Skip to main content
Journal of Physical Therapy Science logoLink to Journal of Physical Therapy Science
. 2015 Jun 30;27(6):1823–1825. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.1823

Effects of chewing gum on driving performance as evaluated by the STISIM driving simulator

Ingyu Yoo 1, Eun-Joo Kim 1,*, Joo-Hyun Lee 2
PMCID: PMC4499992  PMID: 26180329

Abstract

[Purpose] The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of chewing gum on driving performance in a driving simulator. [Subjects] In total, 26 young licensed drivers participated. [Methods] The driving scenario was typical of an urban environment: a single-carriageway, two-way road consisting of a mix of curved and straight sections, with considerable levels of traffic, pedestrians, and parked cars. Mean distance driven above the speed limit, lane position, mean distance driven across the center line, and mean distance driven off the road were used as estimates of brake, accelerator, and steering control. The results were compared with those of a non-chewing gum control condition. [Results] The driving performance while chewing gum was significantly better: the mean distance driven above the speed limit was 26.61% shorter, and the mean distance driven off the road was 31.99% shorter. Lane position and mean distance driven across the center line did not differ significantly between the two conditions. [Conclusion] Chewing gum appears to enhance driving performance during a sustained attention driving task.

Key words: Attention, Driver, Performance

INTRODUCTION

Generally, driving requires a high level of arousal for awareness and responsiveness1). However, cognitive function tends to decline with age, which can affect the ability to drive safely. The accident rate per mile driven among drivers over the age of 65 has been found to be higher than that among middle-aged drivers, and when injured in a collision, these older drivers are also more likely to die or to sustain serious injuries2). Cognitive psychologists have demonstrated that the anterior attention system plays a major role in higher-level executive control of attention during more complex cognitive tasks, such as problem-solving and decision-making, especially when multi-tasking3). Interestingly, chewing gum has recently been reported to enhance cognitive performance4). For example, Stephens and Tunney5) provided evidence that chewing gum increases scores in immediate, delayed recall, and working memory tests. Similarly, Johnson and Miles6) confirmed that chewing gum during learning and/or recall improves subsequent memory. The cognitive processes improved by chewing have been systematically investigated and include memory, attention, and executive function5). Specifically, gum-chewing during cognitive testing has been found to improve verbal working memory and immediate episodic long-term memory, sustained attention and language-based attention. The results for gum-chewing’s effects on word-association executive function are conflicting7). On the basis of these previous reports, we hypothesized that chewing gum would improve sustained attention and executive function. Thus, the purpose of this study was to determine the effects of chewing gum on driving performance. To assess this, subjects performed simulated driving tasks while chewing gum and then without.

SUBJECTS AND METHODS

The subjects comprised 26 young drivers (10 females) who held valid driving licenses (mean driving distance, 87,200 ± 31,134 km/year; possession of driver’s license, 3.40 ± 2.20 years). All subjects were informed of the purpose and methods of the study prior to participation and provided their informed consent, following the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Participant information was obtained by interview and self-report. Exclusion criteria were (i) inability to chew gum for a period of approximately 30 min, (ii) age younger than 18 or older than 40 years, (iii) consumption of more than 40 units of alcohol per week, (iv) smoking more than 10 cigarettes per day, (v) currently taking medication, (vi) currently experiencing medical problems (including dental problems) or serious medical conditions, and (vii) allergies to gum6). A STISIM Drive M400 driving simulator (System Technology Inc., Hawthorne, CA, USA) was used to provide subjects with various driving environments and to estimate their driving performance. The simulator consists of a car unit with adjustable car seats with a dashboard, a steering wheel, turn signals, and brake and gas pedals, and provides a 135° field of view of a simulated environment. Realistic roadway scenery is projected on a wide computer monitor, placed in front of the center of the steering wheel. Speed and gear numbers are displayed on the screen. Auditory feedback is provided through speakers and includes the sound of the engine, braking, speeding around curves, wheels, and driving off the road. Before assessment in the simulator, subjects practiced for 5 min. If a participant demonstrated comfortable performance in the driving trial, he or she was allowed to participate in the test trials. A 3-day break was provided after each trial: not chewing gum and chewing spearmint sugar-free gum. The order of the conditions was randomized. In the chewing-gum condition, participants chewed gum at their normal speed (1 Hz) during driving. In the control condition, no gum was provided, and no chewing took place. The ~30-min driving scenario consisted of a two-lane highway in each direction, with a lane width of 3.5 m. Subjects were instructed to drive in a steady lateral position in the right (slower) traffic lane, following a lead vehicle, while maintaining a steady speed of 80 km/h. Participants were required to maintain speed limits, comply with traffic rules, and negotiate surrounding traffic. The scenario presented a two-lane road with horizontal curves. Mean distance driven above the speed limit, lane position, mean distance driven across the center line, and mean distance driven off the road were estimated by the simulator equipment and used as estimates of the driver’s abilities of brake, accelerator, and steering control. Differences in driving performance between the no-chewing condition and the chewing gum condition and between genders were assessed using the paired t-test. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test confirmed the data had a normal distribution; therefore, parametric statistics were used. A p value < 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance in all tests.

RESULTS

While chewing gum, drivers drove significantly shorter distances at speeds exceeding the speed limit (26.61% shorter) and off the road (31.99% shorter) than those while not chewing (p < 0.05). However, lane position and mean distance driven across the center line did not differ significantly (p > 0.05; Table 1), and there was no effect of gender in either condition.

Table 1. Effect of gum-chewing on driving capacity (N=26).

Chewing gum condition (mean ± SD)

No gum Chewing gum
Mean distance driven above speed limit (m) 609.9 ± 190.9 456.5 ± 166.9*
Lane position (m) 6.79 ± 0.46 6.81 ± 0.54
Mean distance driven across center line (m) 59.2 ± 34.3 43.5 ± 32.9
Mean distance driven off road (m) 127.0 ± 77.5 86.4 ± 47.9*

*p < 0.05

DISCUSSION

Safe driving demands the capacity to concentrate and to divide attention over multiple sensory events, including visual and auditory modalities; it also requires high-level cognitive decisions in a rapidly changing environment8). In other words, driving skills require the simultaneous integration of many types of information while coordinating movements3). Simulated driving studies indicate that measures of driving behavior, such as speed, lane position, and time/distance across the center line and off the road edge, correlate with a driver’s comfort with the given driving conditions. Driving is a complex task requiring appropriate responses to hazardous situations on the road, and research has shown that attention and executive function correlate with driving status. The STISIM driving simulator is widely used to examine the driving abilities of drivers with poor performance9). This study assessed mean speed, mean distance driven above the speed limit, lane position, mean distance driven across the center line, and mean distance driven off the road edge as proxies for driving performance. Chewing gum significantly reduced the distances above the speed limit and off the road. Previous studies have indicated that chewing gum during specific tasks improves reaction times10) and has positive impacts on activities requiring cognitive function. Gum chewing may improve cognitive function by promoting regional cerebral blood flow and glucose delivery and by increasing the efficiency of attention and concentration11, 12). Poor performance in a task requiring divided attention is frequently observed in adults with dementia, and previous studies have shown that divided attention problems also exist in many individuals with mild cognitive impairment13). Cognitively impaired older drivers often do not intend to restrict or stop driving in the future, and do not consider their cognitive problems to be serious; thus, gum-chewing may reduce the risk of accidents among these individuals while their impairments remain mild3). Our conclusion is that chewing gum enhances alertness and attention while driving, because it makes drivers feel more comfortable during driving activities. Also, this study has provided further evidence that driving simulators can be used to assess responses to road hazards in a safe environment. Driving simulator recordings have previously been used to assess age-related changes in driving performance and cognition14), and to predict potential future crashes by older drivers15). Further research may clarify the mechanism by which chewing gum improves performance in a task, such as driving, that requires sustained attention and executive function.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Hu TY, Xie X, Li J: Negative or positive? The effect of emotion and mood on risky driving. Transp Res, Part F Traffic Psychol Behav, 2013, 16: 29–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carr DB, Meuser TM, Morris JC: Driving retirement: the role of the physician. CMAJ, 2006, 175: 601–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kowalski K, Love J, Tuokko H, et al. : The influence of cognitive impairment with no dementia on driving restriction and cessation in older adults. Accid Anal Prev, 2012, 49: 308–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tucha O, Mecklinger L, Maier K, et al. : Chewing gum differentially affects aspects of attention in healthy subjects. Appetite, 2004, 42: 327–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stephens R, Tunney RJ: Role of glucose in chewing gum-related facilitation of cognitive function. Appetite, 2004, 43: 211–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Johnson AJ, Miles C: Evidence against memorial facilitation and context-dependent memory effects through the chewing of gum. Appetite, 2007, 48: 394–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Onyper SV, Carr TL, Farrar JS, et al. : Cognitive advantages of chewing gum. Now you see them, now you don’t. Appetite, 2011, 57: 321–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Spiers HJ, Maguire EA: Neural substrates of driving behaviour. Neuroimage, 2007, 36: 245–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shechtman O, Classen S, Awadzi K, et al. : Comparison of driving errors between on-the-road and simulated driving assessment: a validation study. Traffic Inj Prev, 2009, 10: 379–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Smith A: Effects of chewing gum on cognitive function, mood and physiology in stressed and non-stressed volunteers. Nutr Neurosci, 2010, 13: 7–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Smith A: Effects of chewing gum on mood, learning, memory and performance of an intelligence test. Nutr Neurosci, 2009, 12: 81–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mehta RK, Parasuraman R: Effects of mental fatigue on the development of physical fatigue: a neuroergonomic approach. Hum Factors, 2014, 56: 645–656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Okonkwo OC, Griffith HR, Belue K, et al. : Cognitive models of medical decision-making capacity in patients with mild cognitive impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc, 2008, 14: 297–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Shin HK, Lee HC: Characteristics of driving reaction time of elderly drivers in the brake pedal task. J Phys Ther Sci, 2012, 24: 567–570. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fukuda T: The influence of aging on balance function in terms of the foot electromyographic reaction time. J Phys Ther Sci, 2012, 24: 191–196. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Physical Therapy Science are provided here courtesy of Society of Physical Therapy Science

RESOURCES