Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Jul 13.
Published in final edited form as: Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007 Dec;2(4):313–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x

The Power of Personality

The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes

Brent W Roberts 1, Nathan R Kuncel 2, Rebecca Shiner 3, Avshalom Caspi 4,5, Lewis R Goldberg 6
PMCID: PMC4499872  NIHMSID: NIHMS678907  PMID: 26151971

Abstract

The ability of personality traits to predict important life outcomes has traditionally been questioned because of the putative small effects of personality. In this article, we compare the predictive validity of personality traits with that of socioeconomic status (SES) and cognitive ability to test the relative contribution of personality traits to predictions of three critical outcomes: mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment. Only evidence from prospective longitudinal studies was considered. In addition, an attempt was made to limit the review to studies that controlled for important background factors. Results showed that the magnitude of the effects of personality traits on mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment was indistinguishable from the effects of SES and cognitive ability on these outcomes. These results demonstrate the influence of personality traits on important life outcomes, highlight the need to more routinely incorporate measures of personality into quality of life surveys, and encourage further research about the developmental origins of personality traits and the processes by which these traits influence diverse life outcomes.


Starting in the 1980s, personality psychology began a profound renaissance and has now become an extraordinarily diverse and intellectually stimulating field (Pervin & John, 1999). However, just because a field of inquiry is vibrant does not mean it is practical or useful—one would need to show that personality traits predict important life outcomes, such as health and longevity, marital success, and educational and occupational attainment. In fact, two recent reviews have shown that different personality traits are associated with outcomes in each of these domains (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Ozer & Benet-Martinez, 2006). But simply showing that personality traits are related to health, love, and attainment is not a stringent test of the utility of personality traits. These associations could be the result of “third” variables, such as socioeconomic status (SES), that account for the patterns but have not been controlled for in the studies reviewed. In addition, many of the studies reviewed were cross-sectional and therefore lacked the methodological rigor to show the predictive validity of personality traits. A more stringent test of the importance of personality traits can be found in prospective longitudinal studies that show the incremental validity of personality traits over and above other factors.

The analyses reported in this article test whether personality traits are important, practical predictors of significant life outcomes. We focus on three domains: longevity/mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment in work. Within each domain, we evaluate empirical evidence using the gold standard of prospective longitudinal studies—that is, those studies that can provide data about whether personality traits predict life outcomes above and beyond well-known factors such as SES and cognitive abilities. To guide the interpretation drawn from the results of these prospective longitudinal studies, we provide benchmark relations of SES and cognitive ability with outcomes from these three domains. The review proceeds in three sections. First, we address some misperceptions about personality traits that are, in part, responsible for the idea that personality does not predict important life outcomes. Second, we present a review of the evidence for the predictive validity of personality traits. Third, we conclude with a discussion of the implications of our findings and recommendations for future work in this area.

THE “PERSONALITY COEFFICIENT”: AN UNFORTUNATE LEGACY OF THE PERSON-SITUATION DEBATE

Before we embark on our review, it is necessary to lay to rest a myth perpetrated by the 1960s manifestation of the person–situation debate; this myth is often at the root of the perspective that personality traits do not predict outcomes well, if at all. Specifically, in his highly influential book, Walter Mischel (1968) argued that personality traits had limited utility in predicting behavior because their correlational upper limit appeared to be about .30. Subsequently, this .30 value became derided as the “personality coefficient.” Two conclusions were inferred from this argument. First, personality traits have little predictive validity. Second, if personality traits do not predict much, then other factors, such as the situation, must be responsible for the vast amounts of variance that are left unaccounted for. The idea that personality traits are the validity weaklings of the predictive panoply has been reiterated in unmitigated form to this day (e.g., Bandura, 1999; Lewis, 2001; Paul, 2004; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). In fact, this position is so widely accepted that personality psychologists often apologize for correlations in the range of .20 to .30 (e.g., Bornstein, 1999).

Should personality psychologists be apologetic for their modest validity coefficients? Apparently not, according to Meyer and his colleagues (Meyer et al., 2001), who did psychological science a service by tabling the effect sizes for a wide variety of psychological investigations and placing them side-by-side with comparable effect sizes from medicine and everyday life. These investigators made several important points. First, the modal effect size on a correlational scale for psychology as a whole is between .10 and .40, including that seen in experimental investigations (see also Hemphill, 2003). It appears that the .30 barrier applies to most phenomena in psychology and not just to those in the realm of personality psychology. Second, the very largest effects for any variables in psychology are in the .50 to .60 range, and these are quite rare (e.g., the effect of increasing age on declining speed of information processing in adults). Third, effect sizes for assessment measures and therapeutic interventions in psychology are similar to those found in medicine. It is sobering to see that the effect sizes for many medical interventions—like consuming aspirin to treat heart disease or using chemotherapy to treat breast cancer—translate into correlations of .02 or .03. Taken together, the data presented by Meyer and colleagues make clear that our standards for effect sizes need to be established in light of what is typical for psychology and for other fields concerned with human functioning.

In the decades since Mischel’s (1968) critique, researchers have also directly addressed the claim that situations have a stronger influence on behavior than they do on personality traits. Social psychological research on the effects of situations typically involves experimental manipulation of the situation, and the results are analyzed to establish whether the situational manipulation has yielded a statistically significant difference in the outcome. When the effects of situations are converted into the same metric as that used in personality research (typically the correlation coefficient, which conveys both the direction and the size of an effect), the effects of personality traits are generally as strong as the effects of situations (Funder & Ozer, 1983; Sarason, Smith, & Diener, 1975). Overall, it is the moderate position that is correct: Both the person and the situation are necessary for explaining human behavior, given that both have comparable relations with important outcomes.

As research on the relative magnitude of effects has documented, personality psychologists should not apologize for correlations between .10 and .30, given that the effect sizes found in personality psychology are no different than those found in other fields of inquiry. In addition, the importance of a predictor lies not only in the magnitude of its association with the outcome, but also in the nature of the outcome being predicted. A large association between two self-report measures of extraversion and positive affect may be theoretically interesting but may not offer much solace to the researcher searching for proof that extraversion is an important predictor for outcomes that society values. In contrast, a modest correlation between a personality trait and mortality or some other medical outcome, such as Alzheimer’s disease, would be quite important. Moreover, when attempting to predict these critical life outcomes, even relatively small effects can be important because of their pragmatic effects and because of their cumulative effects across a person’s life (Abelson, 1985; Funder, 2004; Rosenthal, 1990). In terms of practicality, the −.03 association between taking aspirin and reducing heart attacks provides an excellent example. In one study, this surprisingly small association resulted in 85 fewer heart attacks among the patients of 10,845 physicians (Rosenthal, 2000). Because of its practical significance, this type of association should not be ignored because of the small effect size. In terms of cumulative effects, a seemingly small effect that moves a person away from pursuing his or her education early in life can have monumental consequences for that person’s health and well-being later in life (Hardarson et al., 2001). In other words, psychological processes with a statistically small or moderate effect can have important effects on individuals’ lives depending on the outcomes with which they are associated and depending on whether those effects get cumulated across a person’s life.

PERSONALITY EFFECTS ON MORTALITY, DIVORCE, AND OCCUPATIONAL ATTAINMENT

Selection of Predictors, Outcomes, and Studies for This Review

To provide the most stringent test of the predictive validity of personality traits, we chose to focus on three objective outcomes: mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment. Although we could have chosen many different outcomes to examine, we selected these three because they are socially valued; they are measured in similar ways across studies; and they have been assessed as outcomes in studies of SES, cognitive ability, and personality traits. Mortality needs little justification as an outcome, as most individuals value a long life. Divorce and marital stability are important outcomes for several reasons. Divorce is a significant source of depression and distress for many individuals and can have negative consequences for children, whereas a happy marriage is one of the most important predictors of life satisfaction (Myers, 2000). Divorce is also linked to disproportionate drops in economic status, especially for women (Kuh & Maclean, 1990), and it can undermine men’s health (e.g., Lund, Holstein, & Osler, 2004). An intact marriage can also preserve cognitive function into old age for both men and women, particularly for those married to a high-ability spouse (Schaie, 1994).

Educational and occupational attainment are also highly prized (Roisman, Masten, Coatsworth, & Tellegen, 2004). Research on subjective well-being has shown that occupational attainment and its important correlate, income, are not as critical for happiness as many assume them to be (Myers, 2000). Nonetheless, educational and occupational attainment are associated with greater access to many resources that can improve the quality of life (e.g., medical care, education) and with greater “social capital” (i.e., greater access to various resources through connections with others; Bradley & Corwyn, 2002; Conger & Donnellan, 2007). The greater income resulting from high educational and occupational attainment may also enable individuals to maintain strong life satisfaction when faced with difficult life circumstances (Johnson & Krueger, 2006).

To better interpret the significance of the relations between personality traits and these outcomes, we have provided comparative information concerning the effect of SES and cognitive ability on each of these outcomes. We chose to use SES as a comparison because it is widely accepted to be one of the most important contributors to a more successful life, including better health and higher occupational attainment (e.g., Adler et al., 1994; Gallo & Mathews, 2003; Galobardes, Lynch, & Smith, 2004; Sapolsky, 2005). In addition, we chose cognitive ability as a comparison variable because, like SES, it is a widely accepted predictor of longevity and occupational success (Deary, Batty, & Gottfredson, 2005; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). In this article, we compare the effect sizes of personality traits with these two predictors in order to understand the relative contribution of personality to a long, stable, and successful life. We also required that the studies in this review make some attempt to control for background variables. For example, in the case of mortality, we looked for prospective longitudinal studies that controlled for previous medical conditions, gender, age, and other relevant variables.

We are not assuming that personality traits are direct causes of the outcomes under study. Rather, we were exclusively interested in whether personality traits predict mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment and in their modal effect sizes. If found to be robust, these patterns of statistical association then invite the question of why and how personality traits might cause these outcomes, and we have provided several examples in each section of potential mechanisms and causal steps involved in the process.

The Measurement of Effect Sizes in Prospective Longitudinal Studies

Before turning to the specific findings for personality, SES, and cognitive ability, we must first address the measurement of effect sizes in the studies reviewed here. Most of the studies that we reviewed used some form of regression analysis for either continuous or categorical outcomes. In studies with continuous outcomes, findings were typically reported as standardized regression weights (beta coefficients). In studies of categorical outcomes, the most common effect size indicators are odds ratios, relative risk ratios, or hazard ratios. Because many psychologists may be less familiar with these ratio statistics, a brief discussion of them is in order. In the context of individual differences, ratio statistics quantify the likelihood of an event (e.g., divorce, mortality) for a higher scoring group versus the likelihood of the same event for a lower scoring group (e.g., persons high in negative affect versus those low in negative affect). An odds ratio is the ratio of the odds of the event for one group over the odds of the same event for the second group. The risk ratio compares the probabilities of the event occurring for the two groups. The hazard ratio assesses the probability of an event occurring for a group over a specific window of time. For these statistics, a value of 1.0 equals no difference in odds or probabilities. Values above 1.0 indicate increased likelihood (odds or probabilities) for the experimental (or numerator) group, with the reverse being true for values below 1.0 (down to a lower limit of zero). Because of this asymmetry, the log of these statistics is often taken.

The primary advantage of ratio statistics in general, and the risk ratio in particular, is their ease of interpretation in applied settings. It is easier to understand that death is three times as likely to occur for one group than for another than it is to make sense out of a point-biserial correlation. However, there are also some disadvantages that should be understood. First, ratio statistics can make effects that are actually very small in absolute magnitude appear to be large when in fact they are very rare events. For example, although it is technically correct that one is three times as likely (risk ratio = 3.0) to win the lottery when buying three tickets instead of one ticket, the improved chances of winning are trivial in an absolute sense.

Second, there is no accepted practice for how to divide continuous predictor variables when computing odds, risk, and hazard ratios. Some predictors are naturally dichotomous (e.g., gender), but many are continuous (e.g., cognitive ability, SES). Researchers often divide continuous variables into some arbitrary set of categories in order to use the odds, rate, or hazard metrics. For example, instead of reporting an association between SES and mortality using a point-biserial correlation, a researcher may use proportional hazards models using some arbitrary categorization of SES, such as quartile estimates (e.g., lowest versus highest quartiles). This permits the researcher to draw conclusions such as “individuals from the highest category of SES are four times as likely to live longer than are groups lowest in SES.” Although more intuitively appealing, the odds statements derived from categorizing continuous variables makes it difficult to deduce the true effect size of a relation, especially across studies. Researchers with very large samples may have the luxury of carving a continuous variable into very fine-grained categories (e.g., 10 categories of SES), which may lead to seemingly huge hazard ratios. In contrast, researchers with smaller samples may only dichotomize or trichotomize the same variables, thus resulting in smaller hazard ratios and what appear to be smaller effects for identical predictors. Finally, many researchers may not categorize their continuous variables at all, which can result in hazard ratios very close to 1.0 that are nonetheless still statistically significant. These procedures for analyzing odds, rate, and hazard ratios produce a haphazard array of results from which it is almost impossible to discern a meaningful average effect size.1

One of the primary tasks of this review is to transform the results from different studies into a common metric so that a fair comparison could be made across the predictors and outcomes. For this purpose, we chose the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient. We used a variety of techniques to arrive at an accurate estimate of the effect size from each study. When transforming relative risk ratios into the correlation metric, we used several methods to arrive at the most appropriate estimate of the effect size. For example, the correlation coefficient can be estimated from reported significance levels (p values) and from test statistics such as the t test or chi-square, as well as from other effect size indicators such as d scores (Rosenthal, 1991). Also, the correlation coefficient can be estimated directly from relative risk ratios and hazard ratios using the generic inverse variance approach (The Cochrane Collaboration, 2005). In this procedure, the relative risk ratio and confidence intervals (CIs) are first transformed into z scores, and the z scores are then transformed into the correlation metric.

For most studies, the effect size correlation was estimated from information on relative risk ratios and p values. For the latter, we used the requivalent effect size indicator (Rosenthal & Rubin, 2003), which is computed from the sample size and p value associated with specific effects. All of these techniques transform the effect size information to a common correlational metric, making the results of the studies comparable across different analytical methods. After compiling effect sizes, meta-analytic techniques were used to estimate population effect sizes in both the risk ratio and correlation metric (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Specifically, a random-effects model with no moderators was used to estimate population effect sizes for both the rate ratio and correlation metrics.2 When appropriate, we first averaged multiple nonindependent effects from studies that reported more than one relevant effect size.

The Predictive Validity of Personality Traits for Mortality

Before considering the role of personality traits in health and longevity, we reviewed a selection of studies linking SES and cognitive ability to these same outcomes. This information provides a point of reference to understand the relative contribution of personality. Table 1 presents the findings from 33 studies examining the prospective relations of low SES and low cognitive ability with mortality.3 SES was measured using measures or composites of typical SES variables including income, education, and occupational status. Total IQ scores were commonly used in analyses of cognitive ability. Most studies demonstrated that being born into a low-SES household or achieving low SES in adulthood resulted in a higher risk of mortality (e.g., Deary & Der, 2005; Hart et al., 2003; Osler et al., 2002; Steenland, Henley, & Thun, 2002). The relative risk ratios and hazard ratios ranged from a low of 0.57 to a high of 1.30 and averaged 1.24 (CIs = 1.19 and 1.29). When translated into the correlation metric, the effect sizes for low SES ranged from −.02 to .08 and averaged .02 (CIs = .017 and .026).

TABLE 1.

SES and IQ Effects on Mortality/Longevity

Study N Outcome Years Controls Predictors Outcome Est. r
Abas et al., 2002 2,584 members of
the Medical
Research Council
Elderly
Hypertension Trial
All-cause
mortality
11 years Low scores on the New
Adult Reading Test (IQ)
Low scores on Raven’s
Progressive Matrices
(IQ)
HR = 0.94 (0.86, 1.02)
p = .16
HR = 0.97 (0.88, 1.06)
p = .53
rhr = .03a
re = .03a
rhr = .01a
re = .01a
Bassuk, Berkman, & Amick, 2002 9,025 men from
Boston
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education
Low adult income
Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 1.32 (0.95, 1.83)
HR = 0.94 (0.65, 1.34)
HR = 1.09 (0.86, 1.39)
rhr = .02
rhr = .00
rhr = .01
6,518 women from
Boston
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education

Low adult income
Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 0.74 (0.53, 1.04)
p < .10
HR = 0.80 (0.52, 1.23)
HR = 0.74 (0.57, 0.98)
p < .05
rhr = −.02
re = −.02
rhr = −.01
rhr = −.03
re = −.02
12,235 men from
Iowa
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education
Low adult income
Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = .77 (.56, 1.07)
HR = 1.18 (0.89, 1.58)
HR = 0.93 (0.69, 1.27)
rhr = −.01
rhr = .01
rhr = .00
9,248 women from
Iowa
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education
Low adult income
Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 0.87 (0.61, 1.23)
HR = 1.03 (.76, 1.41)
HR = .57 (.36, .92)
p < .05
rhr = −.01
rhr = .00
rhr = −.02
re = −.02
10,081 men from
Connecticut
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, race, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education

Low adult income

Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 1.30 (0.96, 1.75)
p < .10
HR = 1.62 (1.17, 2.23)
p < .005
HR = 1.20 (0.94, 1.53)
rhr = .02
re = .02
rhr = .03
re = .03
rhr = .01
7,331 women from
Connecticut
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, race, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, activity level, social
ties, having a regular health care
provider, number of chronic
conditions, depressive symptoms,
cognitive function, physical
function, health status
Low adult education
Low adult income

Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 0.96 (0.64, 1.44)
HR = 1.90 (1.09, 3.32)
p < .05
HR = 1.15 (0.83, 1.59)
rhr = .00
rhr = .03
re = .02
rhr = .01
11,977 men from
North Carolina
All-cause
mortality
9 years age, race, smoking, degree of
urbanization, BMI, alcohol
consumption, social ties, having a
regular health care provider,
number of chronic conditions,
depressive symptoms, cognitive
function, physical function, health
status
Low adult education
Low adult income

Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 1.18 (0.84, 1.64)
HR = 1.42 (1.01, 1.84)
p < .01
HR = 1.01 (.78, 1.32)
rhr = .01
rhr = .02
re = .02
rhr = .00
8,836 women from
North Carolina
All-cause
mortality
9 years Age, race, smoking, BMI, alcohol
consumption, social ties, having a
regular health care provider,
number of chronic conditions,
depressive symptoms, cognitive
function, physical function, health
status
Low adult education
Low adult income

Low adult occupational
prestige
HR = 1.04 (0.84, 1.30)
HR = 1.52 (1.11, 2.08)
p < .01
HR = 1.21 (0.97, 1.51)
p < .10
rhr = .00
rhr = .03
re = .03
rhr = .02
re = .02
Beebe-Dimmer et al, 2004 3,087 women from
the Alameda County
Study
All-cause
mortality
30 years Age, income, education,
occupation, smoking, BMI,
physical activity
Low childhood SES
Low adult education
Manual occupation
Low adult income
HR = 1.12 (0.99, 1.27)
HR = 1.17 (0.99, 1.39)
HR = 1.06 (0.87, 1.30)
HR = 1.35 (1.14, 1.60)
rhr = .03
rhr = .03
rhr = .01
rhr = .06
Bosworth & Schaie, 1999 1,218 members of
the Seattle
Longitudinal Study
All-cause
mortality
7 years Sex, age, education Low verbal IQ

Low math IQ

Low spatial IQ
F(1, 1,174) = 17.58,
p < .001
F(1, 1,198) = 3.75,
p < .05
F(1, 1,119) = 3.72,
p <.05
rF = .12
re = .10
rF = .06
re = .06
rF = .06
re = .06
Bucher & Ragland, 1995 3,154 middle-aged
men from the
Western
Collaborative Group
Study
All-cause
mortality
22 years Systolic blood pressure,
cholesterol, smoking, height
Low adult SES RR = 1.45 (1.17, 1.81) rrr = .06
Clausen, Davey-Smith, & Thelle, 2003 128,723 Oslo
natives
All-cause
mortality
30 years Age, adult income Low index of inequality RR men = 2.48
(1.94, 3.16)
RR women = 1.47
(1.06, 2.04)
rrr = .03
rrr = .01
Curtis, Southall, Congdon, & Dodgeon, 2004 23,311 men and
35,295 women of the
National Statistics
Longitudinal Study
All-cause
mortality
10 years Age, sex, marital status,
employment status
Low adult social class OR men = 1.26 (1.10,
1.46)
OR women = .90 (.77,
1.06)
ror = .02
ror = −.01
Davey Smith, Hart, Blane, & Hole, 1998 5,766 men aged 35–
64 in 1970
All-cause
mortality
25 years Age, adult SES, deprivation, car,
risk factors
Low father’s social class HR = 1.19 (1.04, 1.37)
p = .042
rhr = .03
re = .03
Deary & Der, 2005 898 members of the
Twenty-07 Study
All-cause
mortality
24 years Sex, smoking, social class, years of
education
Low IQ HR = 1.38 (1.15, 1.67)
p = .0006
rhr = .15
re = .11
Sex, smoking, years of education,
IQ
Low social class HR = 1.13 (1.01, 1.26)
p = .027
rhr = .07
re = .07
Sex, smoking, social class, IQ Low education HR = 1.06 (0.97, 1.12)
p = .20
rhr = .04
re = .04
Doornbos & Kromhout, 1990 78,505 Dutch
Nationals
All-cause
mortality
32 years Height, health High education level RR = 0.69a (0.57, 0.81)
p < .0001
rhr = −.01a
re = −.01a
Fiscella & Franks, 2000 13,332 National
Health and
Nutrition
Examination Survey
participants
All-cause
mortality
12 years Age, sex, morbidity, income
inequality, depression, self-rated
health
High income HR = 0.80a (0.77, 0.83) rhr = −.10a
Ganguli et al, 2002 1,064 members of
the Monongahela
Valley Independent
Elders Survey
All-cause
mortality
10 years Age, sex, education, functional
disability, self-rated health,
depression, Number of drugs
taken, depression × self-rated
health interaction
Low education

Low cognitive
functioning (MMSE
score)
RR = .99
p = .94
RR = 1.55,
p = .002
re = .002

re = .09
Hardarson et al., 2001 9,773 women and
9,139 men from the
Reykjavik Study
All-cause
mortality
3–30
years
Height, weight, cholesterol,
triglycerides, systolic blood
pressure, blood sugar, smoking
High education

High education
Men’s HR = 0.77 (0.66,
0.88)
Women’s HR = 1.29 (.56,
1.35)
rhr = −.05

rhr = .01
Hart et al, 2003 922 members of the
Midspan Study who
also participated in
the Scottish Mental
Survey of 1932
All-cause
mortality
25 years Sex, social class, deprivation
Sex, IQ, deprivation
Low IQ

Low social class
RR = 1.26 (0.94, 1.70)
p = .038
RR = 1.22 (0.88, 1.68)
p = .35
rhr = .05
re = .07
rhr = .04
re = .03
Heslop, Smith, Macleod, & Hart, 2001 958 Women from
Western Scotland
All-cause
mortality
25 years Age, blood pressure, cholesterol,
BMI, FEV, smoking, exercise,
alcohol
Low lifetime social class HR = 1.48 (1.04, 2.09)
p = .037
rhr = .07
re = .07
Hosegood & Campbell, 2003 1,888 women from
rural Bangladesh
All-cause
mortality
19 years Age No education p = .005 re = .06
Khang & Kim, 2005 5,437 South
Koreans aged 30
years and older
All-cause
mortality
5 years Age, gender, urbanization, number
of family members, biological risk
factors
Low annual household
income
RR = 2.24 (1.40, 3.60) rrr = .05
Korten et al., 1999 897 subjects aged
70 years and older
All-cause
mortality
3.5 years Age, sex, general health, ADLs,
illness, blood pressure, Symbol-
Letter Modalities Test
Low IQ HR = 2.42
(1.27, 4.62)
rhr = .09
Kuh, Hardy, Langenberg, Richards, & Wadsworth, 2002 2,547 women and
2,812 men from the
Medical Research
Council national
survey
All-cause
mortality
46 years Sex, adult SES, education Low father’s social class HR = 1.90 (1.30, 2.70)
p < .001
rhr = .06
re = .05
Kuh, Richards, Hardy, Butterworth, & Wadsworth, 2004 2,547 women and
2,812 men from the
Medical Res.
All-cause
mortality
46 years Sex, adult SES, education Low IQ HR men = 1.80 (1.10,
2.70)
p < .013
rhr = .05
re = .05
Council national
survey
Low IQ HR women = 0.90 (0.52,
1.60)
p = .70
rhr = −.01
re = −.01
Lantz et al, 1998 3,617 subjects aged
25 years and older
All-cause
mortality
7.5 years Age, sex, race, residence Low education
Low income
HR = 1.08 (0.76, 1.54)
HR = 3.22 (2.01, 5.16)
rhr = .01
rhr = .08
Lynch et al, 1994 2,636 Finnish men All-cause
mortality
8 years Age Low childhood SES RR = 2.39 (1.28, 4.44) rrr = .05
Maier & Smith, 1999 513 members of the
Berlin Aging Study
aged 70 years and
older
All-cause
mortality
4.5 years Age, SES, health Low perceptual speed
Low reasoning
Low memory
Low knowledge
Low fluency
RR = 1.53 (1.29, 1.81)
RR = 1.37 (1.19, 1.71)
RR = 1.39 (1.19, 1.63)
RR = 1.33 (1.15, 1.54)
RR = 1.50 (1.27, 1.78)
rrr = .22
rrr = .15
rrr = .18
rrr = .17
rrr = .21
Martin & Kubzansky, 2005 659 gifted children
from Terman Life
Cycle Study
All-cause
mortality
48 years Father’s occupation, poor health in
childhood, Sex
Less high IQb
Father’s occupation
HR = 0.73 (0.59, 0.90)
HR = 0.99 (0.90, 1.08)
rhr = .11
rhr = .01
Osler et al, 2003 7,308 members of
Project Metropolit in
Copenhagen
All-cause
mortality
49 years IQ, birth weight
SES, birth weight
Working class status

Low Harnquist IQ test
HR = 1.30 (1.08,1.57)

HR = 1.53 (1.19, 1.97)
rhr = .03

rhr = .04
Osler et al, 2002 25,728 citizens of
Copenhagen
(12,715 men &
13,013 women)
All-cause
mortality
24–34
years
Smoking status, activity level,
BMI, alcohol consumption,
education, household structure,
Percent of households with
children
High household income Men’s HR = 0.64a (0.57,
0.73)
p < .01
Women’s HR = 0.68a
(0.65, 0.89)
p < .01
rhr = −.06a
re = −.02a

rhr = −.04a
re = −.02a
Pudaric, Sundquist, & Johansson, 2003 8,959 members of
the Swedish Survey
of Living Conditions
All-cause
mortality
7–12
years
Age, health status Low education RR = 1.22 (1.07, 1.38) rhr = .03
Shipley, Der, Taylor, & Deary, 2006 6,424 members of
the UK Health and
Lifestyle Survey
All-cause
mortality
19 years Age, sex, social class, education,
health behaviors, FEV, blood
pressure, BMI
High verbal memory
High visual spatial
ability
HR = 0.95 (0.92, 0.99)
p < .0052
HR = 0.99 (0.96, 1.03)
p = .66
rhr = −.03
re = −.03
rhr = −.01
re = .00
Steenland et al., 2002 550,888 men from
the CPS-I cohort
All-cause
mortality
26 years Age, smoking, BMI, diet, alcohol,
hypertension, menopausal status
(women)
Low education level Men’s RR = 1.14 (1.12,
1.16)
rrr = .02
553,959 women
from the CPS-I
cohort
Women’s
RR = 1.24 (1.21, 1.28)
rrr = .02
625,663 men from
the CPS-II cohort
All-cause
mortality
16 years Age, smoking, BMI, diet, alcohol,
hypertension, menopausal status
(women)
Low education level Men’s
RR = 1.28 (1.25, 1.31)
rrr = .03
767,472 women
from the CPS-II
cohort
Women’s
RR = 1.18 (1.15, 1.22)
rrr = .01
St. John et al., 2002 8,099 Seniors from
the Canadian Study
of Health and Aging
Mortality 5 years Age, sex, education, marital
status, functional status, self-rated
health
High MMSE scores OR = 0.95 (0.93, 0.97) ror = −.05a
Tenconi, Devoti, Comelli, & RIFLE Research Group, 2000 12,361 Italian men
from the RIFLE
pooling project
All-cause
mortality
7 years Age, systolic blood pressure,
cholesterol, smoking
Low adult education
level
Low adult occupational
level
RR = 0.76 (0.56, 1.01)
p = .122
RR = 1.30 (1.04, 1.63)
p = .022
rrr = −.02
re = −.01
rrr = .02
re = .02
Vagero & Leon, 1994 404,450 Swedish
men born in 1946–
1955
Mortality 36 years Adulthood social class Low childhood social
class
OR = 1.52 (1.32, 1.76) ror = .01
Whalley & Deary, 2001 722 Members of the
Scottish mental
survey of 1932
Life
expectancy
76 years Father’s SES, overcrowding High Moray House test
scores (IQ)
Partial r = .19 r = .19

Note. Confidence intervals are given in parentheses. SES = socioeconomic status; HR = hazard ratio; RR = relative risk ratio; OR = odds ratio; rrr = Correlation estimated from the rate ratio; rhr = correlation estimated from the hazard ratio; ror = correlation estimated from the odds ratio; rF = correlation estimated from F test; re = requivalent—correlation estimated from the reported p value and sample size; BMI = body mass index; FEV = forced expiratory volume; ADLs = activities of daily living; MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination; CPS = Cancer Prevention Study; RIFLE = risk factors and life expectancy.

a

The sign of the ratios and correlations based on high SES and high IQ were reversed before these effect sizes were aggregated with remaining effect sizes.

b

IQ scores are referred to as “less high” because the lowest IQ score in the sample was 135.

Through the use of the relative risk metric, we determined that the effect of low IQ on mortality was similar to that of SES, ranging from a modest 0.74 to 2.42 and averaging 1.19 (CIs = 1.10 and 1.30). When translated into the correlation metric, however, the effect of low IQ on mortality was equivalent to a correlation of .06 (CIs = .03 and .09), which was three times larger than the effect of SES on mortality. The discrepancy between the relative risk and correlation metrics most likely resulted because some studies reported the relative risks in terms of continuous measures of IQ, which resulted in smaller relative risk ratios (e.g., St. John, Montgomery, Kristjansson, & McDowell, 2002). Merging relative risk ratios from these studies with those that carve the continuous variables into subgroups appears to underestimate the effect of IQ on mortality, at least in terms of the relative risk metric. The most telling comparison of IQ and SES comes from the five studies that include both variables in the prediction of mortality. Consistent with the aggregate results, IQ was a stronger predictor of mortality in each case (i.e., Deary & Der, 2005; Ganguli, Dodge, & Mulsant, 2002; Hart et al., 2003; Osler et al., 2002; Wilson, Bienia, Mendes de Leon, Evans, & Bennet, 2003).

Table 2 lists 34 studies that link personality traits to mortality/longevity.4 In most of these studies, multiple factors such as SES, cognitive ability, gender, and disease severity were controlled for. We organized our review roughly around the Big Five taxonomy of personality traits (e.g., Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Openness to Experience; Goldberg, 1993b). For example, research drawn from the Terman Longitudinal Study showed that children who were more conscientious tended to live longer (Friedman et al., 1993). This effect held even after controlling for gender and parental divorce, two known contributors to shorter lifespans. Moreover, a number of other factors, such as SES and childhood health difficulties, were unrelated to longevity in this study. The protective effect of Conscientiousness has now been replicated across several studies and more heterogeneous samples. Conscientiousness was found to be a rather strong protective factor in an elderly sample participating in a Medicare training program (Weiss & Costa, 2005), even when controlling for education level, cardiovascular disease, and smoking, among other factors. Similarly, Conscientiousness predicted decreased rates of mortality in a sample of individuals suffering from chronic renal insufficiency, even after controlling for age, diabetic status, and hemoglobin count (Christensen et al., 2002).

TABLE 2.

Personality Traits and Mortality

Study N Outcome Length of study Controls Predictors Outcome Est. ra
Allison et al., 2003 101 survivors of head and
neck cancer
Mortality 1 year Age, disease stage,
cohabitation status
High Optimism OR = 1.12 (1.01, 1.24) ror = −.22
Almada et al., 1991 1,871 members of the
Western Electric Study
All-cause mortality 25 years Age, blood pressure,
smoking, cholesterol,
alcohol consumption
High Neuroticism
High Cynicism
RR = 1.20 (1.00, 1.40)
RR = 1.4 (1.2, 1.7)
rrr = .05
rrr = .09
Barefoot, Dahlstrom, & Williams, 1983 255 medical students All-cause mortality 25 years High Hostility p = .005 re = .18
Barefoot, Dodge, Peterson, Dahlstrom, & Williams, 1989 128 law Students 29 years Age High Hostility p = .012 re = .22
Barefoot, Larsen, von der Lieth, & Schroll, 1995 730 residents of Glostrup
born in 1914
All-cause mortality 27 years Age, sex, blood pressure,
smoking, triglycerid, FEV
High Hostility RR = 1.36 (1.06, 1.75) rrr = .09
Barefoot et al., 1998 100 Older men and women All-cause mortality 14 years Sex, age High Trust RR = 0.46 (0.24, 0.91)
p < .03
rrr = −.23
re = −.22
Barefoot et al., 1987 500 members of the second
Duke longitudinal study
All-cause mortality 15 years Age, sex, cholesterol levels,
smoking, physician ratings
of health
Suspiciousness p = .02 re = .10
Boyle et al., 2005 1,328 Duke University
Medical Center patients
All-cause mortality 15 years Sex, age, tobacco
consumption,
hypertension,
hyperlipidemia, number of
coronary arteries narrowed,
left ventricular ejection
fraction, artery bypass
surgery
High Hostility HR = 1.25 (1.06, 1.47)
p < .007
rhr = .07
re = .07
Boyle et al., 2004 936 Duke University
Medical Center patients
All-cause mortality 15 years Sex, age, tobacco
consumption,
hypertension,
hyperlipidemia, number of
coronary arteries narrowed,
left ventricular ejection
fraction, artery bypass
surgery
High Hostility HR = 1.28 (1.06, 1.55)
p <. 02
rhr = .08
re = .08
Christensen et al., 2002 174 chronic renal
insufficiency patients
Mortality 4 years Age, diabetic status,
hemoglobin
High Conscientiousness HR = 0.94, B = −.066
(.03)
p < .05
rB = −.17
re = −.15
High Neuroticism HR = 1.05, B = .047
(.023)
p <. 05
rhr = .15
re = .15
Danner et al, 2001 180 nuns Longevity 63 years Age, education, linguistic
ability
High Positive Emotion
(sentences)
High Positive Emotion
(words)
HR = 2.50 (1.20, 5.30)
p < .01
HR = 3.20 (1.50, 6.80)
p < .01
rhr = .18
re = .19
rhr = .22
re = .19
Different Positive Emotions HR = 4.30 (1.70,
10.40)
p < .01
rhr = .24
re = .19
Denollet et al, 1996 303 CHD patients Mortality 8 years CHD, age, social
alienation, depression, use
of benzodiazepines
Type D personalityb HR = 4.10 (1.90, 8.80)
p = .0004
rhr = .21
re = .20
Everson et al., 1997 2,125 men from the Kuopio
Eschemic Heart Disease
Risk Factor Study
All-cause mortality 9 years Age, SES Cynical distrust HR = 1.97 (1.26, 3.09) rhr = .06
Friedman et al., 1993 1,178 members of the
Terman Lifecycle Study
Longevity 71 years Sex, IQ High Conscientiousness HR = .33, B = −1.11
(0.37)
p < .01
rhr = .09
re = .08
High Cheerfulnessc HR = 1.21, B = .19
(.07)
p < .05
rhr = −.08
re = −.06
Giltay, Geleijnse, Zitman, Hoekstra, & Schouten, 2004 397 men and 418 women of
the Arnhem Elderly Study
All-cause mortality 9 years Age, smoking, alcohol,
education, activity level,
SES, and marital status
Dispositional optimism Men’s HR = 0.58 (0.37,
0.91)
p = .01
rhr = −.12
re = −.13
Women’s HR = 0.80
(0.51–1.25)
p = .39
rhr = −.05
re = −.04
Grossarth-Maticek, Bastianns, & Kanazir, 1985 1,335 inhabitants of
Crvenka, Yugoslavia
Mortality 10 years Age High Rationalityd p < .001 re = .09
Hearn, Murray, & Luepker, 1989 1,313 University of
Minnesota students
All-cause mortality 33 years Age High Hostility p = .72 re = .01
Hirokawa, Nagata, Takatsuka, & Shimizu, 2004 12,417 males and 14,133
females of the Takayama
Study
7 years Age, smoking, marital
status, BMI, exercise,
alcohol, education, and
number of children
High Rationalityd Men’s HR = 0.96 (0.83,
1.09)
Women’s HR = 0.82,
(0.70, 0.96)
p < .05
rhr = −.01
rhr = −.02
re = −.02
Hollis, Connett, Stevens, & Greenlick, 1990 12,866 men from the
Multiple Risk Factor
Intervention Trial
All-cause mortality 6 years Study group assignment,
age, cigarettes, blood
pressure, cholesterol
High Type A personality RR = 0.94 (0.89, 0.99)
p < .01
rhr = −.02
re = −.02
Iribarren et al., 2005 5,115 members of the
CARDIA study
Non-AIDS, non-
homicide-related
mortality
16 years Age, sex, race High Hostility RR = 2.02 (1.07, 3.81) rrr = .03
Kaplan et al, 1994 2,464 men from the Kuopio
Eschemic Heart Disease
Risk Factor Study
All-cause mortality 6 years Age, income Shyness HR = 1.01 (0.63, 1.62) rhr = .00
Korten et al., 1999 897 subjects aged 70 years
and older
Mortality 4 years Age, sex, general health,
ADLs, illness, blood
pressure, Symbol-Letter
Modalities Test, MMSE
High Neuroticism HR = 0.53 (0.31, 0.90) rhr = −.08
Kuskenvuo et al., 1988 3,750 Finnish male twins All-cause mortality 3 years Age High Hostility RR = 2.98 (1.31, 6.77) rrr = .04
Maruta, Colligan, Malinchoc, & Offard, 2000 839 patients from the Mayo
Clinic
All-cause mortality 29 years Sex, age, expected survival Pessimism HR = 1.20 (1.04, 1.38)
p = .01
rhr = .09
re = .09
Maruta et al, 1993 620 from the Mayo Clinic All-cause mortality 20 years Age, sex, hypertension,
weight
High Hostility p = .069 re = .07
McCarron, Gunnell, Harrison, Okasha, & Davey-Smith, 2003 8,385 former male students All-cause mortality 41 years 25 years Smoking, father’s SES,
BMI, maternal and paternal
vital status
Mental instability RR = 2.05 (1.36–3.09)
p < .01
rrr = .04
re = .03
McCranie, Watkins, Brandsma, & Sisson, 1986 478 physicians All-cause mortality 25 years High Hostility p = .789 re = −.01
Murberg, Bru, & Aarsland, 2001 119 heart failure patients Mortality 2 years Age, sex, disease severity Neuroticism HR = 1.140 (1.027,
1.265)
p = .01
rhr = .23
re = .24
Osler et al, 2003 7,308 members of Project
Metropolit in Copenhagen,
Denmark
All-cause mortality 49 years IQ, birth weight, SES Creativity HR = 1.17 (0.89, 1.54) rhr = .01
C. Peterson, Seligman, Yurko, Martin, & Friedman, 1998 1,179 members of the
Terman Lifecycle Study
Mortality 51 Years Global pessimism OR = 1.26, p < .01 re = .08
Schulz et al., 1996 238 cancer patients Cancer mortality 8 months Site of cancer, physical
symptoms, age
Pessimism OR = 1.07, B = .07
(.05)
rB = .08
Pessimism × Age
interaction
OR = 0.88, B = −.12
(.06),
p < .05
rB = −11
re = .13
Surtees, Wainwright, Luben, Day, & Khaw, 2005 20,550 members of the
EPIC-Norfolk study (8,950
men and 11,600 women)
Mortality 6 years Age, disease, cigarette
smoking history
Hostility Men’s RR = 1.06 (0.99,
1.14)
Women’s RR = 1.00
(.91, 1.09)
rrr = .02

rrr = .00
Surtees, Wainwright, Luben, Khaw, & Day, 2003 18,248 members of the
EPIC-Norfolk study
Mortality 6 years Age, disease, social class,
cigarette smoking history
Strong sense of coherence RR = 0.76 (0.65, 0.87)
p < .0001 (taken from
abstract)
rhr = −.03
re = −.03
Weiss & Costa, 2005e 1,076 members of the
Medicare Primary and
Consumer-Directed Care
Demonstration
All-cause mortality 5 years Gender, age, education,
diabetic status,
cardiovascular disease,
functional limitations, self-
rated health, cigarette
smoking, depression,
Neuroticism,
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness HR = 0.51 (0.31, 0.85)
p < .05
rhr = −.08
re = −.06
Gender, age, education
diabetic status,
cardiovascular disease,
functional limitations, self-
rated health, cigarette
smoking, depression,
Conscientiousness,
Agreeableness
Neuroticism HR = 0.99 (0.97, 1.00)
p < .05
rhr = −.04
re = −.06
Gender, age, education,
diabetic status,
cardiovascular disease,
functional limitations, self-
rated health, cigarette
smoking, depression,
Neuroticism,
Conscientiousness
Agreeableness HR = 0.99 (0.98, 1.00) rhr = −.06
Wilson et al., 2003 851 members of the
Religious Orders Study
All-cause mortality 5 years Age, sex, education, health Trait anxiety RR = 1.04 (0.99, 1.09)
p = .01 (unadjusted)
rrr = .05
re = .09
Trait anger RR = 1.03 (0.95, 1.12)
p = .64 (unadjusted)
rrr= .02
re = .02
Wilson et al., 2005 6,158 members (aged 65
years and older) of the
Chicago Health and Aging
Project
All-cause mortality 6 years Age, sex, race, education Neuroticism

Extraversion
RR = 1.016 (1.010,
1.020)
RR = 0.984 (0.978,
0.991)
rrr= .07

rrr = −.05
Wilson et al., 2004 883 members of the
Religious Orders Study
All-cause mortality 5 years Age, gender, education,
remaining personality traits
Neuroticism RR = 1.04 (1.02, 1.08)
p < .02 (unadjusted)
rrr= .12
re = .09
Extraversion RR = 0.96 (0.94, 0.99)
p < .001 (unadjusted)
rrr= −.08
re = −.11
Openness RR = 1.005 (0.970,
1.040)
p = .014
rrr= .01
re = .08
Agreeableness RR = 0.964 (0.930,
1.000)
p = .011
rrr= −.06
re = −.09
Conscientiousness RR = 0.968 (0.94,
0.99)
p < .001
rrr= −.07
re = −.11

Note. Confidence intervals are given in parentheses. HR = hazard ratio; RR = relative risk ratio; OR = odds ratio; rrr = correlation estimated from the rate ratio; rhr = correlation estimated from the hazard ratio; ror = correlation estimated from the odds ratio; rB = correlation estimated from a beta weight and standard error; re = requivalent (correlation estimated from the reported p value and sample size); FEV = forced expiratory volume; CHD = coronary heart disease; SES =socioeconomic status; BMI =body-ass index; ADLs =activities of daily living; MMSE =Mini Mental State Examination.

a

The direction of the correlation was derived by choosing a positive pole for each dimension (high Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness) and assuming that each dimension, with the exception of Neuroticism, would be negatively related to mortality in its positive manifestation.

b

Type D personality was categorized as a Neuroticism measure as it correlates more consistently with high Neuroticism (De Fruyt & Denollet, 2002), though it should be noted that it has strong correlations with low Extraversion, low Agreeableness, and low Conscientiousness.

c

On the basis of the correlations presented in Martin and Friedman (2000), cheerfulness was categorized as a measure of Agreeableness.

d

Rationality was not categorized into the Big Five because it measures suppression of aggression, which does not easily fall into one of the five broad domains.

e

The discrepancy in the Hazard ratios results from the fact that the Neuroticism scores were continuous and the Conscientiousness scores were trichotomized.

Similarly, several studies have shown that dispositions reflecting Positive Emotionality or Extraversion were associated with longevity. For example, nuns who scored higher on an index of Positive Emotionality in young adulthood tended to live longer, even when controlling for age, education, and linguistic ability (an aspect of cognitive ability; Danner, Snowden, & Friesen, 2001). Similarly, Optimism was related to higher rates of survival following head and neck cancer (Allison, Guichard, Fung, & Gilain, 2003). In contrast, several studies reported that Neuroticism and Pessimism were associated with increases in one’s risk for premature mortality (Abas, Hotopf, & Prince, 2002; Denollet et al., 1996; Schulz, Bookwala, Knapp, Scheier, & Williamson, 1996; Wilson, Mendes de Leon, Bienias, Evans, & Bennett, 2004). It should be noted, however, that two studies reported a protective effect of high Neuroticism (Korten et al., 1999; Weiss & Costa, 2005).

The domain of Agreeableness showed a less clear association to mortality, with some studies showing a protective effect of high Agreeableness (Wilson et al., 2004) and others showing that high Agreeableness contributed to mortality (Friedman et al., 1993). With respect to the domain of Openness to Experience, two studies showed that Openness or facets of Openness, such as creativity, had little or no relation to mortality (Osler et al., 2002; Wilson et al., 2004).

Because aggregating all personality traits into one overall effect size washes out important distinctions among different trait domains, we examined the effect of specific trait domains by aggregating studies within four categories: Conscientiousness, Positive Emotion/Extraversion, Neuroticism/Negative Emotion, and Hostility/Disagreeableness.5 Our Conscientiousness domain included four studies that linked Conscientiousness to mortality. Because only two of these studies reported the information necessary to compute an average relative risk ratio, we only examined the correlation metric. When translated into a correlation metric, the average effect size for Conscientiousness was −.09 (CIs = −.12 and −.05), indicating a protective effect. Our Extraversion/Positive Emotion domain included six studies that examined the effect of extraversion, positive emotion, and optimism. The average relative risk ratio for the low Extraversion/Positive Emotion was 1.04 (CIs = 1.00 and 1.10) with a corresponding correlation effect size for high Extraversion/Positive Emotion being −.07 (−.11, −.03), with the latter showing a statistically significant protective effect of Extraversion/Positive Emotion. Our Negative Emotionality domain included twelve studies that examined the effect of neuroticism, pessimism, mental instability, and sense of coherence. The average relative risk ratio for the Negative Emotionality domain was 1.15 (CIs = 1.04 and 1.26), and the corresponding correlation effect size was .05 (CIs = .02 and .08). Thus, Neuroticism was associated with a diminished life span. Nineteen studies reported relations between Hostility/Disagreeableness and all-cause mortality, with notable heterogeneity in the effects across studies. The risk ratio population estimate showed an effect equivalent to, if not larger than, the remaining personality domains (risk ratio = 1.14; CIs = 1.06 and 1.23). With the correlation metric, this effect translated into a small but statistically significant effect of .04 (CIs = .02 and .06), indicating that hostility was positively associated with mortality. Thus, the specific personality traits of Conscientiousness, Positive Emotionality/Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Hostility/Disagreeableness were stronger predictors of mortality than was SES when effects were translated into a correlation metric. The effect of personality traits on mortality appears to be equivalent to IQ, although the additive effect of multiple trait domains on mortality may well exceed that of IQ.

Why would personality traits predict mortality? Personality traits may affect health and ultimately longevity through at least three distinct processes (Contrada, Cather, & O’Leary, 1999; Pressman & Cohen, 2005; Rozanski, Blumenthal, & Kaplan, 1999; T.W. Smith, 2006). First, personality differences may be related to pathogenesis or mechanisms that promote disease. This has been evaluated most directly in studies relating various facets of Hostility/Disagreeableness to greater reactivity in response to stressful experiences (T.W. Smith & Gallo, 2001) and in studies relating low Extraversion to neuroendocrine and immune functioning (Miller, Cohen, Rabin, Skoner, & Doyle, 1999) and greater susceptibility to colds (Cohen, Doyle, Turner, Alper, & Skoner, 2003a, 2003b). Second, personality traits may be related to physical-health outcomes because they are associated with health-promoting or health-damaging behaviors. For example, individuals high in Extraversion may foster social relationships, social support, and social integration, all of which are positively associated with health outcomes (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000). In contrast, individuals low in Conscientiousness may engage in a variety of health-risk behaviors such as smoking, unhealthy eating habits, lack of exercise, unprotected sexual intercourse, and dangerous driving habits (Bogg & Roberts, 2004). Third, personality differences may be related to reactions to illness. This includes a wide class of behaviors, such as the ways individuals cope with illness (e.g., Scheier & Carver, 1993), reduce stress, and adhere to prescribed treatments (Kenford et al., 2002).

These processes linking personality traits to physical health are not mutually exclusive. Moreover, different personality traits may affect physical health via different processes. For example, facets of Disagreeableness may be most directly linked to disease processes, facets of low Conscientiousness may be implicated in health-damaging behaviors, and facets of Neuroticism may contribute to ill-health by shaping reactions to illness. In addition, it is likely that the impact of personality differences on health varies across the life course. For example, Neuroticism may have a protective effect on mortality in young adulthood, as individuals who are more neurotic tend to avoid accidents in adolescence and young adulthood (Lee, Wadsworth, & Hotopf, 2006). It is apparent from the extant research that personality traits influence outcomes at all stages of the health process, but much more work remains to be done to specify the processes that account for these effects.

The Predictive Validity of Personality Traits for Divorce

Next, we considered the role that SES, cognitive ability, and personality traits play in divorce. Because there were fewer studies examining these issues, we included prospective studies of SES, IQ, and personality that did not control for many background variables.

In terms of SES and IQ, we found 11 studies that showed a wide range of associations with divorce and marriage (see Table 3).6 For example, the SES of the couple in one study was unsystematically related to divorce (Tzeng & Mare, 1995). In contrast, Kurdek (1993) reported relatively large, protective effects for education and income for both men and women. Because not all these studies reported relative risk ratios, we computed an aggregate using the correlation metric and found the relation between SES and divorce was −.05 (CIs = −.08 and − .02), which indicates a significant protective effect of SES on divorce across these studies. Contradictory patterns were found for the two studies that predicted divorce and marital patterns from measures of cognitive ability. Taylor et al. (2005) reported that IQ was positively related to the possibility of male participants ever marrying but was negatively related to the possibility of female participants ever marrying. Data drawn from the Mills Longitudinal study (Helson, 2006) showed conflicting patterns of associations between verbal and mathematical aptitude and divorce. Because there were only two studies, we did not examine the average effects of IQ on divorce.

TABLE 3.

SES and IQ Effects on Divorce

Study N Outcome Length of
study
Control variables Predictor Results Est. r
Amato & Rogers, 1997 1,742 couples from the Panel
Study of Marital Instability
over the Life Course
Divorce 12 years Age at marriage, prior
cohabitation, ethnicity,
years married, church
attendance, education,
employment, husband’s
income, remarriage,
parents divorced
Wife’s income p = .01 re = .06
Bentler & Newcomb, 1978 77 couples (53 males, 24
females)
Divorce 4 years Women’s education
occupation
p = .05
p = .05
re = −.22
re = −.22
Fergusson, Horwood, & Shannon, 1984 1,002 families from the
Christchurch Child
Development Study
Family breakdown 5 years Maternal age, family size,
church attendance,
marriage type, length of
marriage, planning of
pregnancy
SES T = 2.86 rt = −.09
Helson, 2006 98 women Divorce 31 years SAT Verbal
SAT Math
r = −.06
r = .08
Holley, Yabiku, & Benin, 2006 670 mothers from the
Intergenerational Study of
Parents and Children
Divorce 13 years Age at marriage, religion,
church attendance,
previous cohabitation,
number of children
Similarities subtest
from WAIS
t = −3.02 rt = −.12
Jalovaara, 2001 766,637 first marriages from
Finland
Divorce 2 years Duration of marriage, wife’s
age at marriage, family
composition, degree of
urbanization
Wife’s high
education
Wife’s low
occupational class
Wife’s high income
HR = 0.69
(0.66, 0.73)
HR = 1.34
(1.27, 1.42)
HR = 1.03
(0.92, 1.14)
rhr = −.02

rhr = .01

rhr = .00
Husband’s high
education
Husband’s low
occupational class
Husband’s high
income
HR = 0.66
(0.63, 0.69)
HR = 1.51
(1.44, 1.58)
HR = 0.55
(0.51, 0.58)
rhr = −.02

rhr = .02

rhr = −.02
Kurdek, 1993 286 couples Divorce 5 years High education
(husband)
F(1, 284) =
30.28,
p <
.0000000008
rF = −.31
re = −.34
High income
(husband)
F(1, 284) =
9.32,
p = .0025
rF = −.18
re = −.18
High income (wife) F(1, 284) =
5.11,
p = .025
rF = −.13
re = −.13
Orbuch, Veroff, Hassan, & Horrocks, 2002 373 couples
Divorce
14 years
Race
Years education
(wife)
Household income
Years of education
(husband)
B = −.33 (.06)
p = .001
B = .00 (.01)
B = −.20 (.06)
p = .001
rB = −.28
re = −.17
rB = .00
rB = −.17
re = −.17
A.W. Smith & Meitz, 1985 3,737 families from the Panel
Study of Income Dynamics
Divorce 10 years Education level p = .001 re = −.05
Taylor et al, 2005 883 from the Scottish Mental
Survey and Midspan studies
Ever married 39 years Social class IQ OR men = 1.21
(0.85–1.73)
p = .23
ror = .04
re = .04
OR women =
0.50 (0.32–0.78)
p = .002
ror = −.17
re = −.17
IQ Social class OR men = 1.25
(0.92–1.68)
p = .15
ror = .06
re = .06
OR women =
0.67 (0.49–0.92)
p = .015
ror = −.14
re = −.13
Tzeng & Mare, 1995 17,024 from NLSY, NLSYM,
and NLSYW studies
Annual probability
of marital disruption
9–15 Years Age at marriage, presence
of children, family status
while growing up, number
of marriages, race, cohort
Couple education Z = −6.8 rz = −.05
Couple income Z = .51 rz = .00

Note. Confidence intervals are given in parentheses. SES = socioeconomic status; HR = hazard ratio; RR = relative risk ratio; OR = odds ratio; rz = correlation estimated from the z score and sample size; ror = correlation estimated from the odds ratio; rF = correlation estimated from F test; rB = correlation estimated from the reported unstandardized beta weight and standard error; re = requivalent (correlation estimated from the reported p value and sample size); WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; NLSY = National Longitudinal Study of Youth; NLSYM = National Longitudinal Study of Young Men; NLSYW = National Longitudinal Study of Young Women.

Table 4 shows the data from thirteen prospective studies testing whether personality traits predicted divorce. Traits associated with the domain of Neuroticism, such as being anxious and overly sensitive, increased the probability of experiencing divorce (Kelly & Conley, 1987; Tucker, Kressin, Spiro, & Ruscio, 1998). In contrast, those individuals who were more conscientious and agreeable tended to remain longer in their marriages and avoided divorce (Kelly & Conley, 1987; Kinnunen & Pulkkenin, 2003; Roberts & Bogg, 2004). Although these studies did not control for as many factors as the health studies, the time spans over which the studies were carried out were impressive (e.g., 45 years). We aggregated effects across these studies for the trait domains of Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness with the correlation metric, as too few studies reported relative risk outcomes to warrant aggregating. When so aggregated, the effect of Neuroticism on divorce was .17 (CIs = .12 and .22), the effect of Agreeableness was − .18 (CIs = −.27 and −.09), and the effect of Conscientiousness on divorce was −.13 (CIs = −.17 and −.09). Thus, the predictive effects of these three personality traits on divorce were greater than those found for SES.

TABLE 4.

Personality Traits and Marital Outcomes

Study N Outcome Time Controls Predictors Results Est. r
Bentler & Newcomb, 1978 77 couples (53 males,
24 females)
Divorce 4 years Men’s
extraversion
orderliness
Women’s
clothes consciousness
Congeniality
p = .05
p = .05

p = .05

p = .05
re = .27
re = .27

re = −.40

re = −.40
Caspi, Elder, & Bern, 1987 87 men from the
Berkeley Guidance
Study
Divorce 31 years Childhood ill-temperedness p = .02 re = .25
Huston, Caughlin, Houts, Smith, & George, 2001 152 couples Early divorce Few months after
marriage
Gender, affectional
expression, love,
contrariness,
ambivalence, negativity
Gender, affectional
expression, love,
contrariness,
ambivalence, negativity
Responsiveness
(Agreeableness)


Contrariness (Neuroticism)
F(4, 147) =
4.49,
p <.01

F(4, 147) =
1.29, (p
values not
available)
rF = −.17
re = −.21


rF = .09
Jockin, McGue, & Lykken, 1996 1,490 female and 696
male twins
Ever divorced Cross-sectional Positive Emotionality (women)

Positive Emotionality (men)

Negative Emotionality
(women)
Negative Emotionality (men)

Constraint (women)

Constraint (men)
d = .23
p < .01
d = .21
p < .01
d = .21
p < .01
d = .20
p < .01
d = −.34
p < .01
d = −.20
p < .01
rd = .11
re = .07
rd = .10
re = .10
rd = .10
re = .10
rd = .10
re = .10
rd = −.17
re = −.10
rd = −.10
re = −.10
Kelly & Conley, 1987 556 married men and
women
Marital
compatibility
(divorced versus happily married)
45 years Husband’s Neuroticism
Husband’s impulse control
Wife’s Neuroticism
r = .27
r = −.25
r = .38
r = .27
r = −.25
r = .38
Kinnunen & Pulkkinen, 2003 108 women and 109
men from the Jyvaskyla
Longitudinal Study of
Personality and Social
Development
Divorced versus
intact marriage
at age 36
28, 22, or 9 years Women’s age 8 Aggression
Women’s age 8 Lability
Women’s age 27
Conscientiousness
Women’s age 27 Agreeableness
Men’s age 8 Aggression
Men’s age 8 Compliance
Men’s age 14 Aggression
Men’s age 14 Compliance
Men’s age 27
Conscientiousness
Men’s age 27 Agreeableness
d =.69
d = .43
d = −.12

d = −.54
d = .68
d = .59
d = .57
d = .74
d = .82

d = .61
rd = .30
rd = .19
rd = −.05

rd = −.24
rd = .26
rd = .23
rd = .22
rd = .28
rd = .31

rd = .24
Kurdek, 1993 286 couples Divorce 5 years Neuroticism (husband)


Neuroticism (wife)


Conscientiousness (husband)


Conscientiousness (wife)


Positive Emotionality
(husband)
F(1, 284) =
17.34,
p = .000005
F(1, 284) =
14.21,
p = .0002
F(1, 284) =
−2.78,
p = .096
F(1, 284) =
−4.16,
p = 042
d = .21
p < .01
rF = .25
re = .24

re = .22
rF = .22

re = −.10
rF = −.10

rF = −.12
re = −.12

rd = .10
re = .10
Lawrence & Bradbury, 2001 60 couples from Los
Angeles
Divorce 4 years Aggressiveness OR = 2.37
p = .06
re = .24
ro = .23
Loeb, 1966 639 college students Divorce 13 years Women’s MMPI psychopathic
deviancy
Men’s MMPI psychopathic
deviancy
Men’s MMPI hypochondriasis
Men’s MMPI hysteria
Men’s MMPI schizophrenia
p < .025

p < .025

p < .005
p < .025
p < .05
re = .13

re = .13

re = .16
re = .13
re = .11
McCranie & Kahan, 1986 431 physicians Number of
divorces
25 years MMPI psychopathic deviancy r = .13 r = .13
Roberts & Bogg, 2004 99 women from the
Mills Longitudinal
Study
Ever divorced 22 years Responsibility r = −.21 r = −.21
Skolnick, 1981 122 members of the
IHD longitudinal
studies
Divorce versus
satisfied
marriage
Cognitively invested
Emotionally aggressive
Nurturant
Under controlled
p = .06
p = .08
p = .06
p = .008
re = −.17
re = .16
re = −.17
re = .24
Tucker et al., 1998 773 from the Normative
Aging Study
Divorce 26 years Age at marriage,
education
Inadequacy


Anxiety


Sensitivity


Anger


Tension
OR = 2.40
(1.36, 4.35)
p < .01
OR = 2.80
(1.55, 5.15)
p < .001
OR = 2.80
(1.50, 5.25)
p < .01
OR = 2.70
(1.54, 4.71)
p < .001
OR = 1.20
(0.61, 2.51)
ror = .11
re = .09

ror = .12
re = .12

ror = .12
re = .09

ror = .13
re = .12

ror = .02
968 members of the
Terman Life Cycle
Study
Divorce 53 to 78 years Sex, education, age at
marriage
Conscientiousness





Perseverance





Sympathy





Not egotistical
OR parent
rating = 0.92
(0.84, 1.01)
OR teacher
rating = 0.92
(0.83, 1.01)
OR parent
rating = 1.01
(0.92, 1.11)
OR teacher
rating = 0.95
(0.86, 1.05)
OR parent
rating = 0.94
(0.85, 1.02)
OR teacher
rating = 0.95
(0.84, 1.07)
OR parent
rating = 0.95
(0.87,1.03)
OR teacher
rating = 0.96
(0.87, 1.05)
ror = −.07


ror = −.08


ror = .01


ror = −.05


ror = −.06


ror = −.04


ror = −.05


ror = −.04

Note. Confidence intervals are given in parentheses. HR = hazard ratio; RR = relative risk ratio; OR = odds ratio; rd = Correlation estimated from the d score; ror = correlation estimated from the odds ratio; rF = correlation estimated from F test; re = requivalent (correlation estimated from the reported p value and sample size); MMPI = Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory; IHS = Institute of Human Development.

Why would personality traits lead to divorce or conversely marital stability? The most likely reason is because personality traits help shape the quality of long-term relationships. For example, Neuroticism is one of the strongest and most consistent personality predictors of relationship dissatisfaction, conflict, abuse, and ultimately dissolution (Karney & Bradbury, 1995). Sophisticated studies that include dyads (not just individuals) and multiple methods (not just self reports) increasingly demonstrate that the links between personality traits and relationship processes are more than simply an artifact of shared method variance in the assessment of these two domains (Donnellan, Conger, & Bryant, 2004; Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2000; Watson, Hubbard, & Wiese, 2000). One study that followed a sample of young adults across their multiple relationships in early adulthood discovered that the influence of Negative Emotionality on relationship quality showed cross-relationship generalization; that is, it predicted the same kinds of experiences across relationships with different partners (Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2002).

An important goal for future research will be to uncover the proximal relationship-specific processes that mediate personality effects on relationship outcomes (Reiss, Capobianco, & Tsai, 2002). Three processes merit attention. First, personality traits influence people’s exposure to relationship events. For example, people high in Neuroticism may be more likely to be exposed to daily conflicts in their relationships (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995; Suls & Martin, 2005). Second, personality traits shape people’s reactions to the behavior of their partners. For example, disagreeable individuals may escalate negative affect during conflict (e.g., Gottman, Coan, Carrere, & Swanson, 1998). Similarly, agreeable people may be better able to regulate emotions during interpersonal conflicts (Jensen-Campbell & Graziano, 2001). Cognitive processes also factor in creating trait-correlated experiences (Snyder & Stukas, 1999). For example, highly neurotic individuals may overreact to minor criticism from their partner, believe they are no longer loved when their partner does not call, or assume infidelity on the basis of mere flirtation. Third, personality traits evoke behaviors from partners that contribute to relationship quality. For example, people high in Neuroticism and low in Agreeableness may be more likely to express behaviors identified as detrimental to relationships such as criticism, contempt, defensiveness, and stonewalling (Gottman, 1994).

The Predictive Validity of Personality Traits for Educational and Occupational Attainment

The role of personality traits in occupational attainment has been studied sporadically in longitudinal studies over the last few decades. In contrast, the roles of SES and IQ have been studied exhaustively by sociologists in their programmatic research on the antecedents to status attainment. In their seminal work, Blau and Duncan (1967) conceptualized a model of status attainment as a function of the SES of an individual’s father. Researchers at the University of Wisconsin added what they considered social-psychological factors (Sewell, Haller, & Portes, 1969). In this Wisconsin model, attainment is a function of parental SES, cognitive abilities, academic performance, occupational and educational aspirations, and the role of significant others (Haller & Portes, 1973). Each factor in the model has been found to be positively related to occupational attainment (Hauser, Tsai, & Sewell, 1983). The key question here is to what extent SES and IQ predict educational and occupational attainment holding constant the remaining factors.

A great deal of research has validated the structure and content of the Wisconsin model (Sewell & Hauser, 1980; Sewell & Hauser, 1992), and rather than compiling these studies, which are highly similar in structure and findings, we provide representative findings from a study that includes three replications of the model (Jencks, Crouse, & Mueser, 1983). As can be seen in Table 5, childhood socioeconomic indicators, such as father’s occupational status and mother’s education, are related to outcomes, such as grades, educational attainment, and eventual occupational attainment, even after controlling for the remaining variables in the Wisconsin model. The average beta weight of SES and education was .09.7 Parental income had a stronger effect, with an average beta weight of .14 across these three studies. Cognitive abilities were even more powerful predictors of occupational attainment, with an average beta weight of .27.

TABLE 5.

SES, IQ, and Status Attainment

Study N Outcome Time span Control variables Predictor Results
Jencks, Crouse, & Meuser, 1983 1,789 Occupational
attainment
7 years Father and mother’s
SES, earnings, aptitude,
grades, friends
education plans,
educational and
occupational
aspirations, education
Father’s SES
Mother’s education
Parental income
IQ
β = .15
β = .09
β = .11
β = .31
Earnings



Education
Father’s SES
Mother’s education
Parent’s income
IQ
Father’s SES
Mothers education
Parent’s income
IQ
β = −.01
β = .01
β = .16
β = .14
β = .13
β = .13
β = .14
β = .37

Note. SES = socioeconomic status.

Do personality traits contribute to the prediction of occupational attainment even when intelligence and socioeconomic background are taken into account? As there are far fewer studies linking personality traits directly to indices of occupational attainment, such as prestige and income, we also included prospective studies examining the impact of personality traits on related outcomes such as long-term unemployment and occupational stability. The studies listed in Table 6 attest to the fact that personality traits predict all of these work-related outcomes. For example, adolescent ratings of Neuroticism, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness predicted occupational status 46 years later, even after controlling for childhood IQ (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999). The weighted-average beta weight across the studies in Table 6 was .23 (CIs = .14 and .32), indicating that the modal effect size of personality traits was comparable with the effect of childhood SES and IQ on similar outcomes.8

TABLE 6.

Personality Traits and Occupational Attainment

Study N Outcome Time span Control variables Predictor Results
Caspi et al, 1987 182 members of the
Berkeley Guidance Study
Occupational attainment

Erratic work life
31 years

31 years
IQ, education

IQ, education,
occupational attainment
Childhood ill-
temperedness
Childhood ill-
temperedness
β = −.10

β = .45
Caspi, Elder, & Bern, 1988 73 men from the Berkeley
Guidance Study





83 women from the
Berkeley Guidance Study
Age at entry into a stable
career

Occupational attainment



Stable participation in the
labor market
11 years


11 years



11 years
SES, education,
childhood ill-
temperedness
Age at entry into stable
career, education,
childhood ill-
temperedness
SES, education,
childhood ill-
temperedness
Childhood shyness


Childhood shyness



Childhood shyness
β = .27


β = −.05



β = −.19
Helson & Roberts, 1992 63 women from the Mills
Longitudinal Study
Occupational attainment 16 years Work aspirations,
husband’s individuality
Individuality β = .34
Helson, Roberts, & Agronick, 1995 120 women from the Mills
Longitudinal Study
Occupational creativity 31 years SAT Verbal scores, status
aspirations
Creative temperament β = .44
Judge et al., 1999 118 Members from the IHD
longitudinal studies
Extrinsic career success 46 years IQ Neuroticism
Extraversion
Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
β = −.21
β = .27
β = −.32
β = .44
Kokko & Pulkkinen, 2000 311 members of the
Jyvaskyla Longitudinal
Study
Long-term unemployment
between ages 27 and 36
19 years Aggression, child-
centered parenting,
school maladjustment,
problem drinking, lack of
occupational alternatives
at age 27
Age 8 prosociality
(emotionally stable,
reliable, friendly)
β = −.37
Luster & McAdoo, 1996 123 members of the Perry
Preschool sample
Age 27 income 22 years Mother’s education,
maternal involvement in
kindergarten, preschool
attendance, academic
motivation, IQ score, 8th
grade achievement,
educational attainment at
age 27
Age 5 personal behavior
(teacher ratings of not lying
and cheating, not using
obscene words)
β = .23
Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003a 859 members of the
Dunedin Longitudinal
Study
Occupational attainment 8 years IQ, SES Negative Emotionality
Constraint
Positive Emotionality
β = −.17
β = .18
β = .13
Seibert, Kraimer, & Crant, 2001

Tharenou, 2001
180 alumni from
Midwestern University
2,431 Australian managers
Salary progression

Advancement in
management
2 years

Organizational sector,
organization size,
marriage, number of
children, relocated,
changed organizations,
gender, age, tenure,
education level, training,
challenging work,
occupation type,
managerial promotions,
managerial aspirations,
mentor career support,
career encouragement,
male hierarchy, transition
level
Proactive personality

Masculinity
r = .11

r = .05

Note. SES = socioeconomic status; IHD = Institute of Human Development.

Why are personality traits related to achievement in educational and occupational domains? The personality processes involved may vary across different stages of development, and at least five candidate processes deserve research scrutiny (Roberts, 2006). First, the personality-to-achievement associations may reflect “attraction” effects or “active niche-picking,” whereby people choose educational and work experiences whose qualities are concordant with their own personalities. For example, people who are more conscientious may prefer conventional jobs, such as accounting and farming (Gottfredson, Jones, & Holland, 1993). People who are more extraverted may prefer jobs that are described as social or enterprising, such as teaching or business management (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997). Moreover, extraverted individuals are more likely to assume leadership roles in multiple settings (Judge, Bono, Ilies, & Gerhardt, 2002). In fact, all of the Big Five personality traits have substantial relations with better performance when the personality predictor is appropriately aligned with work criteria (Hogan & Holland, 2003). This indicates that if people find jobs that fit with their dispositions they will experience greater levels of job performance, which should lead to greater success, tenure, and satisfaction across the life course (Judge et al., 1999).

Second, personality-to-achievement associations may reflect “recruitment effects,” whereby people are selected into achievement situations and are given preferential treatment on the basis of their personality characteristics. These recruitment effects begin to appear early in development. For example, children’s personality traits begin to influence their emerging relationships with teachers at a young age (Birch & Ladd, 1998). In adulthood, job applicants who are more extraverted, conscientious, and less neurotic are liked better by interviewers and are more often recommended for the job (Cook, Vance, & Spector, 2000).

Third, personality traits may affect work outcomes because people take an active role in shaping their work environment (Roberts, 2006). For example, leaders have tremendous power to shape the nature of the organization by hiring, firing, and promoting individuals. Cross-sectional studies of groups have shown that leaders’ conscientiousness and cognitive ability affect decision making and treatment of subordinates (LePine, Hollenbeck, Ilgen, & Hedlund, 1997). Individuals who are not leaders or supervisors may shape their work to better fit themselves through job crafting (Wrzesniewski & Dutton, 2001) or job sculpting (Bell & Staw, 1989). They can change their day-to-day work environments through changing the tasks they do, organizing their work differently, or changing the nature of the relationships they maintain with others (Wrzesniewski & Dutton, 2001). Presumably these changes in their work environments lead to an increase in the fit between personality and work. In turn, increased fit with one’s environment is associated with elevated performance (Harms, Roberts, & Winter, 2006).

Fourth, some personality-to-achievement associations emerge as consequences of “attrition” or “deselection pressures,” whereby people leave achievement settings (e.g., schools or jobs) that do not fit with their personality or are released from these settings because of their trait-correlated behaviors (Cairns & Cairns, 1994). For example, longitudinal evidence from different countries shows that children who exhibit a combination of poor self-control and high irritability or antagonism are at heightened risk of unemployment (Caspi, Wright, Moffitt, & Silva, 1998; Kokko, Bergman, & Pulkkinen, 2003; Kokko & Pulkkinen, 2000).

Fifth, personality-to-achievement associations may emerge as a result of direct effects of personality on performance. Personality traits may promote certain kinds of task effectiveness; there is some evidence that this occurs in part via the processing of information. For example, higher positive emotions facilitate the efficient processing of complex information and are associated with creative problem solving (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999). In addition to these effects on task effectiveness, personality may directly affect other aspects of work performance, such as interpersonal interactions (Hurtz & Donovan, 2000). Personality traits may also directly influence performance motivation; for example, Conscientiousness consistently predicts stronger goal setting and self-efficacy, whereas Neuroticism predicts these motivations negatively (Erez & Judge, 2001; Judge & Ilies, 2002).

GENERAL DISCUSSION

It is abundantly clear from this review that specific personality traits predict important life outcomes, such as mortality, divorce, and success in work. Depending on the sample, trait, and outcome, people with specific personality characteristics are more likely to experience important life outcomes even after controlling for other factors. Moreover, when compared with the effects reported for SES and cognitive abilities, the predictive validities of personality traits do not appear to be markedly different in magnitude. In fact, as can be seen in Figures 13, in many cases, the evidence supports the conclusion that personality traits predict these outcomes better than SES does. Despite these impressive findings, a few limitations and qualifications must be kept in mind when interpreting these data.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Average effects (in the correlation metric) of low socioeconomic status (SES), low IQ, low Conscientiousness (C), low Extraversion/Positive Emotion(E/PE), Neuroticism (N), and low Agreeableness (A) on mortality. Error bars represent standard error.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Average effects (in the standardized beta weight metric) of high socioeconomic status (SES), high parental income, high IQ, and high personality trait scores on occupational outcomes.

The requirement that we only examine the incremental validity of personality measures after controlling for SES and cognitive abilities, though clearly the most stringent test of the relevance of personality traits, is also arbitrarily tough. In fact, controlling for variables that are assumed to be nuisance factors can obscure important relations (Meehl, 1971). For example, SES, cognitive abilities, and personality traits may determine life outcomes through indirect rather than direct pathways. Consider cognitive abilities. These are only modest predictors of occupational attainment when “all other factors are controlled,” but they play a much more important, indirect role through their effect on educational attainment. Students with higher cognitive abilities tend to obtain better grades and go on to achieve more in the educational sphere across a range of disciplines (Kuncel, Crede, & Thomas, 2007; Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2001, 2004); in turn, educational attainment is the best predictor of occupational attainment. This observation about cumulative indirect effects applies equally well to SES and personality traits.

Furthermore, the effect sizes associated with SES, cognitive abilities, and personality traits were all uniformly small-to-medium in size. This finding is entirely consistent with those from other reviews showing that most psychological constructs have effect sizes in the range between .10 and .40 on a correlational scale (Meyer et al., 2001). Our hope is that reviews like this one can help adjust the norms researchers hold for what the modal effect size is in psychology and related fields. Studies are often disparaged for having small effects as if it is not the norm. Moreover, small effect sizes are often criticized without any understanding of their practical significance. Practical significance can only be determined if we ground our research by both predicting consequential outcomes, such as mortality, and by translating the results into a metric that is clearly understandable, such as years lost or number of deaths. Correlations and ratio statistics do not provide this type of information. On the other hand, some researchers have translated their results into metrics that most individuals can grasp. As we noted in the introduction, Rosenthal (1990) showed that taking aspirin prevented approximately 85 heart attacks in the patients of 10,845 physicians despite the meager −.03 correlation between this practice and the outcome of having a heart attack. Several other studies in our review provided similar benchmarks. Hardarson et al., (2001) showed that 148 fewer people died in their high education group (out of 869) than in their low education group, despite the effect size being equal to a correlation of −.05. Danner et al. (2001) showed that the association between positive emotion and longevity was associated with a gain of almost 7 years of additional life, despite having an average effect size of around .20. Of course, our ability to draw these types of conclusions necessitates grounding our research in more practical outcomes and their respective metrics.

There is one salient difference between many of the studies of SES and cognitive abilities and the studies focusing on personality traits. The typical sample in studies of the long-term effect of personality traits was a sample of convenience or was distinctly unrepresentative. In contrast, many of the studies of SES and cognitive ability included nationally representative and/or remarkably large samples (e.g., 500,000 participants). Therefore, the results for SES and cognitive abilities are generalizable, whereas it is more difficult to generalize findings from personality research. Perhaps the situation will improve if future demographers include personality measures in large surveys of the general population.

Recommendations

One of the challenges of incorporating personality measures in large studies is the cost–benefit trade off involved with including a thorough assessment of personality traits in a reasonably short period of time. Because most personality inventories include many items, researchers may be pressed either to eliminate them from their studies or to use highly abbreviated measures of personality traits. The latter practice has become even more common now that most personality researchers have concluded that personality traits can be represented within five to seven broad domains (Goldberg, 1993b; Saucier, 2003). The temptation is to include a brief five-factor instrument under the assumption that this will provide good coverage of the entire range of personality traits. However, the use of short, broad bandwidth measures can lead to substantial decreases in predictive validity (Goldberg, 1993a), because short measures of the Big Five lack the breadth and depth of longer personality inventories. In contrast, research has shown that the predictive validity of personality measures increases when one uses a well-elaborated measure with many lower order facets (Ashton, 1998; Mershon & Gorsuch, 1988; Paunonen, 1998; Paunonen & Ashton, 2001).

However, research participants do not have unlimited time, and researchers may need advice on the selection of optimal measures of personality traits. One solution is to pay attention to previous research and focus on those traits that have been found to be related to the specific outcomes under study instead of using an omnibus personality inventory. For example, given the clear and consistent finding that the personality trait of Conscientiousness is related to health behaviors and mortality (e.g., Bogg & Roberts, 2004; Friedman, 2000), it would seem prudent to measure this trait well if one wanted to control for this factor or include it in any study of health and mortality. Moreover, it appears that specific facets of this domain, such as self-control and conventionality, are more relevant to health than are other facets such as orderliness (Bogg & Roberts, 2004). If researchers are truly interested in assessing personality traits well, then they should invest the time necessary for the task. This entails moving away from expedient surveys to more in-depth assessments. Finally, if one truly wants to assess personality traits well, then researchers should use multiple methods for this purpose and should not rely solely on self-reports (Eid & Diener, 2006).

We also recommend that researchers not equate all individual differences with personality traits. Personality psychologists also study constructs such as motivation, interests, emotions, values, identities, life stories, and self-regulation (see Mayer, 2005, and Roberts & Wood, 2006, for reviews). Moreover, these different domains of personality are only modestly correlated (e.g., Ackerman & Heggested, 1997; Roberts & Robins, 2000). Thus, there are a wide range of additional constructs that may have independent effects on important life outcomes that are waiting to be studied.

Conclusions

In light of increasingly robust evidence that personality matters for a wide range of life outcomes, researchers need to turn their attention to several issues. First, we need to know more about the processes through which personality traits shape individuals’ functioning over time. Simply documenting that links exist between personality traits and life outcomes does not clarify the mechanisms through which personality exerts its effects. In this article, we have suggested a number of potential processes that may be at work in the domains of health, relationships, and educational and occupational success. Undoubtedly, other personality processes will turn out to influence these outcomes as well.

Second, we need a greater understanding of the relationship between personality and the social environmental factors already known to affect health and development. Looking over the studies reviewed above, one can see that specific personality traits such as Conscientiousness predict occupational and marital outcomes that, in turn, predict longevity. Thus, it may be that Conscientiousness has both direct and indirect effects on mortality, as it contributes to following life paths that afford better health, and may also directly affect the ways in which people handle health-related issues, such as whether they exercise or eat a healthy diet (Bogg & Roberts, 2004). One idea that has not been entertained is the potential synergistic relation between personality traits and social environmental factors. It may be the case that the combination of certain personality traits and certain social conditions creates a potent cocktail of factors that either promotes or undermines specific outcomes. Finally, certain social contexts may wash out the effect of individual difference factors, and, in turn, people possessing certain personality characteristics may be resilient to seemingly toxic environmental influences. A systematic understanding of the relations between personality traits and social environmental factors associated with important life outcomes would be very helpful.

Third, the present results drive home the point that we need to know much more about the development of personality traits at all stages in the life course. How does a person arrive in adulthood as an optimistic or conscientious person? If personality traits affect the ways that individuals negotiate the tasks they face across the course of their lives, then the processes contributing to the development of those traits are worthy of study (Caspi & Shiner, 2006; Caspi & Shiner, in press; Rothbart & Bates, 2006). However, there has been a tendency in personality and developmental research to focus on personality traits as the causes of various outcomes without fully considering personality differences as an outcome worthy of study (Roberts, 2005). In contrast, research shows that personality traits continue to change in adulthood (e.g., Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006) and that these changes may be important for health and mortality. For example, changes in personality traits such as Neuroticism have been linked to poor health outcomes and even mortality (Mroczek & Spiro, 2007).

Fourth, our results raise fundamental questions about how personality should be addressed in prevention and intervention efforts. Skeptical readers may doubt the relevance of the present results for prevention and intervention in light of the common assumption that personality is highly stable and immutable. However, personality traits do change in adulthood (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006) and can be changed through therapeutic intervention (De Fruyt, Van Leeuwen, Bagby, Rolland, & Rouillon, 2006). Therefore, one possibility would be to focus on socializing factors that may affect changes in personality traits, as the resulting changes would then be leveraged across multiple domains of life. Further, the findings for personality traits should be of considerable interest to professionals dedicated to promoting healthy, happy marriages and socioeconomic success. Some individuals will clearly be at a heightened risk of problems in these life domains, and it may be possible to target prevention and intervention efforts to the subsets of individuals at the greatest risk. Such research can likewise inform the processes that need to be targeted in prevention and intervention. As we gain greater understanding of how personality exerts its effects on adaptation, we will achieve new insights into the most relevant processes to change. Moreover, it is essential to recognize that it may be possible to improve individuals’ lives by targeting those processes without directly changing the personality traits driving those processes (e.g., see Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2005, for an interesting example of how this may occur). In all prevention and intervention work, it will be important to attend to the possibility that most personality traits can have positive or negative effects, depending on the outcomes in question, the presence of other psychological attributes, and the environmental context (Caspi & Shiner, 2006; Shiner, 2005).

Personality research has had a contentious history, and there are still vestiges of doubt about the importance of personality traits. We thus reviewed the comparative predictive validity of personality traits, SES, and IQ across three objective criteria: mortality, divorce, and occupational attainment. We found that personality traits are just as important as SES and IQ in predicting these important life outcomes. We believe these metaanalytic findings should quell lingering doubts. The closing of a chapter in the history of personality psychology is also an opportunity to open a new chapter. We thus invite new research to test and document how personality traits “work” to shape life outcomes. A useful lead may be taken from cognate research on social disparities in health (Adler & Snibbe, 2003). Just as researchers are seeking to understand how SES “gets under the skin” to influence health, personality researchers need to partner with other branches of psychology to understand how personality traits “get outside the skin” to influence important life outcomes.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Average effects (in the correlation metric) of low socioeconomic status (SES), low Conscientiousness (C), Neuroticism (N), and low Agreeableness (A) on divorce. Error bars represent standard error.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this paper was supported by National Institute of Aging Grants AG19414 and AG20048; National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH49414, MH45070, MH49227; United Kingdom Medical Research Council Grant G0100527; and by grants from the Colgate Research Council. We would like to thank Howard Friedman, David Funder, George Davie Smth, Ian Deary, Chris Fraley, Linda Gottfredson, Josh Jackson, and Ben Karney for their comments on earlier drafts of this article.

Footnotes

1

This situation is in no way particular to epidemiological or medical studies using odds, rate, and hazard ratios as outcomes. The field of psychology reports results in a Babylonian array of test statistics and effect sizes also.

2

The population effects for the rate ratio and correlation metric were not based on identical data because in some cases the authors did not report rate ratio information or did not report enough information to compute a rate ratio and a CI.

3

Most of the studies of SES and mortality were compiled from an exhaustive review of the literature on the effect of childhood SES and mortality (Galobardes et al., 2004). We added several of the largest studies examining the effect of adult SES on mortality (e.g., Steenland et al., 2002), and to these we added the results from the studies on cognitive ability and personality that reported SES effects. We also did standard electronic literature searches using the terms socioeconomic status, cognitive ability, and all-cause mortality. We also examined the reference sections from the list of studies and searched for papers that cited these studies. Experts in the field of epidemiology were also contacted and asked to identify missing studies. The resulting SES data base is representative of the field, and as the effects are based on over 3 million data points, the effect sizes and CIs are very stable. The studies of cognitive ability and mortality represent all of the studies found that reported usable data.

4

We identified studies through electronic searches that included the terms personality traits, extroversion, agreeableness, hostility, conscientiousness, emotional stability, neuroticism, openness to experience, and all-cause mortality. We also identified studies through reference sections of the list of studies and through studies that cited each study. A number of studies were not included in this review because we focused on studies that were prospective and controlled for background factors.

5

We did not examine the domain of Openness to Experience because there were only two studies that tested the association with mortality.

6

We identified studies using electronic searches including the terms divorce, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability. We also identified studies through examining the reference sections of the studies and through studies that cited each study.

7

We did not transform the standardized beta weights into the correlation metric because almost all authors failed to provide the necessary information for the transformation (CIs or standard errors). Therefore, we averaged the results in the beta weight metric instead. As the sampling distribution of beta weights is unknown, we used the formula for the standard error of the partial correlation (√N−k−2) to estimate CIs.

8

In making comparisons between correlations and regression weights, it should be kept in mind that although the two are identical for orthogonal predictors, most regression weights tend to be smaller than the corresponding zero-order validity correlations because of predictor redundancy (R.A. Peterson & Brown, 2005).

REFERENCES

  1. Abas M, Hotopf M, Prince M. Depression and mortality in a high-risk population. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;181:123–128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abelson RP. A variance explanation paradox: When a little is a lot. Psychological Bulletin. 1985;97:129–133. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ackerman PL, Heggestad ED. Intelligence, personality, and interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:219–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, Syme SL. Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist. 1994;49:15–24. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.49.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Adler NE, Snibbe AC. The role of psychosocial processes in explaining the gradient between socioeconomic status and health. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003;12:119–123. [Google Scholar]
  6. Allison PJ, Guichard C, Fung K, Gilain L. Dispositional optimism predicts survival status 1 year after diagnosis in head and neck cancer patients. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2003;21:543–548. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2003.10.092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Almada SJ, Zonderman AB, Shekelle RB, Dyer AR, Daviglus ML, Costa PT, Stamler J. Neuroticism and cynicism and risk of death in middle-aged men: The Western Electric Study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1991;53:165–175. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199103000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Amato PR, Rogers SJ. A longitudinal study of marital problems and subsequent divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1997;59:612–624. [Google Scholar]
  9. Ashby FG, Isen AM, Turken AU. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review. 1999;106:529–550. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.3.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ashton MC. Personality and job performance: The importance of narrow traits. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 1998;19:289–303. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OR, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 154–196. [Google Scholar]
  12. Barefoot JC, Dahlstrom WG, Williams RB. Hostility, CHD incidence, and total mortality: A 25-year follow-up study of 255 physicians. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1983;45:59–63. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198303000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barefoot JC, Dodge KA, Peterson BL, Dahlstrom WG, Williams RB. The Cook-Medley hostility scale: Item content and ability to predict survival. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1989;51:46–57. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198901000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Barefoot JC, Larsen S, von der Lieth L, Schroll M. Hostility, incidence of acute myocardial infarction, and mortality in a sample of older Danish men and women. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1995;142:477–484. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a117663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Barefoot JC, Maynard KE, Beckham JC, Brummett BH, Hooker K, Siegler IC. Trust, health, and longevity. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1998;21:517–526. doi: 10.1023/a:1018792528008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Barefoot JC, Siegler IC, Nowlin JB, Peterson BL, Haney TL, Williams RB. Suspiciousness, health, and mortality: A follow-up stuffy of 500 older adults. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1987;49:450–457. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198709000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bassuk SS, Berkman LF, Amick BC. Socioeconomic status and mortality among the elderly: Findings from four U.S. Communities. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;155:520–533. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.6.520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Beebe-Dimmer J, Lynch JW, Turrell G, Lustgarten S, Raghunathan T, Kaplan GA. Childhood and adult socioeconomic conditions and 31-year mortality risk in women. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;159:481–490. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bell NE, Staw BM. People as sculptors versus sculpture: The roles of personality and personal control in organizations. In: Arthur MB, Hall DT, Lawrence BS, editors. Handbook of career theory. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1989. pp. 232–251. [Google Scholar]
  20. Bentler PM, Newcomb MD. Longitudinal study of marital success and failure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1978;46:1053–1070. [Google Scholar]
  21. Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, Seeman TE. From social integration to health. Social Science Medicine. 2000;51:843–857. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Birch SH, Ladd GW. Children’s interpersonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship. Developmental Psychology. 1998;34:934–946. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Blau PM, Duncan OD. The American occupational structure. New York: Wiley; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  24. Bogg T, Roberts BW. Conscientiousness and health behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130:887–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Bolger N, Zuckerman A. A framework for studying personality in the stress process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:890–902. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bornstein RF. Criterion validity of objective and projective dependency tests: A meta-analytic assessment of behavioral prediction. Psychological Assessment. 1999;11:48–57. [Google Scholar]
  27. Bosworth HB, Schaie KW. Survival effects in cognitive function, cognitive style, and sociodemographic variables in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Experimental Aging Research. 1999;25:121–139. doi: 10.1080/036107399244057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Boyle SH, Williams RB, Mark DB, Brummett BH, Siegler IC, Barefoot JC. Hostility, age, and mortality in a sample of cardiac patients. American Journal of Cardiology. 2005;96:64–66. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2005.02.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Boyle SH, Williams RB, Mark DB, Brummett BH, Siegler IC, Helms MJ, Barefoot JC. Hostility as a predictor of survival in patients with coronary artery disease. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2004;66:629–632. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000138122.93942.4a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Bucher HC, Ragland DR. Socioeconomic indicators and mortality from coronary heart disease and cancer: A 22-year follow-up of middle-aged men. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85:1231–1236. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.9.1231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Cairns RB, Cairns BD. Lifelines and risks: Pathways of youth in our time. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  33. Caspi A, Elder GH, Bern DJ. Moving against the world: Life-course patterns of explosive children. Developmental Psychology. 1987;23:308–313. [Google Scholar]
  34. Caspi A, Elder GH, Bern DJ. Moving away from the world: Life-course patterns of shy children. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24:824–831. [Google Scholar]
  35. Caspi A, Roberts BW, Shiner R. Personality development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2005;56:453–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Caspi A, Shiner RL. Personality development. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 6th ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 300–365. (Vol. Ed.) [Google Scholar]
  37. Caspi A, Shiner RL. Temperament and personality. In: Rutter M, Bishop D, Pine D, Scott S, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Thapar A, editors. Rutter’s child and adolescent psychiatry. 5th ed. London: Blackwell; (in press) [Google Scholar]
  38. Caspi A, Wright BR, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. Early failure in the labor market: Childhood and adolescent predictors of unemployment in the transition to adulthood. American Sociological Preview. 1998;63:424–451. [Google Scholar]
  39. Christensen AJ, Ehlers SL, Wiebe JS, Moran PJ, Raichle K, Ferneyhough K, Lawton WJ. Patient personality and mortality: A 4-year prospective examination of chronic renal insufficiency. Health Psychology. 2002;21:315–320. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.21.4.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Claussen B, Davey-Smith G, Thelle D. Impact of childhood and adulthood socioeconomic position on cause specific mortality: The Oslo Mortality Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57:40–45. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.1.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. The Cochrane Collaboration. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions 4.2.5. 2005 May; Retrieved June 1, 2006, from http://www.cochrane.org/resources/handbook/handbook.pdf.
  42. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Turner RB, Alper CM, Skoner DR. Emotional style and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003a;65:652–657. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000077508.57784.da. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Turner RB, Alper CM, Skoner DR. Sociability and susceptibility to the common cold. Psychological Science. 2003b;14:389–395. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:175–199. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Contrada RJ, Cather C, O’Leary A. Personality and health: Dispositions and processes in disease susceptibility and adaptation to illness. In: Pervin LA, John OR, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. pp. 576–604. [Google Scholar]
  46. Cook KW, Vance CA, Spector RE. The relation of candidate personality with selection-interview outcomes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30:867–885. [Google Scholar]
  47. Curtis S, Southall H, Congdon P, Dodgeon B. Area effects on health variation over the life-course: Analysis of the longitudinal study sample in England using new data on are of residence in childhood. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58:57–74. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(03)00149-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Danner DD, Snowden DA, Friesen WV. Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the Nun Study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:804–813. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Davey-Smith G, Hart C, Blane D, Hole D. Adverse socioeconomic conditions in childhood and cause specific adult mortality: Prospective observational study. British Medical Journal. 1998;316:1631–1635. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7145.1631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Deary IJ, Batty D, Gottfredson LS. Human hierarchies, health, and IQ: Comment. Science. 2005;309:703. doi: 10.1126/science.309.5735.703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Deary IJ, Der G. Reaction time explains IQ’s association with death. Psychological Science. 2005;16:64–69. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00781.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. De Fruyt F, Denollet J. Type D personality: A five-factor model perspective. Psychology and Health. 2002;17:671–683. [Google Scholar]
  53. De Fruyt F, Van Leeuwen K, Bagby RM, Rolland J, Rouillon F. Assessing and interpreting personality change and continuity in patients treated for major depression. Psychological Assessment. 2006;18:71–80. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Denollet J, Sys SU, Stroobant N, Rombouts H, Gillebert TC, Brutsaert DL. Personality as independent predictor of long-term mortality in patients with coronary heart disease. Lancet. 1996;347:417–421. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)90007-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Bryant CM. The Big Five and enduring marriages. Journal of Research in Personality. 2004;38:481–504. [Google Scholar]
  56. Doornbos G, Kromhout D. Educational level and mortality in a 32-year follow-up study of 18-year old men in the Netherlands. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1990;19:374–379. doi: 10.1093/ije/19.2.374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Eid M, Diener E. Handbook of psychological assessment: A multimethod perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  58. Erez A, Judge TA. Relationship of core self-evaluations to goal setting, motivation, and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2001;86:1270–1279. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.6.1270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Everson SA, Kauhanen J, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Julkunen J, Tuomilehto J, Salonen JT. Hostility and increased risk of mortality and acute myocardial infarction: The mediating role of behavioral risk factors. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1997;146:142–152. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Shannon FT. A proportional hazards model of family breakdown. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1984;46:539–549. [Google Scholar]
  61. Fiscella K, Franks P. Individual income, income inequality, health, and mortality: What are the relationships? Health Services Research. 2000;35:307–318. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Friedman HS. Long-term relations of personality and health: Dynamisms, mechanisms, tropisms. Journal of Personality. 2000;68:1089–1108. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Friedman HS, Tucker JS, Tomlinson-Keasey C, Schwartz JE, Wingard DL, Criqui MH. Does childhood personality predict longevity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:176–185. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.1.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Funder DC. The personality puzzle. New York: Norton; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  65. Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Behavior as a function of the situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;44:107–112. [Google Scholar]
  66. Gallo LC, Matthews KA. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:10–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Smith GD. Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality in adulthood: Systematic review and interpretation. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2004;26:7–21. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Mulsant BH. Rates and predictors of mortality in an aging, rural, community-based cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2002;59:1046–1052. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.11.1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Giltay EJ, Geleijnse JM, Zitman EG, Hoekstra T, Schouten EG. Dispositional optimism and all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in a prospective cohort of elderly Dutch men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:1126–1135. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Goldberg LR. The structure of personality traits: Vertical and horizontal aspects. In: Funder DC, Parke RD, Tomlinson-Keasey C, Widaman K, editors. Studying lives through time: Personality and development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1993a. pp. 169–188. [Google Scholar]
  71. Goldberg LR. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist. 1993b;48:26–34. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Gottfredson GD, Jones EM, Holland JL. Personality and vocational interests: The relation of Holland’s six interest dimensions to five robust dimensions of personality. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1993;40:518–524. [Google Scholar]
  73. Gottman JM. What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  74. Gottman JM, Coan J, Carrere S, Swanson C. Predicting marital happiness and stability from newlywed interactions. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1998;60:5–22. [Google Scholar]
  75. Grossarth-Maticek R, Bastianns J, Kanazir DT. Psychosocial factors as strong predictors of mortality from cancer, ischaemic heart disease and stroke: The Yugoslav prospective study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1985;29:167–176. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90038-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Haller AO, Portes A. Status attainment processes. Sociology of Education. 1973;46:51–91. [Google Scholar]
  77. Hardarson T, Gardarsdottir M, Gudmundsson KT, Thorgeirsson G, Sigvaldason H, Sigfusson N. The relationship between educational level and mortality: The Reykjavik Study. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2001;249:495–502. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2796.2001.00834.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Harms PD, Roberts BW, Winter D. Becoming the Harvard man: Person-environment fit, personality development, and academic success. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:851–865. doi: 10.1177/0146167206287720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hart CL, Taylor MD, Davey-Smith G, Whalley LJ, Starr JM, Hole DJ, et al. Childhood IQ, social class, deprivation, and their relationships with mortality and morbidity risk in later life: Prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan Studies. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65:877–883. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000088584.82822.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hauser RM, Tsai S, Sewell WH. A model of stratification with response error in social and psychological variables. Sociology of Education. 1983;56:20–46. [Google Scholar]
  81. Hearn MD, Murray DM, Luepker RV. Hostility, coronary heart disease, and total mortality: A 33-year follow-up study of university students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1989;12:105–121. doi: 10.1007/BF00846545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  83. Helson R. [Unpublished data from the Mills Longitudinal Study] Berkeley: University of California; 2006. Unpublished raw data. [Google Scholar]
  84. Helson R, Roberts BW. Personality of young adult couples and wives’ work patterns. Journal of Personality. 1992;60:575–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00921.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Helson R, Roberts BW, Agronick G. Enduringness and change in creative personality and the prediction of occupational creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:1173–1183. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Hemphill JF. Interpreting the magnitude of correlation coefficients. American Psychologist. 2003;58:78–79. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.58.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Heslop P, Smith GD, Macleod J, Hart C. The socioeconomic position of employed women, risk factors and mortality. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;53:477–485. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00350-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Hirokawa K, Nagata C, Takatsuka N, Shimizu H. The relationships of a rationality/antiemotionality personality scale to mortalities of cancer and cardiovascular disease in a community population in Japan. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;56:103–111. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00046-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Hogan J, Holland B. Using theory to evaluate personality and job-performance relations: A socioanalytic perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2003;88:100–112. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Holley P, Yabiku S, Benin M. The relationship between intelligence and divorce. Journal of Family Issues. 2006;27:1723–1748. [Google Scholar]
  91. Hollis JF, Connett JE, Stevens VJ, Greenlick MR. Stressful life events, Type A behavior, and the prediction of cardiovascular and total mortality over six years. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1990;13:263–280. doi: 10.1007/BF00846834. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Hosegood V, Campbell OMR. Body mass index, height, weight, arm circumference, and mortality in rural Bangladeshi women: A 19-year longitudinal study. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003;77:341–347. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/77.2.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Hurtz GM, Donovan JJ. Personality and job performance: The Big Five revisited. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2000;85:869–879. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.6.869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Huston TL, Caughlin JP, Houts RM, Smith SE, George LJ. The connubial crucible: Newlywed years as predictors of marital delight, distress, and divorce. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:237–252. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Iribarren C, Jacobs DR, Kiefe CI, Lewis CE, Matthews KA, Roseman JM, Hulley SB. Causes and demographic, medical, lifestyle and psychosocial predictors of premature mortality: the CARDIA study. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;60:471–482. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jalovaara M. Socio-economic status and divorce in first marriages in Finland 1991–1993. Population Studies. 2001;55:119–133. [Google Scholar]
  97. Jencks C, Crouse J, Mueser P. The Wisconsin model of status attainment: A national replication with improved measures of ability and aspiration. Sociology of Education. 1983;56:3–19. [Google Scholar]
  98. Jensen-Campbell LA, Graziano WG. Agreeableness as a moderator of interpersonal conflict. Journal of Personality. 2001;69:323–361. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Jockin V, McGue M, Lykken DT. Personality and divorce: A genetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:288–299. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Johnson W, Krueger RF. How money buys happiness: Genetic and environmental processes linking finances and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;90:680–691. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Judge TA, Bono JE, Ilies R, Gerhardt MW. Personality and leadership: A qualitative and quantitative review. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2002;87:765–780. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Judge TA, Higgins CA, Thoresen CJ, Barrick MR. The big five personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span. Personnel Psychology. 1999;52:621–652. [Google Scholar]
  103. Judge TA, Ilies R. Relationship of personality to performance motivation: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2002;87:797–807. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Kaplan GA, Wilson TW, Cohen RD, Kauhanen J, Wu M, Salonen JT. Social functioning and overall mortality: Prospective evidence from the Kuopio Ischemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study. Epidemiology. 1994;5:495–500. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory, methods, and research. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:3–34. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Kelly EL, Conley JJ. Personality and compatibility: A prospective analysis of marital stability and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52:27–40. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Kenford SL, Smith SS, Wetter DW, Jorenby DE, Fiore MC, Baker TB. Predicting relapse back to smoking: Contrasting affective and physical models of dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:216–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Khang Y, Kim HR. Explaining socioeconomic inequality in mortality among South Koreans: An examination of multiple pathways in a nationally representative longitudinal study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;34:630–637. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Kinnunen U, Pulkkinen L. Childhood socio-emotional characteristics as antecedents of marital stability and quality. European Psychologist. 2003;8:223–237. [Google Scholar]
  110. Kokko K, Bergman LR, Pulkkinen L. Child personality characteristics and selection into long-term unemployment in Finnish and Swedish longitudinal samples. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2003;27:134–144. [Google Scholar]
  111. Kokko K, Pulkkinen L. Aggression in childhood and long-term unemployment in adulthood: A cycle of maladaptation and some protective factors. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:463–472. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.4.463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Korten AE, Jorm AF, Jaio Z, Letenneur L, Jacomb PA, Henderson AS, et al. Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 1999;53:83–88. doi: 10.1136/jech.53.2.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Koskenvuo M, Kaprio J, Rose RJ, Kesaniemi A, Sarna S, Heikkila K, Langinvainio H. Hostility as a risk factor for mortality and ischemic heart disease in men. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1988;50:330–340. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198807000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Kuh D, Hardy R, Langenberg C, Richards M, Wadsworth MEJ. Mortality in adults aged 26–54 years related to socioeconomic conditions in childhood and adulthood: Post war birth cohort study. British Medical Journal. 2002;325:1076–1080. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7372.1076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Kuh D, Maclean M. Women’s childhood experience of parental separation and their subsequent health and socioeconomic status in adulthood. Journal of Biosocial Science. 1990;22:121–135. doi: 10.1017/s0021932000018435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Kuh D, Richards M, Hardy R, Butterworth S, Wadsworth MEJ. Childhood cognitive ability and deaths up until middle age: A post-war birth cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;33:408–413. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Kuncel NR, Crede M, Thomas LL. A comprehensive meta-analysis of the predictive validity of the Graduate Management Admission Test (GMAT) and undergraduate grade point average (UGPA) Academy of Management Learning and Education. 2007;6:51–68. [Google Scholar]
  118. Kuncel NR, Hezlett SA, Ones DS. A comprehensive meta-analysis of the predictive validity of the Graduate Record Examinations: Implications for graduate student selection and performance. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:162–181. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Kuncel NR, Hezlett SA, Ones DS. Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2004;86:148–161. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Kurdek LA. Predicting marital dissolution: A 5-year prospective longitudinal study of newlywed couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;64:221–242. [Google Scholar]
  121. Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Williams DR, Mero RP, Chen J. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1998;279:1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.21.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Lawrence E, Bradbury TN. Physical aggression and marital dysfunction: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Family Psychology. 2001;15:135–154. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.15.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Lee WE, Wadsorth MEJ, Hotopf M. The protective role of trait anxiety: S longitudinal cohort study. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36:345–351. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. LePine JA, Hollenbeck JR, Ilgen DR, Hedlund J. Effects of individual differences on the performance of hierarchical decision-making teams: Much more than g . Journal of Applied Psychology. 1997;82:803–811. [Google Scholar]
  125. Lewis M. Issues in the study of personality development. Psychological Inquiry. 2001;12:67–83. [Google Scholar]
  126. Loeb J. The personality factor in divorce. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 1966;30:562. doi: 10.1037/h0024025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Lund R, Holstein BE, Osler M. Marital history from age 15 to 40 years and subsequent 10-year mortality: A longitudinal study of Danish males born in 1953. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;33:389–397. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Luster T, McAdoo H. Family and child influences on educational attainment: A secondary analysis of the High/Scope Perry preschool data. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32:26–39. [Google Scholar]
  129. Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Cohen RD, Kauhanen J, Wilson TW, Smith NL, Salonen JT. Childhood and adult socioeconomic status as predictors of mortality in Finland. Lancet. 1994;343:424–527. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(94)91468-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Maier H, Smith J. Psychological predictors of mortality in old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 1999;54B:44–54. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54b.1.p44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Martin LT, Friedman HS. Comparing personality scales across time: An illustrative study of validity and consistency in life-span archival data. Journal of Personality. 2000;68:85–110. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Martin LT, Kubzansky LD. Childhood cognitive performance and risk of mortality: A prospective cohort study of gifted individuals. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;162:887–890. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Maruta T, Colligan RC, Malinchoc M, Offord KP. Optimists vs. pessimists: Survival rate among medical patients over a 30-year period. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 2000;75:140–143. doi: 10.4065/75.2.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Maruta T, Hamburgen ME, Jennings CA, Offord KP, Colligan RC, Frye RL, Malinchoc M. Keeping hostility in perspective: Coronary heart disease and the hostility scale on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Mayo Clinic Proceedings. 1993;68:109–114. doi: 10.1016/s0025-6196(12)60156-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Mayer JD. A tale of two visions: Can a new view of personality help integrate psychology? American Psychologist. 2005;60:294–307. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.4.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. McCarron P, Gunnell D, Harrison GL, Okasha M, Davey-Smith G. Temperament in young adulthood and later mortality: Prospective observational study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57:888–892. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.11.888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. McCranie EW, Kahan J. Personality and multiple divorce. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1986;174:161–164. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198603000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. McCrainie EW, Watkins LO, Brandsma JM, Sisson BD. Hostility, coronary heart disease (CHD) incidence, and total mortality: Lack of association in a 25-year follow-up study of 478 physicians. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1986;9:119–125. doi: 10.1007/BF00848472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Meehl RE. High school yearbooks: A reply to Schwarz. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1971;77:143–148. doi: 10.1037/h0031999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Mershon B, Gorsuch RL. Number of factors in personality sphere: Does increase in factors increase predictability of real life criteria? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;55:675–680. [Google Scholar]
  141. Meyer GJ, Finn SE, Eyde LD, Kay GG, Moreland KL, Dies RR, et al. Psychological testing and psychological assessment. American Psychologist. 2001;56:128–165. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Miller GE, Cohen S, Rabin BS, Skoner DR, Doyle WJ. Personality and tonic cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and immune parameters. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 1999;13:109–123. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1998.0545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Mischel W. Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  144. Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Personality change influences mortality in older men. Psychological Science. 2007;18:371–376. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01907.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Murberg TA, Bru E, Aarsland T. Personality as predictor of mortality among patients with congestive heart failure: A two-year follow-up study. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:749–757. [Google Scholar]
  146. Myers DG. The American paradox: Spiritual hunger in an age of plenty. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  147. Orbuch TL, Veroff J, Hassan H, Horrocks J. Who will divorce: A 14-year longitudinal study of black couples and white couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2002;19:179–202. [Google Scholar]
  148. Osler M, Andersen AN, Due P, Lund R, Damsgaard MT, Holstein BE. Socioeconomic position in early life, birth weight, childhood cognitive function, and adult mortality. A longitudinal study of Danish men born in 1953. Journal of Epidemiology Community Health. 2003;57:681–686. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.9.681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Osler M, Prescott E, Gronbaek M, Christensen U, Due P, Enghorn G. Income inequality, individual income, and mortality in Danish adults: Analysis of pooled data from two cohort studies. British Medical Journal. 2002;324:1–4. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7328.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Ozer DJ, Benet-Martinez V. Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:401–421. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Paul AM. The cult of personality. New York: Free Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  152. Paunonen SV. Hierarchical organization of personality and prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:538–556. [Google Scholar]
  153. Paunonen SV, Ashton MC. Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:524–539. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Pervin LA, John OP. Handbook of personality theory and research. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  155. Peterson C, Seligman MER, Yurko KH, Martin LR, Friedman HS. Catastrophizing and untimely death. Psychological Science. 1998;2:127–130. [Google Scholar]
  156. Peterson RA, Brown SR. On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2005;90:175–181. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131:925–971. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Pudaric S, Sundquist J, Johansson S. Country of birth, instrumental activities of daily living, self-rated health and mortality: A Swedish population-based survey of people aged 55–74. Social Science and Medicine. 2003;56:2439–2503. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00284-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Rapee RM, Kennedy S, Ingram M, Edwards S, Sweeney L. Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73:488–497. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Reiss HT, Capobianco A, Tsai FT. Finding the person in personal relationships. Journal of Personality. 2002;70:813–850. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.05025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Roberts BW. Blessings, banes, and possibilities in the study of childhood personality. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 2005;51:367–378. [Google Scholar]
  162. Roberts BW. Personality development and organizational behavior. In: Staw BM, editor. Research on organizational behavior. Greenwich, CT: Elsevier Science/JAI Press; 2006. pp. 1–41. [Google Scholar]
  163. Roberts BW, Bogg T. A 30-year longitudinal study of the relationships between conscientiousness-related traits, and the family structure and health-behavior factors that affect health. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:325–354. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00264.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt T. Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:582–593. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Roberts BW, Robins RW. Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: The intersection of the Big Five dimensions and major life goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2000;26:1284–1296. [Google Scholar]
  166. Roberts BW, Walton K, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:1–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Roberts BW, Wood D. Personality development in the context of the neo-socioanalytic model of personality. In: Mroczek D, Little T, editors. Handbook of personality development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  168. Robins RW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Two personalities, one relationship: Both partners’ personality traits shape the quality of a relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:251–259. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Robins RW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. It’s not just who you’re with, it’s who you are: Personality and relationship experiences across multiple relationships. Journal of Personality. 2002;70:925–964. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.05028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Roisman GE, Masten AS, Coatsworth D, Tellegen A. Salient and emerging developmental tasks in the transition to adulthood. Child Development. 2004;75:123–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00658.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Rosenthal R. How are we doing in soft psychology. American Psychologist. 1990;45:775–777. [Google Scholar]
  172. Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Rev. ed. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  173. Rosenthal R. Effect sizes in behavioral and biomedical research. In: Bicman L, editor. Validity and social experimentation: Don Campbell’s legacy. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 2000. pp. 121–139. [Google Scholar]
  174. Rosenthal R, Rubin DB. Requivalent: A simple effect size indicator. Psychological Methods. 2003;8:492–496. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Ross L, Nisbett RE. The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Company; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  176. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development. 6th ed. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 99–166. (Vol. Ed.) [Google Scholar]
  177. Rozanski A, Blumenthal JA, Kaplan J. Impact of psychological factors on the pathogenesis of cardiovascular disease and implications for therapy. Circulation. 1999;99:2192–2217. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.99.16.2192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Sapolsky RM. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science. 2005;308:648–652. doi: 10.1126/science.1106477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Sarason IG, Smith RE, Diener E. Personality research: Components of variance attributable to the person and the situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1975;32:199–204. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.32.2.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Saucier G. An alternative multi-language structure for personality attributes. European Journal of Personality. 2003;76:179–205. [Google Scholar]
  181. Schaie KW. The course of adult intellectual development. American Psychologist. 1994;49:304–313. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.49.4.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Scheier MF, Carver CS. On the power of positive thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1993;2:26–30. [Google Scholar]
  183. Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:262–274. [Google Scholar]
  184. Schulz R, Bookwala J, Knapp JE, Scheier M, Williamson GM. Pessimism, age, and cancer mortality. Psychology and Aging. 1996;11:304–309. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.2.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Seibert SE, Kraimer ML, Crant JM. What do proactive people do? A longitudinal model linking proactive personality and career success. Personnel Psychology. 2001;54:845–874. [Google Scholar]
  186. Sewell WH, Haller AO, Portes A. The educational and early occupational process. American Sociological Review. 1969;34:82–92. [Google Scholar]
  187. Sewell WH, Hauser RM. The Wisconsin longitudinal study of social and psychological factors in aspirations and achievements. Research in Sociology of Education and Socialization. 1980;1:59–101. [Google Scholar]
  188. Sewell WH, Hauser RM. The influence of The American Occupational Structure on the Wisconsin model. Contemporary Sociology. 1992;21:598–603. [Google Scholar]
  189. Shiner RL. An emerging developmental science of personality: Current progress and future prospects. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2005;51:379–387. [Google Scholar]
  190. Shipley BA, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Cognition and all-cause mortality across the entire adult age range: Health and lifestyle survey. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:17–24. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195867.66643.0f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Skolnick A. Married lives: Longitudinal perspectives on marriage. In: Eichorn DH, Clausen JA, Haan N, Honzik MP, Mussen PH, editors. Present and past in midlife. New York: Academic Press; 1981. pp. 270–300. [Google Scholar]
  192. Smith AW, Meitz JEG. Vanishing supermoms and other trends in marital dissolution, 1969–1978. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1985;47:53–65. [Google Scholar]
  193. Smith TW. Personality as risk and resilience in physical health. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2006;15:227–231. [Google Scholar]
  194. Smith TW, Gallo L. Personality traits as risk factors for physical illness. In: Baum A, Evenson T, Singer J, editors. Handbook of health psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. pp. 139–174. [Google Scholar]
  195. Snyder M, Stukas A. Interpersonal processes: The interplay of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activities in social interaction. Annual Review of Psychology. 1999;50:273–303. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Steenland K, Henley J, Thun M. All-cause and cause-specific death rates by educational status for two million people in two American Cancer Society cohorts, 1959–1996. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;156:11–21. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. St. John PD, Montgomery PR, Kristjansson B, McDowell I. Cognitive scores, even with the normal range, predict death and institutionalization. Age and Ageing. 2002;31:373–378. doi: 10.1093/ageing/31.5.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Suls J, Martin R. The daily life of the garden-variety neurotic: Reactivity, stressor exposure, mood spillover, and maladaptive coping. Journal of Personality. 2005;73:1485–1509. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00356.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Luben R, Day NE, Khaw K. Prospective cohort study of hostility and the risk of cardiovascular disease mortality. International Journal of Cardiology. 2005;100:155–161. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Luben R, Khaw K, Day NE. Sense of coherence and mortality in men and women in the EPIC-Norfolk United Kingdom prospective cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;158:1202–1209. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Taylor MD, Hart CL, Davey-Smith G, Whalley LJ, Hole DJ, Wilson V, Deary IJ. Childhood IQ and marriage by mid-life: The Scottish Mental Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38:1621–1630. [Google Scholar]
  202. Tenconi MT, Devoti G, Comelli M RIFLE Research Group. Role of socioeconomic indicators in the prediction of all causes and coronary heart disease mortality in over 12,000 men—The Italian RIFLE pooling project. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2000;16:565–571. doi: 10.1023/a:1007640424387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Tharenou P. Going up? Do traits and informal social processes predict advancing in management? Academy of Management Journal. 2001;44:1005–1017. [Google Scholar]
  204. Tucker JS, Kressin NR, Spiro A, Ruscio J. Intrapersonal characteristics and the timing of divorce: A prospective investigation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 1998;15:211–225. [Google Scholar]
  205. Tzeng JM, Mare RD. Labor market and socioeconomic effects on marital stability. Social Science Research. 1995;24:329–351. [Google Scholar]
  206. Vagero D, Leon D. Effect of social class in childhood and adulthood on adult mortality. Lancet. 1994;343:1224–1225. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(94)92432-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Watson D, Hubbard B, Wiese D. General traits of personality and affectivity as predictors of satisfaction in intimate relationships: Evidence from self-and partner-ratings. Journal of Personality. 2000;68:413–449. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Weiss A, Costa PT. Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among Medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:1–10. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181272.58103.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Whalley LJ, Deary IJ. Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. British Medical Journal. 2001;322:1–5. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7290.819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA, Bennet DA. Negative affect and mortality in older persons. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;9:827–835. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Gu L, Bienas JL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Neuroticism, extraversion, and mortality in a defined population of older persons. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:841–845. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000190615.20656.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  212. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Personality and mortality in old age. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2004;59:110–116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.3.p110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Wrzesniewski A, Dutton JE. Crafting a job: Revisioning employees as active crafters of their work. Academy of Management Review. 2001;26:179–201. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES