooming or partaking in any social behavior stated above, including not actively feeding, foraging, tool using or observing.
LOCATION
Arboreal Location of chimpanzee is in a tree or a vine, i.e., off the ground.
Terrestrial Location of chimpanzee is at ground level.
HUMAN CONTACT
Yes Individual is in direct physical contact with a human caretaker.
No Individual is not in direct physical contact with a human caretaker.

Table 4.

Plants and insects or insect products consumed during bush-outings by the “TDT” and “Nursery” groups between January 28–May 18 2012. Data includes species, family and part of plant eaten (THV: terrestrial herbaceous vegetation).

No. Species Family Type of Plant Part eaten
1 Adansonia digitata Bombacaceae Tree fruit
2 Afzelia Africana Caesalpinioideae Tree honey on leaves
3 Allophyllus africanus Sapindaceae Tree fruit
4 Andropogon gayanus Poaceae Grass leaf
5 Bombax costatum Bombacaceae Tree flower, leaf
6 Cassia sieberiana Caesalpinioideae Tree seed
7 Carapa procera Meliaceae Tree fruit
8 Cola cordifolia Sterculiaceae Tree flower, leaf stem, fruit, bark
9 Cordia myxa Boraginaceae Tree fruit
10 Costus afer Zingiberaceae THV stalk
11 Daniella oliveri Caesalpinioideae Tree flower, seeds, new leaf
12 Detarium microcarpum Caesalpinioideae Tree fruit, bark
13 Dialium guineensis Caesalpinioideae Tree fruit
14 Diospyros mespiliformis Ebenaceae Tree fruit, new leaf
15 Dioscorea sp. Dioscoreaceae Vine leaf
16 Ficus sp. Moraceae Tree fruit
17 Ficus sur Moraceae Tree fruit
18 Ficus thonninguii Moraceae Tree fruit
19 Gardenia erubescens Rubiaceae Tree fruit
20 Hanna undulate Simaroubaceae Tree fruit
21 Khaya senegalensis Meliaceae Tree leaf, leaf stem
22 Kigelia africana Bignoniaceae Tree fruit
23 Landolphia heudelotti Apocynaceae Vine bark, fruit, leaf
24 Lannea acida Anacardiaceae Tree seed
25 Lannea microcarpa Anacardiaceae Tree leaf, bark
26 Manguifera indica Anacardiaceae Tree leaf, fruit
27 Marantochloa cuspidate Marantaceae THV stalk
28 Opilia celtidifolia Opilianaceae Vine bark, leaf
29 Oxytenanthera abyssinica Poaceae Grass stalk
30 Parkia biglobosa Mimosaceae Tree flower, fruit
31 Piliostigma thoningui Caesalpinioideae Tree seed
32 Pterocarpus erinaceus Fabaceae Tree flower, new leaf
33 Pterocarpus sp. santalinoides Fabaceae Tree flower
34 Pterocarpus senegalensis Fabaceae Tree flower
35 Raphia sp. Arecaceae Tree stalk
36 Saba comorensis Apocynaceae Vine fruit
37 Saba senegalensis Apocynaceae Vine fruit
38 Siphonochilus sp. Zingiberaceae THV root, flower
39 Smilax anceps Smilaceae Vine new leaf
40 Tamarindus indica Caesalpinioideae Tree bark
41 Uapaca somon Euphorbiaceae Tree fruit
42 Ximenia Americana Olacaceae Shrub fruit
43 Xylopia aethiopica Annonaceae Tree seed
44 Oecophylla longinoda Formicidae Insect ant
45 Procubitermes Termitidae Insect termite
46 Trigona sp. Apidae Honey honey

2.3. Analysis

A Pearson’s correlation was performed on the percentage of time spent engaged in social behaviors (i.e., social play and allo-grooming) and the percentage of time spent engaged in foraging (i.e., eating, searching for food and subsistence tool use), in order to assess whether there was any behavioral trade-off between these two domains of behavior. Both percentages were calculated using the continuous focal data and the total time spent engaged in these activities was divided by the total focal time for each individual.

Continuous focal follows served to calculate activity budgets across nine main behavioral state categories, i.e., eating, searching for food, resting, traveling, observing conspecifics and humans, social behavior, solitary behavior as well as tool use and nest building (Table 3). The total duration of each activity was divided by the total focal time for each individual and computed as an overall mean percentage of activity.

In order to analyze what variables predicted observation of conspecifics’ foraging and dietary knowledge, i.e., the total number of food parts consumed by an individual, we used a general linear model (GLM) Univariate analysis in SPSS version 19. A Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was first run on all covariates and dependent variables to test the data for normality. Since all variables showed a normal distribution, all further statistical tests performed on the data were parametric. All tests were two-tailed and the significance level was p < 0.05. All means are reported with a ±1 standard deviation (SD) unless otherwise specified.

The first GLM analysis examined the effects of individual age, sex and mean ABS on individual dietary knowledge. This model included additional covariates such as the amount of time individuals watched conspecifics foraging and percentage of time spent in contact with humans. Since the majority of food items consumed during the bush-outings occurred in an arboreal setting (see Result’s section: The overall percentage of food items which were located in an arboreal setting was 82%), the percentage of time spent arboreal was also examined in the model. Dietary knowledge was expected to be higher for older individuals. Individuals who spend more time in contact with humans were expected to present less dietary knowledge and to have exhibited higher levels of ABS. The second model explored whether the percentage of time spent observing conspecifics’ foraging was influenced by individual age, sex and the mean ABS as well as individual dietary knowledge and percentage of time spent foraging. More knowledgeable individuals, as reflected by their broader individual dietary repertoire, were expected to spend less time observing others, and individuals who spent more time foraging were expected to spend less time observing conspecifics’ foraging and vice versa.

3. Results

Findings failed to show a significant correlation between percentage of time spent in social and foraging behaviors (Pearson correlation: n = 10, r = −0.537, p = 0.109). There was therefore no apparent trade-off at the individual level between time spent in one or the other of these domains of behavior.

3.1. Activity Budget, Location and Human Contact

When examining the mean individuals’ activity budget based on continuous focal follows, the chimpanzees on bush-outings spent on average nearly 70% of their time engaged in either resting (22.2 ± 6.7%), feeding (22.0 ± 9.3%) or traveling (22.6 ± 3.1%) (Figure 2). They spent a quarter of their time engaged in social (13.9 ± 5.3%) or solitary (11.2 ± 2.7%) behaviors, while tool use (0.5 ± 0.4%) and nesting behavior (0.6 ± 0.5%) occurred more rarely. The chimpanzees also spent a mean percentage of time of 1.5 ± 1.2% searching for food and 1.9 ± 1.1% observing others (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Mean individual percentage of time spent (±1 SE) across the nine main behavioral categories recorded during continuous focal follows.

Based on the continuous focal data, individuals spent on average 55.5 ± 20.6% of their time terrestrial and 43.6 ± 21.2% of their time in an arboreal location. Also, individuals spent relatively less time in direct contact with human caretakers (19.0 ± 10.9%) than not (80.2 ± 10.9%).

3.2. Dietary Knowledge

During this study, subjects consumed an individual mean of 27 ± 4.3 food items. The mean individual percentage of food items located and ingested in trees was 82 ± 6.5%. Data from focal follows revealed that the subjects of this study consumed altogether 46 different species including 63 different food items during the four-month study period (for full list see Table 4). The highest percentage of food parts consumed included fruit, leaves and flowers (33%, 19% and 13%, respectively); roots, insects and honey contributed the least to their overall dietary repertoire (Figure 3). The Univariate GLM model examined the influence of mean ABS, age, sex, as well as the percentage of time spent arboreal, in human contact, and observing conspecifics foraging upon individual variation in dietary knowledge. Only the percentage time spent arboreal emerged as significant (GLM: F1,10 = 10.27, p = 0.049).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Percentage of food parts consumed (n = 63) during bush-outings during the four-month study period.

Individual dietary knowledge and the percentage of time spent arboreal indeed showed a highly significant positive correlation (Pearson correlation: n = 10, r = 0.81, p = 0.005). Individuals who spent more time arboreal therefore showed a greater dietary knowledge than counterparts who spent more time on the ground. However, the percentage of time spent in human contact failed to correlate negatively with time spent arboreal (Pearson correlation: n = 10, r = −0.36, p = 0.31). Hence, contact with humans did not affect as predicted the chimpanzees’ ability to spend time arboreal.

3.3. Observing Conspecifics Foraging

During bush-outings individuals spent little of their total percentage time observing humans and conspecifics (Figure 2). The rehabilitants spent on average 54.4 ± 24.4% of their time observing conspecifics as a function of the total time spent observing others, i.e., humans and conspecifics. Only 28.9 ± 24.9% of the total time observing others was spent observing conspecifics foraging. In addition, there were no recorded instances of individuals observing humans foraging, confirming that during this study social information on foraging could only be acquired via observation of conspecifics. Neither sex nor the covariates included in the GLM Univariate analysis, i.e., age, individual mean ABS, individual dietary knowledge, individual percentage time spent foraging or in human contact, significantly explained the recorded variation in time spent observing conspecifics foraging.

4. Discussion

4.1. Activity Budget

The percentage of time spent engaged in social play or allo-grooming did not correlate significantly with individual percentage time spent foraging. This finding suggests that CCC individuals showed no behavioral trade-off between these two core domains of activity. Our study rehabilitants spent on average slightly more time terrestrial than arboreal, although there was a great deal of variation across individuals. They also spent on average less time in direct contact with humans than not. The rehabilitant who spent the most time in human contact during this study happened to be the youngest and the newest arrival (2 years old with 46% of time spent in human contact).

Traveling, resting and foraging were the predominant activities recorded during bush-outings and contributed on average equally to the individuals’ activity budget. Although the chimpanzees were always fed before going out on bush-outings, they spent nearly a quarter of their time foraging (24.0 ± 10.9%). While provisioned, individuals were fed a balanced diet, which mirrored that of individuals who remained in cages or enclosures. Individuals who partook in bush-outings therefore continued to forage for wild foods either because they needed to supplement their provisioned diet and/or they preferred wild foods over provisioned foods.

4.2. Dietary Knowledge

Our study period coincided with the end of the dry season, a period of fruit scarcity in the HNNP [38]. Nevertheless, during this study, fruit was the predominant food item consumed with as many as 21 different fruit species recorded as ingested, followed by leaves and flowers, with 12 and eight different plant species targeted respectively (Figure 3). High fruit consumption concurred with observations of wild chimpanzees recorded elsewhere [39]. The great majority of food items consumed were located up in the trees. We uncovered a significant positive correlation between the individual percentage time spent arboreal and dietary knowledge, which potentially highlights the significance of ecological opportunities in contributing to the expansion of individuals’ dietary knowledge. Unlike reported findings in Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) [32], there was no effect of percentage time spent in human contact on individual dietary knowledge. Finally, contrary to expectation, neither age, sex nor mean ABS upon arrival predicted individual variation in dietary knowledge.

4.3. The Role of Observation

Although previous studies both in wild and captive settings revealed gender, age and rearing history effects on conspecific observation and individual social learning [19,28,40,41], none were detected in this study. In addition, while several studies have highlighted the importance of decreasing human contact to promote species-specific behaviors, especially for individuals rehabilitated for the purpose of release [12,32,42], contact with humans and human presence in the context of the CCC failed to predict potential observational learning of conspecifics engaged in foraging behavior. Albeit raised as an issue with rehabilitated orangutans [32], contact with humans during bush-outings did not affect the foraging development of CCC rehabilitants. However, during this study, rehabilitants spent a limited percentage of time observing others engaged in foraging activities. This low percentage may be attributed to the low frequency of the occurrence of complex food processing events during the study period, i.e., the end of the dry season. Indeed, conspecific observation among young wild great apes is most often recorded when the ‘demonstrator’ is engaged in complex processing skills [31], such as nut cracking and ant dipping in chimpanzees [28,41]. Tool use at the CCC occurs predominantly during the rainy season due the availability of target foods, i.e., driver ants Dorylus sp. or Afzelia africana pods which younger individuals crack with stones to access the seeds within during rainy season months [43]. Therefore, due to seasonality, there were fewer opportunities to gather data on subjects observing conspecifics performing complex foraging tasks such as tool use behaviors (Figure 2). Added to this, our definition of observation, i.e., <3 m and observing an individual for more than 3 seconds, may be too narrow, precluding observation events occurring at greater distances or lasting for shorter durations. Hence, the role of observation in behavioral acquisition in our study may have been underestimated. Future studies should be performed over a longer sampling period to account for seasonality and across more subjects, and the definition of “observation” should potentially be revised to capture more effectively observation events of shorter duration and occurring at greater distances. Finally other behaviors, such as scrounging and co-feeding [30], should also be recorded to assess the contribution of other socially-biased learning mechanisms on dietary acquisition.

4.4. The “Super Chimpanzee” Theory

The dietary repertoire of our subjects (i.e., 46 total number of species and 63 total food items consumed) was nearly equivalent to that reported in a three-year study of wild chimpanzees in Fongoli, Senegal, which registered the consumption of 47 different species and 60 different food items [18]. The latter site shares similar ecological conditions to the PNHN where the CCC is located [13,18,33]. Due to our comparatively short study period and smaller number of study subjects, the dietary knowledge of our study individuals and CCC rehabilitants as a whole could possibly far exceed that of Fongoli. If true, this difference could be attributed to a lower floristic diversity at Fongoli compared with the HNNP [13,18,33], but also to the unique conditions afforded by a sanctuary setting.

The relatively broad dietary knowledge recorded among the CCC subjects during our study may stem from two sanctuary-specific factors; i.e., the young age of residents partaking in bush-outings and their different geographical origins. Most studies on wild chimpanzees suggest that social learning processes contribute the most to the acquisition of knowledge of edible foods and processing techniques, e.g., [25,28]. In this sense, the dietary knowledge of a group may be broadened (1) via individual acquisition of novel foods via trial and error (innovation) followed by social transmission (diffusion) or (2) the social transmission of new knowledge carried by migrant individuals (dissemination) [44]. Since young chimpanzees present a higher propensity for innovation and social learning than older subjects [28], the likelihood of innovation and behavioral diffusion may be higher among groups comprised of young rehabilitated orphans on bush-outings in an environment offering innovation opportunities than older individuals who are restricted to their enclosure.

Cultural transmission has been documented in both wild and captive settings and this process can occur both within and between groups [28,31,45,46]. All subjects of our study and the majority of orphans at the CCC are wild-born and originate from different regions of Guinea or West Africa [47]. Each individual therefore carries with it its own environmental and cultural knowledge. Studies of wild chimpanzees have revealed that juveniles and infants, including individuals under 2 years old, are able to successfully perform tool use tasks such as using a probe to access an embedded food resource [41,46,48]. In addition, albeit not yet weaned, chimpanzees as young as 6 months old start to consume solid foods and are therefore beginning to build up familiarity with different edible food parts [49]. Therefore, in spite of an early separation from their mothers, young arrivals (average age of arrival for sample subjects was 1.8 years) likely possess dietary knowledge of their own and are familiar with different edible or non-edible food items. Since the environment around the CCC is highly heterogeneous [13] and many species recorded as consumed during this study are widely distributed across West Africa [50], some overlap in food species between the rehabilitants’ native environment and the CCC’s is highly probable. Rehabilitants could therefore be contributing their dietary knowledge additively to a pool of information of edible foods. Anecdotally, evidence of the ‘super chimpanzee’ theory was witnessed during our study when a recently arrived individual climbed an oil palm tree, Elaeis guineensis, and removed a frond to consume the petiole. Oil palm petiole feeding has been witnessed among wild chimpanzees elsewhere in Guinea and Africa [51]. No other individual in the group had ever been seen doing this behavior before. The following day, two other members were seen performing this behavior [52].

5. Conclusion

Until adolescence, the CCC offers the opportunity of bush-outings to its rehabilitants as daily enrichment. These bush-outings, during which human contact is principally initiated by the rehabilitants, provide highly stimulating settings offering learning opportunities and enabling the development of a range of behavioral skills. Daily bush-outings favor the chimpanzees’ immersion in natural surroundings where they may learn and perfect nest building skills, and broaden their dietary knowledge, as well as their social skills during interactions with conspecifics [13]. Neither sex nor age of our young subjects affected their rehabilitation performance in terms of their dietary knowledge. Our study also revealed that individuals with signs of psychological and emotional trauma upon arrival had an equivalent dietary knowledge compared to less traumatized orphans. Nonetheless, historical and latent stereotypic behaviors may serve to predict difficulties in the social domain [53]. Nevertheless, CCC staff have noted that, after several years of bush-outings, some chimpanzees ceased to demonstrate stereotypic behaviors, indicating that this form of rehabilitation can effectively help to curtail initial psychological and/or emotional trauma [54].

Data across sanctuaries, if gathered systematically using equivalent observational protocols and behavioral ethograms, could contribute to a better understanding of the key factors predicting rehabilitation success, individual progress and eventually release success. Collaborative research programs across sanctuaries and captive settings could therefore further help inform best rehabilitation practices [16,32,55]. For example, activity budget studies and evaluations of individual’s skillsets and behavior in both the physical and social domain could serve to compare rehabilitation approaches and environments both within and among sanctuaries. An activity budget may also monitor the evolution of individuals with respect to their history and personality, as well as group composition and dynamics. This study has begun to explore indicators and/or the learning and developmental processes underlying successful rehabilitation. These data can serve to inform future identification of suitable release candidates and also to help to improve the welfare of individual rehabilitants especially since the CCC keepers are now trained in behavioral data collection and are monitoring individuals. It is apparent that both social and ecological skills are required to maximize release success [12] and special attention should be given to both during rehabilitation since they are two distinct domains requiring differing skill sets. In addition, as proposed by others [12,42], there is a growing necessity for behavioral data on chimpanzees both at the individual and group level pre- and post-release to start identifying key behavioral indicators of release success. This study is a good start in the right direction. We hope to encourage more studies among rehabilitant chimpanzees aimed at better predicting and understanding rehabilitation and potentially release success among our closest living relatives.

Acknowledgments

Many people contributed to the success of this study, notably the CCC’s staff and volunteers. We are of course especially grateful to all the CCC orphans for marveling us daily on their resilience, empathy and patience and for offering us such amazing insights into their cognitive abilities and determination to survive against all odds. We would also like to thank the Guinean government including the Direction des Eaux et Forêts and the Direction Nationale des Aires Protegées, especially Aboubacar Oulare, for their continued collaboration and support, and the numerous funders including the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Services (USFW), the Arcus Foundation, the Edith J. Goode Trust fund, the Fondation Brigitte Bardot, Fondation Le Pal Nature, IPPL-UK, IPPL-US, the Tusk foundation, the Sweden Chimpanzee Trust and Fauna and Flora International (FFI) who have contributed over the years to the CCC’s rehabilitation and release activities.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References and Notes

  • 1.Noon C. Chimpanzees and Retirement. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 1999;2:141–146. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0202_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Teleki G. Sanctuaries for Ape Refugees. In: Beck B., Stoinski T., Hutchins M., Maple T., Norton B., Rowan A., Stevens E., Arluke A., editors. Great Apes and Humans, the Ethics of Coexistence. Smithsonian Press; Washington, DC, USA: 2001. pp. 133–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Farmer K. Pan-African Sanctuary Alliance: Status and Range of Activities for Great Ape Conservation. Am. J. Primatol. 2002;58:117–132. doi: 10.1002/ajp.10054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Campbell G., Kuehl H., N’Goran Kouamé P., Boesch C. Alarming Decline of West African Chimpanzees in Côte d’Ivoire. Curr. Biol. 2008;18:R903–R904. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schipper J., Chanson J.S., Chiozza F., Cox N.A., Hoffmann M., Katariya V., Lamoreux J., Rodrigues A.S., Stuart S.N., Temple H.J., et al. The Status of the World’s Land and Marine Mammals: Diversity, Threat, and Knowledge. Science. 2008;322:225–230. doi: 10.1126/science.1165115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Beck B. Chimpanzee Orphans: Sanctuaries, Reintroduction and Cognition. In: Lonsdorf E., Ross S., Matsuzawa T., editors. The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives. University of Chicago Press; Chicago, IL, USA: 2010. pp. 332–346. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Morimura N., Idani G., Matsuzawa T. The First Chimpanzee Sanctuary in Japan: An Attempt to Care for the “Surplus” of Biomedical Research. Am. J. Primatol. 2011;73:226–232. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Faust L., Cress D., Farmer K., Ross S., Beck B. Predicting Capacity Demand on Sanctuaries for African Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Int. J. Primatol. 2011;32:849–864. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9505-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Beck B., Walkup K., Rodrigues M., Unwin S., et Tara Stoinski D.T. Best Practice Guidelines for the Reintroduction of Great Apes. IUCN; Abu Dhabi, UAE: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hannah A., McGrew W. Primate Responses to Environmental Change. Chapman and Hall; London, UK: 2005. Rehabilitation of Captive Chimpanzees; pp. 167–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yeager C. Orangutan Rehabilitation in Tanjung Puting National Park, Indonesia. Conserv. Biol. 1997;11:802–805. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Farmer K., Honig N., Goossens B., Jamart A. Habitat Ecologique et Liberte des Primates: Re-Introduction of Central Chimpanzees to the Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo. In: Soorae P., editor. IUCN/SSC: Global Re-Introduction Perspectives: Additional Case-Studies from Around the Globe. Re-Introduction Specialist Group; Abu Dhabi, UAE: 2010. pp. 231–237. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Humle T., Colin C., Laurans M., Raballand E. Group Release of Sanctuary Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Haut Niger National Park, Guinea, West Africa: Ranging Patterns and Lessons So Far. Int. J. Primatol. 2010;32:456–473. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Reimers M., Schwarzenberger F., Preuschoft S. Rehabilitation of Research Chimpanzees: Stress and Coping after Long-Term Isolation. Hormones Behav. 2007;51:428–435. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fritz J., Maki S., Nash L., Martin T., Matevia M. The Relationship Between Forage Material and Levels of Coprophagy in Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biol. 1992;11:313–318. doi: 10.1002/zoo.1430110503. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wobber V., Hare B. Psychological Health of Orphan Bonobos and Chimpanzees in African Sanctuaries. PLoS ONE. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Doran D. Influence of Seasonality on Activity Patterns, Feeding Behavior, Ranging, and Grouping Patterns in Taï Chimpanzees. Int. J. Primatol. 1997;18:183–206. doi: 10.1023/A:1026368518431. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Preutz J. Feeding Ecology of Savanna Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in Fongoli, Senegal. In: Hohmann G., Robbins M., Boesch C., editors. Feeding Ecology in Apes and Other Primates: Ecological, Physical, and Behavioral Aspects. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2006. pp. 161–182. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Goodall J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Boesch C., Boesch H. Hunting Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1989;78:547–573. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330780410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Yamakoshi G. Dietary Responses to Fruit Scarcity of Wild Chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: Possible Implications for Ecological Importance of Tool Use. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 1998;106:283–295. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199807)106:3&#x0003c;283::AID-AJPA2&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Marshall A., Wrangham R. Evolutionary Consequences of Fallback Foods. Int. J. Primatol. 2007;28:1219–1235. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9218-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.van-Lawick-Goodall J. Cultural Elements in a Chimpanzee Community. In: Menzel E.W., editor. Precultural Primate Behavior. Karger; Basel, Switzerland: 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pusey A. Mother-Offspring Relationships in Chimpanzees after Weaning. Anim. Behav. 1983;31:363–377. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80055-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M. Sex Differences in the Behavioral Development of Chimpanzees at Mahale. In: Heltne P., Marquardt L., editors. Understanding Chimpanzees. Harvard University Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 1989. pp. 104–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Van Schaik C. Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture. Science. 2003;299:102–105. doi: 10.1126/science.1078004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Galef B., Jr., Giraldeau L. Social Influences on Foraging in Vertebrates: Causal Mechanisms and Adaptive Functions. Anim. Behav. 2001;61:3–15. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Biro D., Inoue-Nakamura N., Tonooka R., Yamakoshi G., Sousa C., Matsuzawa T. Cultural Innovation and Transmission of Tool Use in Wild Chimpanzees: Evidence from Field Experiments. Anim. Cognit. 2003;6:213–223. doi: 10.1007/s10071-003-0183-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lonsdorf E. Sex Differences in the Development of Termite-Fishing Skills in the Wild Chimpanzees, (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii), of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Anim. Behav. 2005;70:673–683. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.12.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rapaport L., Brown G. Social Influences on Foraging Behavior in Young Nonhuman Primates: Learning What, Where, and How to Eat. Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 2008;17:189–201. doi: 10.1002/evan.20180. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jaeggi A., Dunkel L., Van Noordwijk M., Wich S., Sura A., Van Schaik C. Social Learning of Diet and Foraging Skills by Wild Immature Bornean Orangutans: Implications for Culture. Am. J. Primatol. 2010;72:62–71. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Riedler B., Millesi E., Pratje P. Adaptation to Forest Life During the Reintroduction Process of Immature Pongo abelii. Int. J. Primatol. 2010;31:647–663. doi: 10.1007/s10764-010-9418-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brugiere D., Dia M., Diakite S., Gbansara M., Mamy M., Saliou B., Magassouba B. Large- and Medium-Sized Ungulates in the Haut Niger National Park, Republic of Guinea: Population Changes 1997–2002. Oryx. 2005;39:50–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fleury-Brugiere M., Brugiere D. High Population Density of Pan troglodytes verus in the Haut Niger National Park, Republic of Guinea: Implications for Local and Regional Conservation. Int. J. Primatol. 2010;31:383–392. doi: 10.1007/s10764-010-9391-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Likert R. A Technique for the Measurement of Attitudes. Arch. Psychol. 1932;140:55–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Altmann J. Observational Study of Behavior: Sampling Methods. Behaviour. 1974;49:227–267. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Martin P., Bateson P. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. 3rd ed. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, UK: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Humle T. School of Anthropology and Conservation, University of Kent; Canterbury, UK: 2008. Unpublished data. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Humle T. Location and Ecology. In: Matsuzawa T., Humle T., Sugiyama Y., Matsuzawa T., Yamagiwa J., editors. The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba. Springer; Tokyo, Japan: 2011. pp. 13–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Boesch C., Boesch-Achermann H. The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford University Press; New York, NY, USA: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Humle T., Snowdon C., Matsuzawa T. Social Influences on Ant-Dipping Acquisition in the Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Anim. Cognit. 2009;12:37–48. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0272-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Baker K. Guidelines for Nonhuman Primate Re-Introductions. Re-Introduction News. 2002;21:29–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Humle T., Ongman L. School of Anthropology and Conservation, University of Kent; Canterbury, UK: 2012. Personal observation. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Humle T. Primate Material Culture. In: Hicks D., Beaudry M., editors. The Oxford Handbook of Material Culture Studies. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2010. pp. 406–424. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Whiten A., Spiteri A., Horner V., Bonnie K., Lambeth S., Schapiro S., de Waal F. Transmission of Multiple Traditions within and between Chimpanzee Groups. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:1038–1043. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Humle T., Snowdon C. Socially Biased Learning in the Acquisition of a Complex Foraging Task in Juvenile Cottontop Tamarins, (Saguinus oedipus) Anim. Behav. 2008;75:267–277. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Chimpanzee Conservation Center. 2012. Somoria, Faranah, Republic of Guinea. Unpublished dataset.
  • 48.Lonsdorf E. What is the Role of Mothers in the Acquisition of Termite-Fishing Behaviors in Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Anim. Cognit. 2006;9:36–46. doi: 10.1007/s10071-005-0002-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nishida T., Turner L. Food Transfer Between Mother and Infant Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Int. J. Primatol. 1996;17:947–968. doi: 10.1007/BF02735296. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hawthorne W., Jongkind C. Woody Plants of Western African Forests. Royal Botanic Gardens; Kew, UK: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Humle T., Matsuzawa T. Oil Palm Use by Adjacent Communities of Chimpanzees at Bossou and Nimba Mountains, West Africa. Int. J. Primatol. 2004;25:551–581. doi: 10.1023/B:IJOP.0000023575.93644.f4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ongman L. School of Anthropology and Conservation, University of Kent; Canterbury, UK: 2012. Personal observation. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ongman L., Colin C., Raballand E., Humle T. Individual History and the Social Dimensions of Rehabilitation in Chimpanzees. 2013. in preparation. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 54.Raballand E., Colin C. Personal observation. 2012. Chimpanzee Conservation Center, Somoria, Faranah, Republic of Guinea.
  • 55.Farmer K., Buchanan-Smith H.M., Jamart A. Behavioral Adaptation of Pan troglodytes troglodytes. Int. J. Primatol. 2006;27:747–765. doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9041-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Animals : an Open Access Journal from MDPI are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES