Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jul 10.
Published in final edited form as: J Res Child Educ. 2015 Apr 1;29(2):212–225. doi: 10.1080/02568543.2015.1008659

“No-o-o-o Peeking”: Preschoolers’ Executive Control, Social Competence, and Classroom Adjustment

Susanne A Denham 1, Hideko H Bassett 1, Yana S Sirotkin 1, Chavaughn Brown 1, Carol S Morris 1
PMCID: PMC4498669  NIHMSID: NIHMS669121  PMID: 26166925

Abstract

The goals of this study were to evaluate (1) how specific aspects of executive control, briefly assessed, predict social competence and classroom adjustment during preschool; and (2) differences between two aspects of executive control, according to child’s age, socioeconomic risk status, and gender. The facets of executive control were defined as cool executive control (CEC; affectively neutral, slow acting, and late developing) and hot executive control (HEC; more emotional, fast acting, and early developing). Two hundred eighty-seven 3- to 5-year-old children from private child care and Head Start centers were directly assessed during executive control tasks, and preschool teachers provided information on their school success. Aspects of executive control varied with age, socioeconomic risk, and gender. Specifically, older children performed better on CEC tasks across three age levels; for HEC tasks, change was seen only between 3-year-olds and 4-year-olds. Children of mothers with less formal education performed less well on CEC than those whose mothers had more education; girls performed better than boys on HEC tasks. Further, facets of executive control were differentially related to later social competence and classroom adjustment. HEC predicted social competence, whereas CEC uniquely predicted classroom adjustment. Implications for everyday practice and specific curricula formulation are discussed.

Keywords: executive control, social competence, classroom adjustment


Young children’s school readiness is defined as mastery of certain basic skills or abilities, including literacy, numeracy, and social skills, which help ensure success in school (Hair, Halle, Terry-Humen, Lavelle, & Calkins, 2006). According to kindergarten teachers, executive control is a crucially needed readiness skill (Rimm-Kaufmann, Pianta, & Cox, 2000). Specifically, a growing body of work emphasizes the impact of children’s executive control on both social adjustment and academic success, including social competence (Bierman, Nix, Greenberg, Blair, & Domitrovich, 2008; Blair, Granger, & Razza, 2005; Raver et al., 2011), as well as classroom adjustment (e.g., early learning behaviors and attitudes toward learning), mathematics ability, and literacy (Bierman et al., 2008; Blair & Razza, 2007; Brock, Rimm-Kaufman, Nathanson, & Grimm, 2009; Ponitz, McClelland, Matthews & Morrison, 2009; Pritchard & Woodward, 2011; Sesma, Mahone, Levine, Eason, & Cutting, 2009; Welsh, Nix, Blair, Bierman, & Nelson, 2010).

In this study, using a short direct assessment tool, we focus upon the contributions of two aspects of executive control to preschoolers’ social competence and classroom adjustment. These outcomes highlight the child’s success in the new preschool environment with teachers and peers alike, accompanied by crucial learning-related attitudes and behaviors that allow them to become immersed in the many new tasks put before them. Hence, social competence can be defined as skills associated with successful interactions with peers and teachers, such as cooperating, taking into account others’ feelings, refraining from either aggression directed at, or withdrawal from, one’s peers. Classroom adjustment can be defined as young children’s behaviors and attitudes associated with learning in the classroom environment, such as positive attitudes about school, the ability to participate cooperatively in classroom activities, and the ability to engage in self-directed activities (Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999).

These two sets of abilities go hand-in-hand—without either, experiences in the early years of schooling are apt to be less positive. The classroom is a very social place, and social competence undergirds classroom adjustment. A socially competent child may be able to pay more attention to academic tasks, plan better, and devote more resources to learning than a less socially competent one, because they can benefit more from teachers’ instructions, giving and getting academic information from peers, sharing academic resources with peers, and modeling peers’ learning skills (Ladd, Buhs, & Seid, 2000). Moreover, children who demonstrate classroom adjustment are more accepted by classmates and teachers, and are given more instruction and positive feedback by teachers (Birch & Ladd, 1997; Ladd et al., 1999; Ladd, Kochenderfer, & Coleman, 1997). Thus, we view both social competence and classroom adjustment as crucial, and related, outcomes for a successful introduction to schooling.

Definition and Structure of Executive Control

Cognitive and affective/motivational processes that modulate attention, emotion, and behavior to a given situation/stimulus, for the purpose of pursuing a goal and supported by prefrontal and other cortical involvement, are implicated in what is termed executive control (as opposed to the broader and often ill- or multiply-defined construct, self-regulation) (EC; Calkins & Howse, 2004; Posner & Rothbart, 2007). Such modulation of attention, emotion, and behavior should contribute to successful social competence and classroom adjustment.

Recent advances in both developmental psychobiological theorizing and research, as well as neuroimaging, suggest that two types of EC are distinguishable, both neurally and behaviorally, and that such distinctions can be important both theoretically and practically (Willoughby, Kupersmidt, Voegler-Lee, & Bryant, 2011; Zelazo & Müller, 2002). Thus, EC may be seen as including cool executive control (CEC; more affectively neutral, slow acting, and late developing) and hot executive control (HEC; more reflexive, fast acting, early developing, and under stimulus control; Willoughby et al., 2011). Although because of its responsibility for higher-order cognitive processes, such as working memory, focusing and shifting attention, and inhibiting prepotent behaviors while activating alternative, subdominant responses, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) plays a central role in both CEC and HEC, there are distinguishing neural structures involving each type of EC (Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, 2010; Garon, Bryson, & Smith, 2008).

CEC, in which the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DL-PFC) plays a special role (Best & Miller, 2010; Garon et al., 2008), encompasses a wide array of increasingly organized, flexible, goal-directed cognitive processes in response to relatively non-affective and novel situations, as well as complex cognitive tasks (Diamond, 2006). School tasks mandate that children use CEC abilities to purposefully shift or focus their attention, and thus flexibly respond to conflicting stimuli (Blair et al., 2005). Hence, CEC skills have been found to predict early literacy skills (McClelland et al., 2007; Willoughby et al., 2011), and mediate the intervention effects of research-based curricula (Bierman et al., 2008). Preschoolers with good CEC may be better adjusted in the classroom; for example, they can pay attention to and remember teachers’ instructions and directions, and also restrain themselves when necessary (e.g., sitting still, waiting in line when they would like to move around). In terms of social competence, they may be able to shift attention to others’ feelings, and suppress prepotent responses in order to be more cooperative.

Young children also need to demonstrate EC that requires more affective and motivational processes (e.g., not touching a toy that belongs to someone else)—in other words, HEC. Guided by both emotional information from the limbic system and orbitofrontal cortical braking, as well as the PFC (Calkins & Marcovitch, 2010; Lewis & Todd, 2007; Willoughby et al., 2011), HEC enables children to, for example, regulate their anger and approach systems and purposefully deploy attention during emotional arousal (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Preschoolers better able to control their impulses and balance their own self-defined needs with societal norms are ready to be motivated and engaged in schooling (Blair & Razza, 2007; Rothbart & Jones, 1998); for example, in terms of social competence, they can refrain from hitting when they are provoked, or, in terms of classroom adjustment, delay the gratification of biting into their snack until everyone is served. It is important to consider both subtypes of EC because of their somewhat varying source and functions, but much overlap would be expected between them.

EC and Child Characteristics

Moving from definitional issues and relations with social competence and classroom adjustment, it is important to consider individual differences due to age, socioeconomic risk, or gender. Researchers have found age-related differences on EC tasks (e.g., Carlson, 2005; Carlson & Moses, 2001; Diamond, Carlson, & Beck, 2005; Jones, Rothbart, & Posner, 2003; Li-Grining, 2007), probably because so many of the processes involved (e.g., inhibitory control, working memory, and attention) show dramatic development between ages of 3 to 5 years (Jones et al., 2003; Welsh et al., 2010). Knowledge of age differences in EC can have not only important measurement and developmental considerations (e.g., what tasks are appropriate for ages across the preschool range?), but also important applications, such as appropriate expectations for preschoolers’ EC.

In terms of SES-related risk, children growing up in poverty may experience stressful life events, low levels of social support, and limited opportunities for scaffolded exploration of their social and physical environments, resulting in potential developmental delays in EC (Bierman et al., 2008). Dilworth-Bart (2012) has further suggested that home disorganization, chaos, housing disruption, noise (i.e., major stressors), and lack of environmental/familial resources from which to benefit, are attributes of lower SES that make development of EC difficult for children. Ponitz et al. (2009) found that children from higher-SES families achieved higher self-regulation scores than children from lower-SES families. Pinpointing differences in the development of age-appropriate EC in children varying in SES can translate into foci for preschool programming; in other words, children living in poverty can benefit from a focus on EC (Raver et al., 2011).

Regarding gender, differences between girls and boys on EC could shed light on the achievement gap between the genders (Matthews, Ponitz, & Morrison, 2009). Gender differences are not always found; in fact, Matthews et al. note that researchers have not often focused on such potential differences. However, when such differences have been tested, results favoring girls have been seen on both CEC tasks (Carlson & Moses, 2001) and HEC tasks (Clark, Sheffield, Wiebe, & Espy, 2013; Eisenberg et al., 2010; Li-Grining, 2007). Ponitz et al. (2009) also found that girls achieved slightly higher behavioral self-regulation scores than boys. These gender differences may emerge on several different measures of EC, as early as 2 ½ years of age (Eisenberg et al., 2010), with this gap remaining wide until the spring of kindergarten (Matthews et al., 2009). Importantly, Matthews et al. also found that this gender difference was particularly true for boys whose initial performance was the lowest. Discerning gender differences could help early childhood educators in targeting programming to those most in need, who are often boys. Finally, as already noted, preschoolers’ EC is increasingly being found to relate to various indices of school readiness, including social competence and classroom adjustment.

The Current Study

The Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA; Smith-Donald, Raver, Hayes, & Richardson, 2007) is a direct assessment of aspects of EC. This measure has been shortened based on recent research; in Authors (2012b), six PSRA tasks were examined for ceiling effects at 35–65 months. In that research, with four remaining tasks showing no ceiling effects, confirmatory factor analyses showed two components at each of two longitudinal time points—CEC and HEC. Four-year-olds, children of higher socioeconomic status, and girls outperformed 3-year-olds, those at socioeconomic risk, and boys. Children, especially girls, scored higher on HEC. Finally, aspects of EC differentially predicted teacher-reports of school readiness at both times of assessment, with age, socioeconomic risk status, and gender controlled.

The current study’s general aims are to corroborate and extend this previous work with a new cohort of preschoolers, and to further refine the PSRA for applied usage by shortening it even further. Two of the PSRA tasks that showed no ceiling effects, one for CEC and one for HEC, were selected and administered across a broader age range than in the previous work, to: (1) evaluate differences in CEC and HEC according to child characteristics (age, socioeconomic risk status, and gender); and (2) describe associations of PSRA components with teachers’ reports on children’s social competence and classroom adjustment, after holding child characteristics constant. Based on the nature of CEC and HEC, and on earlier work, we expected that CEC would be more highly associated with classroom adjustment than HEC, with the converse true for social competence. We expected that older children, those in higher SES families, and girls would show higher scores on indices of EC. Results commensurate with earlier work but with a shortened measure could have important applications; for example, early childhood educators could make use of these tasks in evaluating young children’s EC for ongoing assessment and programming decisions.

Method

Participants

The present study was part of a larger longitudinal study focused on creating a portable assessment battery of preschoolers’ social-emotional competence related to school readiness (Authors, 2005). Participants were preschoolers enrolled in Head Start and private child care centers, who varied in terms of race, ethnicity, and income, in the greater Northern Virginia area. The 287 participating children (183 in private child care, 104 attending Head Start) included, at the study’s inception, 64 3-year-olds: M = 42.3 mos, SD = 3.2; 138 4-year-olds: M = 54.0 mos, SD = 3.4; and 85 5-year-olds: M = 62.6 mos, SD = 1.8). Approximately 50% of the participants were male, with a majority of children identified by their parent as either Caucasian or African American (of the 92% of parents reporting, 58% self-reported as Caucasian, 32.2% as African-American, 7.6% as multi-racial, 1.1% as Asian, and 1.2% as other). Fifteen percent of parents self-reported as Hispanic/Latino. Approximately half of the mothers had attained high school graduation or less. The ordinal value for level of maternal education was used as a proxy for socioeconomic risk status in regression analyses to follow (Desai & Alva, 1998); for the group comparisons, a dichotomous variable for mothers’ education was created in which low equaled high school diploma or less, and high equaled associates’ degree or higher).

Procedures

Our research design was a short-term longitudinal, correlational study. Parents reported on demographics in the late fall to early winter. Assessments of EC were conducted from late winter to spring, in quiet areas of centers. Teacher measures were collected at the end of the academic year. For each participating child in their classroom, these teachers were paid $20 in compensation for their time in the completion of the questionnaires. Children received stickers after completion of their task.

Measures

EC: PSRA (Smith-Donald et al., 2007)

Two PSRA tasks (Pencil Tap and Toy Peek) were used to capture strengths and weaknesses in preschoolers’ EC. Authors (2012a, 2012b) showed concurrent and predictive validity for four to seven of the original PSRA tasks; to further this inquiry and in keeping with study goals, two tasks from Authors (2012b) were chosen due to lack of ceiling effects and high standardized factor loadings in confirmatory factor analyses. The Pencil Tap task was included as a measure of CEC, and the Toy Peek task was included as a measure of HEC. The tasks were administered by trained and certified research assistants who live-coded performance levels or latencies, as appropriate, for each task. For the Pencil Tap task, the child was asked to tap an unsharpened pencil, once after the assessor tapped twice, and twice after the assessor tapped once; scores equaled the percentage of correct trials over a total of 16 trials. For the Toy Peek task, the child was asked not to peek for one minute while the assessor wrapped a toy in tissue paper and a gift bag; scores equaled latency in seconds to the first peek up to the maximum of 60 seconds.

Inter-assessor reliability via intra-class correlations equaled .95 for Pencil Tap, and .79 for Toy Peek, ps < .001. As for internal consistency of scales for each task, Cronbach’s alpha across 16 trials for Pencil Tap was .79; for Toy Peek, across scores for presence of peeking, number of peeks, and time to peeking, Cronbach’s alpha equaled .96. Scores used in analyses were the percentage of correct pencil taps across trials, and latency to peeking (up to 60 sec allotted time).

Social Competence: Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation (SCBE-30; LaFreniere & Dumas, 1996)

Social competence can be defined as skills associated with successful interactions with peers and teachers—cooperating, taking into account others’ feelings, refraining from either aggression directed at, or withdrawal from, one’s peers. The SCBE-30 taps these dimensions of children’s behavior; teachers rated children’s social competence and behavior on 30 items using a 1 – 5 point scale. Ten-item scales included Angry/Aggressive (e.g., “easily frustrated”), Sensitive/Cooperative (e.g., “comforts or assists children in difficulty”), and Anxious/Withdrawn (e.g., “avoids new situations”). Internal consistency was good for the scales in our data (α = .91, .82, and .81, respectively). Validity has been previously shown with normative, clinical, and cross-cultural samples (Denham et al., 2003; LaFreniere & Dumas, 1996). For this study, scales were correlated (mean r (281) = .39, p < .001); an aggregate of z-scores for each scale (with Angry/Aggressive and Anxious/Withdrawn reversed) was created, α = .66.

Classroom Adjustment: Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale (PLBS; McDermott, Leigh, & Perry, 2002)

Classroom adjustment can be defined as young children’s behaviors and attitudes associated with learning in the classroom environment, such as positive attitudes about school, the ability to participate cooperatively in classroom activities, and the ability to engage in self-directed activities (Ladd et al., 1999). The PLBS is a 29-item measure that taps aspects of classroom adjustment; teachers rate children’s approaches to learning (Fantuzzo, Perry, & McDermott, 2004; McDermott et al., 2002). Internal consistency reliability for the three subscales was good in these data: Competence Motivation (11 items, e.g., reluctant to tackle a new activity; α = .87), Attention/Persistence (nine items, e.g., tries hard, but concentration soon fades and performance deteriorates; α = .88), and Attitudes Toward Learning (seven items, e.g., does not achieve anything constructive when in a sulky mood; α = .78). Multi-method, multi-source analyses have validated the PLBS for use with this study’s population (Fantuzzo et al., 2004).

Classroom Adjustment: Teacher Rating Scale of School Adjustment (TRSSA; Ladd et al., 1997)

Classroom adjustment was assessed using the TRSSA. The TRSSA includes 52-items rated on a 3-point scale, across four scales: School Liking, Cooperative Participation, Self-Directiveness, and Comfort with Teacher. Adequate reliability was found in this study for School Liking (five items, e.g., “likes going to school”; α = .75), Cooperative Participation (eight items, e.g., “follows teacher’s directions”; α = .91), Self-Directiveness (nine items, e.g., “works independently”; α = .87), and Comfort with Teacher (five items, e.g., “initiates conversations with the teacher”; α = .70). The TRSSA has been found to be valid across social economic status and ethnicity (Ladd et al., 1997). For this study’s classroom adjustment score, a summed aggregate of z-scores for each PLBS and TRSSA scale was created, α = .88; average r (279) = .52, p < .001.

Results

The first goal of this study was to examine differences in both CEC and HEC due to age (measured cross-sectionally), socioeconomic risk status, and gender. To meet this goal, a 3 (Age: 3-year-old, 4-year-old, 5-year-old) × 2 (SES: low maternal education versus high maternal education) × 2 (Gender: boy, girl) MANOVA was run using CEC and HEC as dependent variables. Results of this analysis can be seen in Table 1. Older children showed more EC; follow-up one-way ANOVAs showed that age differences were significant for both CEC and HEC, Fs (2, 273) = 48.26 and 20.638, respectively, ps < .001. Bonferroni multiple comparisons for age differences in CEC showed a linear progression, with groups scoring higher as age increased. Similar comparisons for age differences in HEC showed only differences between 3-year-olds and both other age groups. SES differences favoring children less at risk were found only for CEC, F (1, 273) = 18.90, ps < .001, and gender differences favoring girls were significant only for HEC, F (1, 273) = 7.63.

Table 1.

Age, Gender, and Maternal Education Comparisons for HEC and CEC

Gender Age Maternal Education

F (2, 272) = 4.08* F (4, 544) = 22.70*** F (2, 272) = 10.24***
Partial η2 = .029 Partial η2 = .143 Partial η2 = .070

Boys Girls 3-yr 4-yr 5-yr High School or Less Associate Degree or More
CEC 52.41
(2.57)
57.83
(2.64)
28.14
(3.73)
61.32
(2.49)
75.90
(3.23)
47.12
(2.69)
63.13
(2.51)
HEC 38.64
(1.83)
45.98
(1.88)
29.50
(2.66)
46.08
(1.77)
51.36
(2.30)
42.08
(1.92)
42.54
(1.79)

Notes: Fs evaluated by Pillai’s Trace. Standard errors in parentheses.

*

p≤ .05,

**

p≤ .01,

***

p≤ .001.

The second goal of the study was to assess CEC’s and HEC’s contributions to variance in later indicators of teacher-rated social competence and classroom adjustment. Regression analyses were used to address this goal, with age, maternal education, and gender entered in the first block of each equation, and CEC and HEC entered in the second block. As can be seen in Table 2, after controlling for significant contributions of age, maternal education, and gender, HEC contributed to a significant increment in variance explained for social competence. For classroom adjustment, after controlling for significant contributions of age and gender, CEC marginally contributed, and HEC significantly contributed, to increments in variance explained.

Table 2.

Prediction of Social Competence and Classroom Adjustment, Given Age, Maternal Education, and Gender

Social Competence Classroom Adjustment
B SE B β ΔR2 B SE B β ΔR2
Block 1 .162*** .122***
Age .021 .005 .217*** .012 .004 .173**
Maternal Education −.095 .033 −.157** −.033 .025 −.076
Gender .420 . 082 .280*** .312 .061 .285***
Block 2 .019* .036**
CEC .000 .002 −.019 .002 .001 .118+
HEC .005 .002 .156* .004 .002 .149**
+

p < .10.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p <.001.

To consider whether EC explained a significant increment of variance in classroom adjustment over and above social competence, a third regression equation was calculated, with social competence entered in the first block along with the child characteristic variables. Given the significant contribution of social competence to classroom adjustment (r (285) = .62, β = .576, p < .001), the β for HEC became nonsignificant, and that for CEC became significant, β = .129, p < .05.

Discussion

The study’s first goal was to evaluate differences in CEC and HEC according to child characteristics (i.e., age, socioeconomic risk status, and gender), for 3- to 5-year-old children in Head Start and private child care centers. Next, contributions that both CEC and HEC made to social competence and classroom adjustment were examined, after holding child characteristics constant. Findings corroborated and extended our earlier work (Authors, 2012b) with a new cohort of preschoolers, and a much-shortened PSRA (two subtests instead of the original six).

Child Characteristics and EC

Age

Age differences in both CEC and HEC were found, as would be expected given earlier findings, including our own (Authors, 2012b). In that earlier research, however, only 3- and 4-year-olds were included, and age differences were found for both CEC and HEC. For this cohort, which included 5-year-olds, the age change was continuous between each age level for CEC; in contrast, change was seen only between 3-year-olds and 4-year-olds for HEC.

These differing results suggest that motivational/emotional aspects of EC, which predominate in HEC, may mature before the more exclusively frontally involved CEC abilities. Although others have also shown age change across this span for CEC-type tasks (Carlson, 2005; Jones et al., 2003; Welsh et al., 2010), checking others’ findings on the trajectory of HEC age change is difficult, because studies less often include this aspect of EC. When HEC tasks are included, often only 3- and 4-year-olds are tested, or it is difficult to tease apart relative CEC/HEC age differences on a multiplicity of tasks (e.g., Espy, Kaufmann, Glisky, & McDiarmid, 2001). However, current results do stand in contrast to those of Carlson (2005), who found increases in HEC, using the same task, through age 5. Future research targeting the difference found here could be useful, especially because our age data are cross-sectional. Given that many child care and preschool programs still separate children into age-graded groups, knowing what to expect of these groups is necessary in applied settings. For example, it would be useful to know that 3-year-olds would probably not be as successful at waiting during transitions as their older counterparts; teachers could modify practice accordingly, employing supportive methods to assist 3-year-olds to respond to such demands.

Socioeconomic Risk

Children of mothers with less formal education performed less well on the CEC task in this study. Similar correlational results were found by Blair et al. (2011) and others previously reviewed (e.g., Bierman et al., 2008; Dilworth-Bart, 2012). As part of a latent variable for risk, maternal education marginally predicted CEC even when cortisol, parenting, and IQ were included in the structural model (Blair et al., 2011). It is clear that children at risk for living in poverty are likely to need support in developing CEC; living in chaos and stress is likely to limit aspects of brain development that underlies the emergence of EC. This need should be acknowledged in practice and policy; for example, programming like that reported on by Raver et al. (2011), including use of clear rules, rewards for positive behavior, and redirection of negative behavior, should be implemented to support the development of EC among children from low-income households.

Gender

Earlier research (e.g., Carlson & Moses, 2001; Clark et al., 2013; Eisenberg et al., 2010; Li-Grining, 2007) has found gender differences on both CEC and HEC, but a number of other reports have not (e.g., some, such as Wiebe, Espy, & Charak, 2008, have examined only CEC), and differences only on HEC were found here. These findings bear replication, but it could be that girls are socialized such that they have more practice, via socializers’ expectations, in the motivational-emotional aspects of HEC (e.g., adults may communicate expectations to girls that they can and should be able to wait and delay gratification; Raffaelli, Crockett, & Shen, 2005). Furthermore, these differences could also be impacted by differential maturation and temperamental reactivity. Regardless of the etiology, it appears that boys could benefit from experiences that allow them to develop these skills. For example, increases in HEC might result in lessened aggression and negativity, which are typically seen more often in boys (Raffaelli et al., 2005).

Predicting Social Competence and Classroom Adjustment

The next goal of the study was to see whether CEC and HEC predicted preschoolers’ social competence and classroom adjustment. HEC predicted social competence, which may not be surprising given the myriad ways in which being able to delay gratification could bolster one’s abilities to get along with others—cooperating, taking into account others’ feelings, and refraining from either aggression directed at, or withdrawal from, one’s peers. Waiting for a turn and choosing a nonaggressive solution to a problem, for example, require such skill.

As already noted, classroom adjustment works together with social competence. Analyses parsing the influence of EC on social competence and classroom adjustment were attempted here. We found that although classroom adjustment was predicted by both HEC and CEC (marginally) in initial analyses, subsequent analyses that included social competence in the prediction of classroom adjustment showed that CEC, rather than HEC, was the unique predictor of “social competence-free” classroom adjustment. This study joins many others in demonstrating that CEC predicts aspects of school success; in contrast with earlier work (e.g., Ponitz et al., 2009), however, criterion measures here were softer, but equally important, indicators of school success.

Conclusions and Implications for Early Childhood Education

In summary, this study uncovered important findings on aspects of preschoolers’ EC using a much-shortened version of the PSRA, which could be potentially useful for early childhood educators as part of an assessment battery measuring aspects of preschoolers’ social-emotional school readiness. Aspects of EC varied with age, socioeconomic risk, and gender. Moreover, they were differentially related to later social competence and classroom adjustment.

Thus, the current emphasis on EC as a precursor to such success is warranted in research, policy, and practice, and should be informed by the important within- and between-child characteristics emphasized here. These conclusions must be fleshed out in a number of ways.

First, if EC (i.e., cognitive flexibility in divided and sustained attention, working memory, inhibition of behaviors when required, and emotion regulation) is such an important contributor to early school success, effective general frameworks for classroom practice are required. Regarding such frameworks for practice, clear expectations and limit-setting, paired with allowance of autonomy and supportiveness from adults, promote the emergence of EC. A classroom where there is warm support and a predictable order to the day provides a sturdy foundation for the development of EC (Bierman et al., 2008; Diamond & Lee, 2011). Children are more likely to advance in environments where they are aided by caring adults in both following rules and doing things for themselves.

The scaffolding involved in such developmentally appropriate practices supports children’s use of their focused attention and careful behavioral responding. For example, imagine a classroom where children are shown how to put on their own coats, given opportunities to try, fail, keep trying, and work out the correct techniques to get dressed appropriately for outdoor winter play. These children are utilizing focused attention, controlling competing responses and emotional meltdowns, and gaining EC.

Further, encouraging children to verbally label the means of EC appears helpful (Kray & Ferdinand, 2013). For example, repeating a rule to a game using self-talk can help a child focus on and remember it. Teachers’ verbal redirection techniques (e.g., “talk like a butterfly” when quiet voices are desired) are also useful, as are physical props like holding a ball when it is your turn to speak (i.e., when you do not have the ball, it is not your turn).

Next, practicing tasks requiring EC, with progressive increases in difficulty, is important (Diamond & Lee, 2011; Kray & Ferdinand, 2013). For example, children can take turns holding a real egg when enacting the story of Humpty Dumpty, play games like “Simon Says,” or solve increasingly difficult puzzles. Repeating activities that require one or more elements of EC allow children to move through successive approximations of more and more mature levels of EC.

Second, intervention programming can allow classroom teachers to promote EC in a more intentional, organized fashion. Diamond and Lee (2011) discussed attributes of successful interventions to promote EC, and noted that diverse activities have proven useful. Working with computer games has shown significant but somewhat circumscribed effects so far (Thorell, Lindqvist, Bergman Nutley, Bohlin, & Klingberg, 2009). In terms of noncomputerized games, Röthlisberger, Neuenschwander, Cimeli, Michel, and Roebers (2012) demonstrated gains in elements of preschoolers’ and kindergartners’ EC via the use of a game-centered curriculum involving mostly small groups of children, playing games such as assembling puzzles using shapes to match a picture, scanning pictures for missing objects, and dimensional card sorting. In addition, exercise programs, especially cognitively engaging ones (i.e., those that require children to create, monitor, and modify a cognitive plan to meet task demands) also show promise (Best, 2010). Even introducing early childhood versions of yoga and mindfulness are theoretically appealing (Zelazo & Lyons, 2012). However, Diamond and Lee (2011) point out that it may be as useful or even more useful, in tandem with helpful classroom practices and use of verbal and other scaffolding, to focus less narrowly on activities of EC than on broader emotional and social development (as did Bierman et al., 2008), or on physical development.

Third, it should be reiterated that the current results enjoin early childhood teachers to keep several child characteristics in mind when considering their support of ongoing executive control development. First, expectations should be tailored to the child’s age; for both CEC and HEC, younger children are less capable, as are boys and children from lower SES households. These children, in particular, and any who are less capable at CEC and HEC, deserve particular attention. Children with worse executive functions may benefit most from the practices and activities listed here (Matthews et al., 2009); thus, early training may help to avert later difficulties.

Fourth and finally, along with the suggestions already put forward, teachers could use the simple PSRA tasks utilized here in ongoing assessment of children’s performance to understand the children’s current functioning and forecast its impact on broader classroom adjustment. In summary, although continued effectiveness and efficacy research needs to be performed, EC deserves continued attention in classrooms, in terms of everyday practice, general and specific curricula, individual child characteristics, and assessment.

Acknowledgments

The present study was funded by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development grant #R01HD51514. We are grateful to the many children, families, and teachers who participated in this study, and the directors of the facilities who worked so cooperatively with us. We also thank Alyssa Perna, Sara Kalb Thayer, and Erin Way for their unstinting assistance in study organization and data collection.

References

  1. Authors. Social-emotional assessment of child's school readiness. 2005 Grant 5 R01 HD051514, National Institutes for Child Health and Human Development. [Google Scholar]
  2. Authors. Factor structure of self-regulation in preschoolers: Testing models of a field-based assessment for predicting early school readiness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012a;111:386–404. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Authors. Refining the Preschool Self-Regulation Assessment (PSRA) for use in preschool classrooms. Infant and Child Development. 2012b;21:596–616. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernier A, Carlson SM, Whipple N. From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development. 2010;81:326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: Contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Review. 2010;30:331–351. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development. 2010;81:1641–1660. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bierman K, Nix R, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Domitrovich C. Executive function and school readiness intervention: Impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:821–843. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Birch SH, Ladd GW. The teacher-child relationship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology. 1997;35:61–79. [Google Scholar]
  9. Blair C, Granger D, Razza RP. Cortisol reactivity is positively related to executive function in preschool children attending Head Start. Child Development. 2005;76:554–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00863.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blair C, Granger DA, Willoughby M, Mills-Koonce R, Cox M, Greenberg MT The FLP Investigators. Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development. 2011;82:1970–1984. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blair C, Razza RP. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development. 2007;78:647–663. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brock LL, Rimm-Kaufman SE, Nathanson L, Grimm KJ. The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2009;24:337–349. [Google Scholar]
  13. Calkins SD, Howse RB. Individual differences in self-regulation: Implications for childhood adjustment. In: Philippot P, Feldman RS, editors. The regulation of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004. pp. 307–332. [Google Scholar]
  14. Calkins SD, Marcovitch S. Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: Integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning. In: Calkins SD, Bell MA, editors. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 37–57. [Google Scholar]
  15. Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:595–616. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development. 2001;72:1032–1053. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark CAC, Sheffield TD, Wiebe SA, Espy KA. Longitudinal associations between executive control and developing mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development. 2013;84(2):662–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01854.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Denham SA, Blair KA, DeMulder E, Levitas J, Sawyer K, Auerbach-Major S, Queenan P. Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence. Child Development. 2003;74:238–256. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Desai S, Alva S. Maternal education and child health? Is there a strong causal relationship? Demography. 1998;35:75–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Diamond A. The early development of executive functions. In: Bailystok E, Craik F, editors. Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 70–95. [Google Scholar]
  21. Diamond A, Carlson SM, Beck DM. Preschool children’s performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task: Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2005;28:689–729. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dilworth-Bart JE. Does executive function mediate SES and home quality associations with academic readiness? Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2012;27(3):416–425. [Google Scholar]
  24. Eisenberg N, Vidmar M, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND, Edwards A, Gaertner B, Kupfer A. Mothers’ teaching strategies and children’s effortful control: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46(5):1294–1308. doi: 10.1037/a0020236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Espy KA, Kaufmann PM, Glisky ML, McDiarmid MD. New procedures to assess executive functions in preschool children. The Clinical Neurospsychologist. 2001;15:46–58. doi: 10.1076/clin.15.1.46.1908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fantuzzo J, Perry MA, McDermott P. Preschool approaches to learning and their relationship to other relevant classroom competencies for low-income children. School Psychology Quarterly. 2004;19:212–230. [Google Scholar]
  27. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134:31–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hair E, Halle T, Terry-Humen E, Lavelle B, Calkins J. Children’s school readiness in the ECLS-K: Predictions to academic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2006;21:431–454. [Google Scholar]
  29. Jones LB, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. Development of executive attention in preschool children. Developmental Science. 2003;6:498–504. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kray J, Ferdinand NK. How to improve cognitive control in development during childhood: Potentials and limits of cognitive interventions. Child Development Perspectives. 2013 n/a–n/a. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ladd GW, Birch SH, Buhs ES. Children’s social and scholastic lives in kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development. 1999;70:1373–1400. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ladd GW, Buhs E, Seid M. Children’s initial sentiments about kindergarten: Is school liking an antecedent of early classroom participation and achievement? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2000;46:255–279. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ladd GW, Kochenderfer BJ, Coleman CC. Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: Distinct relation system that contribute uniquely to children’s school adjustment? Child Development. 1997;68:1181–1197. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01993.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. LaFreniere PJ, Dumas JE. Social competence and behavioral evaluation in children ages 3 to 6 years: The short form (SCBE-30) Psychological Assessment. 1996;8:369–377. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lewis MD, Todd RM. The self-regulating brain: Cortical-subcortical feedback and the development of intelligent action. Cognitive Development. 2007;22:406–430. [Google Scholar]
  36. Li-Grining CP. Effortful control among low-income preschoolers in three cities: Stability, change, and individual differences. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:208–221. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Matthews JS, Ponitz CC, Morrison FJ. Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2009;101(3):689–704. [Google Scholar]
  38. McClelland MM, Cameron CE, Connor CM, Farris CL, Jewkes AM, Morrison FJ. Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:947–959. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McDermott PA, Leigh NM, Perry MA. Development and validation of the Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale. Psychology in the Schools. 2002;39:353–365. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ponitz CC, McClelland MM, Matthews JS, Morrison FJ. A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:605–619. doi: 10.1037/a0015365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pritchard VE, Woodward LJ. Preschool executive control on the Shape School task: Measurement considerations and utility. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23:31–43. doi: 10.1037/a0021095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Raffaelli M, Crockett LJ, Shen Y-L. Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2005;166(1):54–75. doi: 10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Raver CC, Jones SM, Li-Grining C, Zhai F, Bub K, Pressler E. CRSP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: Self-regulation as a mediating mechanism. Child Development. 2011;82:362–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rimm-Kaufman S, Pianta RC, Cox M. Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2000;15:147–166. [Google Scholar]
  46. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3 Social, emotional and personality development. New York, NY: Wiley; 1998. pp. 105–176. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rothbart MK, Jones LB. Temperament, self-regulation, and education. School Psychology Review. 1998;27:479–491. [Google Scholar]
  48. Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Roebers CM. Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Infant and Child Development. 2012;21:411–429. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sesma HW, Mahone EM, Levine T, Eason SH, Cutting LE. The contribution of executive skills to reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychology. 2009;15:232–246. doi: 10.1080/09297040802220029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Smith-Donald R, Raver CC, Hayes T, Richardson B. Preliminary construct and concurrent validity of the Preschool Self-regulation Assessment (PSRA) for field-based research. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2007;22:173–187. [Google Scholar]
  51. Thorell LB, Lindqvist S, Bergman Nutley S, Bohlin G, Klingberg T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science. 2009;12:106–113. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Welsh JA, Nix RL, Blair C, Bierman KL, Nelson KE. The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2010;102:43–53. doi: 10.1037/a0016738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wiebe SA, Espy KA, Charak D. Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44(2):575–587. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575. 10.1037/0012-1649.44,2.575.supp. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Willoughby M, Kupersmidt J, Voegler-Lee M, Bryant D. Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2011;36:161–180. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Zelazo PD, Lyons KE. The potential benefits of mindfulness training in early childhood: A developmental social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6:154–160. [Google Scholar]
  56. Zelazo PD, Müeller U. Executive function in typical and atypical development. In: Goswami U, editor. Handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford, England: Blackwell; 2002. pp. 445–469. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES