Abstract
Background & Aims
Five-year-olds with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) often struggle with mastering grammatical morphemes. It has been proposed that verbal morphology is particularly problematic in this respect. Previous research has also shown that in young typically developing children grammatical markers appear later in more phonologically challenging contexts. The main aim of the present study was to explore whether grammatical deficits in children with SLI are morphosyntactic in nature, or whether phonological factors also explain some of the variability in morpheme production. The analysis considered the effects of the same phonological factors on the production of three different morphemes: two verbal (past tense -ed; 3rd person singular -s) and one nominal morpheme (possessive -s).
Methods & Procedures
The participants were 30 children with SLI (21 boys) aged 4;6–5;11 years (mean=5;1) The data were collected during grammar test sessions, which consisted of question/answer elicitations of target forms involving picture props. A total of 2301 items were analysed using binary logistic regression; the predictors included: 1) utterance position of the target word, 2) phonological complexity of its coda, 3) voicing of the final stem consonant, and 4) syllabicity (allomorph type); 5) participant accounting for the individual differences in the responses.
Outcomes & Results
The results showed a robust effect of syllabicity on the correct morpheme production. Specifically, syllabic allomorphs (e.g., She dresses) were significantly more challenging than the segmental ones (e.g., He runs) for all three morphemes. The effects of other factors were observed only for a single morpheme: coda complexity and voicing helped explain variability in past tense production, and utterance position significantly affected children’s performance with the possessive. The participant factor also had a significant effect, indicating high within-group variability – often observed in SLI population.
Conclusions & Implications
The systematic effect of syllabicity across both verbal and nominal morphemes suggests morphophonological influences in the grammatical development of children with SLI that cannot be fully explained by syntactic deficits. Poorer performance in producing syllabic allomorphs can be accounted for by much lower overall frequency of these forms, and by the “tongue-twisting” effect of producing similar segments in succession, as in added [ædəd], washes [wɒʃəz]. Interestingly, the greater acoustic salience of the syllabic allomorphs (an extra syllable) does not enhance children’s abilities to produce them. These findings suggest that the interconnections between different levels of language have a stronger effect on the grammatical development of children with SLI than might be expected. Allomorphy should, therefore, be taken into account when designing language assessments and speech therapy, ensuring that children receive sufficient practice with the entire set of allomorphic variants.
Keywords: specific language impairment, grammatical morphemes, morphophonology, allomorphy, coda complexity, utterance position
Introduction
Early acquisition of grammar
Children’s understanding of the organisational principles of their first language becomes deeper and more complex over time. This process of gradual development of the linguistic competence in young children has long been an object of intense scientific interest (Bloom 1970; Brown 1973). For English-speaking children, one of the major characteristics of early acquisition are morphological errors of omission (Clark 2003), when children use bare stems without any morphological markers, as in Every day she dance or Two bus.
Some have proposed that these omissions may be caused by children’s incomplete syntactic or semantic representations (Wexler 1994). However, more recently it has also been shown that phonological factors have a significant effect on early morpheme production, with some phonological contexts being more challenging for adding grammatical morphemes than others. These constraints include, for example, the phonological complexity of the coda and the position of the target word within the utterance – two factors that have been found to systematically affect children’s early verbal (Song, Sundara & Demuth 2009) and nominal morpheme productions (Theodore, Demuth & Shattuck-Hufnagel 2011; Mealings, Cox & Demuth 2013).
The systematicity and robustness of these effects across morphemes provide extensive evidence that phonology is an important component of morphological development. For example, the plural form buses is often produced by 2-year-olds as ‘buseh’ [bɐsə] or ‘buss’ [bɐss] (Mealings et al. 2013). Such partial realisations suggest that it is phonological or articulation difficulties which make production of these morphemes challenging. If so, this would indicate a possible dissociation between young children’s receptive and expressive skills, demonstrating their awareness that the morpheme is required, despite omitting it in actual speech.
Morpheme acquisition in children with SLI
Variability in early morpheme production is also typical for children with SLI, who, despite their normal physical and cognitive abilities, lag behind their peers in terms of language development (Leonard 1998; Bishop & Norbury 2008). Since this language impairment is diagnosed by excluding other possible causes, the term “SLI” can potentially cover a broad range of deficits in receptive and expressive skills. In other words, children with SLI form a heterogeneous population (Dale & Cole 1991; Leonard 1998; Conti-Ramsden & Botting 1999). Nevertheless, it has been shown that, in general, English-speaking children with SLI find it particularly challenging to use auxiliary verbs (Cleave & Rice 1997; Grela & Leonard 2000) and bound grammatical morphemes (Leonard 1998; Norbury, Bishop & Briscoe 2001; Rice, Wexler & Cleave 1995), and they continue to omit a large proportion of these morphemes for an extended period of time.
According to Optional Infinitive/Extended Optional Infinitive hypothesis, these errors of omission are syntactic in nature (Rice, Wexler & Cleave 1995), affecting primarily verbal tense and agreement morphology. However, other studies have shown that at least some of the variability in the SLI children’s use of grammatical morphemes can be explained by the effects of phonological context (Marshall & van der Lely 2007; Polite 2011), suggesting that children have some knowledge of the morpheme despite their failure to reliably produce it. In addition, some of the recent findings show that the interconnections between different levels of language have stronger effects on the grammatical development in children with SLI than might be anticipated. Specifically, morphophonological processes, such as allomorphic variation, liaison, contraction and elision may be especially challenging for these children (Royle & Stine 2013). If so, the concept of impaired morphophonological abilities should be integrated into descriptive models of SLI.
Despite the findings mentioned above, it is not yet clear whether the phonological factors that influence the production of grammatical morphemes in younger typically developing (TD) children have the same effect on children with SLI across various morphemes. The aim of this study was therefore to provide a more in-depth investigation of this issue, examining the effects of phonological constraints and morphophonological alternations on morpheme production in children with SLI to determine whether they reveal the same tendencies as have been found for TD children.
Phonological constraints on morphological development
The factors of interest in this study are four constraints on morpheme production: coda complexity, stem coda voicing, utterance position and syllabicity (syllabic vs. segmental/non-syllabic allomorphs). We will first consider the significance of these effects for TD children and then discuss current findings in children with SLI.
Evidence from TD children
Previous studies have found that grammatical development in English-speaking 2-year-olds is significantly affected by phonological complexity of the target coda. Specifically, children are more accurate when adding grammatical morphemes to lexical stems that end in a vowel rather than in a consonant. In other words, items ending in a simple coda (e.g., plays) are presumably easier to articulate than those ending in complex codas/consonant clusters (e.g., sits) (Song, Sundara & Demuth 2009).
Voicing can also affect children’s abilities to produce morphemes, due to the difference in the order of acquisition between voiced and voiceless stops and fricatives. It has been shown that English voiceless stops (e.g., [p], [t], [k]) are usually acquired earlier in coda position than their voiced counterparts ([b], [d], [g]) (Kehoe & Stoel-Gammon 2001), and [s] is acquired before [z] (Smit 1993). Moreover, lexical stems ending in a voiced consonant require adding a voiced allomorph (e.g., stands [stændz]), thus creating clusters of voiced consonants that should be more challenging to produce than unvoiced clusters (e.g., sits [sıts]). In Berko’s classical study (1958), no significant differences were found between voiced and voiceless conditions. However, these voicing contrasts were studied within different allomorphs. Thus, the voiced condition included both phonologically simple (e.g., plays) and complex (e.g., stands) codas. Although simple codas are always voiced, they are typically acquired earlier. Therefore, not taking the coda complexity factor into account might have mitigated the results, masking possible voicing effects.
It has also been demonstrated that TD children are sensitive to the utterance position of the target form. Specifically, they are more likely to produce grammatical morphemes when the target word is in utterance-final rather than utterance-medial position, e.g., Every day he reads vs. He reads every day, and this has been found to affect both verbal and nominal morpheme production (Song, Sundara & Demuth 2009; Theodore, Demuth & Shattuck-Hufnagel 2011). This could be due to a) final phrase lengthening, which provides greater time for producing the final syllable and all segments, or b) increased articulation ease due to the absence of a following word.
It has further been shown that syllabic allomorphs (e.g., washes) are usually later-acquired by TD children than segmental allomorphs (e.g., climbs). This has been supported by evidence from children’s spontaneous speech (Brown 1973), elicited productions (Berko 1958), and elicited imitations (Mealings et al. 2013). However, the systematicity and robustness of this pattern for different morphemes and across age groups has not yet been given full consideration. For example, there is a question about the source of the delayed acquisition of the syllabic allomorphs: is it driven by the challenge of articulating similar sounds in succession (e.g., added), or due to the lower frequency of these allomophs in the speech input children hear and produce?
Evidence from children with SLI
Due to their more mature chronological age, 5-year-olds with SLI might not be affected in the same way by the phonological effects observed in TD 2-year-olds. However, there is evidence suggesting that, for example, coda complexity might have a similar effect on morpheme production in children with SLI as it does in TD children. Thus, some studies have reported a higher proportion of morpheme omission in the context of complex codas (Polite 2011), and even in older 9–16-year-old children with SLI (Marshall & van der Lely 2007). However, the age of the participants in the latter study raises some concerns. Specifically, in TD children the reported regular past tense forms are typically acquired by about 3;6 (Brown 1973). Since overall children with SLI demonstrate an approximate 2-year delay in their language development (Rice 2013), those participants who continue omitting grammatical markers at the age of 9–16 are likely to have additional problems (e.g., articulatory deficits) on top of their difficulties in acquiring morphology. Furthermore, the participants in the Polite’s study (2011) showed high overall accuracy and very small differences between the two conditions – 77% vs. 74% correct productions for simple versus complex codas, respectively. Therefore, it remains unclear how robust the effect of the coda complexity might be for children with SLI.
The effects of voicing on the speech of children with SLI have been investigated in a number of studies, and no significant differences have been established between voiced and voiceless conditions (Oetting & Horohov 1997; Marchman, Wulfeck & Ellis Weismer 1999). However, just like in TD children, the possible confounding factors such as coda complexity have not been controlled for when examining this issue. This problem therefore requires further investigation.
It has also been shown that utterance position significantly affects SLI morpheme production when using past tense -ed suffix. Specifically, participants have been significantly more accurate in producing correct forms in sentence-final position (Dalal & Loeb 2005). This finding is consistent with what has been previously established for TD populations. However, we wanted to investigate the robustness of this effect across more than one morpheme before generalising this result.
It has further been proposed that syllabic allomorphs tend to be later acquired by children with SLI (Oetting & Horohov 1997; Marchman, Wulfeck & Ellis Weismer 1999). As before, this pattern mirrors the observations in TD population. However, in these studies the analysis was based on a small number of syllabic tokens; it thus requires further empirical evidence before making confident inferences about the SLI population in general. Although it seems natural that increasing the word length by adding another syllable could make production more challenging, the longer duration of the syllabic allomorphs should also make them more perceptually salient (Mealings et al. 2013). The greater acoustic content thus might serve as an additional cue for children with SLI, improving their abilities to perceive the morphemes and enhancing acquisition.
To summarise, phonological factors seem to explain some of the variability in morpheme production in both TD and SLI populations. However, the systematicity and robustness of the phonological effects in children with SLI is not yet clear. Since these children are older and thus have better motor control skills than TD 2-year-olds, it seems important to investigate how this might compensate for the phonological constraints on morpheme production observed in much younger TD children. It could be the case that the effects of phonological factors extend beyond articulatory difficulties, and that acquiring morphophonological regularities is another problematic area for language development in children with SLI.
The aim of the present paper was to analyse coda complexity, voicing, utterance position and syllabicity within one model, considering their possible interactions, and to compare the results across morphemes of different types. At a glance, these factors seem to be of a diverse nature. But all these phenomena involve different levels of segmental, syllabic, and phrasal phonology that affect the realisation of inflectional morphemes. Due to their possible interactions, it seems essential to study them within one model to ensure there are no confounds.
When addressing these issues, we were guided by the general hypothesis that children with SLI should reveal similar patterns of morpheme acquisition to that of younger TD children. Furthermore, if there is a morphophonological component to the problem of inflectional morpheme realisation, we would expect to observe similar effects on both verbal and nominal morphemes.
Method
Data
The data were drawn from speech samples collected during the investigation of the efficacy of various intervention methods on the language development of children with SLI (Smith-Lock et al. 2013a, b). It focused on studying these children’s abilities to correctly use grammatical morphemes. Data collection spanned three years (2010–2012), aiming at establishing whether the intervention programs significantly improved children’s performance in general, and if so, which methods and activities gave the best results for more rapid language development.
Before and after treatment, each participant was tested on the same set of 30 target items for a particular grammatical morpheme. This paper includes only data from the pre-intervention sessions, and compares children’s production of three grammatical markers: 1) past tense -ed, 2) 3rd person singular -s, and 3) possessive marker -s.
Typically developing controls were not included in the original experiment. However, when designing the stimuli, the researchers first tested all target items on a group of twenty TD children age-matched with the SLI group. The final set of stimuli consisted only of the forms that were successfully elicited from the TD children 100% of the time. The items used in the experiment were all familiar verbs and nouns (both common and proper) balanced on the basis of the required allomorph type. For example, the past tense morpheme can be realised as [t], [d], or [əd]; the targets therefore included 10 words for each allomorph with mostly monosyllabic stems. However, they were not perfectly balanced in terms of coda types; thus, the majority of words in the voiced allomorph [d] condition had CVC/CVCC stems as in buzzed, and also a few CCV ones, as in cried. Although most items had monosyllabic stems, there were a few disyllables (e.g., the CVCV stem in watered1). The full list of target forms is presented in table 1.
Table 1.
Morpheme | Target items for each condition and their counts (number) | Utterance position: medial (proportion) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Segmental allomorphs | Syllabic allomorphs | |||
Simple codas* | Complex codas | |||
Past tense-ed | watered, cried, stirred (77) |
squeezed, paddled, smiled, crawled, combed, buzzed, climbed, hopped, skipped, touched, danced, walked, shopped, dropped, licked, jumped, kicked (414) |
pointed, ended, needed, twisted, added, folded, counted, landed, painted, melted (239) |
474 (.65) |
Present tense-s | wears, plays, cries (94) |
needs, opens, reads, drives, climbs, smiles, runs, coughs, skips, kicks, walks, cuts, laughs, picks, sits, counts, jumps (481) |
touches, freezes, watches, hisses, squeezes, brushes, squashes / crushes, kisses, mashes, washes (222) |
601 (.75) |
Possessive-s | bee’s, May’s, Mary’s, boy’s (98) |
Doug’s, Carl’s, dog’s, Em’s, Bob’s, man’s, Hope’s, Jack’s, Pat’s, Brett’s, Blake’s, Kate’s, Pip’s, cat’s, duck’s, sheep’s (447) |
Joyce’s, church’s, Josh’s, Grace’s, Blanche’s, Mitch’s, Rich’s, Trish’s, horse’s, fish’s (229) |
118 (.15) |
Participants
All the participants included in this study had been attending one of the Language Developmental Centres in Western Australia. These centres provide specialised language and academic intervention for children with SLI. Entry into the school requires being diagnosed as having SLI by a speech language pathologist. Children’s language skills were assessed either with the Preschool Language Scale (PLS) (Zimmerman et al. 2002) or the Clinical Evaluation of Language Function (CELF) (Wiig et al. 2006) as one part of an extensive assessment process for referral to the school. Referral information also included evidence that children’s non-verbal cognitive skills were within the normal range, as attested by a psychologist or paediatrician, using one of the following assessment tools: Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler 2002), Cognitive Adaptive Test (Accardo & Capute 2005), Denver Developmental Screening Test (Frankenburg et al. 1992), and Griffiths Mental Development Scales (Griffiths 1970). Although the scores on every standardised assessment were not available for the researchers in the present study, all the participants in this project were professionally attested as having (1) at least moderately impaired receptive or expressive language, i.e., 1–1.5 SD below the mean on the standardised language assessments; (2) normal non-verbal IQ, i.e., within the normal range (85–115 points) on the standardised cognitive skills tests; and (3) no hearing loss, neurological impairment or other diagnosis that would account for their language impairment.
In addition to standardised tests, all participants had to pass an articulation screening – to ensure their ability to produce the relevant phoneme combinations in non-morphemic contexts. This involved repetition of monosyllabic non-words with the target sounds on the end of the stems, as in [pept] for the past tense targets. Since the aim of the present study was to examine possible phonological constraints on children’s morpheme production, only those children who made no more than one error in this screening task were included in the analysis.
In order to examine variability in the children’s morpheme productions, we set an additional inclusion criterion. Thus, only those children who used the target morpheme correctly in 15–85% of the obligatory contexts were included in study. Out of a total of 47 participants who passed the articulation screening, 30 met this criterion. Although this excluded several potential participants, it also avoided any ceiling or floor effects.
The final analysis for this project was therefore based on the data from 9 girls and 21 boys with SLI aged 4;6–5;11 years (mean age = 5;1). The participants were tested either on their ability to add morphemes of tense/agreement (past tense -ed; 3rd person singular -s), or possessive -s. In the original experiment two target morphemes were assigned for each child, depending on their performance in a brief grammar screening test (see Smith-Lock et al. 2013a, b for a detailed description of the procedure). For the most part our data contain information on one target morpheme for each participant. However, there were four children who showed sufficient variability in producing two target morphemes. Thus, the analysis was based on data from 11 participants on past tense morpheme -ed (mean age = 5;1), 10 on the present tense -s (mean age = 5;2), and 13 on the possessive -s (mean age = 4;6).
Procedure
The experiment was administered during a separate 15–20-minute one-on-one session between the speech language pathologist (SLP) and the child, which took place after the preliminary screening assessments. The task consisted of question–answer elicitations of the 30 target items, which were presented along with picture props in random order across subjects. For example, the SLP could give the following description of a scene and say: “This man loves running. What does he do every day?” The child was then expected to give answers like “He runs” or “He runs every day”. No practice trials were used during the experiment. However, if the participant failed to produce the target form, the SLP provided an additional prompt like “Does he run? Yes. Now you tell me that.” Up to 3 attempts to give the correct response were allowed for each form, after which the tester moved to the next item. The elicitation process went in accordance with a standardised protocol (see Appendix for the examples). Prior to the testing the SLPs were trained in both group and individual sessions, which consisted of an explanation of the test and observation of its administration. The SLPs were also observed while testing pilot children and were provided with hands-on feedback and demonstration. The frequency with which the tester completely failed to elicit the required form varied from child to child, depending on the severity of their problems with learning grammatical morphemes. However, for those children whose data were analysed in the present study, responses were available for at least 28 out of 30 items.
The analysis presented in this paper includes the full set of 30 stimuli for each morpheme, plus any additional spontaneous responses of non-target words elicited during the session that contained the target form. These were not numerous, however, and did not exceed 10 % of the data for each child. For example, a child might reply “He jogs and he runs”, in which case both jogs and runs were included in the list of analysed items, even though jogs was not a target form. If the child used the form multiple times, all the attempts were counted, regardless of whether the items contained the target morpheme or not. For example, from the sentence “Because he wanted to hop and hop, and he hopped, and he hop, and he hop” four items were used for the analysis of the past tense forms – two as correct productions (wanted, hopped), and other two as the cases of omission (the last two instances of ‘he hop’).
Other types of unsuccessful attempts, such as grammatically or semantically incomplete answers or ambiguous forms, direct imitations or delayed responses were not included in the analysis. For instance, in the following example the SLP attempted two times to elicit the possessive form of ‘cat’:
-
(1)
The SLP points at a picture with a cat standing near a ball.
SLP: This ball belongs to the cat. Whose ball is it? Child: The cat! SLP: Yes, but who does the ball belong to? This is the… Child: The cat’s ball.
The first use of the target word was excluded from the analysis as it was not clear from the recording whether the child was referring to the animal (as in There is the cat) or using the possessive without the grammatical marker (i.e., intended: cat’s, produced: cat). Therefore, only the second utterance was retained in the data set. Likewise, in the following example, the child’s form was both potentially grammatical and discourse appropriate, but did not contain the target grammatical morpheme, apparently, due to the truncated nature of the child’s utterance (i.e., no subject included).
-
(2)
The SLP points at a picture with a man running.
SLP: The man likes to run. What does he do every day? Child: Run.
Such forms were again ambiguous, and were therefore not included in the analyses reported here. In total, approximately 220 items (about 11 % of the initial data set) were excluded from the analysis. See the Appendix for other examples of correct versus incorrect/error responses.
Analysis
The data were transcribed from the audio recordings, and then coded, depending on whether the morpheme was produced, omitted, or appeared with an error, such as overgeneralisation or partial realisation. In cases where the presence of the morpheme was not clear to the transcriber (less than 8% of data), the token was re-examined by a second transcriber, and a final decision was made by consensus.
After transcription, the data were coded according to error type. Most often these were errors of omission, when a target word would be produced as a bare stem (e.g., He kick the ball every day). In a very few cases (less than 5% of the data) a child would make an error of overgeneralisation (e.g., She pickses flowers) or produce a partial realisation of the morpheme (e.g., She twist-t (meaning ‘twisted’) the stick). The analysed data included only those instances of full realisations and full omissions, where the child’s response was ungrammatical if the morpheme was missing.
A total of 2301 sentences were analysed: 730 for the past tense, 797 for the 3rd person singular -s, and 774 for the possessive morpheme -s. Coding the tokens according to their utterance position and syllabicity was applicable in all cases. Phonological complexity and voicing were only relevant for the segmental allomorphs.
Results
We applied a binary logistic regression model with five predictors to determine whether any could account for variability in morpheme production across the target markers. These predictors included the four main factors of interest (coda complexity, voicing, utterance position and syllabicity) and the additional variable participant, to account for possible individual differences in children’s results. Since our model included multiple parameters, the significance level was set at p=.01. The results of the Hosmer-Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test indicated that our data fit the model for all three morphemes (see table 2).
Table 2.
Morpheme | Factors | Hosmer-Lemeshow test | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Coda complexity | Voicing | Utterance position | Syllabicity | Participant | ||
Past -ed | *p<.001, OR=4.22 | *p<.001, OR=.34 | p=0.224, OR=0.78 | *p<.001, OR=.21 | *p<.001, df=10 | p=.078 |
Present -s | p=0.023, OR=1.96 | p=0.240, OR=0.76 | p=0.394, OR=1.2 | *p<.001, OR=.11 | *p<.001, df=9 | p=.82 |
Possessive -s | p=0.339, OR=0.76 | p=0.298, OR=0.80 | *p<.001, OR=.30 | *p<.001, OR=.02 | *p<.001, df=12 | p=.495 |
Note: The odds ratios (OR) compare the likelihoods of the correct productions with the binary terms introduced in the following order: a) Coda complexity: 1) simple codas, 2) complex codas; b) Voicing: 1) voiced, 2) voiceless; c) Utterance position: 1) final; 2) medial; d) Syllabicity: 1) syllabic allomorph, 2) segmental allomorph.
The analysis showed that two factors had a robust effect on morpheme production across all three suffixes: participant and syllabicity. Specifically, the syllabic allomorphs appeared to be significantly more challenging than the segmental ones. The boxplots in figure 1 illustrate these differences in production rates across all the morphemes. The empty circles stand for the average performance of each participant on a particular condition, illustrating high within group variability; the black circles mark the outliers.
Since the participant predictor had a significant effect on production, we concluded that, as was expected, participants showed high within-group variability in their performance. However, the model for each of the three morphemes accounted for the possible influence of this factor on other predictors, indicating their respective significance despite within-group differences.
Other predictors, i.e., coda complexity, voicing and utterance position, did not show a systematic effect across the target morphemes. Their significance for the individual suffixes is discussed in the respective subsections below.
Past tense morpheme
Analysis of the past tense morpheme production showed that, although utterance position did not have a significant effect on children’s performance, all three other factors (coda complexity, voicing and syllabicity) contributed to its variable production. This is illustrated in figure 2.
As anticipated, based on the findings for TD populations, consonant clusters were more problematic than simple codas, with the latter being correctly produced 4 times more often (OR=4.22). In addition, morphemes resulting in voiceless clusters proved to be less challenging, and were roughly 3 times more accurately produced than those which created voiced clusters (OR=.34). Utterance position did not show any significant effect on morpheme production. However, when adding syllabic allomorphs, the participants were approximately 5 times less accurate than when using segmental allomorphs (OR=.21).
Present tense morpheme
The results for 3rd person singular production showed that only syllabicity had a significant effect on the accuracy, with the syllabic allomorphs being about 10 times less likely to be added correctly (OR=.11). As illustrated in figure 3, coda complexity, voicing or utterance position did not show any significant effect on morpheme production.
Possessive morpheme
Apart from syllabicity, the only factor that significantly affected children’s performance was utterance position. This is illustrated in figure 4. As before, the syllabic allomorphs were much more challenging than the segmental ones. In this case, the difference between the two conditions was very large: syllabic suffixes were 50 times less likely to be produced correctly (OR=.02).
Interestingly, the possessive morpheme was added correctly more often when the target appeared utterance-medially (OR=.3), i.e. in the more phonologically challenging context. This was the opposite of what has been reported for TD children, who were more accurate when the morpheme appeared utterance finally (cf. Mealings & Demuth, 2014). However, this difference may be explained by the nature of the present data. As shown by table 1, our data contained a much lower overall proportion of the possessives appearing in medial position (only 15% of the total) than was observed for other morphemes (65% and 75% for the verbal suffixes). Thus, children’s responses to the possessive -s morpheme questions were more often single-noun utterances, and much less frequently full noun phrases – both acceptable in the context of a dialogue:
-
(3)
SLP: Whose book is this? Child: The man’s, or The man’s book.
However, this disproportion in numbers suggests that the problem might be syntactic in nature: leaving out the head of the noun phrase led to an increase in morpheme omission – since there was no need to underline the syntactic relationship between the words; similarly, preserving the head of the noun phrase could be bootstrapping morpheme production, i.e. serving as an additional reminder of the need to indicate the relationship between two nouns.
Discussion
The results of the study suggest that syllabicity has a robust effect on morpheme production in children with SLI. It accounted for much of the variability in children’s performance, not only for the verbal morphemes of tense and agreement (past tense, 3rd person singular), but also for the nominal marker (possessive). This suggests that, as with TD children, the process of morpheme acquisition in children with SLI has a phonological component that cannot be fully explained by incomplete morphosyntactic representations.
Possible explanations for the poorer performance on the syllabic allomorphs include frequency effects and difficulties in producing similar segments in succession. It has previously been shown that TD children’s early productions reflect the frequency with which different syllable and word structures appear in the input they hear (e.g., Roark & Demuth 2000; Levelt Schiller & Levelt 2000). Guided by this observation, we calculated the frequency of syllabic vs. segmental allomorphs in child-directed speech using the Providence Corpus (Demuth, Culbertson & Alter 2006), found on the CHILDES database (MacWhinney 2000). The data were drawn from the transcribed and morphologically coded utterances produced by six parents talking to their children between the ages of 2;10–3;1. This age-group was chosen so as to approximate the SLI children’s level of language development. The final set of items included 1407 utterances (467 items for past tense -ed; 698 – for 3rd person singular -s; and 242 – for the possessive -s), all of which contained the target morphemes in their regular forms. The items were sorted according to allomorph type, whose frequencies were calculated and proportions compared. A simple one-proportion test was used to estimate the significance of the differences in the proportions for the syllabic vs. segmental allomorphs for each morpheme. The results showed that, for all three morphemes, the proportion of segmental allomorphs was significantly greater than the syllabic ones (see table 3). This finding confirms the idea that the delayed acquisition of syllabic allomorphs can be caused by their less frequent occurrence in the speech children hear.
Table 3.
Morpheme | One-proportion test results and confidence intervals (CI) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Total tokens | Proportion of segmental allomorphs | 95% CI | p-value | |
Past -ed | 467 | 0.83 | (0.79, 0.86) | *p<.001 |
Present -s | 698 | 0.95 | (0.93, 0.96) | *p<.001 |
Possessive -s | 242 | 0.90 | (0.86, 0.94) | *p<.001 |
Alternatively, children may find it more difficult to produce same/similar segments in succession separated by only the short reduced vowel schwa (e.g., added [ædəd], brushes [brʌʃəz]). On the one hand, repetition of similarly sounding segments might incorrectly signal to the child that the morpheme is redundant – as if it were already added to the stem. This was proposed by Berko (1958). On the other hand, perhaps repeating similar word-final segments in an unstressed syllable presents challenges in terms of articulation (Mealings et al. 2013). This pattern can be likened with to the so-called “tongue-twister effect” when sentences containing a large proportion of the same/similar word-initial phonemes become increasingly challenging to produce and perceive (Keller, Carpenter & Just 2003).
In the Introduction we discussed studies that have previously shown the significance of both utterance position and coda complexity on morpheme production in children with SLI (Dalal & Loeb 2005; Marshall & van der Lely 2007; Polite 2011). The fact that we did not systematically find this effect in our set of data may be due to the diversity of methods used across studies or to the wide variety of language deficits observed in children with SLI. In other words, since these children form a heterogeneous population, they may have deficits in more than one language domain; therefore, it is possible that the participants in the above studies had additional problems with articulation or syntax.
Recall that in the papers cited above there was no mention of any additional articulatory tests having been conducted. In contrast, the current study presents results only from those children who successfully passed the articulation screening prior to testing. The importance of applying this assessment tool is supported by evidence from the intervention therapy: children who were able to produce the relevant segments in non-morphemic contexts significantly improved after the treatment program, whereas those who failed the articulation screening were not as much affected by the intervention (Smith-Lock et al. 2013a). This raises the methodological question of whether, when studying phonological constraints and morphophonological alternations, it would be advisable to use a similar articulation test prior to the experiment. This would ensure that children’s performance was not affected by articulation deficits, thus providing evidence from a more homogeneous population.
The robust effect of syllabicity demonstrated in the present study suggests that forms which require syllabic suffixes may present a particular challenge for children with SLI. Due to their longer duration, these suffixes carry greater phonetic content than their segmental counterparts, but this does not seem to improve the accuracy of these children’s productions. It is still possible, however, that the longer duration of the syllabic allomorphs could improve children’s performance in a perception or a grammaticality judgement task. These are areas for further research.
To summarise, our data suggest that children with SLI tend to acquire syllabic allomorphs later than segmental ones. This is likely due to the lower frequency of the syllabic forms or their greater articulatory complexity. Since TD children tend to master the syllabic forms later as well, this supports the idea of delay rather than deviance in these SLI children’s language development. The systematicity of this morphophonological effect across verbal and nominal morphemes also has important theoretical implications. In particular, it suggests that, counter to predictions of the Optional Infinitive hypothesis, morpheme omissions cannot be fully explained by children’s incomplete syntactic representations. Rather, morpheme omission is likely to be affected by a combination of factors, including phonological constraints and morphophonological processes. Specifically, finding the robust effect of allomorph type supports the idea proposed by Royle and Stine (2013) that morphophonology may be another problematic area that restricts these children’s grammatical abilities, and thus it should be included in the descriptive models of SLI. In addition, learning morphophonological regularities may be particularly problematic in languages other than English, where rich allomophic variation, as well as contractions, liaison and elisions occur. In such languages problems in acquiring various morphophonological patterns might even serve as a clinical marker of SLI. Further research is required to investigate the effects of morphophonological and phonological factors on SLI children’s use of grammatical morphemes crosslinguistically.
Our results have also practical implications. Since screening tests often include only a few tokens for each grammatical morpheme, it is important to ensure that the types of items tested are balanced in terms of phonological context, and the allomorphic forms they take. This applies to intervention programs as well: taking into account the allomorphy and practicing with a full range of morpheme realisations may significantly improve children’s overall morphological development.
The nature of the data presented in this study has its limitations. Firstly, the number of the original stimuli for each category was not always balanced. For example, there were fewer tokens ending in simple codas compared to those ending in a consonant cluster or in a syllabic allomorph (see table 1). Secondly, although all items should have been familiar everyday words known by the typical 5-year-old, the selected target words have different frequencies in the input children hear, which might have affected children’s performance. For example, the list of nouns used for testing possessive -s morpheme production contains a number of proper names, which naturally have much lower frequency than common nouns from the same list, e.g., cat, dog, horse. However, including word frequency as a factor was not possible as there was not enough data for building an adequate model. The phonetic contexts in which the items appeared in the children’s utterances were also not controlled, since they were chosen by the participants themselves. This again led to unequal number of events in each category. In addition, although our model accounted for the possible differences between the participants, the fact that the children were only tested on one or two target morphemes does not allow us to study the effects of phonological factors within every child. Finally, our data give evidence of children’s production skills, which may differ from their abilities to perceive the morphophonological contrasts. Follow-up studies involving both perception and production tasks are needed to control for lexical frequency and phonological structure, the number of words/syllables in the utterance and the target word’s position in the sentence. This will provide a more thorough understanding of the nature of the morphophonological deficits in SLI speech.
This study has shown that 5-year-old children with SLI exhibit significant challenges producing syllabic allomorphs. The low frequency of the syllabic forms in children’s input and speech may cause learning problems, persistently leading to their delayed acquisition across morphemes. The fact that these challenges are found for both verbal and nominal morphemes confirms that this may be due to phonological and frequency effects, and not limited to morphosyntactic problems with tense and agreement. Since allomorphy has such a robust effect on morpheme acquisition in children with SLI, it is advisable to use stimuli that are balanced in terms of the types of morpheme realisations they require during classroom assessments and intervention. Apparently, being particularly sensitive to frequency effects, children with SLI may need to have additional practice using less frequent morpheme realisations, such as syllabic allomorphs (e.g., She dresses as opposed to She runs for 3rd person singular -s), in order to successfully master grammatical morphemes. Our findings also suggest that children learning other languages – particularly those with highly complex morphophonology – may also exhibit persistent learning problems with low frequency allomorphs. Examining how morphophonological alternations are learned, using evidence from both perception and production tasks, will shed further light on the factors influencing language learning in children with SLI. This will in turn help inform more focussed assessment and intervention.
What this paper adds.
What is already known on this subject?
Delayed acquisition of grammatical morphology in children with SLI is often thought to be associated with morphosyntactic deficits. However, there is some evidence that phonological factors may also play a role in explaining their difficulties in learning grammar. This paper explores various phonological interactions across different types of grammatical morphemes in order to better understand the factors affecting grammatical development in children with SLI.
What this study adds?
This study examined verbal (past tense -ed; 3rd person singular -s) and nominal (possessive -s) morphemes, demonstrating that production of all three morphemes are systematically affected by syllabicity (e.g., allomorph type). Specifically, adding syllabic allomorphs (e.g., She dresses) appears to be significantly more challenging than adding segmental ones (e.g., He runs). The findings suggest that learning the patterns of morphoponological alternations may be particularly problematic for children with SLI, leading to additional difficulties in mastering grammar. Therefore, allomorphy and possibly other morphophonological effects require special attention from speech therapists during assessment and intervention.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by funding from Macquarie University, the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders (CE110001021), ARC DP110102479, and NIH R01 HD057606. We thank Farrah Alameddine for her important contribution to this project, research assistants Joanne Dixon, Caroline Dixon, Shannon Golding, Lilian Skarz, Rachel Reid, Catherine Wilkinson, and Sarah Arnott for data collection, and the staff and students of Fremantle and West Coast Language Development Centres for their participation in the project. We are also very grateful to the staff and students of the Child Language Lab at Macquarie University for their helpful assistance and feedback.
Appendix: Sample elicitations for the target morphemes
1. Past tense -ed
-
Sample Stimulus: [child is shown a picture of a girls jumping over a skipping rope]
SLP: ‘These girls love to skip. They did it yesterday. What did they do yesterday?’
Correct response: ‘They/the girls skipped (yesterday)’
Incorrect responses: ‘They skip’, ‘They are skipping’
-
Additional prompts (if a verb other than ‘skip’ was used):
SLP: ‘Did they skip?’
If the child says ‘yes’, SLP continues: ‘You tell me that. What did they do yesterday?’
If the child says ‘no’, SLP continues: ‘I think they did. You tell me that. What did they do yesterday?’
2. Present tense -s
-
Sample Stimulus: [child is shown a picture of a running man]
SLP: ‘This man likes to run, he does this every morning. What does he do every morning?’
Correct response: ‘He/the man runs (every morning)’
Incorrect responses: ‘He run’, ‘He is running’
-
Additional prompts (if a verb other than ‘run’ was used):
SLP: ‘Does he run?’
If the child says ‘yes’, SLP continues: ‘You tell me that. What does he do every morning?’
If the child says ‘no’, SLP continues: ‘I think he does. You tell me that. What does he do every morning?’
3. Possessive -s
-
Sample Stimulus: [child is shown a picture of a man reading a book]
SLP: ‘Look, this man has a book. Whose book is it?’
Correct response: ‘Man’s (book)’
Incorrect responses: ‘Man book’
-
Additional prompt (if a noun other than ‘man’ was used):
SLP: ‘This book belongs to the man. Whose book is it?’
Footnotes
Note that Australian English is non-rhotic, so words like “water” have a CVCV structure.
Declaration of Interest
None of the authors has any conflict of interest with respect to the study and results reported in the paper.
Contributor Information
Ekaterina Tomas, Email: ekaterina.tomas@mq.edu.au, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, North Ryde NSW 2190, Australia; telephone: +61 29850 2936.
Katherine Demuth, Email: katherine.demuth@mq.edu.au, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University, North Ryde NSW 2190, Australia; telephone: +61 29850 8783.
Karen M. Smith-Lock, Email: karen.smith-lock@mq.edu.au, ARC Centre of Excellence in Cognition and its Disorders, Department of Cognitive Science, Macquarie University, North Ryde NSW 2109, Australia; telephone: +61 29850 9599; fax: +61 29850 6067
Peter Petocz, Email: peter.petocz@mq.edu.au, Department of Statistics, Macquarie University, North Ryde NSW 2109, Australia; telephone: +61 29850 9174.
References
- 1.ARCHIBALD LMD, JOANISSE MF, MUNSON B. Motor Control and Nonword Repetition in Specific Working Memory Impairment and SLI. Topics in Language Disorders. 2013;33:255–267. doi: 10.1097/TLD.0b013e31829cf5e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.ACCARDO PJ, CAPUTE AJ. Cognitive Adaptive Test. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 3.BERKO J. The Child’s learning of English morphology. Word. 1958;14:150–177. [Google Scholar]
- 4.BISHOP D, NORBURY CF. Speech and language impairments. In: Thapar A, Rutter M, Bishop D, Pine D, Scott SM, Stevenson J, Taylor E, editors. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 2008. pp. 782–801. [Google Scholar]
- 5.BLOOM L. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- 6.BROWN R. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- 7.CLARK EVEV. First Language Acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- 8.CLEAVE PL, RICE ML. An examination of the morpheme BE in children with specific language impairment: The role of contractibility and grammatical form class. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:480–492. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4003.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.CONTI-RAMSDEN G, BOTTING N. Classification of children with specific language impairment: longitudinal considerations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:1195–204. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4205.1195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.DALAL R, LOEB D. Imitative production of regular past tense -ed by English-speaking children with specific language impairment. The International Journal of Communication Disorders. 2005;40:67–82. doi: 10.1080/13682820410001734163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.DALE PS, COLE KN. What’s normal? Specific language impairment in an individual differences perspective. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1991;22:80–83. [Google Scholar]
- 12.DEMUTH K, CULBERTSON J, ALTER J. Word-minimality, epenthesis, and coda licensing in the acquisition of English. Language & Speech. 2006;49:137–174. doi: 10.1177/00238309060490020201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.FRANKENBURG WK, DODDS J, ARCHER P, SHAPIRO H, BRESNICK B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.GRELA BG, LEONARD LB. The influence of argument-structure complexity on the use of auxiliary verbs by children with SLI. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2000;43:1115–1125. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4305.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.GRIFFITHS R. The Abilities of Young Children. High Wycombe: Cournswood House; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- 16.KEHOE M, STOEL-GAMMON C. Development of syllable structure in English-speaking children with particular reference to rhymes. Journal of Child Language. 2001;28:393–432. doi: 10.1017/s030500090100469x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.KELLER TA, CARPENTER PA, JUST MA. Brain imaging of tongue-twister sentence comprehension: Twisting the tongue and the brain. Brain and Language. 2003;84:189–203. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00506-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.LEONARD LB. Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 19.LEVELT CC, SCHILLER NO, LEVELT WJ. The acquisition of syllable types. Language Acquisition. 2000;8:237–264. [Google Scholar]
- 20.MacWHINNEY B. The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 21.MARCHMAN VA, WULFECK B, ELLIS WEISMER S. Morphological productivity in children with normal language and SLI: a study of the English past tense. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:206–219. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4201.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.MARSHALL CR, VAN DER LELY HKJ. The impact of phonological complexity on past tense inflection in children with Grammatical-SLI. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology. 2007;9:191–203. [Google Scholar]
- 23.MEALINGS KT, COX F, DEMUTH K. Acoustic investigations into the delayed acquisition of the syllabic -es plural in 2-year-olds’ speech. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2013;56:1260–1271. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0163). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.MEALINGS KT, DEMUTH K. Cluster reduction and compensatory lengthening in the acquisition of possessives. Journal of Child Language. 2014;41:690–714. doi: 10.1017/S0305000913000093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.NORBURY CF, BISHOP DVM, BRISCOE J. Production of English finite verb morphology: A comparison of SLI and mild-moderate hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2001;44:165–178. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.OETTING JB, HOROHOV J. Past tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:62–74. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4001.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.POLITE E. The contribution of part-word phonological factors to the production of regular noun plural -s by children with and without specific language impairment. First Language. 2011;31:425–441. [Google Scholar]
- 28.MARSHALL CR, HARCOURT-BROWN S, RAMUS F, VAN DER LELY HKJ. The link between prosody and language skills in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and/or dyslexia. International journal of language & communication disorders / Royal College of Speech & Language Therapists. 2009;44:466–488. doi: 10.1080/13682820802591643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.RENFREW C. Action Picture Test. Camberwell, VIC: ACER; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 30.RICE ML, WEXLER K, CLEAVE PL. Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1995;38:850–863. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3804.850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.ROARK B, DEMUTH K. Prosodic constraints and the learner’s environment: A corpus study. Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. 2000;24:597–608. [Google Scholar]
- 32.ROYLE P, STINE I. The French noun phrase in preschool children with SLI: morphosyntactic and error analyses. Journal of Child Language. 2013;40(5):945–970. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.SMIT AB. Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska articulation norms project: Consonant singletons. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993;36:533–547. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3603.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.SMITH-LOCK KM, LEITAO S, LAMBERT L, NICKELS L. Effective intervention for expressive grammar in children with Specific Language Impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2013a;48:265–282. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.SMITH-LOCK KM, LEITAO S, LAMBERT L, PRIOR P, DUNN A, CRONJE J, NEWHOUSE S, NICKELS L. Daily or weekly? The role of treatment frequency in the effectiveness of grammar treatment for children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2013b;15:255–267. doi: 10.3109/17549507.2013.777851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.SONG JY, SUNDARA M, DEMUTH K. Phonological constraints on children’s production of English 3rd person singular s. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2009;52:623–642. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0258). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.THEODORE R, DEMUTH K, SHATTUCK-HUFNAGEL S. Acoustic evidence for position and complexity effects on children’s production of plural -s. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2011;54:539–548. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0035). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.WECHSLER D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of IntelligenceTM — Third Edition (WPPSITM – III) Sydney, NSW: Pearson; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 39.WEXLER K. Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations. In: Lightfoot D, Hornstein N, editors. Verb Movement. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, U.K: 1994. pp. 305–350. [Google Scholar]
- 40.WIIG E, SECORD W, SEMEL E. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals Preschool – Second Edition, Australian and New Zealand Standardised Edition (CELF P-2 Australian and New Zealand) Sydney, NSW: Pearson; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 41.ZIMMERMAN I, STEINER V, POND R. Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition (PLS-4) Australian Language Adapted. Sydney, NSW: Pearson; 2002. [Google Scholar]